A preview of this full-text is provided by American Psychological Association.
Content available from Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
160
An Operant Analysis of Joint Attention Skills
Per Holth
Abstract
Joint attention, a synchronizing of the attention of two or more persons, has been an increasing focus of
research in cognitive developmental psychology. Research in this area has progressed mainly outside of behavior
analysis, and behavior-analytic research and theory has tended to ignore the work on joint attention. It is argued here,
on the one hand, that behavior-analytic work on verbal behavior with children with autism needs to integrate the
research body on joint attention. On the other hand, research on joint attention should integrate behavior-analytic
principles to produce more effective analyses of basic processes involved. An operant analysis of phenomena
typically considered under the heading of joint attention is followed by examples of training protocols aimed at
teaching joint attention skills, such as social referencing, monitoring, gaze following, and such skills interwoven with
mands and with tacts. Finally, certain research questions are pointed out.
Keywords: Joint attention, Language training, autism.
During the last 25 years, there has been an increasing preoccupation with ‘joint attention’ as a crucial
area in children’s ‘social-cognitive development.’ Research has focused on normative patterns of emergence
of joint attention skills (e.g., Corkum & Moore, 1995) and on how such skills are related to later developing
skills summarized as ‘symbolic abilities’ (Hobson, 1993; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1993), ‘language abilities’
(Baldwin, 1995; Bates, Benigni, Bretherton, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1979; Bruner, 1975; Tomasello, 1988),
and ‘general social-cognitive processes in children’ (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Bruner, 1975; Mundy, 1995;
Tomasello, 1995). Moreover, it appears that children diagnosed with autism may display a syndrom-specific
deficit in joint attention skills (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1989, Mundy & Crowson, 1997; Sigman & Kasari, 1995;
Sigman, Kasari, Kwon, & Yirmiya, 1992). It seems strange, then, that behavior analysts, and even those
working in the field of autism, have not paid much attention to the work on joint attention.
Research within the cognitive-developmental tradition has typically focused on identifying
characteristic patterns of responding in different groups of children and on the consistency of responding
across situations and over time (cf., Moore & Dunham, 1995). In spite of the fact that the whole body of
“cognitive” research on joint attention focuses on behavior that needs to be analyzed in great detail, this field
appears to have developed almost completely apart from behavior analysis. Some researchers have even
argued specifically against behavior-analytic interpretations in this area (e.g., Bruner, 1995; Tomasello, 1995).
Recently, however, other researchers have occasionally called for some joint ventures of traditional
joint-attention researchers and behavior analysts in an effort to develop intervention programs that might
effectively remedy joint attention deficiencies in children with autism (e.g., Mundy, 2001; Mundy &
Crowson, 1997).
The general aim of the present article is to show that an operant analysis is basically relevant to
research on joint attention and to the aim of developing procedures that might help remedy basic deficiencies
in joint attention typically displayed by children with autism. Specific aims of the current presentation are (1)
to decompose the concept of joint attention sufficiently to make it amenable to an operant analysis, (2) to
show how certain well-established basic behavioral processes can be utilized in interventions that attempt to
correct deficiencies in joint attention skills in children with autism, and (3) to outline additional basic operant
research that is needed in order to account for the variables of which joint attention skills are a function.
The concept of ‘joint attention’
Issues treated under the heading of ‘joint attention’ range from the early work by Bruner and
colleagues on gaze following (e.g., Scaife & Bruner, 1975) to issues related to children’s so-called
development of a “Theory of Mind” (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1991; Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Sherman,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
161
1986). Therefore, ‘joint attention’ may not be particularly useful as a technical term unless the diversity of
phenomena currently referred to by the concept prove to covary as a unitary phenomenon. In order to
evaluate the concept as such, we need to analyze the different behavioral phenomena now listed as examples
of ‘joint attention.’ Before turning to an overview of some of the phenomena typically treated within this
realm, let me just briefly consider some attempted definitions.
Defining ‘joint attention’
According to Baldwin (1995), “technically speaking, joint attention simply means the simultaneous
engagement of two or more individuals in mental focus on one and the same external thing” (p. 132). Very
often, however, a lot more may be implied by ‘joint attention.’ Sigman and Kasari (1995) distinguished
between a narrow and a broad definition of ‘joint attention.’ The narrower definition refers simply to “looking
where someone else is looking.” Their broader definition includes what they called “responsive and initiating
behaviors as well as the checking of another person’s face that occurs while the infant is playing with
something, when the infant has accomplished some task, after the infant has pointed to something, or in an
ambiguous situation” (p. 189). Further, according to Bruner (1995), “joint attention involves knowing that
another is looking at and experiencing something in the visual world’” (p. 7). Tomasello (1995) includes in his
definition that “both participants are monitoring the other’s attention to the outside entity,” and that “the
coordination that takes place in joint attentional interactions is accomplished by means of an understanding
that the other participant has a focus of attention to the same entity as the self” (p. 105-107). Finally, Sarria,
Gomez, & Tamarit (1996) observed that although joint attention “typically refers to coordination of visual
attention, . . .[it] may be achieved through other sensory modalities, such as vocalizations or physical contact”
(p. 49).
In A Preliminary Manual for the Abridged: EARLY SOCIAL COMMUNICATION SCALES (ESCS),
Mundy, Hogan, & Doehring (1996) suggested that “the function of [joint attention] behaviors is to share
attention with the interactive partner or to monitor the partner’s attention. They differ from Requesting bids in
that they do not appear to serve an instrumental or imperative purpose.” However, it is clear according to
Corkum & Moore (1995), “joint attention plays an integral part in both the protodeclarative and
protoimperative gestures” (p. 64).
Although empirical studies usually rely on some narrower operational definition of joint attention such
as those specified in the ESCS by Mundy et al. (1996), people working in this area seem to agree that the
concept of ‘joint attention’ implies something in addition to those operationalized skills. This additional
implication has been described as “knowing that another is looking at and experiencing something in the visual
world” (Bruner, 1995, p. 7), “understanding that the other participant has a focus of attention to the same
entity as the self” (Tomasello, 1995, p. 107), or “the recognition that mental focus on some external thing is
shared” (Baldwin, 1995, p. 132). Such “knowing,” “understanding,” or “recognition” is, of course, harder to
specify. Dunham and Moore (1995) predicted that research will, eventually, lead to the decomposing of joint
attention into “the series of transformations that are presumably occurring in social cognition across
developmental time” (p. 23). From a behavior-analytic perspective, a decomposition may be required in order
to bring the phenomena under investigation within reach of its scientific principles.
Phenomena treated under the heading ‘joint attention’
In order to take on the task of pinpointing the behavioral phenomena of which such phenomena can
consist, let us start by delineating some of the cruder categories that are typically conceived of as involving
‘joint attention.’ These include ‘gaze following,’ ‘social referencing,’ ‘protoimperative gestures,’
‘protodeclarative gestures,’ and ‘monitoring.’
(a) Look or gaze following. Perhaps the simplest examples of joint attention skills are those referred
to as ‘responsive joint attention,’ in which one looks where someone else is pointing or touching (following
proximal point/touch) or is looking in the direction of someone’s gaze or beyond the end of someone’s index
finger (following line of regard; see Mundy, Hogan, & Doehring, 1996). However, not even in these cases is
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
162
joint attention a purely formal class. As pointed out by Tomasello (1995), there are two common types of
adult—child interactions that may look like joint attention, but which lack the criterion of “knowing,”
“understanding,” or “recognition.” These are “onlooking” and “cued looking,” in which something may catch
the attention of two people simultaneously, but without one person’s attention influencing the other person’s
attention.
(b) Social referencing. When confronted with some novel stimulus, a child (typically) will look
toward a familiar person and subsequently react to the novel stimulus in accord with the displayed expression
of the familiar person. As in the case of gaze following, however, joint attention in such ‘social referencing’
requires that the child “understands” that the familiar person is attending to the same thing or event as the
child attends to. If, for instance, the child simply looks to a familiar person as a kind of ‘comfort seeking,’ this
will not count as ‘joint attention’ (cf. Baldwin, 1995).
(c) Protoimperative. Protoimperative gestures have been described as “gestures intended to make
another person do something for one’s benefit” (Sarriá et al., 1996). However, a simple contingency between
a gesture and a ‘beneficial effect’ can occur without ‘joint attention.’ Sometimes, the term ‘protoimperative’
has been preserved for cases that involve some type of “coordination of attention with other people” (Sarria
et al., 1996). In accord with this, Tomasello (1995) wrote: “My interpretation of protoimperative pointing in
the 12- to 14-month period, therefore is that the child is attempting not just to obtain the object but to change
the adult’s intentions so as that they become aligned with its own” (p. 111).
(d) Protodeclarative. Bates, Camaioni, and Volterra (1975) defined the protodeclarative as a
preverbal effort to direct other’s attention to an object or event. Tomasello (1995) interpreted the
protodeclarative as having “the purely social motive of sharing attention to something” (p. 111).
(e) Monitoring. Gaze or attention monitoring can take place in a simple responsive manner, as it
certainly does when we are just observing other people, as in a movie. However, such monitoring can be
interactive and involve acting to influence the other person’s attention. It appears that an emerging criterion
for using the term ‘joint attention’ or ‘true joint attention’ in all types of cases mentioned above is exactly the
interactive monitoring of another person’s attention. In developmental psychology, researchers have tried to
capture the essence of ‘joint attention’ in social-cognitive terms.
Why behavior analysts should study joint attention
There are, at least, three specific, good reasons why behavior analysts should be interested in
studying performances typically grouped under the heading of “joint attention.” We will explore each in this
section.
First, because children diagnosed with autism seem to display a syndrome-specific deficit in joint
attention skills (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1989, Mundy & Crowson, 1997; Sigman & Kasari, 1995; Sigman, et al.,
1992), outcome studies of applied work with children with autism should include measures of relevant joint
attention skills. Until intervention studies (e.g., Lovaaas, 1987; McEachin, Smith, & Lovaas, 1993) include
outcome measures that address the cardinal social and social-cognitive symptoms of the syndrome, their
results will remain open to the criticism that no children have been shown to even nearly “recover” from
autism. Even the best-outcome children of behavioral interventions, who do demonstrate major gains on
measures of IQ and social development, may continue to exhibit equally important difficulties on specific
social and cognitive skills (Mundy & Crowson, 1997).
Second, because research has linked joint attention skills to later developing ‘symbolic abilities’
(Hobson, 1993; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1993), ‘language abilities’ (Baldwin, 1995; Bates et al., 1979;
Bruner, 1975; Tomasello, 1988), and ‘general social-cognitive processes in children’ (Baron-Cohen, 1995;
Bruner, 1975; Mundy, 1995; Tomasello, 1995), the development of intervention technologies specifically
aimed to produce joint attention holds the potential of a significant breakthrough in interventions for children
with autism.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
163
Third, some cognitive psychologists have insisted that joint attention is not amenable to a learning
explanation and that behavior analysis is essentially irrelevant to this field of inquiry (e.g., Bruner, 1995;
Tomasello, 1995). The demonstration of how basic behavior principles may be involved in the establishment
of phenomena treated under the heading of ‘joint attention’ will be a substantial support to behavior-analytic
view that the role of behavior principles in ‘psychological development’ has been vastly underestimated in the
field of developmental psychology.
As noted by Schlinger (1993), “developmental psychologists have provided valuable information
about child development. Unfortunately, such information lacks a strong unifying theoretical background and
fails to impart practical knowledge that can enable psychologists to reliably change behavior in natural
settings”(p. viii). In many recent psychology textbooks and articles, the discussion of learning, not to mention
behavior analysis, is almost nonexistent, and even when sections on learning principles are included, authors
rarely refer back to these principles in analyses of complex phenomena. It is quite astonishing to observe that
not even the basic, well-documented principle of operant reinforcement is incorporated in developmental
psychology. In order to more fully appreciate the potential of an operant analysis of joint attention
phenomena, some knowledge of behavior principles and technical terms are required. These will be briefly
outlined here.
Behavior principles and some technical terms
A widespread misconception suggests that behavior analysis confines itself to what can be directly
observed and to responses that result from a direct conditioning history. For instance, according to Bruner
(1973) there is a tradition in psychology that prefers to stop at the level of behavior, dispensing with notions
like intention, “but it is a necessity for the biology of complex behavior, by whatever label we wish to call it”
(p. 2). The field of “intention” is, of course, the very field of operant behavior. An operant analysis will never
stop at the level of behavior, by whatever label we may wish to call it. On the contrary, an operant analysis
will instantly move on to the variables of which the behavior is a function.
Operant reinforcement
Whenever, colloquially speaking, someone intends to obtain an effect, we could say that the behavior
operates upon the environment (social or otherwise) to produce that effect. The operant reinforcement
principle is likely to be, at least partly, familiar to researchers in developmental psychology: When behavior is
followed by certain consequences, the frequency of such responses increase as a result. Basic behavioral
research has amply demonstrated the robustness of the reinforcement principle in human as in other animal
behavior. Even so, authors sometimes reject a behavior-analytic view as “less plausible” and assert that some
of the coordinated action in ‘joint attention’ is just an unlikely candidate for the conditioning explanation. For
instance, Tomasello (1995) contended that:
. . . while the conditioning explanation can never be ruled out completely, children’s spontaneous
gaze alternations, and the way they are coordinated with their ongoing social interactions at around 12
months of age, makes less plausible the conditioning explanation and more plausible the view that the
child understands that the adult is a separate person who has intentions and attention that may differ
from its own. (p. 109)
However, that the child “understands that the adult is a separate person” only sums up the current
structure of behavior-environment relations and obviously does not preclude the relevance of a conditioning
history. ‘Understanding’ can be considered as a summary label for complex sets of performances, but it does
not point to independent variables of which performance is a function.
Operant discrimination
In an operant analysis, ‘attention’ boils down to ‘stimulus control.’ Whenever, in colloquial terms, we
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
164
say that a child attends to something, behavior analysts will move on to specify the child’s behavior that is
controlled by that something. Why, then, would anyone prefer such a technical vocabulary? There is good
reason for the focus on relations between behavior and events in the environment. When that is
accomplished, a large body of research literature on how to establish and change stimulus control becomes
directly relevant to our work in this field. This literature includes work on simple stimulus control (e.g.,
Herrick, Myers, & Korotkin, 1959; Blough, 1958; Reynolds, 1961), compound stimulus control (cf.
Dinsmoor, 1995; Donahoe & Palmer, 1994), simultaneous and successive discrimination (e.g., Loess &
Duncan, 1952; Zentall & Clemet, 2001). More recently, there is a growing body of experimental research on
more complex stimulus control, to be noted below.
Conditioned reinforcement and behavior chains
There is sometimes a preconception regarding what can reasonably function as a reinforcer. For
instance, some have indicated that on the basis of the observations of “how social or sharing or reciprocal
such attentional activity is . . . it was . . . inevitable that we grew uncomfortable with learning theory
explanations of how eye-to-eye contact came into being, or how it shifted over to shared attention on
common objects. With respect to the former, there were even studies indicating that eye-to-eye contact itself
was reinforcing in learning tasks” (Bruner, 1995, p. 2). Similarly, according to Tomasello (1995), “in [the
case of declaratives] the child simply shows or shares something with an adult, which would not seem
amenable to a conditioning explanation as there are no apparent rewards involved.” Although he admits that
“if human beings are rewarded by smiles and other signs of acknowledgement from adults, then they might be
conditioned in their use of protodeclaratives as well,” he adds that “this stretches the conditioning explanation
somewhat out of shape” (p. 111). Why this should be stretching the conditioning explanation somewhat out
of shape is not explained, and I can think of no other reason for this suggestion than some sort of
preconception of what can possibly function as reinforcers, for instance couched in terms of a drive-reduction
theory (cf. Chomsky’s 1959 review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior).
The consequences of behavior that can function as reinforcers can be either: (1) purely material
things, (2) social stimuli, or (3) stimuli correlated with access to other (high-probability) activities (i.e., the
Premack Principle). Further, some reinforcers function as such without requiring any type of prior “learning,”
while others come to function as such only after they appear in certain types of relation to other reinforcers.
Although the details of the principles involved in the establishment of new, conditioned, reinforcers
may still need to be explored in some detail (cf. Fantino & Logan, 1979) we do know a lot about how to
establish new things or events as reinforcers. The standard procedure that is suggested in the literature of
applied behavior analysis (e.g., Lovaas et al., 1981; Maurice, Green, & Luce, 1996) is a “pairing” of stimuli
that one wants to establish as conditioned reinforcers with unconditioned or primary reinforcers. A safer, and
possibly more effective, procedure is to establish the new, to-be-conditioned, reinforcer as an SD for a
response that produces the unconditioned reinforcer (e.g., Dinsmoor, 1950; Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950;
Lovaas, Freitag, Kinder, Rubenstein, Schaeffer, & Simmons, 1966; Skinner, 1938). Behavior chains will then
build up, in which the reinforcing consequence of one behavioral element constitutes the occasion for other
behavior which typically produces reinforcement.
Generally, the effectiveness of conditioned reinforcers will depend on the presence of the establishing
operation (e.g., deprivation) that the primary reinforcement effect depends on. However, if the conditioned
reinforcer obtains its effect through a similar relation to a number of different primary reinforcers, it will
become a generalized conditioned reinforcer. The effectiveness of such reinforcers is less dependent upon
each specific establishing operation upon which each of the unconditioned reinforcers may depend.
Conditional discriminations
The three-term contingency SD àR à SR is, perhaps, the most robust behavior-analytic formula, but
behavior analysis is not limited to it. The three-term contingency can be placed under conditional or
contextual control: A response may be followed by a reinforcing event in the presence of a particular stimulus,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
165
but this relation may hold only in the presence of some additional stimulus (e.g., Sidman, 1986). For instance,
you may dial a telephone number in front of you to produce the voice of some interesting person, but only
when the dialing tone is present first. Similarly, in many social settings, responding to certain features in the
environment will be reinforced by other people, but only when they, too, attend to those features of the
environment as well. Features of conditional discrimination training have been studied in great detail over the
last 20 years and have been reported in the literature on stimulus equivalence (e.g., JEAB, 1996; Sidman,
1994).
Joint control
Sometimes, behavior depends on the simultaneous, or joint, control by two different stimuli over a
single response (e.g., Lowenkron, 1998). For instance, if somebody requests a 14 mm socket, you may
repeat “14mm” as you scan a number of sockets until you see one which controls the same response (saying
“14 mm”), such as one with “14mm” printed on it, before you stretch out and pick up that socket. In social
interactions, when you try to locate an object or event to which another person attends, it may be helpful if
you are told a name or otherwise given a description of that object or event. You may then visually scan the
environment until you see something that controls the same verbal response in you. In the absence of a verbal
description, you may simply respond, at least in part, like you observe the other person to do, and scan the
environment in that other person’s visual field until you see something that controls that same response (e.g.,
smiling or frowning) in you.
Conjugate reinforcement
Reinforcement is not just an on-or-off issue. In what has been termed ‘conjugate reinforcement,’
there is a contingent relation between the intensity (e.g., frequency) of the response and the intensity of some
continuously available stimulus, and changes in the intensity of the continuously available stimulus functions
as a reinforcer. (e.g., Lovitt, 1967; Rovee-Collier & Gekoski, 1979). Much of what may function as social
reinforcers, such as other persons’ attention may often not be an on-or-off matter, but a matter of intensity
typically related to an intensity of responding. For example, when guiding someone else’s attention, we may
be sensitive to small changes in the direction of the person’s looking in the right or wrong direction.
Continuous repertoires
Sometimes reinforcement is contingent upon a correspondence between response dimensions and
stimulus dimensions, as in what has been referred to as continuous fields (Skinner, 1953). Such continuous
fields may lead to continuous repertoires in which intermediate values on the stimulus dimension control
intermediate values on the response dimension, and extreme values on the stimulus dimension control
corresponding extreme values on the response dimension (Wildemann & Holland, 1972). Crude gaze
following, for example, may result from the direct training of only a limited number of different exemplars.
Observing responses
Organisms are, of course, not only passively exposed to stimuli. They operate on the environment as
if “gathering information” relevant to the issue of how to respond next. However, “gathering information”
may not accurately describe the function of such behavior. In an experiment on observing behavior by
Dinsmoor (1983), pigeons were exposed to a multiple schedule in which pecks on one key were extinguished
in the presence of a red light and reinforced according to a variable ratio schedule in the presence of a green
light. Under such circumstances, key pecking that produce either green or red light (that is correlated with
reinforcement and nonreinforcement, respectively) will be maintained. In terms of information value, the red
light and the green light should be equal. However, if responses on the observation key only produces green
light when the reinforcement schedule operates, pecking the observation key is maintained, whereas if only
the red light is produced when the extinction schedule is operating, responding to the observation key is not
maintained (Dinsmoor, 1983). To the extent that this finding can be extrapolated to human behavior,
monitoring the behavior of other persons is best maintained when some properties of their behavior serve as
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
166
positive discriminative stimuli, i.e., occasions for doing something that produces a reinforcing event.
Correspondingly, such monitoring may not be well maintained when distinct properties of the other person’s
behavior mainly function as negative discriminative stimuli (S∆) in the presence of which behavior is not
reinforced.
An operant analysis of joint attention performances
In colloquial terms, joint attention can be said to involve the detection of what another person attends
to. A fairly general principle seems to be that people particularly attend to things, events, or properties that are
novel. Such preference for novel stimuli is well documented even in infants and utilized in experiments on so-
called “recognition memory” using habituation procedures (e.g., Bornstein, 1976), paired-comparison (novelty
preference) procedures (e.g., Fantz, 1964), or novelty discrimination procedures (e.g., Werner & Siqueland,
1978). Further, when it comes to verbal skills and listening skills, people tend to report on deviations from
standard patterns of events and to listen to such reports with more interest than in reports on routine events or
things that do not change, except when invariability itself is novel. Obviously, novelty does not exist by itself
but only as a property of the history of each person with respect to particular things and events. Although
some types of events are likely to be novel to most people, a detailed knowledge of what is novel to, and
likely to exert stimulus control over some perceptual behavior of, a particular person will require a more
detailed knowledge of the history of that person.
An operant analysis of gaze following.
Mundy et al. (1996) distinguished between a lower and a higher level of responding to joint attention.
Lower level behavior consists of orienting head and eyes in accord with another person’s proximal point or
touch. Higher level behavior involves following someone’s line of regard (beyond the index finger if pointing
is involved) to some object or event.
In behavior-analytic terms, the lower-level behaviors can occur as standard discriminated operants –
the product of a standard three-term contingency: The adult’s pointing or touching is the occasion upon which
looking in that direction is typically followed by reinforcing consequences. These reinforcing consequences
may very well be purely visual.
Higher-level performances can involve very much more complex skills. As Bruner (1995) pointed
out, the child’s action must not only be started by the adult’s gaze, but it must also stop “when the infant
finds a visual target out there” (p. 7). A relatively simple version of a higher-level behavior could consist of a
two-component behavior chain in which an adult’s gaze in a particular direction serves as a discriminative
stimulus for the child’s turning the head/eyes to look in that general direction. In that vicinity, something
irregular is happening which functions simultaneously as a conditioned reinforcer for turning the head/eyes
and as an SD for visual focusing and further looking. However, if the child, eventually, focuses on something
for reasons totally apart from what the adult was initially attending to, this may not fulfill the stricter criteria of
true joint attention adhered to by some authors. In true joint attention, the child must focus (and stop
scanning) not just dependent upon seeing something that singularly reinforces the child’s seeing, but which is
likely also to have functioned as an SD for the adult’s look. In traditional terms, then, we want to know the
basis on which the child determines what the adult is attending to. More specifically, the child may focus (and
stop scanning) when looking is jointly controlled by the adult’s gaze and some novel or irregular thing or
event.
An operant analysis of ‘social referencing’.
Simple forms of social referencing may be built up similarly to observing behavior as studied in the
laboratory (e.g., Dinsmoor, 1983). Again, if true joint attention is involved in the sense that the child can be
said to “understand” that the familiar person attends to the same event as the child does, the principle of joint
control must be involved. Hence, social referencing with joint attention requires that the child behaves in
accord with the behavior of the familiar person towards a novel stimulus, but only contingent upon an event
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
167
of joint control in which the child could be said to infer that the familiar person’s behavior is controlled by the
same novel event as the child’s own behavior.
An operant analysis of the ‘protoimperative’.
A definition of protoimperative gestures as “gestures intended to make another person do something
for one’s benefit” (Sarriá et al., 1996) may correspond closely to Skinner’s preliminary definition of a mand
as “a verbal operant in which the response is reinforced [through the mediation of other persons] by a
characteristic consequence and is therefore under the functional control of relevant conditions of deprivation
or aversive stimulation” (Skinner, 1957, pp. 35-36).
Protoimperatives (or mands) can also occur without features of joint attention. A child may simply
persist in doing what has previously produced reinforcers through the mediation of other persons without
otherwise being sensitive to whether or not anyone attends at the moment. However, protoimperatives usually
work more smoothly and reliably when the child engages in observing behavior that establishes another
person’s attention to what the child is pointing at.
An operant analysis of the ‘protodeclarative’. Whereas protoimperatives correspond to Skinner’s (1957)
definition of a mand, protodeclaratives may correspond to a rudimentary version of a tact, which is
established by reinforcement “with many different reinforcers or with a generalized reinforcer” (Skinner,
1957, p. 83). Specifically, “the purely social motive of sharing attention to something” (Tomasello,1995, p.
111), may imply that behavior is typically reinforced by social consequences, such as other persons’ nods,
smiles, visual orienting, uttering “yes,” “oh,” “look at that,” or other relevant comments that in Skinner’s
(1957) terminology constitute intraverbals. Thus, joint attention is central to the ‘protodeclarative’ in the
sense that the joint attention of other persons constitutes the reinforcement that characterizes this function.
An operant analysis of monitoring
Instead of just responding to discrete instances of other persons’ looking or pointing, a child may
“keep an eye on” someone in order to detect such instances. As lower-level joint attention, such continuing
observing behavior, or vigilance, may be automatically and abundantly reinforced when the child is
monitoring parents or others who may be particularly qualified at focusing on events that may reinforce the
child’s perceptual behavior. On the other hand, true joint attention in such monitoring would seem to require
a contingency of the type that characterizes the ‘protodeclarative’ or tact and involve similar social
reinforcers.
In sum
The present operant interpretation of joint attention skills points out seven basic factors.
(1) In social interactions that involve visual joint attention, the visual orienting of one person is under
discriminative control of the pointing or visual orienting of another person. (2) Such discriminative control
may be conditional upon other stimuli. For instance, such point or gaze following may be particularly likely in
the presence of certain facial expressions, when someone says “Look!” or when you have asked for
directions. (3) In a three-dimensional world, a great many different objects, events, or properties of objects
and events may exist in the direction of someone’s look, so that identifying the particular stimuli at which
someone else is focusing must be jointly controlled by the direction of the look and something else. (4) Both
the extent to which someone follows another person’s orienting, and the extent to which one operates to get
others to follow one’s own orienting depends on previous consequences of such behavior. (5) When one
directs the attention of someone else, small changes in the right direction may function as reinforcers, and
when following someone else’s direction, a novel stimulus may, typically, function as a reinforcer. (6) In both
cases, the reinforcers may have gained in strength because they are typical precursors of the moment of joint
attention which, in turn, constitutes an occasion upon which other behavior (e.g., verbal behavior) is likely to
be reinforced. (7) A limited number of exemplars of successfully following and directing others’ attention may
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
168
suffice to produce a continuous repertoire of such joint attention skills.
Advantages of an operant analysis: Implications for the applied field
Generally, the focus on accessible variables in behavior analysis makes it directly applicable to
practical issues. Having found that “joint visual attention is not spontaneously demonstrated by infants until
about 10 months of age” and that, “given the appropriate feedback infants are able to acquire a gaze-
following response from about 8 months on,” Corkum and Moore (1995, p. 78) concluded that “learning is a
possible mode of acquisition for joint visual attention.” Such acceptance of “learning as a possible mode” may
be a first step towards an analysis of the variables of which joint attention skills are a function. If joint
attention skills are amenable to an operant analysis, learning protocols aimed at the establishment of such
skills appear to be a rather straightforward matter. Here are some examples:
1. Social referencing: Establishing normal social stimuli as reinforcers
If social stimuli that function as reinforcers for behavior in most people, including children, do not do
so for behavior in children with autism, a crucial step may be to establish such events as reinforcers. The
following outline of a training procedure will focus on establishing others’ nodding and smiling as reinforcers.
Training: Trainer and child are seated face-to-face at opposite sides of a table. Spread approximately
10 small edible reinforcers around the table. Any attempt from the child to take pieces from the table should
be blocked. When the child sits quietly, nod and smile before you let the child take one item. If the child does
respond, repeat the nod and smile, and prompt the child to take one item from the table. Then, as long as you
do not nod and smile, block any attempts the child may make to take things from the table, and when you
nod and smile, let the child take another item, and so on. Let the time vary between each time you nod and
smile. When the child takes items from the table only immediately following your nods and smiles, this
constitutes a simple version of social referencing. Further, it is appropriate to say that your nods and smiles
function as an SD for the child’s response in taking items from the table, which is also a reliable indication that
your nods and smiles will function as a conditioned reinforcer for any behavior in the child that produce your
nods and smiles as consequences. An early change in the child’s behavior will be an obvious increase in the
child’s visual attention to your face. Your nods and smiles can then be utilized to establish useful social
behavior in the child, such as calling your name and, later, directing your attention to other objects and
events.
Clearly, the simple procedure described above is only a start, and a large number of problems remain
and will have to be solved. First, your nods and smiles are likely to function as reinforcers only when those
edible reinforcers are visible at the table. Second, although your nods and smiles now function as a
conditioned reinforcer, they will not be generalized: They will only function as reinforcers as long as those
edibles are reinforcing, i.e., as long as relevant deprivation is maintained. Third, nods and smiles by others
than the trainer may still go unnoticed by the child. Fourth, in addition to nods and smiles, other persons’
uttering “yes,” “oh,” “look at that,” and other relevant comments (intraverbals) will also need to be
established as conditioned reinforcers in order to establish a general interest in the normal social consequences
of engaging in standard “communication.”
2. Establishing monitoring
Additional monitoring may be established effectively by having the child actively guiding someone
else’s behavior through several steps that are necessary in order for the child to make that other person locate
and deliver a reinforcer. An example of a sequence of relevant tasks is the following: First, attach envelopes,
say 5-6, on a horizontal line on the wall. Let the child sit and watch from a distance of 3-4m that someone
puts some snack (or other potential reinforcer) into one of the envelopes. Then, tell the child to instruct you
on where to find the snack for him or her. Start pointing to some random envelope and have the child
prompted to guide you by pointing further to the left, further to the right, or by saying “stop” as your pointing
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
169
finger moves before the envelope in which the reinforcer is located. In a second version of the task, have the
envelopes arranged in a vertical row and have the child guide you by pointing further up, further down, or
saying “stop”. Next, combine the tasks by having envelopes pasted over a large area on the wall. In more
advanced version of the task, the child may be taught to specify in more detail how you should move from
your current pointing position, such as “next one to the right and two up!” Let the child confirm that you have
the right position by saying “stop” or “yes, that’s the one” before you pick up the reinforcer, and to make
“mistakes” occasionally, so that the child cannot successfully relax his/her monitoring of your behavior.
3. Establishing gaze or point following
Pretaining: Trainer and child are seated face-to-face at opposite sides of a table. The trainer shows
the potentially reinforcing stimulus to the child, asks the child to turn around (or otherwise makes sure that
the child cannot observe), puts a potentially reinforcing stimulus under one of two opaque cups turned upside
down on the table. Next, the trainer says “ready” and makes sure that the child observes the cups and
chooses one of them by pointing to it. The trainer lifts up the cup and, if the reinforcer is located under the
cup to which the child pointed, the child is allowed to grab it. If the reinforcer is located under the other cup,
the child is just allowed to observe it before it is removed by the trainer and a new trial is started. The
pretraining continues until the child turns around within a couple of seconds when asked to do so, and turns
back and chooses one of the cups within 5s when the trainer says “ready.”
Training: Use the same arrangement as during pretraining, except that the trainer moves his/her face
as close to the cup that contains the reinforcer as is necessary to make the child look at the trainer’s face
before being allowed to choose one of the cups. Repeat this until the child observes the trainer’s face and
consistently (e.g., four successive times) chooses the object with the reinforcer placed under it. Next, the
trainer fades his face away from the cup on successive trials until the child observes the trainer’s face and
chooses the “correct” cup even when the trainer sits laid back and just looks at the cup under which the
reinforcer is placed.
General ideas for training extensions: Hide the reinforcer behind different objects in different places,
use different reinforcers and different trainers.
4. Establishing mands with joint attention
Mands (protodeclaratives and declaratives) without joint attention are evident when they typically
occur indiscriminate of a listener’s attention. For instance, even a well developed for, such as “Can I have
that chocolate?” may occur and simply be repeated even in the absence of evidence that any listener attends.
Training: Let the child observe you putting potential reinforcers away so that they are not accessible
to the child without your participation. When the child produces the first mand, do not deliver the terminal
reinforcer, but prompt an attention-getting response in the child, such as calling your name, which will
typically be followed by your appropriate listener behavior, such as answering “yes,” and visually orienting in
the child’s direction. Then, let this constitute the occasion upon which child’s mand is typically reinforced.
5. Establishing tacts
Joint attention appears to be particularly important to verbal behavior under stimulus control, such as
tacts. Skinner (1957) defined the tact as “a verbal operant in which a given response form is evoked (or at
least strengthened) by a particular object or event or a property of an object or event.” According to
Skinner’s technical analysis, the unique relation to a discriminative stimulus, rather than to a specific
establishing operation, is obtained by (1) many different reinforcers or (2) generalized reinforcers. However,
additional analyses are required in order to work out an effective intervention plan regarding how to
strengthen a tact repertoire in persons who demonstrate a distinct lack in that domain. First, a normal tact
repertoire is not likely to be practiced and maintained if normal listeners’ responses do not function as
generalized (conditioned) reinforcers. Hence, procedures for establishing generalized conditioned
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
170
reinforcement need to be based on naturalistic observations of the specific events that are likely to constitute
listener reactions to tacts in the speaker’s natural language. A large literature on ‘joint attention’ seems
particularly relevant to this issue. Another person’s joint attention, in the form of visual orienting, nodding,
smiling, and uttering different types of “relevant comments,” constitutes the reinforcement that characterizes
the tact function. Hence, unless those responses that can be summarized as joint attention from another
person actually function as reinforcers for a child’s behavior, there is no basis for a development of tacts.
Numerous attempts to establish conversational skills (such as tacts and intraverbals) in children with
autism appear to have succeeded mainly within the limits of an artificial training setting in which verbal
behavior has been reinforced by characteristic consequences that typically produce mands. A successful tact
training program, then, must ensure that the consequences that typically follow and maintain tacts in the
natural environment do, in fact, function as reinforcers. Hence, training along the lines described in previous
sections (1) Social referencing: Establishing normal social stimuli as reinforcers, and (2) Establishing gaze and
point following, may turn out as pivotal (e.g., Burke & Cerniglia, 1990; Koegel, Koegel, Harrower, & Carter),
or prerequisites, for successful tact training. Once normal consequences do function properly as reinforcers, it
is possible that exposure to naturalistic conditions may suffice to foster commenting and other conversational
skills. However, we may want to speed up such a development through additional training. First, there may
be a large number of “names” of objects and events that may initially be most expediently established through
traditional discrete trial training (e.g., as described in Lovaas et al., 1981).
Second, in order to produce a high frequency of “learn units” (e.g., Greer & McDonough, 1999), it
may be preferable to establish child-initiated training during many different naturally occurring circumstances
by initially reinforcing tacting abundantly whenever it occurs.
Third, it may be wise to teach the kinds of verbal skills that are most likely to be reinforced by
standard listeners. What does seem more likely to be reinforced in natural settings over time is commenting
on things or events that are novel in some way. A deficiency in this area may be particularly evident in many
children with autism. As one parent wrote to an internet discussion group on applied behavior analysis for
children with autism, “Does anyone have any ideas on how to develop a program on teaching a child to
comment? My son . . . does not make comments. A purple cow could walk by and he wouldn’t mention it.”
A series of tasks that may teach the necessary skills in discriminating novel stimuli may start with simple
“What’s missing?” tasks (e.g., Lovaas et al.,1981) and similar training focused on “What’s added?”, “What’s
changed?” and “What’s strange?” In order to increase the rate of spontaneous commenting in natural settings,
instructions may be faded by increasing the time and the distance from instructions to opportunities to
respond. Novel stimulus constellations can be arranged in other rooms and gradually in more distant places so
that the child is given opportunities to respond in the absence of immediate instructions.
Research questions derived from an operant analysis
According to an operant analysis, it is entirely possible that normal development of behavioral
repertoires in children exposed to normal environments relies fundamentally on normal social reinforcing
stimuli to function as such from very early on. If even the parents’ visual attention and smiling does not
function as reinforcers for the behavior of an infant, important early forms of social skills related to joint
attention may not develop. A number of important research questions follow from this interpretation:
(1) Do other’s visual attention, nodding and smiling normally function as a reinforcer from birth, or
does the reinforcing effect of such stimuli develop later, possibly mainly as a result of operant conditioning
procedures? This could be investigated by using a conjugate schedule (see Rovee-Collier & Gekoski, 1979) in
which the degree of visual orientation towards the child, nodding, and/or smiling of a human face on a
monitor is changed contingent on the rate of sucking a non-nutritive nipple.
(2) Is there a difference even at birth in the extent to which visual attention, nods, and smiles function
as reinforcers for the behavior of typically developing children as compared with children with autism? The
conjugate schedule procedure just mentioned could, in principle, be used to investigate whether such a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
171
difference between children with autism and normally developing children exists even shortly after birth.
Diagnosis and a possible initiation of corrective measures within the first few weeks after birth would seem
like an interesting option.
(3) If the reinforcing effect of other persons’ visual attention, smiling and nodding typically depends
on other, primary reinforcers, what are those primary reinforcers, and what are the relevant procedures to
which children are typically exposed in their natural environments?
To the extent that other’s visual attention, nodding and smiling normally function as a reinforcer
already from birth on, social interaction may function as what has been termed an autocatalytic process (e.g.,
Skinner, 1953) in which the reinforcing effect of such social events gain in strength because such events, in
addition to being reinforcing in the first place, also constitute occasions upon which additional behavior is
likely to produce more of the same. It is possible that the reinforcing effect may typically increase over time
because such social events are typically correlated with a higher rate of positive reinforcement, such as
other’s compliance with requests (mands). Further, it seems likely that other’s visual attention combined with
nodding and smiling is typically correlated with a low frequency of aversive social stimuli. Research in this
field could include initial naturalistic observation of parent—child interactions and proceed with systematic
exaggeration of these features of children’s environments.
(4) Can conditioned reinforcers established through contrived contingencies be maintained as
reinforcers at near-normal rates of back-up (primary) reinforcement? When other persons’ nodding and
smiling do not have a reinforcing effect, such an effect can be produced by differentially reinforcing some
behavior in the presence of such nodding and smiling. However, such an arrangement may work only when
visible, or in the presence of “therapists” with a history of using it. Moreover, such an arrangement with
obviously contrived reinforcers is potentially stigmatizing, particularly as the child advances to otherwise more
normalized social environments. Hence, explicit conditioning of normal social reinforcers can only lead to a
lasting normalized social skills repertoire if the conditioned reinforcing effect is maintainable at near-normal
rates of primary reinforcement. How far can contrived contingencies of primary reinforcement be faded
towards a non-conspicuous level without loosing their effect?
Conclusion
The literature on joint attention has identified elements of social interaction that appear to be crucial
for normal social functioning in general, and for verbal behavior in particular. Joint attention deficits seem to
characterize children with autism, and a thorough operant analysis seems required in order to identify
variables of which joint attention skills are a function. Hopefully, the current operant interpretation will spark
off experimental analyses from which more advanced and effective intervention plans can be developed.
References
Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore &
P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role in Development, (pp. 131-158). Hillsdale, N. J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint attention deficits in autism: Towards a cognitive analysis.
Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185-189.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In A.
Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development, and simulation of everyday mindreading (
pp. 234-251). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role
in Development, (pp. 41-59. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
172
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). From gesture to the first
word: On cognitive and social prerequisites. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Interaction, conversation,
and the development of language, (pp. 247-307). New York: Wiley.
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly – Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 205-226.
Blough, D. S. (1958). A method for obtaining psychophysical thresholds from the pigeon. Journal of
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 31-43.
Bornstein, M. H. (1976). Infants’ recognition memory for hue. Developmental Psychology, 12, 185-
191. Bruner, J. (1973). Going Beyond the Information Given. New York: Norton.
Bruner, J. (1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255-
287. Bruner, J. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies
in motherB infant interaction, (pp. 271-289). New York: Academic Press.
Bruner, J. (1995). From joint attention to the meeting of minds: An introduction. In C. Moore & P. J.
Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role in Development, (pp. 189-203). Hillsdale, N. J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Burke, J. C, & Cerniglia, L. (1990) Stimulus complexity and autistic children's responsivity:
Assessing and training a pivotal behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 233-253.
Chomsky, N. (1959). Verbal behavior. By B. F. Skinner. Language, 35, 26-58.
Corcum, V, & Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In C. Moore & P.
J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role in Development, (pp. 61-83). Hillsdale, N. J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dinsmoor, J. A.(1950). A quantitative comparison of the discriminative and reinforcing functions of a
stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 458-472.
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983). Observing and conditioned reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
6, 693-728.
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1995). Stimulus-control 2. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 253-269.
Donahoe, J. W. & Palmer, D. C. (1994). Learning and Complex Behavior. Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
Dunham, P. J., & Moore, C. (1995). In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its
Origin and Role in Development, (pp. 15-28). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Evans, R. I. (1968). B. F. Skinner: The man and his ideas. New York: Dutton & Co.
Fantino, E., & Logan, C. A. (1979). The Experimental Analysis of Behavior: A Biological
perspective. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman & Company.
Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to
novel ones. Science, 146, 668-670.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
173
Fetterman, J. G. (1996). Dimensions of stimulus complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 3-18.
Greer R. D., & McDonough S. H. (1999). Is the learn unit a fundamental measure of pedagogy?
The Behavior Analyst, 22, 5-16.
Herrick, R. M., Myers, J. L., & Korotkin, A. L. (1959). Changes in SD and in S-delta rates during
the development of an operant discrimination. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52,
359-363.
Hobson, P. (1993). Understanding persons: The role of affect. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-
Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism, (pp 204-227).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior (JEAB) (1996). Vol. 65, pp.183-353.
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, C. M. (1999). Pivotal response
intervention I: Overview of approach. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24,
174-185.
Loess, H. B., & Duncan, C. P. (1952). Human discrimination learning with simultaneous and
successive presentation of stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 215-221.
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in
young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3-9.
Lovaas, O. I., Ackerman, A., Alexander, D., Firestone, P., Perkins, J., & Young, D. Teaching
Developmentally Disabled Children: The Me Book. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed Publishing Company, 1981.
Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G, Kinder, M. I., Rubenstein, B. D., Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q.
(1966). Establishment of Social Reinforcers in Two Schizophrenic Children on the Basis of Food. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 4, 109-125.
Lovitt, T.C. (1967). Use of conjugate reinforcement to evaluate the relative reinforcing effects of
various narrative forms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 5, 164-171.
Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some Logical Functions of Joint Control. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327-354
Maurice, C., Greene, G. & Luce S. C. (1996). Behavioral Intervention for Young Children with
Autism. Austin, Texas: Pro Ed.
McEachin, J. J, Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long-term outcome for children who received
early intensive behavioral treatment. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 97, 359-372.
Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J. (1995). Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role in Development.
Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention, social-emotional approach in children with autism. Development
and Psychopathology, 7, 63-82.
Mundy, P. (2001). Joint attention, social development and a transactional-neurodevelopmental
model for autism. Paper presented during the 27th annual convention of the Association for Behavior
Analysis, New Orleans.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
174
Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention and early social communication: Implications
for research on intervention with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 27, 653-676.
Mundy, P., Hogan, A., & Doehring, P. (1996). A preliminary manual for the abridged Early
Social Communication Scales. Coral Gables: University of Miami,
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1993). The theory of mind and joint attention deficits in
autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds:
Perspectives from autism (pp. 181-293). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. A., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits in
autism: The contributions of nonverbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 27, 657-669.
Reynolds, G. S. (1961). Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
4, 203-208.
Rovee-Collier, C. K., & Gekoski, M. J. (1979). The economics of infancy: A review of conjugate
reinforcement. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol.
13, pp. 195-255). New York: Academic.
Sarriá, E., Gómes, J.C. & Tamarit, J. (1996) Joint attention and alternative language intervention in
autism: Implications of theory for parctice. In: S. von Tetzchner, & M.H. Jensen (Eds.), Augmentative and
alternative communication: European perspectives (s. 49-64). London: Whurr.
Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253,
265-266.
Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D.
Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp 213-245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence Relations and Behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors
Cooperative.
Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1995). Joint attention across contexts in normal and autistic children. In
C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role in Development, (pp. 189-203).
Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sigman, Kasari, Kwon, & Yirmiya, 1992. Responses to the negative emotions of others in autistic,
mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796-807.
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. Acton: Copley Publishing Group.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York: MacMillan.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice Hall.
Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint attention in early language development. Language
Sciences, 11, 69-88.
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.),
Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role in Development, (pp. 103-130). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Werner, J. S., & Siqueland, E. R. (1978). Visual recognition memory in the preterm infant. Infant
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
JEIBI VOLUME 2, ISSUE NO. 3, Fall, 2005
175
Behavior and Development, 1, 79-98.
Wildemann, D. G. & Holland J. G. (1972). Control of a continuous response dimension by a
continuous stimulus dimension. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, 419-434.
Zentall T. R., & Clement, T. S. (2001). Simultaneous discrimination learning: Stimulus interactions.
Animal Learning & Behavior, 29, 311-325. Author’s Note
The current article is based upon a paper entitled “TACTs and the role of 'Joint Attention': An
Operant Analysis,” delivered as an invited address at the 6th International Congress on
Behaviorism and the Sciences of Behavior, August 2002.
Please address correspondence to:
Per Holth
The Behavioral Center
P.O. Box 1565
Vika, 0118
Oslo, Norway
e-mail: per.holth@atferd.unirand.no
Advertising in the Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention
Advertising is available in JEIBI. All advertising must be paid for in advance. Make your check
payable to Joseph Cautilli. The ad copy should be in our hands at least 3 weeks prior to publication. Copy
should be in MS Word or Word Perfect, RTF format and advertiser should include graphics or logos
with ad copy.
The prices for advertising in one issue are as follows:
1/4 Page: $50.00 1/2 Page: $100.00 vertical or horizontal Full Page: $200.00
If you wish to run the same ad in both issues for the year, you are eligible for the following
discount:
1/4 Pg.: $40 - per issue
1/2 Pg.: $75 - per issue -vertical or horizontal
Full Page: $150.00-per issue.
For more information, or place an ad, contact Halina Dziewolska by phone at (215) 462-
6737or e-mail at: halinadz@hotmail.com
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Content uploaded by Per Holth
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Per Holth on Jun 13, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.