ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

A growing body of research suggests an association between exposure to interparental conflict and attention problems in children. This study examined whether a videotaped couple conflict decreases child’s short-term attention performance, comparing the effect to the known disruptive impact of watching action films. Participants were 60 children, aged 11–13 years. Children’s performance in an attention task was measured prior to and immediately after video exposure and their skin conductance level (SCL) was assessed throughout stimulus presentation in two experimental conditions: (i) couple conflict condition and (ii) action film condition. Results indicate that the simulated couple conflict more harmfully disrupted children’s accuracy performance (i.e. error ratio) although being less physiologically arousing than the action film. No significant group differences were present concerning concentration performance as a behavioral outcome. The present study adds to the evidence that interparental conflict might be crucial in understanding more profoundly attention difficulties in children.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by: [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich]
On: 01 July 2014, At: 08:22
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Family Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rfsc20
Interparental conflict impairs children’s short-termed
attention performance
Martina Zemp
a
, Guy Bodenmann
a
& Steven R.H. Beach
b
a
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
b
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
Published online: 27 Jun 2014.
To cite this article: Martina Zemp, Guy Bodenmann & Steven R.H. Beach (2014) Interparental conflict impairs children’s
short-termed attention performance, Family Science, 5:1, 43-51
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19424620.2014.933742
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Interparental conict impairs childrens short-termed attention performance
Martina Zemp
a
*, Guy Bodenmann
a
and Steven R.H. Beach
b
a
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland;
b
Department of Psychology, University of Georgia,
Athens, GA, USA
(Received 29 April 2013; accepted 3 June 2014)
A growing body of research suggests an association between exposure to interparental conict and attention problems in
children. This study examined whether a videotaped couple conict decreases childs short-term attention performance,
comparing the effect to the known disruptive impact of watching action lms. Participants were 60 children, aged 11
13 years. Childrens performance in an attention task was measured prior to and immediately after video exposure and their
skin conductance level (SCL) was assessed throughout stimulus presentation in two experimental conditions: (i) couple
conict condition and (ii) action lm condition. Results indicate that the simulated couple conict more harmfully disrupted
childrens accuracy performance (i.e. error ratio) although being less physiologically arousing than the action lm. No
signicant group differences were present concerning concentration performance as a behavioral outcome. The present
study adds to the evidence that interparental con ict might be crucial in understanding more profoundly attention difculties
in children.
Keywords: marital conict; attention problems; skin conductance; action movies
Introduction
Over many years, interparental conict has emerged as a
primary powerful predictor of child maladjustment (e.g.
Barletta & OMara, 2006; Cummings & Davies, 2010).
Considerable evidence exists that exposure to high levels
of destructive interparental conict increases childrens
risk for a wide array of adjustment problems, including
impairments in school performance (Ghazarian & Buehler,
2010; Harold, Aitken, & Shelton, 2007). Attention pro-
blems are being increasingly discussed as one of the major
mechanisms that may account for the detrimental impact
of marital discord on childrens school adjustment
(Davies, Woitach, Winter, & Cummings, 2008). This is
the rst study, to our knowledge, to examine a couple
conict as a cause for attention problems in children by
measuring childrens attention performance prior to and
after conict exposure in an experimental approach.
Childrens reactivity to marital conict is, according to
the emotional security theory (EST; Davies & Cummings,
1994), an expression of perceived threat to their felt
security and safety, a central goal for children in the family
setting. EST holds that children from high-conict homes
are likely to develop insecure representations of the inter-
parental relationship, for instance, loss in condence in
parents abilities to manage difculties for the purpose of
preserving family stability (Davies & Cummings, 1998).
Recently, it was assumed that concerns about emotional
security may interfere with cognitive functioning outside
the home too, for example in school (Davies, Winter, &
Cicchetti, 2006). In line with this hypothesis, childrens
insecure representations of the parental relations hip were
identied as a primary intervening mechanism in the link
between observational ratings of marital discord and child
as well as teacher reports on childrens school adjustment
over a 2-year period (Sturge-Apple, Davies, Winter,
Cummings, & Schermerhorn, 2008). Strikingly, Davies
et al. (2008) revealed that attention difculties accounted
for 34% of the association between childrens insecurity
and their academic performance. Childrens insecure
representations of the parental relationship predicted sub-
sequent childrens attention problems, measured by atten -
tion tasks and parent report 1 year later, which in turn
were associated with teacher reports of childrens school
problems longitudinally over a further year. Parental
discord therefore appears to have a substantial impact on
childrens functioning in school, with attention difculties
as a powerful explanatory mechanism.
Attention dif
culties in relation to child exposure to
parental conict are explicable in light of resourc e alloca-
tion models of cognitive neuroscience which indicate that
human cognitive processing is limited by a central
resource pool (Kahneman, 1973). If too many distracting
stimuli or tasks require resources, performance diminishes
(Schneider & Fisk, 1982). Affective experiences particu-
larly redirect humans attention away from task perfor-
mance toward the arousing stimulus (Beal, Weiss,
Barros, & Macdermid, 2005). Translated to an EST frame-
work, one interpretation is that prolonged insecurity and
*Corresponding author. Email: martina.zemp@psychologie.uzh.ch
Family Science, 2014
Vol. 5, No. 1, 4351, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19424620.2014.933742
© 2014 Taylor & Francis
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
vigilance due to parental conict may deplete the common
reservoir of a childs psychological resources. That is,
heightened reactivity associated with emotional insecurity
requires considerable expenditure of effort to regul ate
attention, affect, and action (Davies & Sturge-Apple,
2007). As a result, the energy required to regai n security
may absorb the resources children need to successfully
pursue other signicant tasks in development.
Correspondingly, Davies, Manning, and Cicchetti (2013)
recently reported that toddlers insecurity in the inte r-
parental relationship predicted subsequent behavior pro-
blems, pa rtly mediated by their difculties in mastering
stage-salient developmental tasks (i.e. emotion regulation,
autonomy, and problem-solving). In a similar vein, expo-
sure to parental arguments may disrupt childrens short-
term attention capacity by undermining their ability to
focus and sustain attention (Davies et al., 2006). To the
best of our knowledge, this is the rst study to experimen-
tally examine this hypothesis measuring childrens atten-
tion performance prior to and immediately after exposure
to an analog couple conict.
Analog designs employing simulations of couple dis-
putes emerged as a powerful experimental tool for the
investigation of causal effects of marital conict on child
adjustment considering the potential of direct stimulus
manipulation (Cummings, 1995). A large number of
studies in this eld have established the validity of the
experimental exposure to simulated conict (e.g. Davies,
Myers, Cummings, & Heindel, 1999; Goeke-Morey,
Cummings, Harold, & Shelton, 2003). To date, we know
from two studies that examined the immediate effect of
parental conict on childrens cognitive functioning using
this approach. First, OBrien and Chin ( 1998) demon-
strated that 10- to 12-year-old childrens internal conict
representations, after being experimentally activated by
audiotaped vignettes of angry couple interactions, biased
their recognition memory for conict-related words
assessed by a word recognition task. Children were
instructed to listen to constructive and aggressive conict
words and to report whether they had heard them pre-
viously in the vignettes. It was shown that children from
high- compared to low-conict homes showed a greater
tendency to false-posi tive memory errors concerning the
aggressive words (i.e. mislabeling aggressive words not
already presented as having been heard) but made fewer
errors in recognizing valid (presented) aggressive words.
Second, Medina, Margolin, and Wilcox (2000) exam-
ined the impact of childrens history of family hostility
(i.e. maternal child-abus e potential and marital conict) on
their performance on a verbal attention task, measured
prior to and after exposure to audiotaped simulations of
couple disagreements. They found that children from
high-exposure family backgrounds improved their audi-
tory verbal attention following the conict exposure in
comparison with performance prior to the stimulus. The
authors suggested that heightened reactivity to the conict
stimulus mig ht be one underlying pathway through which
these children achieved enhanced scores on the post-sti-
mulus assessment. These ndings revealed that childrens
history of parental conict does affect thei r reactivity to
simulated parental conict in terms of memory and atten-
tion performance but, limiting their implications, no com-
parisons were made to a control group exposed to stimuli
unrelated to marital conict.
Taken together, there is emerging evidence to assume
considerable effects of interparental conict on a childs
attention performance. Childrens felt threat about safety
when faced with parental strife may disrupt their executive
functioning by depleting the common reservoir of cogni-
tive resources (Davies et al., 2006). Since focusing and
sustaining attention requi re high levels of neuropsycholo-
gical resources, emotional insecurity may be particularly
likely to undermine this dimension of functioning. Despite
initial ndings supporting this hypothesis prospectively
and not based on an experimental approach (Davies
et al., 2008), we think the most appropriate way to
examine allocation of attention resources in the context
of interparental conict might be within an experimental
study which assesses childrens attention performance
prior to and immediately after exposure to analog conict,
and to compare the impact of the conict stimulus to a
conict-unrelated stimulus.
The current study
This experimental study examines the short-term effects of
a videotaped couple conict on childrens attention per-
formance compared to a control group in a community
sample. Childrens attention performance was assessed
prior to and immediately after the video exposure by an
attention task and their skin conductance level (SCL) was
measured continuously throughout stimulus presentation.
The conict situation represented a verbal couple dispute
with no conict resolution but an escalation outcome. This
type of unresolved conict is especially prone to elicit
childs reactivity (Cummings, Simpson, & Wilson,
1993). Children of the control condition were exposed to
a physiologically arousing, but conict-unrelated stimulus;
that is, a sequence of an action lm for children showing
two kids eeing from policemen in a racing car. This lm
scene excludes any interadult interaction in content. This
control condition was explicitly chosen because (i) only a
conict-unrelated stimulus can avoid potential confound
due to classical conditioning. Based on the model of
classical conditioning, it is likely that even nonconict
adult interaction can trigger negative reactions in children
autonomously if previous aversive experiences with
parental interactions were made. In a similar context,
conditioning and generalization of fear has been experi-
mentally demonstrated in healthy children (Glenn et al.,
44 M. Zemp et al.
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
2012) and seems to be manifested in skin conductance
particularly (Neumann, Waters, Westbury, & Henry,
2008). (ii) We chose a particularly rigorous control condi-
tion since previous research showed that action lms
characterized by fast pace and high level of physical
movement and visual change are highly physiologically
arousing and attention-grabbing for children (Huston
et al., 1981), and known to disrupt childrens attention
on tasks following exposure to the lm (Lillard &
Peterson, 2011). These ndings are in line with the limited
capacity model of television viewing stating that the
rapidly changing scenery and attention-grabbing editing
of action lms underm ine childs attention capacity with
regard to less exciting tasks (Lang, Bolls, Potter, &
Kawahara, 1999). Comparing the couple conict to an
action lm might therefore yield further-reaching implica-
tions than comparison with an alternative couple
interaction.
Children aged 1113 were recruited since there is
considerable evidence that their internal representations
of the interparental relationship at this developmental
stage have become fairly elaborate and play an increas-
ingly important role in information processing (Demorest,
1992). Consequently, 10- to 12-year-olds showed schema-
consistent processing of conict words after being
exposed to a simulated conict not evident in children of
79 years (OBrien & Chin, 1998). Accordingly, it has
been found that early adolescents more accurately used
affective cues to evaluate conict endings (Davies, Myers,
& Cummings, 1996 ) and made sharper discriminations
between resolved and unresolved interadult anger than
younger children (Cummings et al., 1993).
Hypotheses
In a preliminary analysis, we rst examine if manipulation
of the arousing control stimulus was successful. Both lm
sequences were supposed to elicit physiological arousal,
but signicantly higher levels should result in the action
lm group compared to the couple conict group (H1)
(Huston et al., 1981). As main hypothesis, the conict
exposure was expected to have more detrimental impact
on childrens short-termed attention performance, i.e. their
performance in accuracy (H2) and concentration (H3) in
the attention task. We controlled for childrens perception
of interparental conict (CPIC) and childs gender in all
analyses given that previous ndings reported a signi
cant
impact of these variables on the impact of parental conict
(e.g. Cummings & Davies, 2010). However, we did not
propose specic hypotheses concerning gender differences
because reviews in this regard summarize that prior
research has been inconsistent (Davies & Lindsay, 2001;
Snyder, 1998), with studies showing higher reactivity in
girls (Shelton, Harold, Goeke-Morey, & Cummings,
2006), in boys (David & Murphy, 2004), or no gender
effects (Goeke-Morey, Cummings, & Papp, 2007). A con-
servative analytic stance was adopted; two-tailed tests of
signicance were used throughout.
Method
Participants
Participants were motherchild dyads recruited by means
of advertisements in local newspapers or magazines and a
letter to parents in local schools. Inclusion criteria for
participation were the child being between 11 and
13 years of age, that both mother and child were uent
in German, and that they were living together with the
childs father or stepfather in the same household. N =60
children (30 boys and 30 girls) participated in this study.
Three outliers (>3 SD above the mean) with regard to
attention performance had been removed from the initial
sample because these data points were not compatible
with the remaining data. According to the test manual of
the attention task used in this study cases for which there
is a large gap in the value of performance from the
remainder cannot be meaningfully interpreted due to
potential simulation or comprehension problems
(Brickenkamp, Schmidt-Atzert, & Liepmann, 2010). The
average age of children was 11.67 years (SD = .71).
Seventy percent of the children attended elementary
school, 23% secondary school, and 7% another type of
regular school. Most (95%) were living together with both
biological parents, only three children were living with
their biological mother and a stepfather since toddler age.
The average level of interparental conict reported by the
children was M = 1.98 (SD = .72, range = 1.003.38),
assessed on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never)
to 5 (very often) (Gödde & Walper, 2001). Ninety-seven
percent of parents were married, 3% cohabitated. Mean
parental relationship duration was M = 18.58 years
(SD = 4.25, range = 1028 years). Mothers were primarily
Swiss (82%), 7% were Germans, and 11% were from
other countries, but all were uent in German. Five per-
cent completed secondary school, 45% graduated voca-
tional school, and 50% attained higher educational
qualication, respectively. The majority of the mothers
(83%) reported to have a pretax annual family income of
about CHF 80,000 (approximately 89,740$), which is at
the level of the national mean income of Switzerland
couple households (CHF 81,000; Swiss Federal
Statistical Ofce, 2011).
Procedure
Interested mothers were screened by telephone and
received an informed consent for participation in the
study. Eligible children and their mothers were then
invited to our laboratory. After the introduction by the
Family Science 45
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
investigator, mot hers were asked to sign the declaration of
consent and were instruct ed to wait in the next room.
Electrodes were then attached to the childs nondominant
hand. Childrens SCL was rst measured for a 3-min
baseline period (SCL-B). Pictures neutral in valence of
the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang,
Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2008) which have been validated
concerning self-report and SCL (Lang, Greenwald,
Bradley, & Hamm, 1993 ) were shown during this period.
Next, SCL was recorded while children were viewing a
1-min video sequence (SCL-R) to which they were ran-
domly assigned (couple conict vs. action lm). Block
randomization was used to implement the random assign-
ment to condition in order to ensure an equal allocation to
both conditions. The person responsible for the random
assignment to condition was not involved in the assess-
ment of outcomes.
Group 1 (i.e. the couple conict group; n = 29) was
exposed to a 1-min videot aped couple dispute characteri-
zed by intense verbal anger of the woman, complaining
about her husbands lack of understanding of her daily
exhaustion and the husbands highly defensive reaction.
The argument ended in escalation with shouting by the
wife and a display of contempt by the husband. Children
of group 2 (i.e. the action lm group; n = 31) watched a 1-
min sequence of an action lm appropriate to childrens
age. The sequence showed two kids eeing from police-
men in a racing car. In this sequence no interpersonal
aspects were present, but there were perceptually salient
visual change, hectic sound, high speed, and physical
movement. Prior to and after video exposure, all children
completed the d2-R test of attention (Brickenkamp et al.,
2010).
At the end, all children were thoroughly debriefed.
They were told that conicts were common in normal
families but that constructive conict resolutions were
important. The debrieng happened rst alone, then with
the mother present. Participants were not rewarded nan-
cially. However, each mother was given a DVD of the
Couples Coping Enhancement Training (CCET;
Bodenmann, Schaer, & Gmelch, 2008), a self-directed
marital distress prevention tool, and they were able to
obtain a study report. The children received a bag of
sweets and a certicate for study participation.
Measures
Childrens perception of interparental conict
The subscale Persisting conict of the German short ver-
sion of the CPIC Scale (Gödde & Walper, 2001; originally
developed by Grych, Seid, & Fincham, 1992) was used to
assess the childrens perceptions of the frequency and
conict resolution of parental conicts. A largely consis-
tent factor structure to the original of the CPIC and good
psychometric properties have been shown for the German
version (Gödde & Walper, 2001). Children completed the
eight items (e.g. My parents are mean to each other;
When my parents disagree about something, they usually
come up with a solution) on a 5-point Likert scale ran-
ging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). Internal consistency
for the Persisting conict subscale was α = .86 in the
current study.
Attention performance
The d2-R test of attention (Brickenkamp et al., 2010) was
used to assess childrens attention performance. This
paperpencil test consists of 14 rows each of 57 characters
(p and d with one to four dashes above and/or below
each letter), whereby the rst and the last row are not
included in the nal scoring of outcome measures. All d
with two dashes are target symbols. The subjects were
asked to cancel as many target symbols as possible within
20 seconds per row. Total test time is 4ʹ40ʺ without pause
between test rows. The manual provides several process-
ing measures according to standardized formulae.
Concentration (total characters correctly processed minus
errors of commission) and accuracy (error ratio, i.e. false-
positive errors and errors of omission divided by the total
characters processed) were calculated in this study since
all other parameters enter in these two formulae. It should
be noted that low scores on error ratio imply high
accuracy.
The d2 test is a very popular and frequently used
attention task in German-speaking countries. Cronbachs
alpha within the relevant age range were α = .82.86
regarding accuracy and α = .92.95 with regard to con-
centration (Brickenkamp et al., 2010). The authors had
already pointed out in previous editions that considerable
training effects are to be expected (Brickenkamp, 2002),
which has been con rmed empirically (Bühner, Ziegler,
Bohnes, & Lauterbach, 2006). It there fore had to be
assumed that all children would enhance performance
from pre- to post-measurement but to different degrees.
Skin conductance level reactivity
SCL was measured for a 3-min baseline period (SCL-B)
and continuous ly throughout stimulus exposure (SCL-R)
using two Ag-AgCl electrodes lled with isotonic elec-
trode gel (0.5% saline in a neutral base). Two electrodes
were placed on the volar surfaces of the distal phalanges
of the rst and second ngers of the childrens nondomi-
nant hand having been washed with pure water. An SCL
response amplier using a constant voltage (0.5 V) tech-
nique and a 16-channel A/D converter were used to
amplify and digitize the signals. The AcqKnowledge
data acquisition and analysis software (Biopac Systems,
Inc.) collected SCL assessments at a rate of 1000 readings
46 M. Zemp et al.
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
per second. Averages (expressed in microsiemens) for
SCL-B and SCL-R were calculated. SCL data were not
available for four children because of equipment failure or
measurement artifacts. These children were excluded from
the whole study.
Results
Descriptive analyses
Descriptive statistics of the study variables are presented
in Table 1. As it is immanent in data of repeated measures,
high correlations wer e found between the repeated mea-
sures concerning childrens physiological reactivity and
attention performance in the present study. Random
assignment could be considered as succes sful since no
signicant group differences among the variable means
at prestimulus assessment were present (see t-tests in
Table 1). Additionally, a chi-square test was computed in
order to assure that boys and girls were equally distributed
across the two experimental conditions (χ(1) = 2.11; ns).
Childrens reactivity to video exposure
In order to test the group-by-time interactions, a series of
repeated meas ures analyses of covariance (ANCOVA)
were computed with different dependent variables accord-
ing to the study hypotheses, that is, SCL-B, accuracy, and
concentration performance in the attention task prior to
lm exposure as premeasurement and SCL-R, accuracy,
and concentration performance after stimulus exposure as
post-measurement, respectively. We controlled for chil-
drens gender as a between-subjects factor and CPIC (i.e.
Persisting conict) as a covariate.
As listed in Table 2, the rst hypothesis was supported
by a signicant 2 (couple conict vs. action lm) × 2
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, correlations, and t-tests for comparing group means among study variables.
12345 6 7
Group 1
Mean (SD)
Group 2
Mean (SD) t
1. Age 11.76 (.69) 11.58 (.72) .98
2. Persisting conict .16 2.05 (.65) 1.90 (.77) 1.48
3. SCL-B .16 .10 10.28 (3.28) 9.89 (3.68) .42
4. SCL-R .18 .05 .84*** 11.57 (3.44) 13.14 (4.50) 1.47
5. Accuracy (pre) .01 .13 .20 .11 5.80 (4.24) 8.06 (8.35) 1.34
6. Accuracy (post) .01 .11 .14 .12 .65*** 4.31 (3.84) 3.47 (3.62) .87
7. Concentration (pre) .33* .01 .19 .03 .54*** .33** 128.31 (23.44) 122.42 (26.21) .92
8. Concentration (post) .32* .07 .10 .05 .30* .21 .92*** 155.76 (26.55) 154.00 (30.65) .24
Notes: Group 1 = couple conict group; Group 2 = action lm group; SCL-B = skin conductance level baseline; SCL-R = skin conductance level
reactivity. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Table 2. Multivariate results of the repeated measures ANCOVA.
Pillai F df 1 df 2 p η
p
2
I. Skin conductance
Time .06 3.19 1 52 .08 .06
Time * Persisting conict .02 1.10 1 52 .30 .02
Time * Group .22 14.63 1 52 .00 .22
Time * Sex .02 .81 1 52 .37 .02
Time * Group * Sex .00 .00 1 52 .95 .00
II. Accuracy performance
Time .03 1.81 1 55 .18 .03
Time * Persisting conict .00 .10 1 55 .76 .00
Time * Group .10 6.07 1 55 .02 .10
Time * Sex .02 1.30 1 55 .26 .02
Time * Group * Sex .03 1.52 1 55 .22 .03
III. Concentration performance
Time .52 59.14 1 55 .00 .52
Time * Persisting conict .02 1.00 1 55 .32 .02
Time * Group .02 1.12 1 55 .29 .02
Time * Sex .03 1.65 1 55 .20 .03
Time * Group * Sex .00 .11 1 55 .74 .00
Notes: Time = Measurements prior to and after stimulus exposure; Persisting conict = childrens perception of interparental conict; Group = couple
conict vs. action lm; Sex = childs gender.
Family Science 47
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
(SCL-B vs. SCL-R) interaction indicating that the action
stimulus induced signicantly higher physiological arou-
sal than the couple con ict (F(1,52) = 14.63; p < 0.001;
η
p
2
= .22). No other main or interaction effects achieved
signicant values in this analysis (H1 conrmed).
As expected due to general training effects in the
attention task, children in both groups improved their
performance in the d2-R test from pre to post; however,
concerning accuracy, there was a signicant g roup × time
effect (F(1,55) = 6.07; p < .05; η
p
2
= .10). In accordance
with our hypothesis 2, children exposed to the couple
conict improved their accuracy performance after video
exposure compared to prior to the stimulus to a lesser
extent than children exposed to the action lm indicating
more serious cognitive interference by the couple conict
(H2 conrmed). Regarding the concentration performance
in the attention task, all children improved signicantly
from pre- to post-measurement (F(1,55) = 59.14; p < .001;
η
p
2
= .52), but the prepost change in the couple conict
group did not differ from the action lm group
(F(1,55) = 1.12; ns). Hypothesis 3 was therefore not con-
rmed. The two-way interactions between childs gender
and time or the three-way interactions between childs
gender, time, and group were nonsignicant in all
analyses.
Discussion
In the current study, childrens performance in an attention
task was assessed prior to and immediately after a video-
based stress inducing stimulus (either couple conict or
action lm) and childrens physiological reactivity (i.e.
SCL) was measured throughout video exposure. While
taking into account that our ndings are preliminary due
to the relatively small sample size, we found signicant
evidence that a 1-min couple conict interfered detrimen-
tally in childrens cognitive functioning on the level of
accuracy performance in the attention task, even after
controlling for CPIC at home. To the best of our knowl-
edge, this study contributes to the pertinent literature by
providing the rst empirical test of short-term resource
allocation owing to marital conict. The presen t results
are particularly notewort hy since effects were rigorously
tested comparing them with a sequence of an action lm, a
conservative control stimulus in this context given that it
induced signicantly more physiological arousal than the
couple conict. Previous research underlined the signi-
cant association between exposure to arousing television
programs and childs cognitive impairments in both short-
tem (Lang et al., 1999; Lillard & Peterson, 2011) and
longitudinally (Landhuis, Poulton, Welch, & Hancox,
2007; Swing, Gentile, Anderson, & Walsh, 2010).
While we cannot reveal from our data what underlying
psychological mechanisms may be involved, results match
theoretical frameworks. Con ceptions offered by the EST
(Davies & Cummings, 1994) and resource allocation mod-
els (e.g. Kahneman, 1973 ) suggest that childrens emo-
tional insecurity elicited by a simulated parental conict
places demands on their central resources, and thus task
performance (in this instance, atte ntion performance) may
deteriorate. Strong support for this hypothesis was
recently provided by two studies: (i) Davies et al. (2013)
reported that toddlers insecurity in the interparental rela-
tionship predicted failure in stage-salient tasks 1 year later,
which was linked to their subsequent behavior problems
and (ii) it was established that emotional insecurity follow-
ing interparental discord disrupts childrens adjustment in
school longitudinally by undermining their ability to focus
and sustain attention (Davies et al., 2008 ). Our study
added substantial evidence to these emerging lines of
research by conrming short-term attention impairment
following exposure to a couple conict within an experi-
mental approach. A major plausible reason for this could
be that thoughts and concerns about family stability
assume primacy in childrens cognitive functioning, lead-
ing to an inability to focus concentration in other contexts.
It must be assumed, thus, that parental conicts are a
potential cause for attention problems in children.
However, it is worth mentioning again that results
were not consistent across both task outcomes, indi cating
that performed accuracy was affected but concentration
was not. One explanation might be that training effects
in the d2 test are particularly observable with regard to
concentration (Bühner et al., 2006) and this measure
seems to be much more robust which is reected in its
resistance to falsication (Brickenkamp et al., 2010). In
the current context, this fact could have made the concen-
tration measure less amenable to the impact of the
observed couple argument. On the other hand, accuracy
may be more inuenced by affects and cognitive processes
and therefore exposure to experimental stimuli yielded
stronger effects on this variable.
The ndings reported in the present study are intri-
guing for several reasons. First, previous studies
revealed that childrens history of interparental discord
might be crucial i n predicting cognitive distortion fol-
lowing conict exposure (Medina et al., 2000;OBrien
&Chin,1998). In this study, however, we were able to
show that watching a short display of interadult anger is
as such disturbing to children, independent of their back-
ground of family conicts. Second, it is remarkable that
an impact of interparental conict on childrens accuracy
was found even though the stimulus presentation was
only of very short duration (i.e. 1 min). Considering that
interparental conicts in the family context usually last
longer and chronic marital discord is particularly harmful
for childrens well-being, it can be assumed that the
effects of parents disputes in family daily routine
might be considerably stronger. Third, the impact of
the childrens own parents conicts may be much
48 M. Zemp et al.
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
higher than effects of an unfamiliar videotaped couple as
it was used in this study. Fourth, previous research
indicated that interparental discord characterized by phy-
sical or verbal aggression is the most detrimental
(Cummings, Vogel, Cummings, & El-Sheikh, 1989 ).
Thus, it is noteworthy that our stimulus, representing
an angry but not hostile, aggressive or violent interaction
already showed such strong effects. As conicts at home
are often characterized by aggression or violence (in
Switzerland 24% of women experience physical aggres-
sion at least one time during th eir l ifetime ( Gillioz, De
Puy, & D ucret, 1997)), again effects in real life are likely
to be more salient. Fifth, as children often are the subject
of the conict (e.g. Papp, Cumm ings, & Goeke-Morey,
2002) or actively engage in parental conicts by playing
an active part in family conict situations (Jenkins,
Smith, & Graham, 1989), effects may be more pro-
nounced in real family life than in the lab where there
was no involvement of the child in t he c onict.
On the other hand, several limitations of this study
merit discussion. First, the experi mental design allows
high internal validity but potentially reduces external
validity. Childrens reactions to marital conict stimuli in
the laboratory are not equivalent to responses to dispu tes
between their parents at home. Second, the present study
used a conict-unrelated stimulus without any adult inter-
action as control condition in order to avoid negative
reactivity in children due to potential classical condition-
ing of interparental conict following childrens family
history. To compare a marital dispute with either a dis-
agreement among peers or siblings or with a constructive
or neutral interparental conversation is therefore an impor-
tant future research direction. Third, since experimentally
in nature, this study focused on short-termed effects of
parental conict on childs attention performance.
Although Davies et al. (2008) reported attention difcul-
ties to be one of the pathogenic processes in the impact of
interparental discord on childs school adjustment, we
cannot establish from our data whether the presented
mechanisms might undermine childrens development
(most likely, their academic performance) in the longer
term. In a simil ar vein, though considerations from
different theoretical assumptions were discussed to inter-
pret our ndings, they were not directly tested. Thus,
additional studies employing more sophisticated research
designs, including longitudinal data, are needed. Fourth,
the trainings effects inherent to the attention task used in
this study make interpretation of results more complex.
The application of a task with available parallel forms
would have been more advantageous. Fifth, given the
nature of the study a self-selection bias in terms of over-
representation of well-adjusted families has to be assumed
which limits the generalizability of results. However, this
could also mean that even stronger effects might be
expected in a more representative community sample
including also more families with low income and poorer
education.
Conclusion
Taken toget her, our study added to the evidence that
interparental conict deserves particular consideration in
explaining attention problems in children. The current
ndings are alarming considering that the impact of the
analog conict stimulus used in this study may be minor
in comparison with frequent and intense pa rental disputes
in real family life. If replicated elsewhere in larger sample
sizes and more sophisticated research designs including
observational measures in the family context, our ndings
have several practical implications. Rather than exclu-
sively improve parenting and child-rearing abil ities in
parents, prevention, and intervention programs concerning
child maladjustment should further stress the enhancement
of the parental relationship itself (Emery, Fincham, &
Cummings, 1992). Authors have already pointed toward
the relevance of early prevention on the parents level in
this context (Heinrichs, Bodenmann, & Hahlweg, 2008).
There is increasing evidence that relationship education
with an emphasis on con
ict communication is effective
in reducing psychological problems in children as has
been shown, for instance, in the context of the CCET
(e.g. Bodenmann, Cina, Ledermann, & Sanders, 2008).
Although further research is urgently needed, the present
ndings on the signicance of couples relationships for
childrens attention problems are promising.
References
Barletta, J., & OMara, B. (2006). A review of the impact of
marital conict on child adjustment. Australian Journal of
Guidance and Counselling, 16,91105. doi:10.1375/
ajgc.16.1.91
Beal, D. J., Weiss, H. M., Barros, E., & Macdermid, S. M.
(2005). An episodic process model of affective inuences
on performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(6),
10541068. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1054
Bodenmann, G., Cina, A., Ledermann, T., & Sanders, M. R.
(2008). The efcacy of the triple P-positive parenting pro-
gram in improving parenting and child behavior: A compar-
ison with two other treatment conditions. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 46(4), 411427. doi:10.1016/j.
brat.2008.01.001
Bodenmann, G., Schaer, M., & Gmelch, S. (2008). Paarlife
glücklich zu zweit trotz alltagsstress: Eine interaktive DVD
zur verbesserung der partnerschaftlichen kompetenzen.
Freiburg: Universitätsverlag.
Brickenkamp, R. (2002). Test d2. Aufmerksamkeits- und konzen-
trationstest: Manual [Test d2. Attention and concentration
test: Manual]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Brickenkamp, R., Schmidt-Atzert, L., & Liepmann, D. (2010).
Test d2 Revision. Aufmerksamkeits- und konzentrationstest:
Manual [Test d2 Revision. Attention and concentration
test: Manual]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Family Science 49
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
Bühner, M., Ziegler, M., Bohnes, B., & Lauterbach, K. (2006).
Übungseffekte in den TAP untertests test go/nogo und
geteilte aufmerksamkeit sowie dem aufmerksamkeits-belas-
tungstest (d2) [Training effects in the TAP subtests go/nogo
and divided attention as well as in the attention to detail test
d2]. Zeitschrift Für Neuropsychologie, 17(3), 191199.
doi:10.1024/1016-264X.17.3.191
Cummings, E. M. (1995). Usefulness of experiments for the
study of the family. Journal of Family Psychology, 9(2),
175185. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.9.2.175
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2010). Marital conict and
children. An emotional security perspective. New York, NY:
The Guilford Press.
Cummings, E. M., Simpson, K. S., & Wilson, A. (1993).
Childrens responses to interadult anger as a function of
information about resolution. Developmental Psychology,
29, 978985. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.29.6.978
Cummings, E. M., Vogel, D., Cummings, J. S., & El-Sheikh, M.
(1989). Childrens responses to different forms of expression
of anger between adults. Child Development, 60, 1392
1404. doi:10.2307/1130929
David, K. M., & Murphy, B. C. (2004). Interparental conict and
late adolescents sensitization to conict: The moderating
effects of emotional functioning and gender. Journal of
Youth & Adolescence, 33(3), 187200. doi:10.1023/B:
JOYO.0000025318.26238.40
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conict and
child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis.
Psychological Bulletin, 116(3), 387411. doi:10.1037/0033-
2909.116.3.387
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1998). Exploring childrens
emotional security as a mediator of the link between marital
relations and child adjustment. Child Development, 69(1),
124139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06138.x
Davies, P. T., & Lindsay, L. L. (2001). Does gender moderate the
effects of marital conict on children?. In J. H. Grych & F.
D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conict and child develop-
ment: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 6497). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Davies, P. T., Manning, L. G., & Cicchetti, D. (2013). Tracing
the cascade of children s insecurity in the interparental rela-
tionship: The role of stage-salient tasks. Child Development,
84(1), 297312. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01844.x
Davies, P. T., Myers, R. L., & Cummings, E. M. (1996).
Responses of children and adolescents to marital conict
scenarios as a function of the emotionality of conict end-
ings. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly-Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 42(1), 121.
Davies, P. T., Myers, R. L., Cummings, E. M., & Heindel, S.
(1999). Adult conict history and childrens subsequent
responses to conict: An experimental test. Journal of
Family Psychology, 13(4), 610628. doi:10.1037/0893-
3200.13.4.610
Davies, P. T., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2007). Advances in the
formulation of emotional security theory: An ethologically
based perspective. Advances in Child Development and
Behavior, 35,87137. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-009735-
7.50008-6
Davies, P. T., Winter, M. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2006). The
implications of emotional security theory for understand-
ing and treating childhood psychopathology. Development
and Psychopathology, 18(3), 707735. doi:10.1017/
S0954579406060354
Davies, P. T., Woitach, M. J., Winter, M. A., & Cummings, E. M.
(2008). Childrens insecure representations of the
interparental relationship and their school adjustment: The
mediating role of attention difculties. Child Development,
79(5), 15701582. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01206.x
Demorest, A. P. (1992). The role of social cognition in childrens
social maladjustment. Social Cognition, 10(2), 211233.
doi:10.1521/soco.1992.10.2.211
Emery, R. E., Fincham, F. D., & Cummings, E. M. (1992).
Parenting in context: Systemic thinking about parental con-
ict and its inuence on children. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 60(6), 909
912. doi:10.1037/0022-
006X.60.6.909
Ghazarian, S. R., & Buehler, C. (2010). Interparental conict and
academic achievement: An examination of mediating and
moderating factors. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 39(1),
2335. doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9360-1
Gillioz, L., De Puy, J., & Ducret, V. (1997). Domination et
violence envers la femme dans le couple [Domination and
violence against women in the family]. Lausanne: Payot.
Glenn, C. R., Klein, D. N., Lissek, S., Britton, J. C., Pine, D. S.,
& Hajcak, G. (2012). The development of fear learning and
generalization in 813 year-olds. Developmental
Psychobiology, 54(7), 675684. doi:10.1002/dev.20616
Gödde, M., & Walper, S. (2001). Elterliche konikte aus der
sicht von kindern und jugendlichen: Die deutsche kurzfas-
sung der childrens perception of interparental conict scale
(CPIC) [The german short version of the childrens percep-
tion of interparental conict scale]. Diagnostica, 47(1), 18
26. doi:10.1026//0012-1924.47.1.18
Goeke-Morey,M.C.,Cummings,E.M.,Harold,G.T.,&
Shelton, K. H. (2003). Categories and continua of
destructive and constructive marital c onict tactics from
the perspective of U.S. and welsh children. Journa l o f
Family Psychology, 17(3), 327338. doi:10 .1037/0893-
3200.17.3.327
Goeke-Morey, M. C., Cummings, E. M., & Papp, L. M. (2007).
Children and marital conict resolution: Implications for emo-
tional security and adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology,
21(4), 744753. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.744
Grych, J. H., Seid, M., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Assessing
marital conict from the childs perspective: The childrens
perception of interparental conict scale. Child
Development, 63(3), 558572. doi:10.2307/1131346
Harold,G.T.,Aitken,J.J.,&Shelton,K.H.(2007).Inter-
parental conict and childrens academic attainment: A
longitudinal analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 48 (12), 12231232. doi:10.1111/j.1469-
7610.2007.01793.x
Heinrichs, N., Bodenmann, G., & Hahlweg, K. (2008).
Prävention bei paaren und familien. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Huston, A. C., Wright, J. C., Wartella, E., Rice, M. L., Watkins,
B. A., Campbell, T., & Potts, R. (1981). Communicating
more than content: Formal features of childrens television
programs. Journal of Communication, 31(3), 3248.
doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1981.tb00426.x
Jenkins, J. M., Smith, M. A., & Graham, P. J. (1989). Coping
with parental quarrels. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(2), 182189.
doi:10.1097/00004583-198903000-00006
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Landhuis, C. E., Poulton, R., Welch, D., & Hancox, R. J. (2007).
Does childhood television viewing lead to attention pro-
blems in adolescence? Results from a prospective longitudi-
nal study. Pediatrics, 120(3), 532537. doi:10.1542/
peds.2007-0978
50 M. Zemp et al.
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
Lang, A., Bolls, P., Potter, R. F., & Kawahara, K. (1999). The
effects of production pacing and arousing content on the
information processing of television messages. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 43(4), 451475.
doi:10.1080/08838159909364504
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008).
International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective rat-
ings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report A-
8). Gainesville: University of Florida.
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O.
(1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and
behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261273.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x
Lillard, A. S., & Peterson, J. (2011). The immediate impact of
different types of television on young childrens executive
function. Pediatrics, 128(4), 644649. doi:10.1542/
peds.2010-1919
Medina, A. M., Margolin, G., & Wilcox, R. R. (2000). Family
hostility and childrens cognitive processes. Behavior
Therapy, 31(4), 667684. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(00)
80037-4
Neumann, D. L., Waters, A. M., Westbury, H. R., & Henry, J.
(2008). The use of an unpleasant sound unconditional stimu-
lus in an aversive conditioning procedure with 8- to 11-year-
old children. Biological Psychology, 79(3), 337342.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.08.005
OBrien, M., & Chin, C. (1998). The relationship between chil-
drens reported exposure to interparental conict and mem-
ory biases in the recognition of aggressive and constructive
conict words. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
24(6), 647656. doi:10.1177/0146167298246008
Papp, L. M., Cummings, E. M., & Goeke-Morey, M. C. (2002).
Marital conicts in the home when children are present
versus absent. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 774783.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.774
Schneider, W., & Fisk, A. D. (1982). Concurrent automatic and
controlled visual search: Can processing occur without
resource cost? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning Memory and Cognition, 8(4), 261278.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.8.4.261
Shelton, K. H., Harold, G. T., Goeke-Morey, M. C., &
Cummings, E. M. (2006). Children
s coping with marital
conict:Theroleofconict expression and gender.
Social Development, 15(2), 232247. doi:10.1046/
j.1467-9507.2006.00338.x
Snyder, J. R. (1998). Marital conict and child adjustment: What
about gender? Developmental Review, 18(3), 390420.
doi:10.1006/drev.1998.0469
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Winter, M. A., Cummings, E.
M., & Schermerhorn, A. C. (2008). Interparental conict and
childrens school adjustment: The explanatory role of chil-
drens internal representations of interparental and parent-
child relationships. Developmental Psychology, 44(6),
16781690. doi:10.1037/a0013857
Swing, E. L., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Walsh, D. A.
(2010). Television and video game exposure and the devel-
opment of attention problems. Pediatrics, 126(2), 214221.
doi:10.1542/peds.2009-1508
Swiss Federal Statistical Ofce. (2011). Income, consumption
and wealth. Retrieved from http://www.bfs.admin.ch
/bfs/portal/de/index/themen/20/02/blank/key/einkommen0/
niveau.html
Family Science 51
Downloaded by [UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zürich] at 08:22 01 July 2014
... Negative Auswirkungen partnerschaftlicher Konflikte auf das kindliche Befinden konnten auch im experimentellen Setting festgestellt werden: Eine Experimentalstudie untersuchte die Effekte einer einminütigen Filmszene eines Paarkonfliktes auf die Aufmerksamkeitsleistung von 11-bis 13-jährigen Kindern im Vergleich zu einer Sequenz aus einem Actionfilm, die aufgrund von hohem Tempo, schnellen Bewegungen und dramatischer Hintergrundmusik die kindliche Aufmerksamkeit erregte. Obgleich der Paarkonflikt bei den Kindern eine geringere physiologische Erregung auslöste, interferierte er im Vergleich zur Actionfilmsequenz stärker mit der kindlichen Aufmerksamkeitsleistung, vor allem im Hinblick auf die erfasste Genauigkeit (Fehlerwert) in einem Aufmerksamkeitstest, der vor und unmittelbar nach der Videodarbietung von den Kindern bearbeitet wurde (Zemp, Bodenmann & Beach, 2014). Kinder aus konfliktreichen Familien mit hoher physiologischer Stressreaktivität zeigten besonders starke Beeinträchtigungen in der Aufmerksamkeitsleistung (Zemp, Bodenmann & Cummings, 2014). ...
... Children from families of high hostility improved their attention performance after stimulus exposure in comparison with children from low-conflict families. Another experimental study revealed that an unresolved videotaped couple conflict impaired children's short-term attention performance more harmfully than a physiologically arousing action film (Zemp, Bodenmann, & Beach, 2014). Further analyses showed that children's family background of interparental conflict and their skin conductance reactivity moderated these effects (Zemp, Bodenmann, & Cummings, 2014). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Previous research suggests that exposure to interparental conflict may affect children’s attention and memory performance, but few studies have examined this hypothesis experimentally. Objective: The goal of this study is to address this gap by an experimental approach using audiotaped couple conflict simulations. Method: School classes of 5th- and 6th-graders were randomly assigned to listen to either (1) an unresolved couple, (2) a resolved couple conflict, or (3) a neutral couple conversation. Children’s attention and memory performance were measured before and after stimulus exposure by performance tasks and their emotional insecurity was assessed by self-report. Results: Children’s emotional insecurity moderated the impact of the simulated couple conflict on their memory performance, but not on attention. At low levels of emotional insecurity, children performed better in the memory task after hearing the unresolved couple conflict compared to the control group. Conclusion: Children’s responses to simulated conflicts differ by a child’s history of interparental conflict and conflict characteristics, such as whether arguments are resolved. Implication: Consideration of the family background is warranted when studying or treating child cognitive performance problems.
... Decline in couple relationship satisfaction across the transition to parenthood is associated with less sensitivity in mothers and less positive parent-child interaction (Petch & Halford, 2008), and negative parental interactions are associated with insensitive parenting and inconsistent discipline (Cina & Bodenmann, 2009). Additionally, offspring from dissatisfied parents have been shown to suffer more often from physical problems such as infectious diseases (Henriksen & Thuen, 2015) and sleep disorders (El-Sheikh, Buckhalt, Mize, & Acebo, 2006), are at elevated risk for mental difficulties (Troxel & Matthews, 2004), and problems at school and in their academic career (Ghazarian & Buehler, 2010;Zemp, Bodenmann, & Beach, 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Der Einfluss der Familie in der Entstehung und Aufrechterhaltung der ADHS bei Kindern ist wissenschaftlich gut fundiert, jedoch hat sich die einschlägige Forschung weitgehend auf das elterliche Erziehungsverhalten oder die Qualität der Eltern-Kind-Beziehung konzentriert. Die Rolle der elterlichen Partnerschaft in betroffenen Familien wurde in der Literatur bislang vernachlässigt. Diese Übersichtsarbeit fasst den aktuellen Forschungsstand zu den Zusammenhängen von Partnerschaftsstörungen und kindlicher ADHS zusammen und diskutiert chronische Paarkonflikte als Risikofaktor für die Entwicklung von ADHS-Symptomen. Basierend auf dem gegenwärtigen Kenntnisstand wird ein integratives Modell postuliert, das von einer genetischen Vulnerabilität für die kindliche ADHS ausgeht, mit der Partnerschaftsstörungen interagieren. Es stellt dar, wie Partnerschaftsstörungen mit ihren möglichen direkten und indirekten Folgen für die Ätiologie, Aufrechterhaltung und/oder Verstärkung von kindlichen ADHS-Symptomen bedeutsam sein können, welche wiederum auf die Paarbeziehung zurückwirken. Das Review verdeutlicht die Wichtigkeit von Paarpräventionsprogrammen zur Vorbeugung von kindlichen Störungen sowie die angemessene Berücksichtigung von Partnerschaftskonflikten in der Diagnostik und Therapie von Familien mit ADHS-betroffenen Kindern.
Article
Full-text available
Fauber and Long's (1991) overview of research on family therapy with children is a valuable integration of the literatures on the family correlates of and treatments for childhood disorders. Several concerns apply to some of the inferences they draw from risk research, however. Their assertion that various sources of family distress have effects that are mediated primarily through parenting is questionable, as is their suggestion that parenting therefore is the appropriate focus of family treatment. The conceptual issues of reductionism, linearity, holism, and change in defining causality are discussed in questioning these conclusions about etiology and treatment. Other empirical and methodological issues are raised briefly, particularly as they relate to statistical models of direct and indirect influences and to the body of correlational and analogue research on how parental conflict influences children.
Article
Full-text available
This article presents a review of research literature regarding the association between marital conflict and child adjustment in intact families. Factors such as the child temperament and gender, the relational dynamics between parent/s and child, the child's developmental stage and cognitive appraisal of the conflict — including aspects of the child's physiological system, as well as parenting practices — are explored to outline contributions to the understanding of issues associated with this aspect of family process. Developmental psychopathology is offered as a useful resource in terms of a process oriented theoretical framework providing a multi-dimensional approach to exploring and explaining the impact of marital conflict on children.
Chapter
Interparental Conflict and Child Development is a 2001 text that provides an in-depth analysis of the rapidly expanding body of research on the impact of interparental conflict on children. Emphasizing developmental and family systems perspectives, it investigates a range of important issues, including the processes by which exposure to conflict may lead to child maladjustment, the role of gender and ethnicity in understanding the effects of conflict, the influence of conflict on parent–child, sibling, and peer relations, family violence, and interparental conflict in divorced and step-families. It also addresses the implications of this research for prevention, clinical intervention, and public policy. Each chapter examines relevant conceptual and methodological questions, reviews on pertinent data, and identifies pathways for future research. Thus, the book serves to chart the course for continued investigation into the links between marital and child functioning.
Article
A total of 70 Latino children between the ages of 7 and 12 years completed measures of exposure to interparental conflict and listened to audio taped simulations of interparental conflict. Children's memory for aggressive and constructive conflict words used during the session was then assessed with a word recognition task. Results indicate that older children who report witnessing high levels of marital conflict made significantly more false positive and fewer false negative memory errors for aggressive words than did all other children. This study provides preliminary support for the theory that children develop marital conflict representations that reflect their socialization experiences with interparental conflict and that guide information processing in the context of novel marital conflict.