ArticlePDF Available

Exploring the relationship between mathematics anxiety and performance: The role of intrusive thoughts

Authors:

Abstract

The current study examined the relationship between math anxiety and arithmetic performance by focusing on intrusive thoughts experienced during problem solving. Participants (N = 122) performed two-digit addition problems on a verification task. Math anxiety significantly predicted response time and error rate. Further, the extent to which intrusive thoughts impeded calculation mediated the relationship between math anxiety and per cent of errors on problems involving a carry operation. Moreover, results indicated that participants experienced a range of intrusive thoughts and these were related to significantly higher levels of math anxiety. The findings lend support to a deficient inhibition account of the math anxiety-to-performance relationship and highlight the importance of considering intrusive thoughts in future work.
MATH ANXIETY, INTRUSIVE THOUGHTS AND
PERFORMANCE
Exploring the relationship between mathematics anxiety and performance: The role of
intrusive thoughts
Thomas E. Hunt
1
, David Clark-Carter
2
, David
Sheffield
1
1
University of Derby, U.K.
2
Staffordshire University
U.K.
Thomas E. Hunt
Department of Psychology,
Faculty of Education, Health & Sciences,
University of Derby, Derby,U.K.
t.hunt@derby.ac.uk
+(44) 1332 592015
Abstract -The current study examined the relationship between math anxiety and arithmetic performance by focusing on intrusive
thoughts experienced during problem solving. Participants (N = 122) performed two-digit addition problems on a verification task.
Math anxiety significantly predicted response time and error rate. Further, the extent to which intrusive thoughts impeded calculation
mediated the relationship between math anxiety and per cent of errors on problems involving a carry operation. Moreover, results
indicated that participants experienced a range of intrusive thoughts and these were related to significantly higher levels of math
anxiety. The findings lend support to a deficient inhibition account of the math anxiety-to-performance relationship and highlight the
importance of considering intrusive thoughts in future work.
Key Words: arithmetic performance; cognitive intrusions; intrusive thoughts; math anxiety
I. INTRODUCTION
Mathematics anxiety can be described as “a feeling of
tension and anxiety that interferes with the manipulation of
numbers and the solving of mathematical problems in a wide
variety of ordinary life and academic situations” (Richardson &
Suinn, 1972, p.551). It is thought to affect a large number of
people (Ashcraft & Moore, 2009), is experienced in children
(Wu, Barth, Amin, Malcarne & Menon, 2012) and adults
(Ashcraft, 2002), and extends to a range of contexts, such as
nursing (McMullan, Jones & Lea, 2012) and consumer
behaviour (Jones, Childers & Jiang, 2012).
There is now substantial empirical evidence to suggest that
math anxiety is negatively correlated with overall math
performance (Ashcraft & Moore, 2009; Hembree, 1990; Ma,
1999). Specifically, research has demonstrated that math
anxiety is more consistently and negatively related to
performance on complex, compared to simple arithmetic (e.g.,
Ashcraft & Faust, 1994), and particularly when a problem
involves a carry operation (Faust, Ashcraft & Fleck, 1996).
However, the mechanisms underlying these relationships are
poorly understood. One possible mechanism linking math
anxiety to performance is intrusive or worrisome thoughts.
Tendency to experience intrusive thoughts has been found to
correlate with performance on a range of cognitive tasks
(Munoz, Sliwinski, Smyth, Almeida & King, 2013) and,
according to processing efficiency theory (Eysenck & Calvo,
1992), on which the more recent attentional control theory
(Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos & Calvo, 2007) is based,
worrisome thoughts interfere with the limited resources
available within the working memory system. Ashcraft and
Krause (2007) propose that preoccupation with one’s fears and
anxieties pertaining to math may act as a ‘secondary task’,
resulting in the depletion of resources necessary for arithmetic
task completion. Related to this, Hopko, Ashcraft, Gute,
Ruggiero & Lewis (1998) argued that high math anxious
individuals may have difficulty in inhibiting attention towards
intrusive or worrisome thoughts. Inhibition theory (Hasher &
Zacks, 1988; Connelly, Hasher & Zacks, 1991) proposes that
there is a mechanism for suppressing, or inhibiting, task-
irrelevant distracters. If this mechanism is not working
adequately then task-irrelevant information may interfere with
working memory processes and consequently result in poor
performance. Indeed, Hopko et al (1998) demonstrated that
high and medium math anxious groups took significantly
longer than a low math anxious group to read through text that
included irrelevant information, suggesting poor inhibitory
control. Similarly, Hopko, McNeil, Gleason and Rabalais
(2002) found that response times of high math anxious
individuals were longer on a card counting task with numeric
stimuli, compared to a card-counting task involving letters,
again implicating inhibitory mechanisms related to working
memory.
Whilst previous math anxiety research has made reference
to worrisome or irrelevant thoughts, surprisingly little research
has attempted to examine the influence of worrisome thoughts
on arithmetic performance. In an early study, Hunsley (1987)
measured math anxiety, test anxiety, performance appraisals,
performance attributions and internal dialogue amongst 96
university students before and after midterm statistics
examinations. Participants’ negative internal dialogue was
measured using the Cognitive Interference Questionnaire (CIQ,
Sarason, 1978), which required participants to rate (on a five-
point scale) the frequency with which they experienced
negative thoughts during the exam. Math and test anxiety
Journal of Education, Psychology and Social Sciences
S C I
CI E NT I F I CS
UBLICATIONP
www.sci-pub.com
-- PSYCHOLOGY --
- 69 -
accounted for 15% and 10% of the variance in CIQ scores,
respectively. Further, Beilock, Kulp, Holt and Carr (2004)
found that students performing arithmetic in a high pressure
condition (involving carry operations) had significantly
increased perceptions of performance pressure and reported
thoughts and worries about the high pressure situation and its
consequences, compared to students in a low pressure
condition. More recently, DeCaro, Rotar, Kendra and Beilock
(2010) reported significant negative correlations between task-
related thoughts and arithmetic problem solving accuracy. Such
findings give support to the idea that poor performance may, in
part, be due to insufficient inhibition of intrusive or worrisome
thoughts. However, math anxiety was not measured, so its
relationship with intrusive thoughts and performance remains
unclear.
II. CURRENT STUDY
The current study aimed to test the deficient inhibition
explanation of the math anxiety-to-performance relationship,
by examining the negative internal dialogue that participants
may experience during math performance. Also, we examined
whether the perceived severity of intrusive thoughts
experienced during a math task, was related to math anxiety. It
was hypothesised that there would be a negative relationship
between math anxiety and performance (increased errors and
longer response times) on complex addition problems involving
a carry operation, but no relationship when a carry operation
is not required. Further, it was hypothesised that there would
be a negative relationship between self-reported impact of
intrusive thoughts (and effort to reduce such thoughts) and
performance. In addition, we explored the types of thought that
participants indicated they had during the math task, and their
relations with math anxiety.
III. METHOD
PARTICIPANTS
Participants were 122 (31 men, 91 women) undergraduate
psychology students from two Midlands universities in the
U.K. The sample included participants from all three years of
undergraduate study. Ages ranged from 18 to 51 years (mean =
24.95; SD = 8.76). The mean age is slightly higher than would
be expected from a student sample at traditional universities,
but is consistent with the slightly higher ages of students in
post-1992 universities in the U.K, which make up
approximately half of the number of universities in the U.K
(Universities Colleges and Admissions Service, 2009).
Participants came from an opportunity sample of the general
university population gained via advertising at the universities.
QUESTIONNAIRE MEASURES
The Mathematics Anxiety Scale-U.K. (MAS-U.K., Hunt,
Clark-Carter & Sheffield, 2011) was used to measure math
anxiety. This is a 23-item scale that uses a five-point Likert-
type scale and asks participants to respond how anxious they
would feel in a variety of situations involving math. The scale
has excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .96) and
very high test-retest reliability between four and ten weeks (r =
.89).
In order to measure intrusive thoughts that may occur
during arithmetic items from the Cognitive Intrusions
Questionnaire (Freeston et al., 1991, English translation by
Freeston, 1994) were selected and modified. The first part of
the original questionnaire involves a list of thoughts that
participants are required to endorse if they experienced them
during a preceding task. This was modified so that the list only
contained thoughts related specifically to the math task
undertaken: “making mistakes”, “time pressure”, “method of
problem solving”, “what people would think”, “panicking”,
“previous math experiences”, and “physical changes”. Next
there was a series of five-point Likert-type scale items, relating
to different aspects of the “most worrisome or troubling”
thoughts. Wording of the original items was modified so that
each one related to the math task and pertained to the
following: frequency of the thoughts, difficulty in removing the
thoughts, extent to which the thoughts impeded calculation,
and the amount of effort used to stop/reduce the thoughts. A
final question asked participants to indicate whether they had
experienced intrusive thoughts that were non-math-task related,
for example relationship problems or health problems.
The study also included a measure of trait anxiety, taken
from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorsuch,
Lushene, Vagg & Jacobs, 1984) but, the zero-order correlations
showed no relationship between trait anxiety and error rates
and response time, so analyses including trait anxiety are not
presented here (these are available from the first author).
EXPERIMENTAL STIMULI AND PROCEDURE
Using the experiment-building software E-prime, 80 two-
digit addition problems were presented using a verification
task, for example ‘37 + 18 = 52’. Sixty of these problems had a
solution that was true; 20 had an incorrect solution. Of the 60
true problems, 30 involved a carry operation, for example 17
+ 18 = 35”, and 30 involved no carry, for example “17 + 12 =
29”. Addends were randomly taken from a range of 10-89.
Problem-size was counterbalanced across addends and
carry/no-carry conditions so that performance could be
attributed to factors other than the size of the problems.
Problems where both addends ending in zeros, for example ‘20
+ 30’, or fives, for example ‘25 + 35’, were not included.
Incorrect problems were divided approximately equally with
splits of +/- 1, +/- 3, and +/- 5, counterbalancing the number of
positive and negative splits.
Participants gave informed consent and completed the
mathematics anxiety scale. Stimuli were presented in the centre
of a VDU, in Times New Roman, size 40, bold font. Following
the on-screen instructions and five practice trials, participants
were asked to respond ‘true’ or ‘false’ to the proposed answers.
This was achieved by pressing the ‘z’ and ‘m’ keys on a
keyboard, for ‘true’ and ‘false’, respectively. There was no
time limit for participants to respond. After responding, a pause
screen, consisting of ‘+++++’ appeared, and this remained until
participants pressed one of the keys to proceed to the next trial.
S C I
CI E NT I F I CS
UBLICATIONP
www.sci-pub.com
ISSN: 1339-1488, VOLUME 2, ISSUE 2, 2014
-- PSYCHOLOGY --
- 70 -
Immediately after completion of the arithmetic task,
participants completed the cognitive intrusions questionnaire.
Finally, they were debriefed and thanked.
GENERAL DATA SCREENING AND DIAGNOSTIC CHECKS
Initially participants were asked to select from a list which
non-task related intrusive thoughts they had experienced.
However, the results demonstrated a bi-modal distribution.
Therefore the decision was made to dichotomise the variable
into the levels “yes” (at least one non-task related thought) and
“no” (no non-task related thoughts), to represent experience of
non-task related thoughts.
Visual inspection of histograms of the data showed the data
to be sufficiently univariately normally distributed. For each
regression, normality of standardised residuals was tested by
visual inspection of histograms; these were found to be normal.
Standardised residuals and standardised predicted values were
also plotted against each and no obvious curvilinear
relationships were apparent, with the display also indicating the
presence of homoscedasticity. Checks for bivariate outliers
were also made using scattergraphs and no outliers were
identified. In order to test for multivariate outliers Cook’s
distance and leverage values were plotted against each other;
no cases appeared to obviously deviate from the main cluster of
cases. In addition, checks of tolerance values and variance
proportions indicated that there were no problems with
multicollinearity among the data.
Reliability analyses for the MAS-U.K demonstrated a
Cronbach’s alpha of .94, indicating excellent internal
consistency.
IV. RESULTS
PROBLEM TYPE ANALYSIS
A within-subjects t-test was used to compare the difference
in per cent of errors between problems with a carry term (mean
= 5.13%; SD = 5.72) and problems without (mean = 2.39%;
SD = 3.32). Significantly more errors were made in response to
problems that included a carry term, t(121) = 5.24, p < .001,
two-tailed test, 95% CIs [1.70, 3.77], d = 0.58, indicating a
medium effect (Cohen, 1988). A within-subjects t-test revealed
that participants took significantly longer to respond to
problems including a carry term (mean = 5730.97ms; SD =
1696.32) than to problems not including a carry term (mean =
3835.15ms; SD = 1052.82), t(121) = 20.60, p < .001, two-tailed
test, 95% CIs [1713.60, 2078.04], d = 1.34.
ZERO-ORDER CORRELATIONS
The zero-order correlations between all variables can be
seen in Table I. Math anxiety was significantly positively
correlated with percentage of errors on problems involving a
carry operation but was not correlated with percentage of errors
to no-carry problems. Math anxiety was also significantly
positively correlated with response time to problems with and
without a carry operation.
Math anxiety was very strongly and highly significantly
positively correlated with perceived frequency of the most
troublesome/worrisome thoughts, effort to reduce thoughts and
perceived impedance of thoughts on calculation. Frequency of
the most troublesome/worrisome thoughts, difficulty removing
the thoughts, effort to reduce the thoughts, and perceived
Variable
Carry errors %
No-carry errors %
Carry RT
No-carry RT
Math anxiety total
Freq. Most
troublesome/
worrisome thought
Difficulty removing
thought
Impeding calculation
Effort to reduce
thought
Carry errors %
1
No-carry errors %
.28**
1
Carry RT
.06
.02
1
No-carry RT
.25**
.17
.83***
1
Math anxiety total
.25**
-.00
.30***
.30***
1
Freq. of most troublesome / worrisome
thought
.19*
.03
.23*
.26**
.59***
1
Difficulty removing
.20*
.10
.11
.16
.56***
.62***
1
Impeding calculation
.35***
.04
.25**
.28**
.52***
.57***
.55***
1
Effort to reduce thought
.21*
.05
.22*
.24**
.61***
.69***
.74***
.65***
1
Journal of Education, Psychology and Social Sciences
S C I
CI E NT I F I CS
UBLICATIONP
www.sci-pub.com
-- PSYCHOLOGY --
- 71 -
impediment to calculation were significantly positively related
to percentage of errors to carry-problems. Perceived frequency
of the most troublesome/worrisome thoughts, effort to reduce
thoughts and perceived impedance of thoughts on calculation
were significantly positively correlated with response time to
both carry-problems and no-carry problems. Perceived
frequency of the most troublesome/worrisome thoughts was
strongly and significantly correlated with perceived difficulty
in removing thoughts, effort to reduce the impact of thoughts
and perceived impedance on calculation.
REGRESSION ANALYSES
A series of hierarchical multiple regressions were then
conducted. In all models tested, math anxiety was included in
the first step, followed by variables related to the self-reporting
impact of the most troublesome/worrisome thoughts, including
frequency of the thoughts, difficulty in removing the thoughts,
and impact of the thoughts on the calculation process. Step
three included self-reported effort in reducing the impact of the
thoughts.
PERCENTAGE OF ERRORS ON CARRY PROBLEMS
The final regression model was significant, F(5, 116) =
3.57, p = .005, accounting for 13.3% (Adj R
2
= .096) of the
variance. As shown in Table II, whilst math anxiety was a
significant predictor at step 1, it became non-significant at step
2, remaining non-significant in the final stage. All other
predictor variables were non-significant, with the exception of
the variable impeding calculation, representing the level of
which the most troublesome/worrisome thoughts impeded the
participant’s calculation of the math problems. Impeding
calculation was significantly positively related to percentage of
errors to carry problems and remained so in the final step,
suggesting a mediation effect. The criteria for mediation,
suggested by Baron and Kenny (1986), were met. As such, the
indirect path between math anxiety, impeding calculation and
percentage of errors was tested using a Sobel test, which
demonstrated a significant indirect path, p = .006.
PERCENTAGE OF ERRORS ON NO-CARRY PROBLEMS
The final regression model was not significant, F(5, 116) =
0.34, p = .89, accounting for <1% (Adj R
2
<.01) of the variance
in percentage of errors to no-carry problems. As shown in
Table 2, no individual variable was a significant predictor.
RESPONSE TIME TO CARRY PROBLEMS
The final regression model was significant, F(5, 116) =
3.35, p = .007, accounting for 12.6% (Adj R
2
= .088) of the
variance in response time to problems involving a carry
operation. As shown in Table III, math anxiety had a high level
of predictive power, being significantly positively related to
response time to carry problems and explaining 9.0% of the
variance. Inclusion of intrusive thoughts measures did not add
significantly to the model. Math anxiety remained significant
through steps two and three.
RESPONSE TIME TO NO-CARRY PROBLEMS
The final regression model was significant, F(5, 116) =
3.31, p = .008, accounting for 12.5% (Adj R
2
= .087) of the
variance in response time to problems that did not involve a
carry operation. Math anxiety was a significant predictor at
step one but no variables were significant in the remaining
steps.
TYPES OF INTRUSIVE THOUGHTS
As shown in Table IV, the most frequent intrusive thought
experienced by participants related to making mistakes.
Approximately half of all participants experienced thoughts
about time pressure or method of problem solving. 41.8% of
participants reported having thoughts that were non-task
related, with similar numbers experiencing thoughts about
previous math experiences or what people might think. Almost
a third of all participants reported thoughts relating to physical
changes and just over one fifth of participants reported having
thoughts about panic. A comparison of math anxiety levels
between those with and those without specific intrusive
thoughts revealed several significant differences. With the
exception of thoughts about method of problem solving,
endorsement of each of the other intrusive thoughts was
associated with significantly higher math anxiety.
TABLE II. RESULTS OF HIERARCHICAL REGRESSION WITH PERCENTAGE OF ERRORS AS THE OUTCOME VARIABLE
*P ≤.05 **P ≤.01
Carry problems
No-carry problems
Step
Variables Entered
Beta
R
2
change
Model R
2
Beta
R
2
change
Model R
2
1
Math anxiety
.251**
.063**
-.004
<.001
2
Math anxiety
.131
.069*
.132**
-.078
.014
.014
Frequency
-.069
-.028
Difficulty in removing
-.018
.147
Impeding calculation
.331**
.016
3
Math anxiety
.141
.002
1.33**
-.078
<.001
.014
Frequency
-.052
-.028
Difficulty in removing
.012
.147
Impeding calculation
.347**
.016
Effort to reduce thoughts
-.074
.002
S C I
CI E NT I F I CS
UBLICATIONP
www.sci-pub.com
ISSN: 1339-1488, VOLUME 2, ISSUE 2, 2014
-- PSYCHOLOGY --
- 72 -
V. DISCUSSION
Consistent with previous findings (e.g., Ashcraft & Faust,
1994) overall response time was significantly longer to
problems involving a carry operation, and significantly more
errors were made on problems involving a carry operation.
Math anxiety was related to poor performance, but once
intrusive thoughts data, namely frequency, difficulty in
removing and impeding calculation, were accounted for, math
anxiety did not predict percentage of errors to either carry or
no-carry problems. However, math anxiety was a significant
predictor of response time for carry problems. In contrast,
intrusive thoughts measures were unrelated to responses times
for carry and no-carry problems. Contrary to expectations, self-
reported frequency of intrusive thoughts did not predict error
rates to either carry problems or no-carry problems. Similarly,
self-reported difficulty in removing intrusive thoughts did not
predict error rates. However, there was a significant positive
relationship between the self-reported extent to which intrusive
thoughts impeded calculation and percentage of errors on
problems involving a carry operation, partially supporting the
hypothesis that the self-reported impact of intrusive thoughts
would be related to performance. Therefore, perceived impact
of intrusive thoughts was a predictor of performance whereas
self-reported frequency of thoughts was not. This is consistent
with other recent findings that showed frequency of thoughts to
be unrelated to working memory performance (Nixon et al.,
2008).
Moreover, the extent to which intrusive thoughts impeded
calculation mediated the relationship between math anxiety and
error rates on problems involving a carry operation; there were
no relationships between math anxiety or intrusive thoughts
measures, on the one hand, and error rates on problems
involving no carry operation, on the other. These findings lend
support to inhibition theory (Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Connelly,
TABLE III. RESULTS OF HIERARCHICAL REGRESSION WITH RESPONSE TIME AS THE OUTCOME VARIABLE
*P ≤.05 **P ≤.01 **P ≤.001
Carry problems
No-carry problems
Step
Variables Entered
Beta
R
2
change
Model R
2
Beta
R
2
change
Model R
2
1
Math anxiety
.299***
.090***
.302***
.091***
2
Math anxiety
.262*
.034
.124**
.213
.033
.124**
Frequency
.105
.114
Difficulty in removing
-.198
-.127
Impeding calculation
.166
.179
3
Math anxiety
.249*
.002
1.26**
.207
.001
.125**
Frequency
.085
.104
Difficulty in removing
-.234
-.144
Impeding calculation
.146
.170
Effort to reduce thoughts
.089
.042
TABLE IV. FREQUENCY OF INTRUSIVE THOUGHTS AND MATH ANXIETY LEVEL AS A FUNCTION OF SPECIFIC THOUGHTS
*BASED ON A POOLED STANDARD DEVIATION
Thought occurrence
Yes
No
Frequency
Math anxiety
Frequency
Math anxiety
T (& p) value
Effect size
(d)*
Nature of
thought
Making mistakes
109
(89.3%)
52.72
(16.51)
13
(10.7%)
43.00
(11.11)
2.06
(.04)
0.61
Time pressure
60
(49.2%)
56.03
(16.67)
62
(50.8%)
47.47
(14.82)
3.00
(.003)
0.54
Method of problem
solving
58
(47.5%)
53.29
(17.08)
64
(52.5%)
50.22
(15.48)
1.04
(.30)
0.19
What people might
think
48
(39.3%)
59.58
(16.53)
74
(60.7%)
46.55
(13.96)
4.68
(<.001)
0.87
Panicking
26
(21.3%)
65.08
(19.01)
96
(78.7%)
48.05
(13.40)
5.22
(<.001)
1.15
Previous math
experiences
49
(40.2%)
55.67
(16.97)
73
(59.8%)
49.00
(15.32)
2.26
(.03)
0.42
Physical changes
36
(29.5%)
60.97
(17.84)
86
(70.5%)
47.79
(13.92)
4.38
(<.001)
0.87
Non task-related
51
(41.8%)
56.43
(15.25)
71
(58.2%)
48.27
(16.22)
2.81
(.006)
0.52
Journal of Education, Psychology and Social Sciences
S C I
CI E NT I F I CS
UBLICATIONP
www.sci-pub.com
-- PSYCHOLOGY --
- 73 -
Hasher & Zacks, 1991) and provide the first data to suggest
that failure to inhibit intrusive thoughts is responsible for the
math anxiety-to-performance relationship, particularly on
problems involving the transitory maintenance of a carry term
(e.g. Faust et al., 1996).
In addition to assessing participants’ perceived impact of
intrusive thoughts, the CIQ (Freeston et al., 1991) permitted
identification of specific intrusive thoughts. Participants
reported a range of intrusive thoughts, with almost 90% having
thoughts about making mistakes and nearly half reporting
thoughts about time pressure and method of problem solving.
Further, participants with higher math anxiety scores were
more likely to endorse intrusive thoughts, particularly relating
to what people might think, panicking and physical changes.
The current study represents the first occasion in which
intrusive thoughts related to completing a math task have been
have examined using the CIQ (Freeston et al., 1991). However,
the precise nature of the relationships observed remains
unclear, in part due to the self-report approach taken. For
example, it is unclear whether thoughts about time pressure
were an antecedent to response time or whether they occurred
following response time. The present study is limited by the
extent to which math anxiety is manipulated. However,
experimental designs could be used to examine the importance
of particular thoughts in math anxious individuals by
manipulating them; for example regular reminders of a time
limit (c.f. Kellogg, Hopko & Ashcraft, 1999) or the use of a
flashing camera light to increase awareness that performance is
being observed. It would also be interesting to investigate the
relationship between math anxiety and arithmetic performance,
and the importance of intrusive thoughts about previous math
experiences, as previous research has emphasised the
importance of negative math experiences as antecedents to
math anxiety (Trujillo & Hadfield, 1999).
In conclusion, math anxiety was shown to be a significant
predictor of response time to both carry and no-carry problems.
In addition, math anxiety was related to a higher error rate on
problems involving a carry operation. Importantly, the extent to
which intrusive thoughts impeded calculation mediated this
relationship. These findings provide support for an inhibition
theory account of math anxiety effects on performance.
Intrusive thoughts should be the focus of future research that
investigates the relationship between math anxiety and
performance.
REFERENCES
[1] M. H. Ashcraft, (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and
cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
11, 181185. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00196
[2] M. H. Ashcraft, M.W. Faust, (1994). Mathematics anxiety and mental
arithmetic performance: An exploratory investigation. Cognition and
Emotion, 8, 97125. doi: 10.1080/02699939408408931
[3] M. H. Ashcraft, a. M. Moore, (2009). Mathematics anxiety and the
affective drop in performance. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 27, 197205. doi:10.1177/0734282908330580
[4] S. L. Beilock, C. A. Kulp, L. E. Holt, T. H. Carr, (2004). More on the
fragility of performance: Choking under pressure in mathematical
problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133,
584-600. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.584
[5] J. Cohen, (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
(2nd Edn.). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[6] S. L. Connelly, L. Hasher, R. T. Zacks, (1991). Age and reading: The
impact of distraction. Psychology and Aging, 6, 533541.
[7] M. S. DeCaro, K. E. Rotar, M. S. Kendra, S. L. Beilock, (2010).
Diagnosing and alleviating the impact of performance pressure on
mathematical problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 63, 1619-1630. doi:
10.1080/17470210903474286
[8] M. W. Faust, M. H. Ashcraft, D. E. Fleck, (1996). Mathematics anxiety
effects in simple and complex addition. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 25
62. doi: 10.1080/135467996387534
[9] M. H. Freeston, R. Ladouceur, H. Letarte, N. Thibodeau, F. Gagnon,
(1991). Cognitive intrusions in a non-clinical population. I. Response
style, subjective experience, and appraisal. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 29, 585-597.
[10] Hasher, R. T. Zacks, (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and
aging: A review and a new view. The Psychology of Learning and
Motivation, 22, 193225.
[11] R. Hembree, (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics
anxiety. Journal of Research for Mathematics Education, 21, 33-46.
[12] D. R. Hopko, M. H. Ashcraft, R. Gute, K. J. Ruggiero, C. Lewis,
(1998). Mathematics anxiety and working memory: Support for the
existence of a deficient inhibition mechanism. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 12, 343355. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(98)00019-X
[13] D. R. Hopko, D. W. McNeil, P. J. Gleason, A. E. Rabalais, (2002). The
emotional Stroop paradigm: Performance as a function of stimulus
properties and self-reported mathematics anxiety. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 26, 157-166. doi: 0147-5916/02/0400-0157/0
[14] J. Hunsley, (1987). Cognitive processes in mathematics anxiety and test
anxiety: The role of appraisals, internal dialogue, and attributions.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 388-392.
[15] T. E. Hunt, D. Clark-Carter, D. Sheffield, (2011). The development and
part-validation of a U.K. scale for mathematics anxiety. Journal of
Psychoeducational Assessment, 29, 455-466. doi:
10.1177/0734282910392892
[16] W. J. Jones, Childers, T. L., & Jiang, Y. (2012). The shopping brain:
math anxiety modulates brain responses to buying decisions. Biological
Psychology, 89, 20113. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.011
[17] J. S. Kellogg, D. R. Hopko, M. H. Ashcraft, (1999). The effects of time
pressure on arithmetic performance. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 13,
591600. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(99)00025-0
[18] X. Ma, (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety
toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 520-540.
[19] M. McMullan, Jones, R., & Lea, S. (2012). Math anxiety, self-efficacy,
and ability in British undergraduate nursing students. Research in
Nursing & Health, 35, 17886. doi:10.1002/nur.21460
[20] E. Munoz, M. J. Sliwinski, J. M. Smyth, D. M. Almeida, H. a. King,
(2013). Intrusive thoughts mediate the association between neuroticism
and cognitive function. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 898
903. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.019
[21] R. Nixon, A. Menne, L. King, A. Steele, J. Barnes, H. Dognt, S. A. Ball,
H. Tyler, (2008). Metacognition, working memory, and thought
suppression in acute stress disorder. Australian Journal of Psychology,
60, 168-174. doi: 10.1080/00049530701867813
[22] F. C.Richardson, R. M. Suinn, (1972). The Mathematics Anxiety Rating
Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19, 551-554.
[23] I. G. Sarason, (1978). The test anxiety scale: Concept and research. In C.
D. Spielberger & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (vol. 5, pp.
193-218). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
[24] C. D. Spielberger, R. L. Gorsuch, R. Lushene, P. R. Vagg, G. A. Jacobs,
(1984). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychological Press,
Inc.
[25] K. M. Trujillo, O. D. Hadfield, (1999). Tracing the roots of mathematics
anxiety through in-depth interviews with preservice elementary teachers.
College Student Journal, 33, 219-232.
S C I
CI E NT I F I CS
UBLICATIONP
www.sci-pub.com
ISSN: 1339-1488, VOLUME 2, ISSUE 2, 2014
-- PSYCHOLOGY --
- 74 -
[26] Universities and Colleges Admissions Service. (2009). Retrieved from
http://www.ucas.ac.uk/about_us/stat_services/stats_online/
[27] S. S. Wu, M. Barth, H. Amin, V. Malcarne, V. Menon, (2012). Math
anxiety in second and third graders and its relation to mathematics
achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 162.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00162
Journal of Education, Psychology and Social Sciences
S C I
CI E NT I F I CS
UBLICATIONP
www.sci-pub.com
-- PSYCHOLOGY --
- 75 -
... By asking participants to report what thoughts they experienced during a computer-based math task, researchers found that 40.2% of participants reported having thought about previous math experiences. 36 They further showed that math anxiety was significantly higher among those who reported having such thoughts. While care must be taken not to assume causal effects, work has emphasized the complex nature of memories associated with prior math events and it appears that negative appraisals may have an important role to play in the development, maintenance, and/or exacerbation of math anxiety. ...
... 4 The scale possesses excellent internal consistency and high test-retest reliability. 36,59,60 ...
... This aligns with previous findings, whereby negative math experiences form a major theme in the narratives of individuals high in math anxiety. 21,25,35 Given the evidence that many people think about previous math experiences during mathematical problem solving, 36 an individual's (negative) appraisal of taking the tests. A conversation could take place in which such incongruency could be explored to modify a student's cognitions regarding their math learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Math anxiety affects many people, from young children through to older adults. While there has been debate concerning the developmental trajectory of math anxiety and negative math attitudes, little attention has been given to the role of appraisals of previous math experiences. We surveyed 308 adults (mean age = 27.56 years, SD = 11.25) and assessed self‐reported measures of math anxiety, mathematical resilience, math attitudes, and appraisal of previous math experiences. As hypothesized, all variables were found to be interrelated. Math anxiety was significantly negatively related to appraisal of previous math experiences, mathematical resilience, and math attitudes. Moreover, appraisal of previous math experiences was shown to mediate the relations between (1) math anxiety and math attitudes, and (2) mathematical resilience and math attitudes. The findings demonstrate the importance of considering current appraisals of previous math experiences and are consistent with an interpretation account of math anxiety. This may help inform cognitive‐based interventions that focus on one's interpretation of past events to support current and future math learning and engagement.
... Indeed, people who are higher in math anxiety tend to perform worse than those lower in math anxiety on assessments of numeracy (Ashcraft 2002;Hembree 1990). Math anxiety is theorized to cause lower numeracy both in the moment because of an anxietyinduced transient reduction in working memory (Ashcraft and Kirk 2001;Hunt et al. 2014) and long term because of an increased avoidance of math and math-related activities, leading to fewer opportunities to hone ones' math skills (Daker et al. 2021;Gunderson et al. 2018;Maloney 2016). ...
... One theory as to why math anxiety is negatively related to numeracy is that math anxiety co-opts important working memory resources needed to perform mathematical computations (Ashcraft and Kirk 2001;Hunt et al. 2014). Another possibility is that being anxious about math has led to a lifetime of avoiding opportunities to enhance numeracy (Maloney 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
We investigate the relations between math anxiety, financial anxiety, and financial literacy while extending previous research in three ways. First, we examine the distinct subconstructs that comprise financial literacy (i.e., financial knowledge, confidence, attitudes, and behaviour). Second, we distinguish between financial knowledge items that are confounded with numeracy versus items that are not. Third, we control for trait anxiety. Using survey data from Canadian adults (N = 241), we demonstrate that math anxiety is negatively related to mathematical financial knowledge but is not related to conceptual financial knowledge, financial confidence, or financial behaviour. Financial anxiety, conversely, is negatively related to both mathematical and conceptual financial knowledge, financial confidence, and ideal financial behaviour. Our data suggest that, when considering financial literacy holistically, financial anxiety is more important than previously thought. These findings highlight the importance of distinguishing between the subconstructs that comprise financial literacy when attempting to understand individual differences that relate to financial literacy. Educators and policymakers looking to improve financial literacy would seemingly benefit from employing a targeted approach to decrease anxiety toward both math and finances.
... Like any other phobia, MA affects individuals on three different levels: physiological reactions, cognitive effects, and avoidance behaviors [9]. Previous studies have found that schoolchildren [10] and adults [11] who scored high in MA showed greater physiological reactivity to mathematics, tended to experience more intrusive thoughts [12], and showed a behavioral disengagement bias specifically away from mathematical stimuli [13]. ...
... In our sample, the children who reported feeling more anxious regarding handling math operations tended to perform math class assignments and exams more poorly, and respond slower and less accurately when performing simple arithmetic calculations. Previous research pointed to the presence of high physiological reactivity to mathematics and intrusive thoughts as pathways that connect MA and math performance [10][11][12]. Interestingly, the numerical processing anxiety factor did not predict Calculation scores whereas other factors, especially non-verbal intelligence, showed a more relevant role in explaining children's differences in arithmetic abilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
Math anxiety (MA) affects students of all age groups. Because of its effects on children’s academic development, the need to recognize its early manifestations has been highlighted. We designed a European-Spanish version of the Scale for Early Mathematics Anxiety (SEMA; Wu et al. (2012)), and assessed its psychometric properties in a sample of children aged 7 to 12 years. The participants (967 typically developing children) were elementary school students recruited from ten schools. Children reported their general and math anxiety levels in an individual session and performed nonverbal IQ and math abilities subtests in a group session. Teachers reported the final math grades. The psychometric indices obtained, and the resulting factor structure revealed that the European-Spanish version of the SEMA developed in this study is a reliable and valid measure to evaluate MA in children from 3rd to 6th grade. Moreover, we explored gender differences, that resulted in small effect sizes, which disappeared when controlling for trait anxiety. Differences across grades were found for both global MA and the numerical processing anxiety factor but not for the situational and performance anxiety factor. Finally, MA was negatively associated with students’ math achievement, although the strength of the associations varied with the MA measure selected, the kind of math achievement analyzed, and the school stage considered. Our findings highlight the relevance of MA in elementary school and highlight the need for an early identification of students at risk of suffering MA to palliate the negative consequences of MA in children’s cognitive and academic development.
... Children who are math anxious have lower involvement in math-related activities, and, in its turn, this can affect how they evaluate themselves and their abilities (Geist, 2010). Further, solving more complex exercises is shown to negatively impact the physiological response in children, who display higher levels of blood pressure (Hunt et al., 2017), and intrusive thoughts that appear during the math-solving process negatively impact their performance while increasing anxiety levels (Hunt et al., 2014). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Math anxiety (MA) represents a widespread problem that affects people of all ages, and some of its effects can be identified as early as primary school. MA hinders academic performance and negatively impacts how children perceive math, which, in the long term, contributes to the avoidance of certain career paths. There are several individual and contextual factors that can be taken into account when looking at how MA develops. In order to address both directions, cognitive tutoring programs are usually employed in order to reduce MA and have two main purposes. On the one hand, to enable children to develop math skills, and on the other hand, through the therapeutical dimension, they focus on reducing one's anxiety levels. More recently, with the development of digital technologies, these programs have become more accessible and have shown promising results with children of different ages. Currently, there is a significant number of such programs that can be used both in teaching activities in the classroom as well as for individual training. We designed a cognitive tutoring program for reducing math anxiety, structured as a game for Romanian primary school children. The aim is to integrate math into a narrative where children can exercise their mathematical problem-solving abilities while they complete the training tasks in a child-friendly stimulating environment.
... Math competency is negatively related to math anxiety (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001;Hembree, 1990;Hill et al., 2016), but this does not mean math anxiety is simply a proxy for poor math competence (Maloney & Beilock, 2012). Math anxiety is theorized to cause poor performance on math-related tasks both in the moment because of anxiety-induced transient reduction in cognitive resources (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001;Hunt et al., 2014) and long term because of an increased avoidance of math-related activities, leading to fewer opportunities to hone ones' math skills (Choe et al., 2019;Hembree, 1990;Maloney, 2016). Thus, it is reasonable to expect the decision-making of math-anxious consumers to suffer on financial tasks that require mathematical reasoning, such as a price comparison task. ...
Article
Full-text available
Comparison shopping is good financial practice, but situations involving numbers and computations are challenging for consumers with math anxiety. We asked North Americans (N = 256) to select the better deal between two products differing in volume and price. As predicted, math anxiety was negatively related to performance on this Price Comparison Task. We then explored the mechanism underlying this relation by testing math competency, price calculation ability, need for cognition, and cognitive reflection as potential mediators. The results from a competing mediator analysis indicated that all factors, apart from need for cognition, served as significant independent mediators between math anxiety and performance on our Price Comparison Task. This study has important implications for how–and why–math anxiety relates to a person’s ability to accurately compare product prices. These data suggest that consumers higher in math anxiety may represent a financially vulnerable population, particularly in the context of financial tasks that are inherently mathematical.
... One possible indicator of MA could be the behaviour of the individual while he or she is engaging with math-related tasks. MA can affect the individual and manifest itself at various levels, such as the physiological level (Hunt et al., 2017), the psychological or cognitive level (Hunt et al., 2014), and the emotional level (Papousek et al., 2012). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Math anxiety is a highly prevalent problem in education that has consistently shown to lead to poor math performance. This study sought to investigate whether certain behaviours are predictive of math anxiety among students. This study involved elementary school students who were low-progressing in math, and is part of an educational intervention program. Ten classifications types of behavioural indicators were identified, such as counting out loud. A multiple linear regression was conducted, identifying three behavioural observations that were positively and significantly associated with their math anxiety. Implications and limitations are discussed.
... Wilson, 2017), even though anxiety and shame are two different emotions (Jenßen, Möller, & Roesken-Winter, 2020). Cognitively, thoughts about one's own failure are reported (Hunt, Clark-Carter, & Sheffield, 2014). The thoughts are directed towards the future or evaluate the current situation as challenging or threatening. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
... Wilson, 2017), even though anxiety and shame are two different emotions (Jenßen, Möller, & Roesken-Winter, 2020). Cognitively, thoughts about one's own failure are reported (Hunt, Clark-Carter, & Sheffield, 2014). The thoughts are directed towards the future or evaluate the current situation as challenging or threatening. ...
Article
Full-text available
Meta-analyses from the 1990s previously have established a significant, small-to-moderate, and negative correlation between math achievement and math anxiety. Since these publications, research has continued to investigate this relation with more diverse samples and measures. Thus, the goal of the present meta-analysis was to provide an update of the math anxiety-math achievement relation and its moderators. Analyzing 747 effect sizes accumulated from research conducted between 1992 and 2018, we found a small-to-moderate, negative, and statistically significant correlation (r = -.28) between math anxiety and math achievement. The relation was significant for all moderator subgroups, with the exception of the relation between math anxiety and assessments measuring the approximate number system. Grade level, math ability level, adolescent/adult math anxiety scales, math topic of anxiety scale, and math assessments were significant moderators of this relation. There is also a tendency for published studies to report significantly stronger correlations than unpublished studies, but overall, large, negative effect sizes are underreported. Our results are consistent with previous findings of a significant relation between math anxiety and math achievement. This association starts in childhood, remains significant through adulthood, is smaller for students in Grades 3 through 5 and postsecondary school, is larger for math anxiety than for statistics anxiety and for certain math anxiety scales, and is smaller for math exam grades and samples selected for low math ability. This work supports future research efforts to determine effective math achievement and math anxiety interventions, which may be most helpful to implement during childhood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Two experiments report on the relationship between level of mathematics anxiety and timed performance on simple and complex addition problems. In both experiments, subjects at differing levels of mathematics anxiety responded to one- and two-column addition problems in a verification task. Anxiety effects on the simple one-column addition problems were largely limited to the false problems. For example, when the incorrect answer differed by 1 from the correct value (e.g. 7+9=15), high anxious subjects were particularly slow to decide ''false'', and became even less accurate when incorrect answers deviated further from correct (e.g. 7+9=39), the opposite of the normal trend. In both experiments, anxiety effects were especially prominent in the two-column addition problems, particularly when the problem involved the carry operation. Experiment 3 tested the same types of stimuli in an untimed task and showed equivalent, high accuracy for all levels of mathematics anxiety, arguing against an alternative interpretation of the anxiety effects based on the covariation between mathematics anxiety and competence. Speed accuracy trade-offs, difficulties in rejecting incorrect addition problems, and large reaction-time differences for complex problems characterise the results and suggest several directions for further empirical tests of the general mathematics anxiety-tocomplexity relationship. In summary, there appear to be several on-line cognitive consequences or correlates of mathematics anxiety.
Article
Full-text available
There is a paucity of information surrounding maths anxiety levels in the British undergraduate student population and, due to terminological issues, existing measures of maths anxiety may not be appropriate measures to use with this population. The current study therefore reports on the development and validation of a new maths anxiety scale. Using a large sample of British undergraduates, the 23-item Mathematics Anxiety Scale-UK (MAS-UK) was shown to be a reliable and valid measure of maths anxiety. Exploratory factor analysis indicated the existence of three factors, highlighting maths anxiety as a multidimensional construct. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed a good-fitting model. Normative data on maths anxiety in a British undergraduate student population are provided, along with comparisons between academic undergraduate subject areas and genders. The MAS-UK may represent an easily administrable, reliable and valid tool for assessing maths anxiety in British and potentially European undergraduate student populations.
Article
Results of 151 studies were integrated by meta-analysis to scrutinize the construct mathematics anxiety. Mathematics anxiety is related to poor performance on mathematics achievement tests. It relates inversely to positive attitudes toward mathematics and is bound directly to avoidance of the subject. Variables that exhibit differential mathematics anxiety levels include ability, school grade level, and undergraduate fields of study, with preservice arithmetic teachers especially prone to mathematics anxiety. Females display higher levels than males. However, mathematics anxiety appears more strongly linked with poor performance and avoidance of mathematics in precollege males than females. A variety of treatments are effective in reducing mathematics anxiety. Improved mathematics performance consistently accompanies valid treatment.
Article
In this mete-analysis I examined 26 studies on the relationship between anxiety toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics among elementary and secondary students. The common population correlation for the relationship is significant (-.27). A series of general linear models indicated that the relationship is consistent across gender groups, grade-level groups, ethnic groups, instruments measuring anxiety, and years of publication. The relationship, however, differs significantly among instruments measuring achievement as well as among types of publication. Researchers using standardized achievement tests tend to report a relationship of significantly smaller magnitude than researchers using mathematics teachers' grades and researcher-made achieve ment tests. Published studies tend to indicate a significantly smaller magnitude of the relationship than unpublished studies. There are no significant interaction effects among key variables such as gender, grade, and ethnicity.
Article
Stroop tasks are considered useful in assessing anxious affect. Although symptomatically mathematics anxiety is similar to other anxiety conditions, demonstrating a Stroop interference effect among individuals with math anxiety has proven difficult. High and low math anxious individuals were administered a Stroop-like card task involving the counting of numerals and letters, along with a standard computer-based Stroop color-naming task using mathematical and neutral control words. On the card task, processing time was significantly longer among high math anxious participants, an effect most pronounced in the numeral condition. Response time did not differ on the standard Stroop color-naming task as a function of anxiety group or word type. Results suggest that interference effects of math anxiety may be a function of inhibitory deficits or excitatory processes (or both) that are compounded when exposed to more salient (i.e. numeric) stimuli. Implications regarding the assessment of mathematics anxiety and Stroop task methodology are discussed.
Article
Although research has established a negative association between trait neuroticism and cognition, little is known about the mechanisms that underlie this relationship. We examined the tendency to experience intrusive thoughts and negative affect as potential mediators of the relationship between neuroticism and cognitive performance. We hypothesized that the tendency to experience intrusive thoughts reflects ineffective attentional control and would account for the relationship between neuroticism and cognitive performance over and above the mediating effect of negative affect. Three hundred seventeen adults (M age =49.43) completed a series of attention-demanding cognitive tasks as well as self-report measures of intrusive thoughts, negative affect, and neuroticism. Intrusive thoughts mediated the association between trait neuroticism and cognitive performance beyond negative affect. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the tendency to experience intrusive thoughts is a mechanism through which trait neuroticism influences cognitive performance.