ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Previous correlational and quasi-experimental studies of arts integration—the pedagogical practice of “teaching through the arts”—suggest its value for enhancing cognitive, academic, and social skills. This study reports the results of a small, preliminary classroom-based experiment that tested effects of arts integration on long-term retention of content. We designed matched arts-integrated (AI) and conventional science units in astronomy and ecology. Four randomized groups of 5th graders in one school completed one unit in the treatment (AI) condition and the other in the control (conventional) condition. To control for teacher effects, four teachers taught the same subject to different groups in each condition. We administered curriculum-based assessments before, immediately after, and 2 months after each unit to measure initial learning and retention. Results showed no differences in initial learning, but significantly better retention in the AI condition. Increases in retention were greatest for students at the lowest levels of reading achievement.
Content may be subject to copyright.
MIND, BRAIN, AND EDUCATION
The Effects of Arts Integration
on Long-Term Retention
of Academic Content
Mariale Hardiman1, Luke Rinne1, and Julia Yarmolinskaya1
ABSTRACT— Previous correlational and quasi-
experimental studies of arts integration—the pedagogical
practice of ‘‘teaching through the arts’’—suggest its value for
enhancing cognitive, academic, and social skills. This study
reports the results of a small, preliminary classroom-based
experiment that tested effects of arts integration on long-term
retention of content. We designed matched arts-integrated
(AI) and conventional science units in astronomy and
ecology. Four randomized groups of 5th graders in one school
completed one unit in the treatment (AI) condition and the
other in the control (conventional) condition. To control
for teacher effects, four teachers taught the same subject
to different groups in each condition. We administered
curriculum-based assessments before, immediately after, and
2 months after each unit to measure initial learning and
retention. Results showed no differences in initial learning,
but significantly better retention in the AI condition. Increases
in retention were greatest for students at the lowest levels of
reading achievement.
Scarce resources in public education and a focus on the
‘‘basics’’ have led to the well-documented narrowing of the
curriculum, often resulting in a reduced role for the arts
in schools (Mishook & Kornhaber, 2006). However, in-
school artistic activity may be valuable not only ‘‘for arts’
sake’’ but also because the arts may improve learning and
student outcomes more broadly. A number of researchers have
proposed that knowledge and skills gained uniquely through
the arts correlate with success in other academic domains
(Catterall, 2002; Deasy, 2002; Fiske, 1999; Spelke, 2008;
Wandall, Dougherty, Ben-Shachar, Deutsch, & Tsang, 2008).
1Johns Hopkins University
Address correspondence to Mariale Hardiman, School of Education, Johns
Hopkins University, 2800 N. Charles St., Baltimore, MD 21218; e-mail:
mmhardiman@jhu.edu
Still others argue that the arts contribute to the development
of more general thinking skills and dispositions that benefit
performance (Hetland, Winner, Veenema, & Sheridan, 2007).
Arts integration, defined here as the infusion of visual and
performing arts activities into instruction in nonarts subjects,
is also thought to enhance content learning (Burnaford, Brown,
Doherty, & McLaughlin, 2007; Martin et al., 2013). Several
quasi-experimental studies reported significant differences in
academic outcomes in schools that instituted arts-integrated
curriculumcomparedtocontrolschools(Barry,2010;Catterall,
Dumais, & Hampden-Thompson, 2012; Phillips, Harper, Lee,
& Boone, 2013; Scripp, Burnaford, Vazquez, Paradis, &
Sienkiewicz, 2013). Yet, to our knowledge, no randomized
controlled trials have tested the effectiveness of arts-integrated
(AI) curricula. Such experiments are important because
schools and/or teachers generally self-select AI instruction,
raising the possibility that differences in student outcomes
may be the result of selection bias. In this article, we report
the results of a small, preliminary randomized experiment
that investigated the effects of AI fifth-grade science units on
long-term retention of content.
Arts Integration and Long-Term Retention of Content
On the basis of the idea that rehearsal of information
consolidates memories for long-term storage (Kandel, 2006),
Hardiman (2003, 2010) posited that AI instruction improves
retention by prompting students to rehearse content through
the use of various visual and performing arts activities which
may enhance student engagement (e.g., Smithrim & Upitis,
2005). Further, Rinne, Gregory, Yarmolinskaya, and Hardiman
(2011) argue that the arts may engage learners in thinking
about new information in ways that improve retention, for
example through semantic elaboration (e.g., Craik & Tulving,
1975), generation of information from a cue (e.g., Slamecka &
Graf, 1978), enactment (e.g., Mohr, Engelkamp, & Zimmer,
1989), oral production (MacLeod, Gopie, Hourihan, Neary,
& Ozubko, 2010), ‘‘effort after meaning’’ (e.g., Zaromb &
144 ©2014 International Mind, Brain, and Education Society and Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Volume 8—Number 3
Mariale Hardiman et al.
Roediger, 2009), emotional arousal (e.g., Cahill & McGaugh,
1995), and pictorial representation (e.g., Paivio, 1971). The
theory that arts integration improves retention of content is
derived from the notion that the arts naturally take advantage
of these strategies whereas conventional instruction typically
does not.
This study contributes to the growing body of research
on arts integration by testing the effects of AI curricula
on academic outcomes through a preliminary study using a
randomized experimental design. We developed matched AI
and conventional versions of 5th grade science units in ecology
and astronomy and tested their effects on retention of content
in four fifth-grade classrooms. We hypothesized that the AI
units would lead to better retention of content on a delayed
posttest. We did not expect that the arts-focused curricula
would facilitate the learning of more content over the course
of the unit—rather, the prediction was that more of what
students had learned would be retained.
METHOD
Participants
Our study was conducted at an urban elementary school that
serves over 600 students (98.5% African American), largely
from low-income families (83.5% are eligible for free and
reduced-price meals [FARM]). The faculty (65.8% African
American), are mostly classified as ‘‘highly qualified teachers’’
(85.3%) according to the school district. All participants in
our study were African American (47% female; Mage =10.8),
and 14% were identified as students with disabilities (eligible
for an individualized education plan [IEP]). All students in the
fifth grade (97) at the school agreed to participate in the study.
Materials
We developed two science units in astronomy and ecology that
spanned 3 weeks of instruction. For each unit, we designed a
control version that utilized conventional forms of instruction
and a treatment, arts-integrated (AI) version that incorporated
a variety of forms of artistic activity (music, visual arts, and
performance arts) into instruction. (Interested educators can
contact us concerning access to these lessons.)
We first mapped the content to be taught over the course
of both units and segmented this content into 15 days of
instruction. We then designed conventional lesson plans
for each day based on the 5E learning cycle model (Bybee
et al., 1989): Engage,Explore,Explain,Elaborate,andEvaluate.
One conventional instructional activity was designed for each
of the five E’s, and activity durations were estimated. We
then compiled the AI units by designing and substituting
in AI learning activities for approximately two-thirds of the
standard activities. Each AI activity was designed to address
the same science content as the standard activity it replaced
and to take the same amount of time as its matched non-
AI activity. In designing the AI activities, we controlled for
a variety of other factors that could potentially influence
retention. These included: the order in which information was
presented or addressed, the ‘‘structure of the activity’’ (e.g.,
individual, pairs, small groups, or whole class), and the use
of multimedia. In addition, we controlled for whether the
activities were student-led or teacher-led and whether the
activities produced student work, with the aim of ensuring
that any student learning effects observed would not be simply
attributable to ‘‘active learning’’ (e.g., Bonwell & Eison, 1991).
Both AI and conventional curricula were designed to be active
and engaging, differing only with respect to the integration of
arts-based activities in place of conventional activities.
Arts-integrated substitutions were designed as follows:
1. Worksheets with written responses were replaced with
activities in which students drew responses to the same
prompts. This substitution was made to take advantage
of the ‘‘picture superiority’’ memory effect (e.g., Paivio,
1971), in which pictures tend to be remembered better
than words, even when verbal tests are administered.
2. Instead of silently reading informational texts, students
in the AI condition silently read stories containing the
same content. Here we aimed to improve recall by adding
elaborative language (e.g., Craik & Tulving, 1975) and
eliciting emotional responses (e.g., Cahill & McGaugh,
1995).
3. Instead of reading text passages out loud, we designed
dramatic scripts that children were asked to perform.
4. Instead of verbal group presentations, students performed
sketches or tableaux (two forms of dramatic play acting)
that we hoped would take advantage of the enactment
effect, which posits that carrying out an action produces
better memory than simply reading or hearing about the
action (Mohr et al., 1989).
5. For vocabulary development, in control lessons students
wrote ‘‘elaborations’’ for each term by expanding on the
definition or providing an example. In the AI condition,
students created ‘‘doodles’’ (simple drawings) to elaborate
the meaning of the new term, which we hoped would
leverage the picture superiority effect (e.g., Paivio, 1971).
6. At the conclusion of each unit, one additional day (Day 16)
was dedicated to review. We selected 10 matched pairs
of activities from across each unit and instructed teachers
to have students present the work they completed during
the activity. In the AI unit, this day was framed as an ‘‘art
exhibition day,’’ and in the control unit, it was simply a
‘‘review day.’’
For examples, in one astronomy activity in the control
condition, students traced the shapes of galaxies, wrote
descriptions in notebooks, and shared them with the class.
Volume 8—Number 3 145
The Effects of Arts Integration
In the matched AI activity, students drew shapes on posters
and used dance movements to depict galaxy shapes in small
performances. In another activity, students in the control
condition described different planets’ features by making
Venn diagrams; in the matched AI activity, students drew
advertising posters to sell real estate on different planets.
Measures
We developed multiple-choice curriculum-based assessments
to measure student learning and retention of the science con-
tent. Assessments consisted of 25 multiple-choice questions as
well as one ‘‘brief constructed response’’ (BCR short-answer)
item. We created three versions of each curriculum-based
assessment by changing question wording and/or modifying
answer choices. These measures were deliberately designed
to be difficult to avoid ceiling effects and make retention
challenging. The order in which the three different forms were
administered for the pretest, posttest, and delayed posttests
was counterbalanced across participants.
Procedures
Prior toimplementation,teachers receivedapproximately 10 hr
of training to review the content and activities in each unit.
To mitigate potential bias, teachers were not informed of
our theory regarding how arts integration might improve
retention. Instead, they were informed only that the study
aimed to test the effectiveness of arts-integration science
curricula versus conventional curricula.
All fifth grade students in the school were randomly
assigned to one of four equally sized groups for 1 hr per day
of science instruction. To ensure that all participants received
equivalent potential benefits from the treatment, students
were taught one unit in the AI condition and the other in the
control condition. After completing the first unit, each group
then completed the second unit with a different teacher in the
alternate condition (see Table 1). Pretests were administered
the day before each unit began, posttests at the conclusion of
each unit, and delayed posttests approximately 8 weeks after
the posttest.
Each teacher taught one randomized group of students in
one condition (treatment or control) and then taught the
Table 1
Randomized Group Assignments
Teacher 1 Teacher 2 Teacher 3 Teacher 4
Astronomy Astronomy Ecology Ecology
Period 1 Group ABCD
Condition AI Control Control AI
Period 2 Group DCBA
Condition Control AI AI Control
matched unit in the other condition to a second randomized
group. Thus, each teacher taught one content area in both
conditions, and the order of conditions was counterbalanced
across teachers. This design allowed for the control of teacher
effects. Fidelity-of-implementation observers were present in
each classroom approximately 60% of the time to ensure that
the units were taught as designed in terms of timing, content,
and activities. Observers were instructed to note any deviation
from the curriculum as well as any differences in instructional
quality across conditions.
RESULTS
Our analysis of student outcomes includes 82 students (of 97)
who completed all assessments and for whom we received
demographic information and standardized test scores from
the district. We calculated each student’s percent correct on
each test, measuring initial learning in terms of the difference
between the initial posttest (T2) and pretest (T1) scores
(i.e., T2–T
1). Meanwhile, consistent with previous studies
(Custers, 2010), retention equaled the delayed posttest (T3)
score as a proportion of the T2score (T3/T2). We identified
three outlying high retention scores (two treatments, one
control) whose scores fell more than three standard deviations
above the mean. These were winsorized to the next highest
value. Students completed the BCR (short-answer) item at the
end of each test at a very low rate (<50%), so these responses
were not analyzed.
We conducted repeated-measures ANOVAs of initial learn-
ing and retention. Factors in each model included condition
order(AIvs.Controlfirst),AIsubject (Astronomy vs. Ecology),
gender, FARM eligibility, IEP eligibility, and reading and math
proficiency levels (Proficient/Advanced vs. Basic). To conserve
statistical power given our relatively small sample size, factors
that did not exhibit significant main effects or interactions
withcondition(AI vs. Control) were removed from our models.
None of the factors listed above was a significant predictor
of initial learning, nor was there a significant effect of the
treatment on initial learning (AI vs. Control), F(1, 82) =1.496,
p=.225. However, our analysis of retention revealed a signif-
icant main effect of AI, F(1, 80) =6.570, p=.012, ηp2=.076,
as well as a significant interaction between AI and reading
proficiency level F(1, 80) =7.070, p=.009, ηp2=.081. Mean
retention was approximately .2 SD greater in the AI condition
(M=.912) than in the control condition (M=.866)—an effect
size that is ‘‘small’’ based on Cohen’s (1988) benchmarks, but
relatively typical of effective educational interventions (Hill,
Bloom, Black, & Lipsey, 2008). The significant interaction
indicates that students at a ‘‘basic’’ level of reading proficiency
(n=16) were the primary drivers of the observed main effect.
Mean retention among these students was approximately .9
SD greater in the AI condition (M=.976) than the control
146 Volume 8—Number 3
Mariale Hardiman et al.
Table 2
Mean Test Scores (Percent Correct) by Test Time, Condition (AI vs.
Control), and Reading Proficiency Level
Condition
Reading
proficiency level Pretest Posttest
Delayed
posttest
Retention
rate
AI Basic 29.0 44.0 39.0 97.6%
Proficient/advanced 38.6 59.0 52.8 89.7%
Control Basic 25.0 46.3 32.5 72.0%
Proficient/advanced 38.9 61.3 54.1 90.2%
condition (M=.720). Interestingly, in the AI condition, poor
readers exhibited better retention than their proficiently
reading peers, though it is important to note that these
students’ long-term learning gains were smaller by about 4
percentage points, meaning they acquired less knowledge
to begin with. See Table 2 for mean scores across the three
test times.
DISCUSSION
Although there was no significant effect of arts integration
on initial learning, arts integration curricula had a significant
effect on retention. Participants learned roughly the same
volume of science content regardless of how the units were
taught, but scores on a delayed posttest showed that students
retained what they learned significantly better when taught
through AI instruction. These findings provide preliminary
support for the theory that arts integration naturally leads
students to interact with academic content in ways that
promote long-term retention.
An important caveat to this finding is that it was the group
of students at a ‘‘basic’’ level of proficiency who showed the
biggest gains in retention. In many ways, this makes sense—AI
instructionrelies less on reading and writing than does conven-
tional instruction, so students who struggle with reading and
writing may benefit more from the opportunity to learn con-
tent through alternative, arts-based means. Thus, for students
who are still developing literacy skills, arts integration may
represent a useful means of improving learning. We emphasize
that we are not endorsing reductions in reading and writing.
If reading and writing activities were scaled back as part of
AI instruction, the possibility of deleterious effects on reading
and writing would need to be addressed, and educators would
presumably need to compensate for this loss elsewhere in
the curriculum.
The generalizability of our findings is limited by our
relatively small sample of participants of the same age from a
single school. In particular, our sample included 16 basic-level
readers. Future research should attempt to replicate our results
with a larger sample. Additionally, it is possible that results
were biased by some teachers’ natural enthusiasm for the use
of the arts in instruction. Further, although teachers were told
that the study aimed to compare the effectiveness of AI and
conventional curricula, the fact that we were interested in this
comparison suggests our hypothesis that the AI curricula may
lead to better outcomes. However, the presence of observers
in the classroom during a majority of instructional time helped
to ensure that the lessons were taught as written.
Despite these limitations, we believe our study provides
important preliminary evidence that many students, partic-
ularly struggling readers, may retain academic content better
when instruction is integrated with the arts, particularly
when mastery requires possession of considerable amounts
of declarative knowledge. We hope that in the future, larger
scale studies of arts integration that utilize randomized
experimental designs will be conducted, and that these
studies would be able to disentangle the underlying causes
of the effects. Such efforts would provide for more effective
measurement of the potential benefits of AI instruction, which
may eventually lead to findings that can enhance educational
practice and policy in important ways.
Acknowledgment—The authors would like to thank the Joseph
P. Drown Foundation for support of this study.
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found in the online
version of this article:
Appendix S1. Sample arts integrated/control lesson plans.
REFERENCES
Barry,N.H.(2010).Oklahoma A+Schools: What the research tells
us 2002–2007. Volume three, quantitative measures. Oklahoma
A+Schools/University of Central Oklahoma. Retrieved from
http://www.okaplus.org/storage/V3%20final.pdf
Bonwell, C. C., & Eison, J. A. (1991). Active learning: Creating excitement
in the classroom (ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report No. 1).
Washington, DC: ERIC Clearinghouse on Higher Education,
The George Washington University.
Burnaford, G., Brown, S., Doherty, J., & McLaughlin, H. J. (2007). Arts
integration frameworks, research and practice. Washington, DC: Arts
Education Partnership.
Bybee, R. W., Buchwald, C. E., Crissman, S., Heil, D. R., Kuerbis, P. J.,
Matsumoto, C., & McInerney, J. D. (1989). Science and technology
education for the elementary years: Frameworks for curriculum and
instruction. Washington, DC: The National Center for Improving
Instruction.
Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1995). A novel demonstration
of enhanced memory associated with emotional arousal.
Consciousness and Cognition,4, 410–421. doi:10.1006/ccog.1995.1048
Catterall, J. S. (2002). The arts and the transfer of learning. In R. J.
Deasy (Ed.), Critical links: Learning in the arts and student academic and
social development (pp. 151–157). Washington, DC: Arts Education
Partnership.
Volume 8—Number 3 147
The Effects of Arts Integration
Catterall, J. S., Dumais, S. A., & Hampden-Thompson, G. (2012). The
arts and achievement in at-risk youth: Findings from four longitudinal
studies. Washington, DC: National Endowment for the Arts.
Retrieved http://arts.gov/file/2684
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd
ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention
of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General,104, 268–294. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.268
Custers, E. (2010). Long-term retention of basic science knowledge:
A review study. Advances in Health Sciences Education,15, 109–128.
Deasy, R. J. (Ed.). (2002). Critical links: Learning in the arts and student
academic and social development. Washington, DC: Arts Education
Partnership.
Fiske, E. (Ed.) (1999). Championsofchange:The impact of theartsonlearning.
Washington, DC: Arts Education Partnership and President’s
Committee on the Arts and Humanities.
Hardiman, M. M. (2003). Connecting brain research with effective teaching:
The brain-targeted teaching model. Lanham, MD: Rowman &
Littlefield Education.
Hardiman, M. M. (2010). The creative-artistic brain. In D. Sousa (Ed.),
Mind, brain, and education: Neuroscience implications for the classroom
(pp. 226–246). Bloomington, IN: Solution Tree Press.
Hetland, L., Winner, E., Veenema, S., & Sheridan, K. (2007). Studio
thinking: The real benefits of arts education. New York, NY: Teachers
College Press.
Hill, C. J., Bloom, H. S., Black, A. R., & Lipsey, M. W. (2008).
Empirical benchmarks for interpreting effect sizes in research.
Child Development Perspectives,2(3), 172–177.
Kandel, E. R. (2006). In search of memory: The emergence of a new science of
mind. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
MacLeod, C. M., Gopie, N., Hourihan, K. L., Neary, K. R., & Ozubko, J.
D. (2010). The production effect: Delineation of a phenomenon.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
36, 671–685. doi:10.1037/a0018785
Martin, A. J., Mansour, M., Anderson, M., Gibson, R., Liem, G. A. D.,
& Sudmalis, D. (2013). The role of arts participation in students’
academic and nonacademic outcomes: A longitudinal study
of school, home, and community factors. Journal of Educational
Psychology,105, 709–727. doi:10.1037/a0032795
Mishook, J. J., & Kornhaber, M. L. (2006). Arts integration in an era
of accountability. Arts Education Policy Review,107(4), 3–11.
Mohr, G., Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1989). Recall and
recognition of self-performed acts. Psychological Research,51(4),
181–187.
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes.NewYork,NY:Holt,
Rinehart, & Winston.
Phillips, J., Harper, J., Lee, K., & Boone, E. (2013). Arts integration and
the Mississippi Arts Commission’s Whole Schools Initiative. Retrieved
from http://www.mswholeschools.org
Rinne, L., Gregory, E., Yarmolinskaya, J., & Hardiman, M.
(2011). Why arts integration improves long-term retention of
content. Mind, Brain, and Education,5(2), 89–96. doi:10.1111/j.1751-
228X.2011.01114.x
Scripp, L., Burnaford, G. Vazquez, O. Paradis, L., & Sienkiewicz,
F. (2013). Partnerships in arts integration research final reports.
Washington, DC: Arts Education Partnership. Retrieved
http://www.artsedsearch.org
Slamecka, N. J., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation
of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Learning and Memory,4, 592–604. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.
592
Smithrim, K., & Upitis, R. (2005). Learning through the arts:
Lessons of engagement. Canadian Journal of Education,28(1),
109–127.
Spelke, E. (2008). Effects of music instruction on developing cognitive
systems at the foundations of mathematics and science. In C.
Asbury & B. Rich (Eds.), Learning, arts, and the brain: The Dana
consortium report on arts and cognition (pp. 17–49). New York, NY:
The Dana Foundation.
Wandall, B., Dougherty, R. F., Ben-Shachar, M., Deutsch, G. K., &
Tsang, J. (2008). Training in the arts, reading and brain imaging.
In C. Asbury & B. Rich (Eds.), Learning,arts,andthebrain:TheDana
consortium report on arts and cognition (pp. 51–59). New York, NY:
The Dana Foundation.
Zaromb, F. M., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). The effects of ‘‘effort after
meaning’’ on recall: Differences in within-and between-subjects
designs. Memory and Cognition,37, 447–463.
148 Volume 8—Number 3
... Art-based learning has also been shown to help develop deeper understanding, critical thinking, creativity, problem-solving skills, and a sense of ownership, which are essential for 21st-century learning skills (Das, 2023). According to Hardiman et al. (2014Hardiman et al. ( , 2017, art integration contributes to long-term retention of subject content, especially among low achievers, making it beneficial for cognitive development. Studies have also found that AIL can improve memory retention, academic 111 skills, and understanding in subjects like science and mathematics (Hardiman et al., 2014;Hardiman et al., 2017;Graham & Brouillette, 2017). ...
... According to Hardiman et al. (2014Hardiman et al. ( , 2017, art integration contributes to long-term retention of subject content, especially among low achievers, making it beneficial for cognitive development. Studies have also found that AIL can improve memory retention, academic 111 skills, and understanding in subjects like science and mathematics (Hardiman et al., 2014;Hardiman et al., 2017;Graham & Brouillette, 2017). ...
... The academic benefits of AIL are significant in enhancing student achievement across various subjects. For instance, studies by Hardiman et al. (2014Hardiman et al. ( , 2017 and Graham & Brouillette (2017) show that AIL improves students' understanding of physical science, leading to better academic outcomes. Art integration not only strengthens knowledge retention but also boosts academic success in mathematics and language proficiency, as observed in studies by Walton (2020) and Sari et al. (2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
To make learning more effective and enjoyable, pedagogical interventions are necessary, particularly those who seek some challenges like academic stress, fear of failure which ultimately results to depression, anxiety, and more psychological issues among the learners. Among these, art-integrated learning emerges as a powerful tool to foster holistic education and emotional well-being. This paper aims to explore the integration of art with various disciplines and highlights the benefits of Art-Integrated Learning as an innovative pedagogical approach/ tool in classroom settings. While numerous studies on this subject have been conducted globally, relatively few conducted in India. The concept of art integration is well-established internationally, with early work by Ives and Pond (1980) demonstrating a relationship between the arts and cognitive development. For art integration to be effective, teachers must adhere to the PAOR (Plan, Act, Observe, Reflect) framework, ensuring structured implementation. However, integrating art into education presents unique challenges in the current digital age, where technology dominates yet falls short in imparting values. To instill values and enhance learning outcomes, educators must adopt diverse pedagogical strategies tailored to real-world contexts.
... When creativity is placed at the center of learning it allows art to be used as a methodology to improve student learning (Anderson, 2018). Through arts integration, students can engage with content on multiple levels at once to increase their understanding and retention of non-arts-based subjects like math and science (Hardiman et al., 2014). As instructional strategies for more effective learning acquisition, arts integration can increase retention for learners with limited language proficiency and for students with lower academic skills to demonstrate mastery of academic content (Hardiman et al., 2014). ...
... Through arts integration, students can engage with content on multiple levels at once to increase their understanding and retention of non-arts-based subjects like math and science (Hardiman et al., 2014). As instructional strategies for more effective learning acquisition, arts integration can increase retention for learners with limited language proficiency and for students with lower academic skills to demonstrate mastery of academic content (Hardiman et al., 2014). ...
... As a pedagogical tool, arts integration can improve academic outcomes in science, math, and other subjects (Hardiman et al., 2014) and as an andragogical tool build teacher agency when the arts are integrated into their professional development as well (Anderson et al., 2023). Arts integration can disrupt the typical instructional approaches in math, science, and other subjects that act as dominant cultural gatekeepers to learning and knowledge acquisition. ...
... Therefore, it develops creative, flexible and critical thinking skills, which are defined as essential for facing 21 st century challenges (Barber, 2023: Kidron, 2019Manalo, 2023). Moreover, integrative learning makes the educational effort relevant to the learner's life since it relates to social, cultural and personal issues while focusing on human lives (Catterall, 2009;Daugherty, 2013;Donovan & Pascale, 2012;Guyotte et al., 2014;Hardiman et al., 2014;Nevo, 2001;Patra & Basantia. 2021;Nevo, 2001;Patra & Basantia, 2021). ...
... The literary discussion about the events that accrued in the novel integrated with relevant scientific knowledge that can be useful to daily life made an impact on the students and increased their positive attitudes towards science lessons. The integration of science with arts and humanities has strong potential to encourage scientific learning and to provide all the skills deriving from it (Catterall, 2009;Daugherty, 2013;Donovan & Pascale, 2012;Guyotte et al., 2014;Hardiman et al., 2014). In many cultures, children are perceived as helpless. ...
Article
Full-text available
Scientific knowledge is required to overcome epidemics, find solutions to climate change, and develop technology that improves our lives. Yet students often perceive science as a dull and challenging discipline. Over the years, many suggestions have been made as to how to teach science and how to allay students’ fears. This study presents a pedagogical model of integrative learning of literature and science, which aims to enhance students’ interest in science and to promote the development of essential skills. The model template consists of six active integrative workshops that engage the learner in the learning process by conducting experiments and building scientific models. We tested the impact of the pedagogical integrative model by measuring students’ attitudes towards science and their self-efficacy. We found that, after six integrative workshops, students expressed more positive attitudes, and a correlation was found between the increase in ability and a positive attitude towards science. This paper highlights the importance of the integrative literature and science model for effective teaching of science.
... Interestingly, 81% of the participants reported that their career choice had not been influenced by their CVD, suggesting that despite the challenges that CVD may present in certain professional settings, it generally does not limit career opportunities [46,51]. However, the challenges experienced in school (62%) and kindergarten (71%) point to a need to increase awareness and adjustments in educational settings to support CVD individuals from a young age [62]. Nowadays, there are several methods that help improve color discriminatory behavior and, in general, visual comfort when viewing digital displays [63,64]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite the extensive literature on the challenges faced by people with color vision deficiency (CVD), significant gaps persist in participant awareness of the age of CVD diagnosis. This study aimed to investigate the extent to which participants are aware of CVD, focusing on their understanding of the deficiency. A total of 283 participants participated in a study designed to assess the age at which they became aware of their CVD, what color vision test their diagnosis was based on, and the challenges they faced. In addition, 190 participants underwent color vision testing using the Color Assessment and Diagnosis (CAD) test, compared to their knowledge of the type of CVD they have. The findings showed that 55% of the participants had found out about their CVD in adulthood, predominantly within occupational contexts. A substantial discrepancy was observed between self-reported diagnoses and clinical findings, with 43% of the participants being unable to specify their type of CVD. There is a necessity for enhanced early diagnostic practices, improved participant education about CVD, and increased public awareness to optimize the quality of life for affected individuals.
... The content remains in their mind for a long time. Hardiman et al., 2014 found in the study that there was better retention in Art Integrated condition when compared with the traditional teaching method. This shows that the Art-Integrated condition is having a positive effect on students' retention power. ...
Article
Learners can construct and demonstrate their understanding through an artistic medium using the ArtIntegrated Learning Approach. AIL Approach can be used as a pedagogical tool & this approach aims for theactive participation of learners in the teaching-learning process. In the present era, children and adolescent’smust-have 21st-century skills to be empowered to deal with challenges and concerns when it comes to theworkplace. These skills can bring positive psychological and behavioral changes in the students. The primarygoal of AIL Approach is to develop skills among the learners because NEP 2020 put light on skill-basedteaching & competency-based teaching. This research paper includes how AIL is important for 21st-centurylearners, characteristics of 21st-century learners, in this era why it is needed to integrate art with learning,how AIL & socio-emotional learning is related to each other and what are the strategies for the implementationof AIL in the classroom. The mindful inclusion of AIL in teaching practice will result in the eradication ofthe rote-learning habit of learners
... The social media platform Pinterest has emerged as a dynamic space where users with similar interests can interact and collaborate. The platform offers various features that encourage users to engage in activities such as sharing, commenting, and collaborating, all centered around visualizing ideas and inspiration (Hardiman et al., 2014). By enabling the formation of groups or shared boards, Pinterest facilitates the exchange of ideas and inspiration among users. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study was conducted to analyze the role of the social media platform Pinterest as a reference for the creativity of Generation Z students. The research method used was qualitative with a case study approach, allowing for an in-depth exploration of how Pinterest influences student creativity. Data collection techniques were carried out through in-depth interviews, observations, and document studies, ensuring a comprehensive understanding of the subject matter. Informants were determined purposively, consisting of students from the Department of Sociology at Universitas Negeri Padang, specifically from both the Sociology Study Program and the Communication Studies Program. This targeted selection ensured that the informants had relevant experience and insights. Data processing utilized the Miles and Huberman data analysis technique, which involves data reduction, data display, and conclusion drawing/verification. This method ensured that the data was systematically analyzed to identify patterns and draw meaningful conclusions. The results of the study indicate that Pinterest plays a crucial role in facilitating the creative processes of Generation Z students. This platform serves as a rich source of visual inspiration, offering a vast array of images and creative ideas that can stimulate the imagination of Generation Z students. Additionally, the ability to save and share creative content broadens the horizons of Generation Z students, enabling them to continuously develop their creative skills and expertise. Pinterest not only provides inspiration but also serves as a collaborative tool where students can exchange ideas and gain feedback from peers, further enhancing their creative potential
... The content remains in their mind for a long time. Hardiman et al., 2014 found in the study that there was better retention in Art Integrated condition when compared with the traditional teaching method. This shows that the Art-Integrated condition is having a positive effect on students' retention power. ...
Article
This study delves into the realm of student conceptual change, examining shifting understandings as important steppingstones on the path to sensemaking and canonical understanding in science education. It explores the potential of a STEAM (Science, Technology, Engineering, Arts, and Math) curriculum, aiming to provide equitable learning opportunities, especially for emerging bilingual (EB) student populations. To achieve this, elementary school educators from randomly assigned schools received professional development training to implement a novel curriculum encompassing both STEM and STEAM (STEM + Arts) approaches to life science instruction. These approaches comprised: (1) an NGSS‐aligned STEM unit employing inquiry‐based science instruction; (2) an NGSS‐aligned STEAM unit utilizing Arts‐based science instruction instead of inquiry methods. The results indicated that a STEAM‐first approach was most beneficial in helping students change from non‐canonical conceptual understanding toward more nuanced canonical science knowledge. Specifically, for EB students, the STEAM‐first approach showed even more promise, signifying its potential to bridge educational disparities. Furthermore, the study revealed that the integration of Arts as an instructional tool to teach science education played a pivotal role in enhancing the overall learning experience among students. Arts integration stimulated motivation, invigorated conceptual understanding, and offered unique avenues for elucidating complex scientific concepts and terminologies. This research contributes valuable insights for improving science education instruction, emphasizing the efficacy of conceptual change toward canonical scientific understanding through patterns of instructional sequencing of effective STEAM integration. It provides educators with evidence‐based strategies to foster inclusive and equitable science learning experiences, ultimately guiding students toward deeper conceptual comprehension.
Article
Full-text available
Art education helps people develop cognitive, emotional, social, and creative skills. This study examines how art can increase critical thinking, problem-solving, emotional intelligence, cultural knowledge, and empathy. Art transforms people into well-rounded people, according to this research. It uses several educational environments and development philosophies. This study examines how art education affects children’s personal and social development. Mixed-methods study involves questionnaires, interviews, and classroom observations. The findings show that frequent exposure to artistic practices promotes mental well-being, self-expression, and teamwork, making education more holistic. According to the report, art education is not just an extracurricular activity but an integral part of a well-rounded education. It promotes its integration into regular education to create adaptable, empathetic, and innovative people who can handle the challenges of a changing world.
Article
Full-text available
The article is devoted to aesthetic experience as a necessary component of students' self-realization. Its main goal is to demonstrate the value and suitability of using the aesthetic component in the psychotherapeutic and educational process, including for students majoring in natural sciences. The article provides a general overview of the available results of domestic and foreign studies on the use of art in the correction of students' psychoemotional states. The psychological components of aesthetic experience and the mechanisms of art's influence on improving the effectiveness of educational activities are considered. The toolkit of the aesthetic component in educational and psychocorrectional activities is quite wide and can be used in various situations. A modern specialist must have a wide range of competencies to solve complex problems. This means the need to use a creative approach and search for non-standard solutions, which is also facilitated by interaction with the creative component of art. The most undeveloped and controversial aspects of the use of art are highlighted, such as the lack of standardized and sufficiently tested materials for work, which requires further research. By analyzing the available materials, it was found that aesthetic experience obtained through art has a positive effect on the psychological state of students. Teaching with the use of art helps improve academic performance, increases the level of memorization of information, improving cognitive and social skills, and is also a condition for personal development, self-determination and awareness of students' opportunities to express their individuality. It is shown that the inclusion of an aesthetic component in teaching is a necessary component of students' self-realization.
Article
Full-text available
Technical Report
Full-text available
This report examines the academic and civic behavior outcomes of teenagers and young adults who have engaged deeply with the arts in or out of school.
Article
Full-text available
This longitudinal study draws on positive youth development frameworks and ecological models to examine the role of school-, home- and community-based arts participation in students’ academic (e.g., motivation, engagement) and nonacademic (e.g., self-esteem, life satisfaction) outcomes. The study is based on 643 elementary and high school students from 15 schools conducted over the course of 2 academic years. Structural equation modeling showed that beyond sociodemographics, prior achievement, and prior variance in outcome measures, school predictors of academic and nonacademic outcomes were arts engagement and in-school arts participation; home predictors were parent–child arts interaction and home-based arts resources; and community arts predictors were participation in and attendance at arts events and external arts tuition (the latter, a negative effect). Implications for theory, policy, funding, and practice are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Five experiments are reported comparing memory for words that were generated by the subjects themselves with the same words when they were simply presented to be read. In all cases, performance in the generate condition was superior to that in the read condition. This held for measures of cued and uncued recognition, free and cued recall, and confidence ratings. The phenomenon persisted across variations in encoding rules, timed or selfpaced presentation, presence or absence of test information, and between- or within-subjects designs. The effect was specific to the response items under recognition testing but not under cued recall. A number of potential explanatory principles are considered, and their difficulties enumerated. It is concluded that the generation effect is real and that it poses an interesting interpretative problem. This is an empirically oriented article whose purpose is to report a set of simple experiments that establish the existence of a robust and interesting phenomenon of memory. This phenomenon, called the generation effect, is robust in that it manifests itself across a variety of testing procedures, encoding rules, and other situational changes. It is interesting in that it does not seem to be easily or satisfactoril y accommodated by any of the currently familiar explanatory notions. We expect that once the phenomenon is described in its initial form, it will be the subject of wider experimental analysis and will eventually become better understood. In contrast to the usual objective reasons for embarking upon a line of research, the present work was neither initiated by any extant theoretical issue nor inspired by any previously published findings. It was carried out with the sole purpose of arriving at a
Article
In this article, we describe the effects on student achievement and attitudes of a Canadian school-wide, arts education approach, Learning Through the Arts (LTTA). Our sample included over 6000 students and their parents, teachers, and principals. We gathered data, both at the outset and after three years of involvement in LTTA on student achievement, student attitudes towards arts and schooling, and out-of-school activities. We found no baseline differences in achievement nor in socioeconomic status in the LTTA and control schools. At the end of three years, the grade-6 LTTA students scored significantly higher on tests of computation than students in control schools. We conclude the article with suggestions for extending this longitudinal research. /// Dans cet article, les auteures décrivent les effets sur le rendement scolaire et les attitudes des élèves du programme Apprendre par les arts (APLA) implanté dans un réseau scolaire. L'échantillon regroupait plus de 6000 élèves, les parents, les enseignants et les directeurs des écoles canadiennes participantes. Les auteurs ont colligé, au début d'implantation du programme APLA et après une période de trois and d'application, des données sur le rendement des élèves, leurs attitudes envers les arts et l'école et les activités extrascolaires. Il n'y avait aucune différence de base quant au rendement ou au statut socioéconomique entre les écoles ayant recours au programme APLA et les écoles témoins. Au terme de l'étude de trois ans, les élèves qui ont appris avec le programme APLA ont obtenu en mathématiques des notes nettement supérieures à celles des élèves des écoles témoins.
Article
This brief summary describes what I consider to be the most important and interesting findings of our book --that is, what in fact are the Critical Links reported in recent research. I will also comment on what I see as the most important implications of these Links for educators and for the future of arts education. There are at least three ways to grasp this book --one is obvious: the book is a guide to research on learning in the visual arts, music, drama, dance, and programs enlisting multiple art forms. Scholars summarize more than 60 studies and outline the contributions to knowledge of each. In addition, two expert researchers offer comments on each study. Each section presents an interpretive essay, and the book offers a concluding chapter on general issues surrounding the transfer of learning from the arts. Another path through the work traces the many skills which the arts touch and cultivate --skills that show up as outcomes in more than one art form: such as literacy, mathematics, and science skills along with student motivation and social competence.