ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Since its emergence in 1998, positive psychology has flourished. Among its successes is the burgeoning field of applied positive psychology (APP), involving interventions to promote wellbeing. However, the remit of APP is currently unclear. As such, we offer a meta-theoretical conceptual map delineating the terrain that APP might conceivably cover, namely, the Layered Integrated Framework Example model. The model is based on Wilber’s (J Conscious Stud 4(1):71–92, 1997) Integral Framework, which features the four main ontological ‘dimensions’ of the person. We then stratify these dimensions to produce a comprehensive conceptual map of the person, and of the potential areas of application for APP. For example, we deconstruct the collective dimensions of Wilber’s framework using the levels of Bronfenbrenner’s (Am Psychol 32(7):513–531, 1977) experimental ecology. The result is a detailed multidimensional framework which facilitates a comprehensive approach to promoting wellbeing, and which charts a way forward for APP.
No caption available
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
1 23
Journal of Happiness Studies
An Interdisciplinary Forum on
Subjective Well-Being
ISSN 1389-4978
J Happiness Stud
DOI 10.1007/s10902-014-9563-y
The LIFE Model: A Meta-
TheoreticalConceptual Map for Applied
Positive Psychology
Tim Lomas, Kate Hefferon & Itai Ivtzan
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and all
rights are held exclusively by Springer Science
+Business Media Dordrecht. This e-offprint
is for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you wish
to self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.
REVIEW ARTICLE
The LIFE Model: A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map
for Applied Positive Psychology
Tim Lomas Kate Hefferon Itai Ivtzan
Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014
Abstract Since its emergence in 1998, positive psychology has flourished. Among its
successes is the burgeoning field of applied positive psychology (APP), involving inter-
ventions to promote wellbeing. However, the remit of APP is currently unclear. As such,
we offer a meta-theoretical conceptual map delineating the terrain that APP might con-
ceivably cover, namely, the Layered Integrated Framework Example model. The model is
based on Wilber’s (J Conscious Stud 4(1):71–92, 1997) Integral Framework, which fea-
tures the four main ontological ‘dimensions’ of the person. We then stratify these
dimensions to produce a comprehensive conceptual map of the person, and of the potential
areas of application for APP. For example, we deconstruct the collective dimensions of
Wilber’s framework using the levels of Bronfenbrenner’s (Am Psychol 32(7):513–531,
1977) experimental ecology. The result is a detailed multidimensional framework which
facilitates a comprehensive approach to promoting wellbeing, and which charts a way
forward for APP.
Keywords Positive psychology Applied psychology Wellbeing Practice
Intervention
It is just over 16 years since Martin Seligman used his 1998 APA presidential address to
inaugurate the new field of positive psychology (PP) (Fowler et al. 1999). Since then, PP
has flourished, drawing in graduate recruits and established scholars, and attracting funding
and interest from diverse sources within academia and beyond (Rusk and Waters 2013).
One of the most successful aspects of PP is the emergent discipline of applied positive
psychology (APP), which can be defined as the science and practice of improving well-
being. At the heart of APP is a growing corpus of positive psychology interventions (PPIs)
designed to promote wellbeing in practical ways, as elucidated throughout this article. APP
T. Lomas (&)K. Hefferon I. Ivtzan
Department of Psychology, University of East London, Stratford Campus, London E15 4LZ, UK
e-mail: t.lomas@uel.ac.uk
123
J Happiness Stud
DOI 10.1007/s10902-014-9563-y
Author's personal copy
has proved to be a very fruitful branch of PP, as reflected in the number of postgraduate
courses devoted to APP. However, in spite of, or rather because of, the rapid emergence of
APP, the development and accumulation of PPIs has accelerated ahead of any systematic
theoretical conceptualisation of the field (Parks and Biswas-Diener 2014). As such, the
present article offers one such conceptualisation, in the form of a model which we have
labelled the Layered Integrated Framework Example (LIFE) model. The article is in three
parts. In the first section, we examine the nature and remit of APP, and consider what
constitutes a PPI. In the second section, we outline the need for a multidimensional
approach to wellbeing. In the third section, we articulate the LIFE model itself, and use it
to organise the field of APP, thus providing a comprehensive strategy for enhancing
wellbeing.
1 Applied Positive Psychology and Positive Psychology Interventions
Turning first to the nature and remit of APP, the spirit of APP is one of praxis. In the
Nichomachean Ethics, Aristotle [2000 (350 BCE)] constructed a tripartite classification of
human activities: poie¯sis (productive/creative disciplines); theo¯ria (contemplative
endeavours); and praxis (practical occupations involving the skilful application of ideas).
In more recent times, an eloquent articulation of praxis was formulated by Marx
[1978(1845), p. 145], who suggested that ‘The philosophers have only interpreted the
world, in various ways. The point, however, is to change it.’ The concept of praxis has been
influential in the social sciences, where it is defined as ‘practical action informed by
theory’ (Foster 1986, p. 96). In the case of APP, this ‘practical action’ is specifically in the
direction of improving wellbeing. This spirit of praxis has seen PP being applied in various
practical domains, from positive education (Seligman et al. 2009) to positive clinical
psychology (Wood and Tarrier 2010). In formulating our LIFE model, we offer a way of
conceptualising the diverse ways in which APP can promote wellbeing across different
areas of life.
Before we introduce this model, it is worth asking what APP does in these applied
domains. PPIs constitute the core of APP; however, there is on-going debate around the
necessary and sufficient criteria for recognising interventions as PPIs. Parks and Biswas-
Diener (2014) identify and critique three broad conceptualisations of PPIs. Content-level
definitions define PPIs as interventions that focus on ‘positive topics.’ A weakness here is
that this encompasses any pleasant activity, with no requirement that any positive outcome
(after the event) be generated. Variable level definitions identify PPIs as interventions that
work upon a positive mechanism (e.g., broaden-and-build; Fredrickson 2001) or produce a
positive outcome (e.g., positive cognitions). While this definition is more selective, at issue
is that the concept of a ‘positive’ outcome is often vaguely operationalized. A third type of
definition identifies PPIs as practices designed to promote wellness, rather than fix dys-
function and alleviate distress. However, this definition overlooks recent developments
around using PPIs for the treatment of mental disorders, e.g., positive psychotherapy for
depression (Seligman et al. 2006). Moreover, all these definitions struggle to accommodate
recent theorising in PP around the inadequacy of simplistic binary polarisations of ‘posi-
tive’ and ‘negative.’ For example, ostensibly positive emotions/qualities can have adverse
outcomes; e.g., optimism is implicated in under-appreciation of risk (Peterson and Vaidya
2003). Conversely, negative emotions may ultimately serve wellbeing; e.g., anxiety can
alert us to potential threats (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi 2000). Going further, Lazarus
(2003) queries the very possibility of even assigning a singular valence to emotions; for
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
example, hope is co-valenced, comprising both the wish for a desired outcome, and fear
that it will not happen. Moreover, from a wider psychosocial perspective, the significance
and desirability of emotions cannot be judged without taking context into account; for
example, forgiveness can be maladaptive in the context of an on-going abusive relationship
(McNulty 2011). More philosophically, the relationship between positive and negative is
inherently dialectical; these are binary terms that are by definition conceptually co-
dependent (Ryff and Singer 2003). Thus, Resnick et al. (2001) caution against polarising
psychology into ‘good’ and ‘bad,’ but urge us to appreciate the complexities of the good
life. This more nuanced approach to wellbeing has been labelled ‘positive psychology 2.0’
(Wong 2011).
Given these conceptual debates around the term ‘positive,’ it can be difficult to state with
certainty what constitutes a PPI. Moreover, it may not be possible to categorise a particular
intervention as being exclusively a PPI. For example, since Kabat-Zinn (1982) operational-
ized the concept of mindfulness, it has been applied in diverse settings: in being used to treat
physical illness, like chronic pain, it could be regarded as a medical intervention (Kabat-Zinn
1982); in being adapted for the treatment of mental illness, like depression, it might be
deemed a clinical psychology intervention (Teasdale et al. 2000). However, mindfulness has
also been extensively used in non-clinical settings, not for alleviating distress per se, but for
promoting general wellbeing (Smith et al. 1995). This last example might offer us one
potential way of identifying PPIs: PPIs may be defined not so much by the practices them-
selves as by the population they are applied to; we could thus broadly define PPIs as
empirically-validated interventions designed to promote wellbeing in a non-clinical popu-
lation. For example, one-to-one PPIs might be viewed as ‘therapy for people who don’t want
therapy.’ It is important to state that this would not stop PPIs still being used to address mental
health issues, as with positive psychotherapy (Seligman et al. 2006). The salient point here is
how one conceptualises such issues, and classifies those people deemed to be suffering from
them. The question of when dysphoria become recognised and treated as clinical disorders is
much debated (Flett et al. 1997). Nevertheless, one can identify situations in which a person is
judged to be experiencing a mental health problem, but this is not regarded as a clinical
disorder, either by clinicians or by the person themselves. For instance, a person may ascribe
their distress to a ‘legitimate’ sense of existential anomie, rather than view it as a psycho-
pathology. In such cases, some people may have previously undertaken psychotherapy;
others may now choose to engage with a psychologist who could provide a relevant PPI.
Furthermore, this general definition of PPIs (interventions to promote wellbeing in a non-
clinical population) would not prevent PP informing treatment in clinical settings; for
example, activities typically regarded as PPIs (e.g., gratitude journals; Emmons and
McCullough 2003) may be used in clinical psychology as adjunctive treatments alongside
more conventional therapies (Wood and Tarrier 2010). Nevertheless, this general definition
does offer a potential heuristic for ascertaining what constitutes a PPI. Having considered
what PPIs are, the LIFE model below can help us identify the areas of application in which
PPIs might be used. However, before articulating this model, we discuss the value of, and
need for, a multidimensional approach to wellbeing.
2 Multidimensional Approaches to Wellbeing
The proposed LIFE model involves a multidimensional conceptualisation of the person,
and therefore offers a multidimensional approach to wellbeing. Such approaches to health
and wellbeing are increasingly common. An early pioneering example is the World Health
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
Organization’s (1948) definition of health as ‘a state of complete physical, mental and
social well-being, and not merely the absence of disease and infirmity.’ This recognises
three main dimensions to the person, and their health/wellbeing: physical, mental, and
social. The same dimensions are also evident in Engel’s (1977) biopsychosocial model of
health. More closely related to PP, Jahoda’s (1958) ‘positive mental health’ construct
featured a biopsychosocial approach to wellbeing. However, our LIFE model is based on a
newer multidimensional conceptualisation of the person, the Integral Framework, devel-
oped by the American philosopher Wilber (1997). We shall first outline Wilber’s frame-
work, and its relevance to PP, before explaining below how we have adapted it through a
process of stratification to create our own LIFE model. Wilber’s framework is described as
an ontological ‘map’ elucidating ‘the basic dimensions of an individual’ (Esbjo
¨rn-Hargens
2010, p. 73). The particular innovation of Wilber’s framework is that it identifies four
dimensions, rather than the three dimensions of the WHO, Engel, and Jahoda models.
These dimensions are produced by juxtaposing two common binaries, creating a logically
appealing framework.
The first binary is the mindbody dichotomy. The relationship between subjective ‘mind’
and objective ‘body’ has perplexed thinkers throughout the ages, and was famously
described by Chalmers (2004) as the ‘hard problem’ of philosophy. Various perspectives
have developed over the centuries: materialistic monism treats the physical body as the
primary (or even only) reality, with subjectivity treated reductively as an illusion or epi-
phenomenon; conversely, transcendental monism (or idealism) views substance as a mental
construct (e.g., a figment of mind); finally, dualistic perspectives acknowledge the reality of
both body and mind, with various theories taking different positions on the nature of their
interaction. Dualism underlies the dominant paradigm in contemporary consciousness
studies, the neural correlates of consciousness (NCC) approach, which proposes that states of
mind are accompanied by analogous neurophysical states (Fell 2004). Currently the para-
digm aims only to chart the neurophysiological correlates of mental states—our under-
standing is not sufficiently advanced to ascertain directional causality, or solve Chalmers’
(2004) ‘hard problem’ (i.e., how brain activity might generate consciousness). Nevertheless,
the NCC approach acknowledges both subjective mind and objective body/brain. This
‘mind–body’ distinction, then, is one of the two binaries that create the Integral Framework.
The second binary is the individual-collective dichotomy. This reflects the idea that there are
two fundamental ‘modes of existence’—‘agency’ and ‘communion’ (Bakan 1966). Agency
refers to the way people exist as discrete, autonomous individuals, whereas communion
reflects the idea that people are also inextricably embedded in socio-cultural networks that
sustain their being, physically and/or mentally. The study of these modes of being has tended
to be somewhat compartmentalised in academia, with psychology focusing on agency for
example, and communion addressed by fields like sociology. However, theorists have begun
to acknowledge the limitations of studying these modes in isolation, and the need to explore
their complex interactions. Thus, the term ‘psychosocial’ has become increasingly prominent
in academia (Martikainen et al. 2002). Consequently, the individual-collective distinction
constitutes the second dichotomy that forms the Integral Framework.
Wilber’s (1997) innovation was to juxtapose these two binaries, creating a two-by-two
matrix of four quadrants, as shown in Fig. 1. Beginning with the top left of the schematic,
the subjective-individual quadrant is the domain of the mind, an umbrella term encom-
passing general subjective experience, including conscious thoughts, feelings and sensa-
tions (as well as unconscious subjective dynamics). The objective-individual quadrant is
the domain of the body/brain, i.e., physiological functioning and behaviour. The sub-
jective-collective quadrant is the domain of relationships, and the common hermeneutic
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
world space these generate, including shared meanings and values; we can refer to this
‘intersubjective’ domain as ‘culture’ (as in ‘the culture’ of a group of people). Finally, the
objective-collective quadrant is the domain of structural social networks, like the material
manifestations of communities (such as transport or housing infrastructure), or more
intangible processes such as economic systems; we can refer to this ‘interobjective’
domain as ‘society.’ Wilber’s framework represents a powerful tool for conceptualising
wellbeing in an integrated way. For example, Hanlon et al. (2010, p. 307) have deployed it
in public health to understand the ‘maze of interconnected problems’ which affect well-
being. Their article presents a hypothetical case-study, which is comparable to the fol-
lowing example. A person is depressed due to unemployment. Considered from the
individual-subjective perspective, their suffering can be appraised in terms of distress,
understood using cognitive theories of mental illness, and addressed through therapy. From
the individual-objective perspective, their suffering can be conceptualised in terms of brain
dysfunction, understood with neurochemical theories, and treated through medication.
From the subjective-collective perspective, their suffering can be apprehended in terms of
cultural meanings around unemployment, understood through social constructionist theo-
ries, and tackled by challenging societal norms. Finally, from the objective-collective
perspective, their suffering can be considered in terms of socio-economic factors that both
contribute to and result from unemployment, understood through economic and political
theories, and addressed through efforts towards a fairer society. Hanlon et al. argue that all
these ‘key dimensions of human experience need to be considered, harmonized and acted
on as a whole’ to fully address the wellbeing of the person (p. 311).
Before setting out our own adaptation of Wilber’s framework in the form of the LIFE
model, we can see how his framework constitutes a powerful device for organising con-
structs within PP. Its power resides partly in the fact that it is ‘content free:’ rather than
advocating theories in a given area, extant theories and research from that area can be
situated within the quadrants (this is why Wilber’s framework, and our LIFE model
adaptation, are meta-theoretical models). First, within the subjective domain, we can sit-
uate the abundance of constructs directly pertaining to mental health and illness. Here,
wellbeing can be conceptualised positively as the presence of desired outcomes, like
hedonic pleasure, or negatively as the absence of mental disorder and distress. Desirable
outcomes include the triumvirate of elements comprising the well-lived life according to
Seligman (2002): the pleasurable life (constructs like SWB); the engaged life (notions like
SUBJECTIVE OBJECTIVE
INDIVIDUAL
COLLECTIVE
SUBJECTIVE
Individual conscious
experience
MIND
OBJECTIVE
Correlated physical
substrates
BODY
CULTURE
Relationships and
shared meanings
INTERSUBJECTIVE
SOCIETY
Material systems
And structures
INTEROBJECTIVE
Fig. 1 Schematic diagram of the four quadrants, adapted from Wilber (1997)
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
flow; Csikszentmihalyi 1990); and the meaningful life [as reflected in Ryff’s (1989) model
of PWB]. (Situating these constructs here does not mean they are unconnected to the other
domains. Ryff’s model encompasses relationships, for instance, which pertains to the
intersubjective domain. Indeed, the point of an integrated framework such as Wilber’s is
that the domains are interlinked.) This domain also includes the array of desirable psy-
chological qualities explored by PP, such as resilience (Masten 2001). Additionally, as
argued above, the remit of PP not only covers these ostensibly ‘positive’ constructs, but
extends to ‘negative’ phenomena like sadness (Wong 2011), which can also be located
here. As the most ‘psychological’ domain, this is the root quadrant; the others are only
relevant to PP to the extent that they impinge on this domain (e.g., affect SWB). Never-
theless, examining the other domains enables us to appreciate the multitude of factors
which can influence wellbeing.
The objective domain concerns the physiological functioning and behaviour of the
body and the brain. First, this quadrant encompasses everything relating to physical
health, and so overlaps with Seligman’s (2008) concept of ‘positive health.’ Larson (1999)
has identified numerous models of health, including: the WHO model (noted above); the
medical model, which defines health as the ‘absence of disease and disability’ (p. 124);
the wellness model, concerned with ‘progress towards higher levels of functioning’ (p.
129); and the environmental model, pertaining to successful adaptation to one’s milieu.
These models can all be situated here, as can the diverse health behaviours which impact
upon physical wellbeing, like exercise (Hefferon and Mutrie 2012). It is worth empha-
sising that some models and concepts may not ‘fit’ precisely within one quadrant, but
intersect or overlap more than one quadrant. For instance, the ‘wellness model’ of health
pertains to connections between physiological functioning and subjective states. Indeed, as
noted above, a central point about Wilber’s framework is that the quadrants are not
hermetically sealed, but interact and reciprocally influence each other. As such, a key
element of research located in this quadrant is the exploration of connections between
bodily and subjective states. For example, embracing the NCC paradigm (Fell 2004), a
‘positive neuroscience’ research program has begun exploring the ‘neural correlates of
wellbeing’ (Urry et al. 2004), such as trait asymmetric activation of the pre-frontal cortex
(Davidson 2000).
The intersubjective domain covers relationships, and the shared ‘culture’ (e.g., val-
ues) that these generate. A key construct pertaining to this domain is social capital,
defined by Bourdieu (1986, p. 248) as the ‘sum total of the resources, actual or virtual,
that accrue to an individual (or a group) by virtue of being enmeshed in a durable
network of more or less institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance and
recognition.’ Social capital is a complex and elastic construct, encompassing all types of
relationship of relevance to PP. These range from bonds within the home, addressed by
specialities like positive relationship science (Fincham and Beach 2010), to relations at
school or at work, explored by positive education (Seligman et al. 2009) and positive
organizational scholarship (Cameron et al. 2003). This domain captures the manifold
ways in which relationships are central to wellbeing, from engendering positive emo-
tions (e.g., love) to being sources of social support (Umberson and Montez 2010). It
also encompasses the emergent forms of ‘culture’ generated by relationships. This
includes the way cultural systems can generate values and worldviews that are generally
considered conducive to wellbeing, like religion (Koenig 2009), or detrimental, like
materialism (Van Boven 2005). (An intriguing question here is whether Wilber’s (1997)
Integral Framework, and our LIFE model, are themselves culturally specific worldviews,
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
as we explore in an endnote.
1
) This domain also includes cultural norms—e.g., in
relation to gender (Lomas 2013)—that influence behaviour and so affect wellbeing.
Lastly, the interobjective domain refers to structural aspects of society: impersonal
processes, institutions and environments which ‘scaffold’ people’s lives. These structures
range from the material conditions of the surrounding environment to macro-economic
forces that influence employment. This domain thus encompasses the work of theorists
across different fields exploring how such structures impact upon wellbeing, from eco-
nomics to international development to politics. In economics, SWB has been used as an
alternative to gross domestic product (GDP) as an index of societal progress, and conse-
quently scholars have assessed the impact of factors like income on SWB (Layard 2005).
From an international development perspective, relevant structural factors include indices
used by the UN (2013) to calculate their ‘human development index,’ i.e., living standards,
health outcomes, and education provision. Political factors influencing wellbeing include
the quality of governance, as assessed by the World Bank for instance (Kaufmann et al.
1999). Further potential factors extend to the quality of the environment (e.g., air pollu-
tion), from a local community level (Burke et al. 2009) to a wider national and even global
level (New Economics Foundation 2013).
3 The LIFE Model
Having introduced Wilber’s (1997) Integral Framework, we can now examine the way we
have adapted this to produce our own LIFE model, represented graphically in Fig. 2.
Essentially, this model introduces further nuance to our understanding of wellbeing by
viewing each domain as being layered or stratified, delineating different strands within
them. There are many possible ways of ‘carving up’ the domains, and the approach
adopted in the LIFE model is by no means the only viable option; indeed, Wilber himself
identifies different strata within his own model (focusing primarily on the emergence of
particular qualities in human evolution). This is the reason our own adaptation is called the
‘Layered Integrated Framework Example’—our model is just one example of how such
layering might be done, and of a multidimensional model generally. Our approach is to
1
The issue of whether Wilber’s (1997) Integral Framework is itself a culturally specific worldview is
somewhat complex. Wilber’s epistemological position might arguably be described as critical realist. While
critical realism recognises that truth and objectivity are problematic, it disavows radical relativism, and
holds that it is ‘‘unwise and premature to abandon wholesale, claims to objectivity and the search for ‘truth’’
(Layder 1998, p. 3). Wilber (1995) acknowledges that all viewpoints (including theories such as his own) are
inextricably perspectival and culturally situated; there is no Archimedean ‘view from nowhere’ (Nagel
1989). However, at the same time, Wilber holds that some viewpoints are more accurate than others, and
even if we cannot achieve absolute objectivity, we can make progress towards it; here he cites Thomas Kuhn
(1970, p. 206), who, despite formulating the powerful concept of shifting scientific paradigms, remained a
‘convinced believer in scientific progress’. As such, Wilber (1995) contends that his framework does
accurately capture the four basic ontological dimensions of the person. At the same time, he allows that this
framework could yet be superseded by still more accurate conceptualisations; for example, while in a
relative sense the distinction between objective body and subjective mind may be valid, people in future
may eventually deem this distinction to be ultimately illusory, as some spiritual teachings suggest [e.g., Sri
Aurobindo 1970]. The authors take a similar critical realist position with respect to the domains of the LIFE
model, which are regarded as provisionally accurate and valid. The issue is slightly different with regard to
the layering of the LIFE model. As set out in the section on layering, the identification here of five specific
layers is somewhat arbitrary; one could easily stratify the domains in any number of ways, choosing a
greater or lesser number of layers, or deciding to focus on different specific layers, depending on one’s
agenda and priorities.
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
view each domain as comprising various ‘levels,’ where each level encompasses or
supersedes the level ‘below’ it. This stratification approach will become clear as we
consider the domains in turn, and outline how we have delineated various levels within. In
keeping with the focus of this paper on applied PP, we shall identify relevant interventions
for each level in turn. In many instances, we draw on existing PPIs; in that sense, the
model simply clarifies the field as it stands. At times though, the model helps us identify
potential PPIs that could be developed in future, thus pointing the way ahead for APP.
Starting with the subjective domain, four different phenomenological ‘strata’ are readily
identifiable: embodied sensations, emotions, cognitions, and conscious awareness. More
contentiously, one could regard these strata as proceeding from ‘lower’ to ‘higher’ in some
respects, with each level superseding the one before. This claim rests on two premises.
First, phylogenetically, these strata arguably emerged in this sequence: embodied sensa-
tions would have preceded discursive cognitions in our evolutionary progression (MacLean
1990). Second, and similarly, this sequence arguably also applies to ontogeny: infants are
thought to develop sensory capacities before they acquire emotions, and complex thoughts
later still (Piaget 1971). To these layers we have added a more contentious fifth stratum,
labelled tentatively as ‘awareness?.’ This level reflects the work of theorists who propose
that conscious awareness can be superseded by further advanced developmental capacities
(Josipovic 2010). Such capacities include ‘non-dual awareness,’ the experiential dissipa-
tion of the dualistic ‘subject-object construct,’ which is regarded as an advanced stage of
consciousness associated with spiritual experience and development (Wilber 1997).
From an APP perspective, we can examine PPIs that ‘work on’ the various subjective
levels. This means psychological techniques for enhancing wellbeing (i.e., the kind one
could do sitting alone at home), in contrast to PPIs involving actions/behaviour (which we
can situate in the objective quadrant), relationships (the intersubjective quadrant), or
structural features of the environment (the interobjective quadrant). Proceeding through the
strata, we can identify PPIs pertaining to each of the levels, thus providing a compre-
hensive arsenal of strategies aimed at the subjective domain. However, we must emphasise
that these levels are not rigidly separated, and most PPIs touch upon more than one level.
For example, PPIs targeted at emotions include interventions to enhance emotional
intelligence (EI; Mayer and Salovey 1997); while evidently pertaining to emotions, EI also
involves emotional awareness (i.e., consciousness) and ratiocination (i.e., cognitions).
Thus, describing PPIs as targeting a particular level is simply a heuristic device, and we
must strive to remain cognizant of the complex interrelationships between the levels.
Nevertheless, in terms of appreciating the range of PPIs available, differentiating these
according to the levels is a useful strategy.
Taking first embodiment, PP has begun to explore the relationship between embodied
subjectivity and wellbeing, for example in relation to post-traumatic growth (PTG)
(Hefferon et al. 2010). Such work has helped address Seligman’s (2008) call, in his paper
on positive health, for PP to become more than a ‘neck-up’ focused discipline. From an
APP perspective, practices like ‘body-mind’ therapy work with embodiment in promoting
wellbeing (Barratt 2009). Moving ‘up’ levels, the importance of positive emotions to PP
can hardly be overstated, these being almost the defining feature of the field. However, in
terms of identifying emotion-focused PPIs, this does not simply mean PPIs that promote
positive emotions as an outcome, as arguably most PPIs are ultimately geared towards
making people feel better in some way (e.g., engendering SWB). Rather, emotion-focused
PPIs are those that develop people’s ability to work with their emotions, i.e., emotional
management skills. There are PPIs designed to cultivate particular attitudinal qualities,
such as loving-kindness meditation to promote compassion (Fredrickson et al. 2008), or
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
Japanese Naikan therapy to engender gratitude (Chan 2010). More comprehensive emo-
tion-focused interventions also exist. For instance, Mayer and Salovey’s (1997) hierar-
chical model of EI comprises four branches: emotional awareness, generation,
understanding, and management. Accordingly, PPIs have been developed which target all
four branches (e.g., Crombie et al. 2011). Moving up to the cognitive level, appreciation of
the relevance of discursive thoughts to wellbeing has generated cognitively-focused PPIs.
Some involve recalling and describing positive events in discursive prose, including
activities based around journaling (e.g., the gratitude journal; Emmons and McCullough
2003) and writing about positive memories (Burton and King 2004). Other interventions
include narrative restructuring exercises, where people reflect on ostensibly negative
experiences and develop meaningful narratives around these. For example, Garland et al.
(2007) created a 6-week intervention for cancer sufferers: through journaling and creative
arts (e.g., writing, drawing), participants engaged in narrative self-exploration, which
fostered PTG. Located here too are innovative therapies using PP in the context of the
treatment of mental health issues, such as wellbeing therapy (Fava and Ruini 2013).
Stepping up to the level of conscious awareness, the exemplar PPI in this regard is
mindfulness. Following Kabat-Zinn’s (1982) pioneering mindfulness-based stress reduc-
tion (MBSR) programme, interest in mindfulness has exploded, with over 500 studies in
2012 alone (Shonin et al. 2013). The MBSR protocol has led to a proliferation of adap-
tations, ranging from mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, designed to prevent depressive
Fig. 2 The layered integrated framework example (LIFE) model
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
relapse (Teasdale et al. 2000), to novel treatments such as smoking cessation (‘surfing the
urge’; Bowen and Marlatt 2009). Finally, we might note that meditation is also associated
with the kind of advanced developmental skills and states of consciousness that we are
tentatively referring to here as ‘awareness?’ (Wilber 1997). Awareness?also serves as a
helpful overarching term for practices that might otherwise be referred to as ‘spiritual.’
Spirituality is a contested concept. Some conceptualisations explicitly bind it to numinous
experiences; the Encyclopaedia of Religion and Society defines it as the ‘quality of an
individual whose inner life is oriented toward God, the supernatural, or the sacred’ (Ya-
mane 1998, p. 492). Less contentiously perhaps (from a metaphysical perspec-
tive),‘intrinsic origin’ definitions depict spirituality simply as an ‘inner search for meaning
and fulfilment’ (Graber 2001, p. 40). Inclusion of spirituality in the LIFE model is in
keeping with Wilber’s (1997) Integral Theory; Wilber himself was influenced by spiritual
philosophers, particularly the Integral Yoga of Sri Aurobindo [1970 (1939–1940)]. Thus,
under the rubric of awareness?we can locate the various practices people worldwide have
created to help them access a sense of spirituality. Most cultures have developed spiritual
practices, usually in the context of specific religious traditions. For instance, in Sufi Islam,
‘Sama’ involves ‘reverently listening to music and/or the singing of mystical poetry’
(Lewisohn 1997, p. 3). In future, PP could draw on such practices in creating new PPIs. An
example in this respect is Mardiyono et al. (2011), who discuss using Islamic ‘relaxation
techniques,’ incorporating prayer and recitation of the Qur’an, in the context of nursing
practice.
Turning to the body/brain domain, hierarchical stratification of levels here is perhaps
easier to envisage: biochemical molecules and atoms (e.g., sodium ions) form neurons;
neurons combine in neural networks; such networks are part of the larger nervous system;
and the nervous system is one aspect of the whole body. (Reinforcing the point about our
stratification being just one ‘example,’ one could easily identify other viable hierarchies,
perhaps featuring more gradations or highlighting other elements.) In terms of APP, we can
examine the influence of each level on wellbeing, and moreover, design interventions to act
on that level. For example, at the biochemical level, mental illness can be conceptualised in
terms of the activity of neurotransmitters like serotonin; interventions here aim to alter
biochemical ‘imbalances,’ as with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) treatments
(Ferguson 2001). Such interventions are presently limited to medical disciplines like
psychiatry, used in the treatment of psychopathology. However, research has pointed to the
potential positive impact on wellbeing of psychoactive drugs such as MDMA (Adamson
and Metzner 1988). It is conceivable that clinicians will in future harness such substances
to pro-actively promote wellbeing; for example, Sessa (2007, p. 220) argues that psychi-
atrists could explore ‘MDMA-assisted psychotherapy’ as an adjunct to more conventional
treatments.
Moving up levels, we can explore the impact of neural networks on wellbeing. These
networks refer to the way mental activities are produced by the interaction of distributed
brain areas, assessed by paradigms such as electroencephalography (EEG). As per the NCC
approach, EEG analysis connects wellbeing to patterns of neural activity, such as greater
left-sided hemispheric activation (Davidson 2000). Moreover, from an APP perspective,
such patterns can be promoted by interventions like neurofeedback; for instance, Gruzelier
et al. (2013) used neurofeedback with children to enhance music creativity and general
wellbeing. Neurofeedback activities can be situated within a larger framework of bio-
feedback, which targets the nervous system more generally. For example, Kleen and
Reitsma (2011) developed a promising intervention that combined heart rate variability
(HRV) biofeedback training (reduced HRV is associated with outcomes like anxiety) with
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
mindfulness. In terms of the body as a whole, scholars have begun to explore the complex
intersections between physical health/illness and SWB and PWB (Hefferon 2013). From an
APP perspective, PPIs focusing on the body include exercise (Hefferon and Mutrie 2012)
and dance therapy (Puig et al. 2006).
Finally, we turn to the collective domains of the LIFE model—the intersubjective and
interobjective realms. Incorporating these domains into PP can help address one of the
most prominent criticisms levelled against the field, namely that it pays insufficient
attention to the social dimensions of wellbeing. Critical theorists such as Becker and
Marecek (2008) argue that conceptualisations of wellbeing in PP reflect a culturally-
specific version of the good life, building upon a North American tradition of expressive
individualism, where happiness is framed as a private concern, achieved through self-
determined choices. Although social institutions are one of Peterson’s (2006) ‘three pillars’
of personal fulfilment, there has been little analysis within PP of structural factors that
might affect a person’s ability to flourish, like educational and economic opportunities
(Prilleltensky and Prilleltensky 2005). As such, learning from such critics, PP can develop
a more comprehensive approach to wellbeing by taking the collective domains into
account. In the LIFE model, we have stratified these domains using Bronfenbrenner’s
(1977) influential experimental ecology, which identifies various socio-cultural ‘levels’
ranging in scale from micro to macro. This model can be used for both collective domains,
as it straddles the two quadrants: one can analyse all levels from either an intersubjective
(e.g., shared values) or an interobjective perspective (e.g., structural aspects of that level).
However, we have omitted the first level of Bronfenbrenner’s model—the individual
person themselves (e.g., their cognitive processes)—from these collective domains, since
in our LIFE model, Bronfenbrenner’s first level has been massively expanded, constituting
in effect the entire subjective and objective domains. Thus, in terms of the intersubjective
and interobjective domains, we begin the stratification at the second tier of Bronfen-
brenner’s model, the ‘microsystem.’ We shall consider these levels in turn, highlighting
examples of PPIs that pertain to each level.
The micro-system is the immediate social setting of the person, e.g., their family or
workplace. To reinforce the point about Bronfenbrenner’s model straddling both collective
domains, we can approach these settings from an intersubjective (e.g., a family’s shared
values) and/or an interobjective perspective (e.g., their material circumstances). When PP
has taken social dimensions into account, it has usually been limited to this level. In PP, the
importance of the micro-system is recognised in studies highlighting the powerful asso-
ciation between relationships and wellbeing; indeed, according to Helliwell and Putnam’s
(2004) analysis of the World Values Survey, close relationships such as marriage are the
most important contextual determinant of wellbeing. More broadly, a wealth of studies
attest to the importance to wellbeing of friendship and good relationships with others
generally (Umberson and Montez 2010). From an APP perspective, one approach to the
micro-system involves enhancing the quality of relationships. We have already touched
upon some relevant PPIs above; for example, practices to promote pro-social emotions,
like loving-kindness meditation, can enhance relational connectedness (Fredrickson et al.
2008). (The fact that many PPIs have positive outcomes across multiple domains serves to
reinforce the point that the four domains—of both the LIFE model and Wilber’s Integral
Framework—intersect and reciprocally influence each other. Moreover, multiple-domain
outcomes are also indicative of the potency of a PPI, i.e., its ability to positively impact
upon manifold aspects of a person’s life.) There are also more comprehensive PPIs aimed
at enhancing relationships. Kauffman and Silberman (2009) highlight the use of PP in
couple’s therapy. For instance, they suggest that ‘growth-fostering relationships’ can be
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
engendered by teaching people effective communication strategies, like responding posi-
tively to good news, so-called ‘active-constructive responding’ (Gable et al. 2004). Such
PPIs aimed at enhancing relationships are transferable to other micro-systems: active-
constructive communication is promoted in positive organizational scholarship as an
effective leadership strategy, for example (Avolio et al. 1999). Alternatively, from an
interobjective perspective, APP might involve improving the material environment of the
micro-setting, e.g., enhancing its aesthetics. Gesler’s (1992) work on therapeutic land-
scapes has emphasised the twin beneficial effects of pleasing environments: rewarding to
appreciate and create. For example, natural light and greenery are conducive to emotional
wellbeing, which is why gardens are often incorporated into healthcare settings (Marcus
and Barnes 1999). Moreover, creating such environments in itself constitutes a PPI: gar-
dening is a therapeutic activity, a potent combination of ‘achievement, satisfaction, and
aesthetic pleasure’ (Milligan et al. 2004, p. 1781), and also social bonding if done
collectively.
Next, the broader network of the meso-system refers to the interaction between micro-
systems. This level recognises that people ‘exist in inter-locking contexts’ which together
affect functioning (Sheridan et al. 2004, p. 7). Indeed, Prilleltensky et al. (2001, p. 151)
argue that ‘clinical and community interventions are inseparable.’ Unfortunately though,
Prilleltensky et al. suggest the meso-system perspective is often overlooked in psychology:
‘we typically psychologize children’s problems and ignore the social and political context
in which their problems occur’ (p. 157). However, researchers are beginning to appreciate
the importance of taking a meso-system approach to wellbeing and general psychological
development. Meso-level PPIs involve working with clients across diverse settings. For
example, Sheridan et al. argue that, for children, their two primary micro-systems are home
and school, which have a ‘bidirectional, reciprocal influence over each other’ (p. 11). As
such, Sheridan et al. have formulated a child-focused meso-system intervention—‘family-
centred positive psychology’ (FCPP)—which establishes partnerships between families
and schools through a process of ‘conjoint behavioural consultation.’ Parents and teachers,
together with an FCPP ‘consultant,’ engage in a structured problem-solving process,
spanning home and school, to address the ‘academic, social, or behavioural needs’ of a
child who is troubled in some way (p. 10). Another successful meso-system intervention is
the ‘Families and Schools Together’ (FAST) programme (McDonald et al. 1997), a
multifamily school-centred intervention, designed to build protective factors for children
(e.g., enhancing bonds with parents), which has been endorsed by Save the Children in the
UK and the United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime.
Scaling up still further, the ‘exo-system’ refers to structures that ‘encompass the
immediate settings,’ such as the wider community in which micro-systems are situated
(Bronfenbrenner 1977, p. 515). Community factors have a significant impact upon well-
being. Burke et al. (2009) identified 120 factors, aggregated into six main categories, which
were a blend of interobjective and intersubjective elements: necessary human and social
services; neighbourhood support (i.e., social capital); green areas and natural environment;
social make-up of people (e.g., age-diversity); neighbourhood affordability; and an absence
of negative community factors (e.g., noise). From an APP perspective, any action to
enhance any of these factors could be deemed a PPI, since this would have the effect of
improving the neighbourhood, thereby enhancing the wellbeing of people within it. This
could be done in a top down way, i.e., local authorities designing neighbourhoods
according to wellbeing principles. For example, open communal spaces where people
can safely interact serve to enhance the collective wellbeing of a community, particularly
if these incorporate greenery, as per the concept of therapeutic landscapes (Chiesura 2004).
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
Improvements can also be enacted in a ‘bottom-up’ way by communities themselves. For
instance, Mongkolnchaiarunya (2005) outlines a community-based waste management
initiative in Thailand. This not only addressed the interobjective factor of waste disposal, it
engendered intersubjective benefits like ‘community empowerment through self-reliance’
(p. 27). Less functionally, but still powerfully, community arts initiatives—such as dec-
orating public spaces—not only improve the aesthetics of the neighbourhood, but promote
social capital and individual wellbeing (Dunphy 2009). More comprehensive interventions
have also been attempted. For example, in the UK, the Well London project was devised to
improve health behaviours among marginalised communities in deprived areas of the city
(Wall et al. 2009). This was a non-prescriptive collaborative project, working with local
communities to identify and meet their needs; as such, the particular projects used with
each community varied. Among the successful projects were ‘Healthy Spaces’ (improving
public spaces to encourage physical activity) and ‘Be Creative Be Well’ (communal
creative arts to promote social capital).
The most expansive of Bronfenbrenner’s (1977, p. 515) levels is the macro-system—
‘overarching institutional patterns such as the economic, social, educational, legal, and
political systems’ of which the other levels are ‘concrete manifestations.’ Exemplar macro-
level analyses include the World Bank’s assessment of the ‘quality of governance’ world-
wide, examining over 200 countries across six dimensions (Kaufmann et al. 1999): voice and
accountability; stability; effectiveness; regulatory framework; rule of law; and control of
corruption. These dimensions can impact upon wellbeing; e.g., there is a negative correlation
between civil rights violations and country-level SWB (Diener et al. 1995). As summarised
by Duncan (2010, p. 165), the happiest communities are not necessarily the wealthiest, but
those who enjoy ‘effective social and political institutions.’ In terms of APP, interventions at a
macro level involve shaping public policy according to wellbeing considerations (Evans
2011). Indeed, recent UN-commissioned analyses of global happiness levels have led to just
these kinds of wellbeing-focused structural recommendations (Helliwell et al. 2013). The
pioneering example of wellbeing as a policy driver is Bhutan, which in 1972 replaced GDP as
their gauge of societal progress with gross national happiness (GNH) (Braun 2009). Cru-
cially, from an APP perspective, GNH is not simply measured, but is reportedly used by the
government to inform policy decisions, with all proposed policies systematically evaluated
according to GNH considerations.
Although Bhutan is ahead of other nations in this regard, the idea of policy being
informed by wellbeing considerations has been gathering strength for some years in other
countries. For example, in the UK, Huppert and Baylis (2004) argued that policies could be
enacted to help counter stereotypes and attitudes that were detrimental to people’s well-
being, such as making occupational age-related discrimination illegal. Such arguments
were heeded, as the Age Discrimination Act was enacted into law in the UK in 2006.
Moreover, recent years have seen more systematic top-down efforts to consider the policy
implications of wellbeing. In the UK, the Office for National Statistics (ONS) began in
2011 to include SWB items within its annual Integrated Household Survey, disseminated
to 200,000 people, thus generating a National Well-being index (ONS 2011). Moreover,
the Prime Minister David Cameron announced in 2010 that this index would help guide
governmental decisions, and consequently established an internal policy unit—the
Behavioural Insights Team, often referred to as the ‘nudge unit’—geared towards this end
(Bache and Reardon 2013). This unit has helped develop policies based on Thaler and
Sunstein’s (2003) concept of ‘libertarian paternalism.’ These policies are designed to
encourage people to make healthy choices, crucially though, without coercion, but by
arranging the ‘choice architecture’ in ways that makes the ‘right’ (i.e., healthy) choice
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
more likely (e.g., by making it the default option). Other macro-system PP initiatives
pertain to economic factors. For example, Haque (2011) has called for a ‘positive eco-
nomic paradigm’ that is not only concerned with profit maximisation, but with fulfilling
‘human potential.’
Finally, we have taken the liberty of ‘adding’ another level to Bronfenbrenner’s original
model, namely the global ecosystem. The biosphere encompasses all the other systems,
being the physical matrix that supports their very existence. This level matters to PP
because, in an existential sense, human wellbeing is ultimately dependent upon planetary
wellbeing (Smith et al. 2013): concerns about flourishing are fundamentally subservient to
the survival of humankind, since one must be alive in order to flourish. This recognition is
reflected in attempts to take ecological variables into account, like societal sustainability,
when calculating macro-levels of wellbeing (New Economics Foundation 2013). More-
over, this level falls within the purview of APP since environmental wellbeing depends to
an extent on human behaviour. As such, we can devise PPIs that might impact positively
on the environment, intervening at any of the levels of Bronfenbrenner’s model to
encourage more sustainable behaviours. At a micro-system level, sustainable energy
consumption can be promoted through smart meters that provide households with feedback
about their energy expenditure (Fischer 2008). From a meso-system perspective, energy
consumption used commuting between micro-systems might be reduced, perhaps through
interventions to encourage ‘active commuting,’ like walking/cycling (Yang et al. 2010).
Exo-system PPIs could include efforts to promote recycling: in intersubjective terms, this
may involve attempting to make it a cultural norm or a valued pro-social behaviour
(Hopper and Nielsen 1991); in interobjective terms, this means enhancing the provision of
recycling services (Read 1999). At a macro-system level, PP could play a role in advo-
cating for regulatory solutions, such as national and international commitments to envi-
ronmental sustainability (Spash 2010), by advancing a wellbeing policy agenda.
4 Conclusion
The LIFE model presented here offers a comprehensive multidimensional approach to
wellbeing. It identifies different dimensions of relevance to PP, including subjective mind,
objective body/brain, intersubjective culture and interobjective society. Moreover, it
stratifies these domains into various levels, introducing further nuance and complexity into
the model. The LIFE model represents the potential terrain for APP, which we define here
as the science and practice of improving wellbeing; we can intervene across all these
dimensions, targeting all of their levels, to actively promote wellbeing. As emphasised
throughout, our LIFE model is but one possible multidimensional model, and one way of
stratifying these dimensions. As such, the model is not intended as a final word on the kind
of multidimensional framework that can drive PP forward. Rather, this paper is just the
beginning of a broader conversation in PP about the value of, and need for, a multidi-
mensional approach to wellbeing.
References
Adamson, S., & Metzner, R. (1988). The nature of the MDMA experience and its role in healing, psy-
chotherapy and spiritual practice. ReVision, 10, 59–72.
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
Aristotle. [2000 (350 BCE)]. In R. Crisp (Ed.), Nicomachean ethics. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Aurobindo, S. [1970 (published serially 1914–19; revised first edition published 1939–40)]. The life divine
(5th edn.). Pondicherry, India: Sri Aurobindo Ashram Trust.
Avolio, B. J., Bass, B. M., & Jung, D. I. (1999). Re-examining the components of transformational and
transactional leadership using the Multifactor Leadership. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, 72(4), 441–462.
Bache, I., & Reardon, L. (2013). An idea whose time has come? Explaining the rise of well-being in British
politics. Political Studies, 61(4), 898–914.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Barratt, B. B. (2009). The emergence of somatic psychology and bodymind therapy. London: Palgrave
MacMillan.
Becker, D., & Marecek, J. (2008). Dreaming the American dream: Individualism and positive psychology.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1767–1780.
Bourdieu, P. (1986). The forms of capital. In J. G. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of theory and research for
the sociology of education (pp. 241–258). New York: Greenwood.
Bowen, S., & Marlatt, A. (2009). Surfing the urge: Brief mindfulness-based intervention for college student
smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23(4), 666–671.
Braun, A. A. (2009). Gross national happiness in Bhutan: A living example of an alternative approach to
progress. (Bachelor’s Thesis), Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychol-
ogist, 32(7), 513–531.
Burke, J., O’Campo, P., Salmon, C., & Walker, R. (2009). Pathways connecting neighborhood influences
and mental well-being: Socioeconomic position and gender differences. Social Science and Medicine,
68(7), 1294–1304.
Burton, C. M., & King, L. A. (2004). The health benefits of writing about intensely positive experiences.
Journal of Research in Personality, 38(2), 150–163.
Cameron, K. S., Dutton, J. E., & Quinn, R. E. (2003). Foundations of positive organizational scholarship. In
K. S. Cameron, J. E. Dutton, & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship: Foundations
of a new discipline (pp. 3–13). San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler.
Chalmers, D. J. (2004). How can we construct a science of consciousness? In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The
cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chan, D. W. (2010). Gratitude, gratitude intervention and subjective well-being among Chinese school
teachers in Hong Kong. Educational Psychology, 30(2), 139–153.
Chiesura, A. (2004). The role of urban parks for the sustainable city. Landscape and Urban Planning, 68(1),
129–138.
Crombie, D., Lombard, C., & Noakes, T. (2011). Increasing emotional intelligence in cricketers: An
intervention study. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 6(1), 69–86.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.
Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity.
American Psychologist, 55(11), 1196–1214.
Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjective Wellbeing of nations. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 851–864.
Duncan, G. (2010). Should happiness-maximization be the goal of government? Journal of Happiness
Studies, 11(2), 163–178.
Dunphy, K. F. (2009). Developing and revitializing rural communities through arts and creativity: Aus-
tralia. Vancouver, BC: Centre for Policy Studies on Culture and Communities, Simon Fraser
University.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental
investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84(2), 377.
Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196(4286),
129–136.
Esbjo
¨rn-Hargens, S. (2010). Integral theory in service of enacting integral education. In S. Esbjo
¨rn-Hargens,
J. Reams, & O. Gunnlaugson (Eds.), Integral education: New directions for higher learning (pp.
57–78). Albany: State University of New York Press.
Evans, J. (2011). Our leaders are all Aristotelians now. Public Policy Research, 17(4), 214–221.
Fava, G. A., & Ruini, C. (2013). Well-being therapy: Theoretical background, clinical implications, and
future directions. In S. A. David, I. Boniwell, & A. C. Ayers (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of hap-
piness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
Fell, J. (2004). Identifying neural correlates of consciousness: The state space approach. Consciousness and
Cognition, 13(4), 709–729.
Ferguson, J. M. (2001). SSRI antidepressant medications: Adverse effects and tolerability. Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry, 3(1), 22–27.
Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. (2010). Of memes and marriage: Toward a positive relationship science.
Journal of Family Theory & Review, 2(1), 4–24.
Fischer, C. (2008). Feedback on household electricity consumption: A tool for saving energy? Energy
Efficiency, 1(1), 79–104.
Flett, G. L., Vredenburg, K., & Krames, L. (1997). The continuity of depression in clinical and nonclinical
samples. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 395–416.
Foster, W. (1986). A critical perspective on administration and organization in education. In K. A. Sirotnik
& J. Oakes (Eds.), Critical perspectives on the organization and improvement of schooling (pp.
95–129). Netherlands: Springer.
Fowler, R. D., Seligman, M. E. P., & Koocher, G. P. (1999). The APA 1998 annual report. American
Psychologist, 54(8), 537–568.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build
theory of positive emotions. The American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives:
Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resour-
ces. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1045–1062.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when things go right? The
intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 87(2), 228–245.
Garland, S. N., Carlson, L. E., Cook, S., Lansdell, L., & Speca, M. (2007). A non-randomized comparison of
mindfulness-based stress reduction and healing arts programs for facilitating post-traumatic growth and
spirituality in cancer outpatients. Supportive Care in Cancer, 15(8), 949–961.
Gesler, W. M. (1992). Therapeutic landscapes: Medical issues in light of the new cultural geography. Social
Science and Medicine, 34(7), 735–746.
Graber, D. R. (2001). Spirituality and healthcare organizations. Journal of Healthcare Management, 46(1),
39–50.
Gruzelier, J. H., Foks, M., Steffert, T., Chen, M. J. L., & Ros, T. (2013). Beneficial outcome from EEG-
neurofeedback on creative music performance, attention and well-being in school children. Biological
Psychology, 95, 86–95.
Hanlon, P., Carlisle, S., Reilly, D., Lyon, A., & Hannah, M. (2010). Enabling well-being in a time of radical
change: Integrative public health for the 21st century. Public Health, 124(6), 305–312.
Haque, U. (2011). Betterness: Economics for humans. Cambridge: Harvard Business Press.
Hefferon, K. (2013). Positive psychology and the body: The psychosomatic side to flourishing. Berkshire:
Open University Press.
Hefferon, K., Grealy, M., & Mutrie, N. (2010). Transforming from cocoon to butterfly: The potential role of
the body in the process of posttraumatic growth. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 50(2), 224–247.
Hefferon, K., & Mutrie, N. (2012). Physical activity as a ‘‘stellar’’ positive psychology intervention. In E.
O. Acevedo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of exercise psychology (pp. 117–130). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Helliwell, J., Layard, R., & Sachs, J. (Eds.). (2013). World happiness report 2013. Geneva: United Nations.
Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of well-being. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 359(1449), 1435–1446.
Hopper, J. R., & Nielsen, J. M. (1991). Recycling as altruistic behavior: Normative and behavioral strategies
to expand participation in a community recycling program. Environment and Behavior, 23(2),
195–220.
Huppert, F. A., & Baylis, N. (2004). Well-being: Towards an integration of psychology, neurobiology and
social science. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 359,
1447–1451.
Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive mental health. New York: Basic Books.
Josipovic, Z. (2010). Duality and nonduality in meditation research. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4),
1119–1121.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the
practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General
Hospital Psychiatry, 4(1), 33–47.
Kauffman, C., & Silberman, J. (2009). Finding and fostering the positive in relationships: Positive inter-
ventions in couples therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(5), 520–531.
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
Kaufmann, D., Kraay, A., & Zoido-Lobato
´n, P. (1999). Aggregating governance indicators. World Bank
Policy Research Working Paper No. 2195. Washington, DC.
Kleen, M., & Reitsma, B. (2011). Appliance of heart rate variability biofeedback in acceptance and com-
mitment therapy: A pilot study. Journal of Neurotherapy, 15(2), 170–181.
Koenig, H. G. (2009). Research on religion, spirituality, and mental health: A review. Canadian Journal of
Psychiatry, 54(5), 283–291.
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago press.
Larson, J. S. (1999). The conceptualization of health. Medical Care Research and Review, 56(2), 123–136.
Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a new science. London: Penguin.
Layder, D. (1998). Sociological practice: Linking theory and social research. London: Sage.
Lazarus, R. S. (2003). Does the positive psychology movement have legs? Psychological Inquiry, 14(2),
93–109.
Lewisohn, L. (1997). The sacred music of Islam: Sama¯’ in the Persian Sufi tradition. British Journal of
Ethnomusicology, 6(1), 1–33.
Lomas, T. (2013). Critical positive masculinity. Masculinities and Social Change, 2(2), 167–193.
MacLean, P. D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution: Role in paleocerebral functions. New York: Springer.
Marcus, C. C., & Barnes, M. (1999). Healing gardens: Therapeutic benefits and design recommendations.
Canada: Wiley.
Mardiyono, M., Songwathana, P., & Petpichetchian, W. (2011). Spirituality intervention and outcomes:
Corner stone of holistic nursing practice. Nurse Media Journal of Nursing, 1(1), 117–127.
Martikainen, P., Bartley, M., & Lahelma, E. (2002). Psychosocial determinants of health in social epide-
miology. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(6), 1091–1093.
Marx, K. [1978(1845)]. Theses on Feuerbach: Thesis 11. In R. C. Tucker (Ed.), The Marx-Engels reader
(2nd edn.). New York: W. W. Norton.
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56,
227–239.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. J. Sluyter (Eds.),
Emotional development and emotional intelligence (pp. 3–31). New York: Basic Books.
McDonald, L., Billingham, S., Conrad, T., Morgan, A., Nancy, O., & Payton, E. (1997). Families and
schools together (FAST): Integrating community development with clinical strategies. Families in
Society: The Journal of Contemporary Social Services, 78(2), 140–155.
McNulty, J. K. (2011). The dark side of forgiveness: The tendency to forgive predicts continued psychological
and physical aggression in marriage. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(6), 770–783.
Milligan, C., Gatrell, A., & Bingley, A. (2004). ‘Cultivating health’: Therapeutic landscapes and older
people in northern England. Social Science and Medicine, 58, 1781–1793.
Mongkolnchaiarunya, J. (2005). Promoting a community-based solid-waste management initiative in local
government: Yala municipality, Thailand. Habitat International, 29(1), 27–40.
Nagel, T. (1989). The view from nowhere. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
New Economics Foundation. (2013). Happy planet index.http://www.happyplanetindex.org/.
Office for National Statistics. (2011). Initial investigation into subjective well-being from the opinions
survey. London: Office for National Statistics.
Parks, A. C., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2014). Positive interventions: Past, present and future. In T. Kashdan &
J. Ciarrochi (Eds.), Mindfulness, acceptance, and positive psychology: The seven foundations of well-
being (pp. 140–165). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Peterson, C. (2006). A primer in positive psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Peterson, C., & Vaidya, R. S. (2003). Optimism as virtue and vice. In E. C. Chang & L. J. Sanna (Eds.),
Virtue, vice, and personality: The complexity of behaviour (pp. 23–27). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Piaget, J. (1971). The theory of stages in cognitive development. New York: McGraw Hill.
Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G., & Peirson, L. (2001). The role of power and control in children’s lives: An
ecological analysis of pathways toward wellness, resilience and problems. Journal of Community &
Applied Social Psychology, 11(2), 143–158.
Prilleltensky, I., & Prilleltensky, O. (2005). Beyond resilience: Blending wellness and liberation in the
helping professions. In M. Ungar (Ed.), Handbook for working with children and youth (pp. 89–103).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Puig, A., Lee, S. M., Goodwin, L., & Sherrard, P. A. (2006). The efficacy of creative arts therapies to
enhance emotional expression, spirituality, and psychological well-being of newly diagnosed Stage I
and Stage II breast cancer patients: A preliminary study. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 33(3), 218–228.
Read, A. D. (1999). ‘‘A weekly doorstep recycling collection, I had no idea we could!’’: Overcoming the
local barriers to participation. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 26(3–4), 217–249.
A Meta-Theoretical Conceptual Map for Applied Positive Psychology
123
Author's personal copy
Resnick, S., Warmoth, A., & Serlin, I. A. (2001). The humanistic psychology and positive psychology
connection: Implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 41(1), 73–101.
Rusk, R. D., & Waters, L. E. (2013). Tracing the size, reach, impact, and breadth of positive psychology.
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(3), 207–221.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069–1081.
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (2003). Ironies of the human condition. Well-being and health on the way to
mortality. In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human strengths (pp.
271–287). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness. New York: Free Press.
Seligman, M. E. P. (2008). Positive health. Applied Psychology, 57, 3–18.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology. In J. E. Gillham (Ed.), The science
of optimism and hope: Research essays in honor of Martin EP Seligman (pp. 415–429). Pennsylvania:
Templeton Foundation Press.
Seligman, M. E. P., Ernst, R. M., Gillham, J., Reivich, K., & Linkins, M. (2009). Positive education:
Positive psychology and classroom interventions. Oxford Review of Education, 35(3), 293–311.
Seligman, M. E. P., Rashid, T., & Parks, A. C. (2006). Positive psychotherapy. American Psychologist,
61(8), 774–788.
Sessa, B. (2007). Is there a case for MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in the UK? Journal of Psychophar-
macology, 21(2), 220–224.
Sheridan, S. M., Warnes, E. D., Cowan, R. J., Schemm, A. V., & Clarke, B. L. (2004). Family-centered
positive psychology: Focusing on strengths to build student success. Psychology in the Schools, 41(1),
7–17.
Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Mindfulness-based interventions: Towards mindful
clinical integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(194). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00194.
Smith, L. M., Case, J. L., Smith, H. M., Harwell, L. C., & Summers, J. K. (2013). Relating ecoystem
services to domains of human well-being: Foundation for a US index. Ecological Indicators, 28,
79–90.
Smith, W. P., Compton, W. C., & West, B. W. (1995). Meditation as an adjunct to a happiness enhancement
program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(2), 269–273.
Spash, C. L. (2010). The brave new world of carbon trading. New Political Economy, 15(2), 169–195.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000).
Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 615–623.
Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2003). Libertarian paternalism. The American Economic Review, 93(2),
175–179.
Umberson, D., & Montez, J. K. (2010). Social relationships and health. Journal of Health and Social
Behavior, 51(1), 54–66.
United Nations. (2013). Human development report 2013. The rise of the south: Human progress in a
diverse world. New York: United Nations Development Programme.
Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C., Dalton, K. M., Mueller, C. J., et al. (2004). Making a
life worth living: Neural correlates of well-being. Psychological Science, 15(6), 367–372.
Van Boven, L. (2005). Experientialism, materialism, and the pursuit of happiness. Review of General
Psychology, 9(2), 132–142.
Wall, M., Hayes, R., Moore, D., Petticrew, M., Clow, A., Schmidt, E., et al. (2009). Evaluation of com-
munity level interventions to address social and structural determinants of health: A cluster randomised
controlled trial. BMC Public Health, 9(1), 207.
Wilber, K. (1995). Sex, ecology, spirituality: The spirit of evolution. Boston: Shambhala.
Wilber, K. (1997). An integral theory of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4(1), 71–92.
Wong, P. T. P. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life.
Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 52(2), 69–81.
Wood, A. M., & Tarrier, N. (2010). Positive clinical psychology: A new vision and strategy for integrated
research and practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 819–829.
World Health Organization. (1948). In Preamble to the constitution of the World Health Organization as
adopted by the international health conference, New York, 19–22 June, 1946. World Health
Organization.
Yamane, D. (1998). Spirituality. In W. E. Swatos Jr (Ed.), Encyclopaedia of religion and society (p. 492).
Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira.
Yang, L., Sahlqvist, S., McMinn, A., Griffin, S. J., & Ogilvie, D. (2010). Interventions to promote cycling:
Systematic review. British Medical Journal, 341, c5293.
T. Lomas et al.
123
Author's personal copy
... Many researchers nowadays agree that well-being is a multidimensional construct that can include physical, emotional, cognitive, and social aspects of personal, professional, and collective functioning (Dodge 2012;Lomas et al. 2014;Linton et al. 2016). However, to the best of our knowledge, there are no comprehensive multidimensional models or theories of well-being that describe and explain what makes software engineers flourish in their personal and professional lives (Singh and Junnarkar 2016). ...
... Furthermore, contrary to many other theories of software engineers' well-being, our theory goes beyond the professional and organizational realm, which is crucial for conceptualizing well-being as a multidimensional construct that includes physical, emotional, cognitive, and social aspects of individual and collective functioning (Dodge 2012;Lomas et al. 2014;Linton et al. 2016). Therefore, the insights gained from the theory can benefit both individual software engineers seeking to improve their wellbeing and work performance as well as the managers and leaders of software engineering teams and organizations working towards establishing a healthy culture and behaviors for their employees. ...
Article
Full-text available
For decades, software engineering research and practice has focused primarily on technological and process-related factors. Today, there is a growing interest in organizational, social, and psychological factors, including well-being. Organizational studies show that well-being contributes to work outcomes, including creativity, performance, and productivity. But despite its importance, the predictors and outcomes of software engineers’ well-being as a multidimensional construct to date are under-researched. This paper reports on the multidimensional well-being of professional software engineers and generates insights for the future research in this area. 44 quantitative survey studies published between 2000 and 2023 were selected and synthesized both quantitatively and qualitatively through a systematic literature review. The results of the review were further analyzed to construct a quantitatively-testable theory, detailing the predictors and outcomes of well-being in software engineering organizations. The total number of research participants included in the selected studies is 16,086 software engineering professionals from at least 42 countries. The literature review identified various measures, constructs, and indicators of well-being, as well as its predictors and outcomes. The theory, based on cumulative results of carefully selected quantitative studies, is an attempt to “correct the record” by establishing well-being in software engineering as a meta-construct of hedonic, eudaimonic, and integrated or hedaimonic qualities predicted by different individual, team and organizational factors and impacting the functioning of software engineers and their organizations. The review highlighted the under-researched aspects of well-being in software engineering and confirmed the need for more advanced quantitative studies. We hope that the theory will benefit researchers in conducting future studies and practitioners in developing nuanced and science-based interventions for improving software engineers’ well-being.
... Scholarship in this area views the human being as being comprised of numerous dimensions, the development of which in childhood in uences their development and expression in adulthood 31 . One such set of dimensions for example, as related to human ourishing, is articulated in the LIFE model 32 , which proposes a four-dimensional model of whole-human wellbeing or human ourishing (behavioural, affective, cognitive, moral). The model is based on the work of Ken Wilber 33 , who synthesized the work of many dozens of human-development theorists 31,34,35,36 . ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Psychology and related fields have historically been Western-centric, influenced especially by the ideas and values of the United States. Consequently, aspects of life that have been relatively neglected in Western cultures have been similarly overlooked in the literature. This includes balance in life (notwithstanding select instances of interest, like work-life balance). However, amidst a broader movement to redress the Western-centricity of academia is an emergent scholarship on this topic. This paper contributes to this by presenting the most ambitious longitudinal study to date of balance, namely as an item – “In general, how often are the various aspects of your life in balance?” – in the Global Flourishing Study, a five-year (minimum) study investigating the predictors of human flourishing involving (in this first year) 202,898 participants from 22 countries. Specifically, this paper looks at the childhood predictors of balance, using random effects meta-analysis to aggregate all findings in the main text, focusing on three research questions. First, how do different aspects of a child's upbringing predict balance in adulthood, for which the most impactful factor was “Feeling like an outsider growing up” (RR = 0.90; 95% CI [0.87,0.92]), and the least was immigration status (RR = 0.98; 95% CI [0.97, 1.00]). Second, do these associations vary by country, with the effect of being an outsider, for example, being strongest in Australia, and weakest (not different than zero) in Türkiye, South Africa, Nigeria, Poland, Egypt, Hong Kong, Indonesia, and the Philippines. Third, are the observed relationships robust to potential unmeasured confounding, as assessed by E-values, which on the whole was the case. These results shed new light on the cross-cultural and causal dynamics of this overlooked topic, and provide the foundation for further enquiry.
... Social support is considered to fulfil these fundamental needs. Life model (Lomas, Hefferon & Ivtzan, 2015) and third wave Positive Psychology arguments (Lomas et al., 2021), also suggest embracing the context of the individual, ...
... Notwithstanding the qualms of those wishing to limit assessment of the good life to hedonic measurement, psychologists have continued to develop eudaimonic measures, most often denoted by terms like flourishing or thriving, which evidences the value scholars place on flourishing (Diener et al., 2010;Fowers et al., 2023;Ryff, 1989;VanderWeele, 2017). Virtually all contemporary conceptualizations of flourishing portray it as a multidimensional set of constructs that is not reducible to a single facet (de Chavez et al., 2005;Lomas et al., 2015). Even when flourishing scholars create or use single dimensional scales, the items of the scales are designed to encompass various flourishing dimensions (Diener et al., 2010;Novak et al., in press). ...
Article
Interest in the topic of human flourishing has burgeoned. This article discusses what is required for a general account of flourishing. It builds on three previous critiques of flourishing conceptualization that clarified the lack of systematic theorizing, the overemphasis on psychometric investigations, and the acultural manner of conceptualization. Addressing these difficulties is necessary to move toward a more cohesive, cumulative science of flourishing. The first theme of the article is a vital first step toward providing a systematic theory of flourishing. The article appropriates Aristotle’s conception of eudaimonia (flourishing or good living) to contemporary concerns. The proposed Eudaimonic Theory defines flourishing, specifies its content in terms of human goods, discusses flourishing as a way of life (i.e., not a one-time achievement or subjective experience), and discusses virtue traits in a flourishing life. A second theme reaffirms the Aristotelian commitment to empirical (broadly conceived) verification. Psychometric evaluations of flourishing measures are useful, but insufficient evidence for a flourishing science. Therefore, hypotheses are provided for heuristic research guidance. The third theme is that flourishing must be made sufficiently capacious to accommodate the substantial cultural variation in flourishing conceptions. The article concludes with a promising proposal for formulating a general account of flourishing.
Article
Full-text available
Positive psychological therapy has been found to be effective in psychological health in the broader cancer patient population. However, specific evidence regarding its effectiveness for adolescent and young adult (AYA) cancer patients remains limited. More research is needed to determine the optimal approaches for alleviating distress in this particular group. We evaluated the efficacy of character strength-based cognitive-behavioral therapy (CS-CBT) versus cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) for distress in AYA patients with cancer. In a randomized controlled three-arm trial, cancer patients aged 15-39 years were assigned to one of three groups using a random number table to ensure equal group sizes: (1) CS-CBT, which incorporated specific exercises focused on identifying and utilizing character strengths within the CBT framework; (2) CBT; or (3) control group. The primary outcome was improvement in patients’ overall mental health on the Distress Thermometer and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale 3 months after assignment to the groups. Analyses used generalized estimating equations. A total of 162 AYAs were randomly and equally assigned to three groups. Improvements in primary outcomes were significantly greater after CS-CBT than after CBT, and no significant differences were found between the CBT and control group. Among all primary and secondary outcomes, the only non-significant difference found was in quality of life with CS-CBT compared to CBT. AYA cancer patients rated their overall mental health and sense of thriving as more improved after CS-CBT than after CBT. However, quality-of-life indicators supported equal intervention effects in both groups. These findings support the effectiveness of the use of CS-CBT intervention in reducing distress and improving thriving.
Article
Full-text available
Concepts like flourishing, well-being, health, and happiness are of increasing interest across many fields, from psychology and medicine to politics and economics. However, these terms are used in diverse and contested ways, which makes it hard to find common ground and understanding. To attempt to help remedy the confusion, this paper offers an overarching conceptual "map" within which these concepts can be situated, thereby providing a common language and framework for their consideration. Moreover, while the overall configuration of this map is conceptually and logically stable, its specific elements are more flexible, particularly in terms of scalable granularity (allowing fine-grained differentiation of internal regions) and epistemological openness (allowing revisions in light of gains in knowledge). As such, the map can be adapted to suit different fields, and updated to accommodate advances in understanding. To that end, we clarify topics of investigation that are still in need of development, providing a roadmap for future research.
Article
Full-text available
This review investigates the relevance of socio-cognitive mindfulness (Langer, 1989) to wellbeing coaching by systematically synthesising the evidence to understand how socio-cognitive mindfulness interventions work. The search yielded 2,867 peer-reviewed studies with twelve papers meeting the eligibility criteria. The interventions induced socio-cognitive mindfulness with non-clinical adults via one or more psychological processes to achieve intrapersonal, interpersonal and environmental wellbeing. Six of the studies employed exercises to produce boosts in wellbeing, whilst six conducted extended programmes, of which three demonstrated sustained wellbeing improvements. The findings indicate that socio-cognitive mindfulness could provide valuable insights for practitioners and synergistic benefits for wellbeing coaching.
Article
This commentary presents certain limitations inherent in the discipline of Psychology that renders the possibility of inclusion or integration of Indian thoughts regarding human subjective experience at a disadvantage vis-à-vis what is termed as ‘modern’ Psychology in the article authored by Prof. Durganand Sinha. Like many other realities, the limitations mostly originate from inequities with regard to power and social justice. The other sources pertain to excessive reliance on positivism. Skewed representation of cultural constructions of the human subjective experience acts as a barrier in making the discipline vibrant and growing in spite of its long years of independent existence. The Concept of Self is illustrated as a case in point.
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary cross-cultural research on flourishing and development has been limited by a focus on Western populations and typically Western priorities, and by attention to only a few indicators of flourishing, such as life satisfaction, life expectancy, or GDP per capita. This paper highlights some significant challenges for robust cross-national and cross-cultural research on the domains and drivers of flourishing. Using data from the recently proposed Global Comparison Framework and the Gallup World Poll, we explore the within- and between-country heterogeneity of flourishing and its determinants across the 22 countries which are the subject of the Global Flourishing Study. Sources of heterogeneity considered include potential tradeoffs among domains of flourishing; the effects of cultural differences on the conceptualization and actualization of flourishing; and the cultural specificity of core analytical concepts, including “life evaluation” and “nation.”
Article
Full-text available
This study evaluated mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT), a group intervention designed to train recovered recurrently depressed patients to disengage from dysphoria-activated depressogenic thinking that may mediate relapse/recurrence. Recovered recurrently depressed patients (n = 145) were randomized to continue with treatment as usual or, in addition, to receive MBCT. Relapse/recurrence to major depression was assessed over a 60-week study period. For patients with 3 or more previous episodes of depression (77% of the sample), MBCT significantly reduced risk of relapse/recurrence. For patients with only 2 previous episodes, MBCT did not reduce relapse/recurrence. MBCT offers a promising cost-efficient psychological approach to preventing relapse/recurrence in recovered recurrently depressed patients.
Article
Full-text available
Historically, depression researchers have examined continuity in terms of whether the symptoms and characteristics of mild, moderate, and severe depression differ in degree along a continuum (i.e., a quantitative difference) or in kind (i.e., qualitative difference). The authors propose a differentiated framework that distinguishes 4 direct tests of continuity (i.e., phenomenological, typological, etiological, and psychometric continuity). They use this framework to suggest that most evidence is consistent with the continuity hypothesis. Moreover, they maintain that the findings of future research can be incorporated into a 2-factor model of depression that allows for both continuities and discontinuities.
Article
Full-text available
Subjective well-being (SWB) in 55 nations, reported in probability surveys and a large college student sample, was correlated with social, economic, and cultural characteristics of the nations. The SWB surveys, representing nations that include three fourths of the earth's population, showed strong convergence. Separate measures of the predictor variables also converged and formed scales with high reliability, with the exception of the comparison variables. High income, individualism, human rights, and societal equality correlated strongly with each other, and with SWB across surveys. Income correlated with SWB even after basic need fulfillment was controlled. Only individualism persistently correlated with SWB when other predictors were controlled. Cultural homogeneity, income growth, and income comparison showed either low or inconsistent relations with SWB.
Article
The effect of a grateful outlook on psychological and physical well-being was examined. In Studies 1 and 2, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 experimental conditions (hassles, gratitude listing, and either neutral life events or social comparison); they then kept weekly (Study 1) or daily (Study 2) records of their moods, coping behaviors, health behaviors, physical symptoms, and overall life appraisals. In a 3rd study, persons with neuromuscular disease were randomly assigned to either the gratitude condition or to a control condition. The gratitude-outlook groups exhibited heightened well-being across several, though not all, of the outcome measures across the 3 studies, relative to the comparison groups. The effect on positive affect appeared to be the most robust finding. Results suggest that a conscious focus on blessings may have emotional and interpersonal benefits.