ArticlePDF Available

The Concept of Domain in Developmental Analyses of Hierarchical Complexity

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Individuals do not operate “at a stage of development.” They operate at a range of different levels of hierarchical complexity depending on skill area, task, context, degree of support, and other variables. It is thus necessary to postulate the concept of domain to refer to the particular conceptual, behavioral, or affective area within which activity operates. The concept raises questions and implications for theory building and application. Such issues are elaborated by discussing a variety of domains and social contexts. A postformal case example of leadership in higher education illuminates the concept of domains and the interrelationships among domains.
Content may be subject to copyright.
World Futures, 64: 330–347, 2008
Copyright c
Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN 0260-4027 print / 1556-1844 online
DOI: 10.1080/02604020802301170
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN IN DEVELOPMENTAL
ANALYSES OF HIERARCHICAL COMPLEXITY
MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
Department of Psychology, Merrimack College, North Andover, Massachusetts, USA
Individuals do not operate “at a stage of development.” They operate at a range of
different levels of hierarchical complexity depending on skill area, task, context,
degree of support, and other variables. It is thus necessary to postulate the concept
of domain to refer to the particular conceptual, behavioral, or affective area within
which activity operates. The concept raises questions and implications for theory
building and application. Such issues are elaborated by discussing a variety of
domains and social contexts. A postformal case example of leadership in higher
education illuminates the concept of domains and the interrelationships among
domains.
KEYWORDS: Development, domain, hierarchical complexity,higher education, leadership,
skill theory.
It is tempting to think of development as a process that involves the transformation
of a single, broad set of competencies in an organism, human or animal. In the
realm of psychological development of people, it seems intuitive to ask, “At what
level of conceptual development does this person function?” This question cannot
be answered. This is because individuals do not function at any one level of
development at any particular time of life. Instead, at any given time, individuals
operate at a range of different developmental levels depending on the particular
skill area, task, context, degree of support, and a suite of other variables. It is
thus necessary to postulate the concept of domain. The concept of domain refers
to the particular conceptual, behavioral, or affective area within which skilled
activity operates. The concept of domain raises several fundamental questions.
What constitutes a domain? How do domains come into being? How do skills
to perform increasingly complex tasks develop within particular domains? Can
development generalize from one domain to another? What are the implications
of the localized notion of domain for theory building and application? These and
related issues are elaborated with reference to conceptual development in a variety
of different domains and social contexts. A postformal case example of leadership
at the Metasystematic stage in higher education is used to illuminate the concept
of domains and the interrelationships among domains. Drawing from insights
Address correspondence to Michael F. Mascolo, Merrimack College, Department of
Psychology, North Amdover, MA 01845, USA. E-mail: michael mascolo@merrimack.edu
330
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 331
suggested by the case, the article concludes with discussing the implications for
higher education and its leadership.
Models of the development of hierarchical complexity have their intellectual
origins in Piaget’s theory of cognitive development (Piaget, 1970). As is well
known, Piaget suggested that the structure of thinking undergoes a series of quali-
tative transformations in development (Piaget, 1971, 1983). The idea of structures
d’ensemble—structures of the whole—was an important part of this theory of
development. The concept of “structures of the whole” refers to the idea that,
within any given stage of development, thinking forms a more or less singular
system that has broad application to many different tasks or task areas. From this
view, an individual’s thinking could not operate in two different stages at the same
time. For example, when an adolescent passes from the abstract to the formal
operational stage, he or she becomes capable of manipulating abstract ideas in a
variety of different areas in logical and systematic ways. If the structure of think-
ing forms a single integrated holistic system as Piaget assumed, one would expect
different formal operational skills to emerge in development at around the same
age in any given individual. However, decades of research and hundreds of studies
have demonstrated that variation rather than synchrony is the rule rather than the
exception in the emergence of cognitive abilities—even for closely related skills
judged to be of the same level of complexity (Fischer, Bullock, Rotenberg, and
Raya, 1993).
The concept of domain is defined in contradistinction to the Piagetian concept of
structures d’ensemble. Rather than developing as a single, homogeneous series of
stages, thinking develops within particular cognitive, behavioral or socioemotional
domains (Fischer, Bullock, Rotenberg, and Raya, 1993; Turiel, 1983)—particular
areas of thinking, feeling, or acting that develop relatively independent of one
another. This conception is common to a variety of models of development that
have been postulated over recent decades (Case, 1992; Case et al., 1996; Com-
mons, Trudeau, Stein, Richards, and Krause, 1998; Demetriou and Efklides, 1994;
Halford, 1999; Fischer and Bidell, 2006; Mascolo and Fischer, 1998, 2005). The-
orists and researchers have proposed and tested sequences of development in a
wide variety of psychological, social, and academic domains in childhood and
adulthood, including moral reasoning (Dawson, 2004; Kohlberg, 1984), social
reasoning (Lamborn, Fischer, and Pipp, 1994), reflective reasoning (Kitchener,
King, and DeLuca, 2005), number sense (Griffin, 2005) narrative knowing (McK-
eough and Genereaux, 2003), ethical reasoning (Perry, 1970), faith (Fowler, 1981),
conceptions of authority and contract (Dawson and Gabrielian, 2003), the devel-
opment of self-evaluative emotions (Mascolo, Fischer, and Li, 2003) and many
others The concept of skill provides a useful way to think about the nature of psy-
chological development as it occurs within specific domains of thinking, feeling,
and acting (Fischer and Bidell, 2006). A skill refers to an individual’s capacity
to control elements of acting, thinking, and feeling within specified contexts and
within particular tasks or task domains. As such, a skill is a type of control structure
for performing particular tasks within particular psychological areas. Skills are not
general structures. There are no general, de-contextualized, or all-purpose skills;
skills are tied to specific tasks and task domains. Skills in different conceptual
332 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
domains (e.g., reading, mathematics, musical appreciation, social interaction, ath-
letic ability) develop relatively independently of each other at different rates and
toward different developmental endpoints. Assessments of the developmental level
of one skill in one conceptual domain (e.g., logical reasoning) will not necessarily
predict the developmental level of skills in a different domain (e.g., classification)
or even in conceptually similar tasks (e.g., classification of familiar versus un-
familiar objects). One can chart developmental sequences only for skills within
particular tasks, domains, and within particular social contexts and assessment
conditions (Dawson, Xie, and Wilson, 2003; Fischer et al., 1993; see Table 3 in
“Introduction to the Model of Hierarchical Complexity,” this issue).
The concepts of domain and hierarchical complexity are applicable to humans
and animals alike (Commons, 2006). A wide literature addresses the issue of the
similarities and differences between human and animal capacities (Bekoff, Allen,
and Burghhardt, 2002; Savage-Rumbaugh, Shank, and Taylor, 2001; Tomasello,
Carpenter, and Call, 2005). Much of this work is aimed toward identifying whether
or not higher-level animals exhibit cognitive capacities normally associated with
humans. The question of whether or not animal cognition is best regarded as qual-
itatively or quantitatively different from human cognition is an important issue.
The Model of Hierarchical Complexity provides a set of tools for understanding
and studying comparative cognition. First, as is the case in humans, animal behav-
ior operates within particular domains. These domains reflect the organism’s need
to adapt to particular classes of selection pressures in its environment. Commons
(2006) described a variety of different domains of animal action, each of which
is likely to exhibit its own trajectory of development. These include mate selec-
tion, attachment, pecking order, prey defense, predator actions, migration and way
finding, food sharing, communication, food selection, and other domains of adap-
tive action. Second, within these domains, rather than attempting to inquire about
whether any given species of animal has or does not have any particular capacity
(e.g., symbolic function, use of imagery, capacity for emotion), one instead asks,
what form and developmental level does an animal’s acting, thinking, and feeling
take in particular contexts and as a result of particular experiential histories. Care-
ful examination of the specific structure of tasks that any given animal performs
allows specification of the developmental level of the animal’s performance, thus
allowing precise comparison of human and animal capacities.
CAN THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN BE PRECISELY DEFINED?
In all of the foregoing, the concept of domain-specificity is defined negatively,
that is, in contrast to the idea of broad-based abilities. This, however, begs the
question of the precise meaning of the concept of domain. Although we know that
development does not move in broad across-the-board stages, we are nonetheless
left with the question: What defines the boundaries of a domain? Is it possible
to identify domains precisely? If so, how many domains of thinking and acting
are there? How do they develop in relation to each other? Different researchers
answer these questions in different ways.
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 333
Domain-Specific Development
Some theorists suggest that the concept of domain can be defined in relatively pre-
cise terms. For example, Case and his colleagues (Case, 1992; Case et al., 1996)
have identified what they call a series of central conceptual structures that begin
to emerge and develop in childhood. Building on this framework, Griffin (2005)
describes a series of central conceptual structures that structure the development
of knowledge about number, narrative, and space (drawing) in children. Table 1
provides an adaptation and extension of her analysis of developmental changes in
each of these domains from early childhood through adulthood. As indicated in
Table 1, it is possible to view mathematical, narrative, and visual representation
(drawing) skills as three distinct conceptual domains, each of which develops
along a distinct pathway. The ages specified for each developmental level reflect
the earliest ages at which skills at the level in question can begin to emerge given
appropriate experience, neurological development, and social support. Although
the complexity of skills at each developmental level is comparable across the
domains, it is clear that the level of skill that any given individual achieves in one
domain does not ordinarily predict the level of skills achieved in other domains.
For example, a person who has achieved a relatively high level of skill in fash-
ioning complex narratives may easily function at a much lower level of skill in
mathematical or drawing tasks. Indeed, in the absence of experience and focused
effort over long periods of time, most individuals will not attain the highest levels
of functioning in any particular conceptual domain.
Relations among Domains of Developing Skills
Although some tasks draw on skills that fall within particular domains, many,
if not most tasks and activities involve the coordination of different conceptual
domains in development. One particularly good example of the coordination of
multiple domains involves the development of moral judgment. Turiel and his col-
leagues (Turiel, 1983, 2002) have elaborated a domain theory of moral and social
development. Turiel’s approach builds on, yet departs from, Kohlberg’s (1984)
seminal theory of stages of moral reasoning. Kohlberg (1963, 1984) suggested
that moral reasoning develops through pre-conventional, conventional, and post-
conventional stages. In this way, moral reasoning develops out of social reasoning.
From this view, genuinely moral reasoning can only emerge in the postformal
stage (also called post-conventional). At this level, adolescents and adults gain
the capacity to differentiate consistently between social conventions—which can
take different forms in different social contexts—to genuinely moral concerns that
transcend social convention.
In contrast to Kohlberg’s approach, Turiel (1983, 2002) has suggested that
moral reasoning does not have its developmental origins in social conventions.
Turiel and his colleagues have suggested that reasoning about morality,social
conventions, and personal issues constitute distinct (albeit connected) domains of
reasoning. They have shown that children as young as 5 years of age are able to
differentiate moral and social conventional issues in structured interview contexts.
For example, when asked if it would be right to hurt another child if a person in
334 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
Table 1
Developmental Transformations in Hierarchical Complexity for Three
Cognitive Domains
Developmental Drawing
Level Number Narrative (Arts)
Metasystematic
(Principles) (25
years +)
Manipulations of
Higher-Order
Mathematical
Structures and
Objects. Study of
relations among
abstract structures
of mathematical
operations (e.g.,
detecting structural
isomorphisms
between groups of
mathematical
operations in
seemingly
disparate areas).
Principled Integration of
Literary Forms and
Genres. Principled
articulation and
integration of relations
among multiple literary
genres, methods, styles,
etc. into a stable and
consolidated style or
narrative system that
organizes a given
narrative.
Principled Consolidation of
Style. Visual expression
organized in terms of
systematic principles that
organize multiple
dimensions of visual,
expressive,
methodological,
conventional forms and
content.
Systematic
(Abstract
Systems) (18–21
years+)
Higher-Order
Mathematical
Relations.
Capacity to
manipulate
abstract relations
involving change
over time (e.g.,
calculus as an
integration of
algebra, geometry,
and arithmetic);
capacity to solve
two simultaneous
abstract relations;
abstract algebraic
proofs.
Narratives Structured by
Integrative Relations.
Complex or interweaving
narratives organized by
relations among multiple
qualities of characters and
events; integrative use of
higher-order literary
devices (e.g., anachrony,
embedded narrative,
higher-order tropes);
violation of standard
forms to produce novel
effects.
Higher-Order
Visual-Conceptual
Integrations.
Manipulation of multiple
visual, conventional
and/or methodological
means to represent
intangible, emotional, or
abstract content.
Modification of
convention or
introduction of novel
means to express abstract,
emotional, and other
visual content.
Formal (Abstract
Mappings)
(14–15 years+)
Transformation of
Algebraic
Relationships.
Able to coordinate
Dialectic Relations among
Stable Characters.
Complex narratives
involving characters
Visual-Conceptual
Integration. Intentional
use of variation in visual
form, content and/or
relations between
two abstract
variables (e.g., f=
m *a; a2+b2=c2)
with inner states
exhibiting continuity over
time. Conflicts derive
from relations among
characters or events.
technique in the service
of a conceptual goal or
outcome (e.g., use of
distortion, variations in
color to represent
emotional themes); use of
visual means that suggest
abstract or themes.
(Continued on next page)
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 335
Table 1
Developmental Transformations in Hierarchical Complexity for Three
Cognitive Domains (Continued)
Developmental Drawing
Level Number Narrative (Arts)
Abstractions (10 years) Simple Algebraic
Representations.
Incipient
representation of
single abstract
variables
representing
quantity (e.g.,
2x=4)
Conflict-Driven
Multi-Lined
Narrative.
Complex stories
involving
characters with
mental states and
motives, organized
plots and subplots
driven by conflicts
and attempts to
resolve conflicts.
Three-Dimensional
Scenes. Draws scenes
exhibiting fore-,
middle-, and
background within an
integrated continuous
space. Fills in details
in realistic ways. Use
of visual metaphor
(e.g., drawing a
teacher as a “witch”).
Primary/Concrete
(Representational
Systems) (6–7 years)
Mental Number
Line.
Understanding
relations between
numbers on a
“mental” number
line; capacity for
addition and
subtraction. By
8–9 years,
multiplication and
division.
Intentional Story
Lines.
Temporal–causal
plot lines involving
characters with
mental states and
motives (e.g., ”We
went to the zoo,
but then I got
hungry so we took
train to go buy
some yummy hot
dogs. . . ”)
Mental Reference Line.
Child can draw
identifiable persons
and objects placed
within a particular
location or scene (e.g.,
person and a house;
flower under the sun),
often with lines
indicating ground or
sky.
Sentential/Preoperational
(Representational
Mappings) (31/2–4 years)
Mental Counting
Line.
Representation of
relations between
numbers;
comparison of
more vs. less.
Causal-Temporal
Action Sequences.
Child relates
multiple actions or
events in time or in
cause-effect
relation (e.g., “We
went to the zoo
and then we got a
hot dog”)
Identifiable Objects and
Figures. Capacity to
draw a recognizable
yet barely articulated
figure or object (e.g.,
person), often depicted
as hovering on the
page.
authority said it was right to do so, most children indicated that the act would
not be right (Kim and Turiel, 1996; Laupa and Turiel, 1986). The differentiation
between the moral and conventional appears to be a ubiquitous one and arises in
a variety of contexts. For example, in highly religious populations, when asked
if stealing or the infliction of pain onto others could be moral if it were God’s
will that it be done, most respondents indicate that God would never will such
acts (Turiel, 2002). Further, Arab women living within the Druze community in
336 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
Northern Israel were asked whether it is appropriate for men, but not women, to
work outside the home. Most women affirmed the prevailing social conventions
and indicated that it would not be appropriate to do so. However, when asked,
most women also indicated that while the prescription against working outside the
home should be followed, the rule itself was also unfair (Wainryb, 1995; Wainryb
and Turiel, 1994).
Turiel (2002) argues that social and cultural differences in moral rules often
arise not as a result of differences in beliefs about the moral aspects of an action,
but instead as a result of social beliefs and background knowledge about the nature
of the issues at hand. For example, Americans differ in their judgments about the
morality of abortion, although most Americans would maintain that the killing
of an innocent child is immoral. One source of differences in moral judgments
about abortion involves different assumptions about what it means to be a “fetus”
or a “child.” These findings, and scores of others, support the proposition that
moral and conventional rules reflect distinct domains of thinking. In addition,
the idea that ostensive differences in moral reasoning often reflect differences in
assumptions about social and other domains of thought provides a framework for
articulating a non-relativistic approach to moral judgment. (Also see Robinette on
moral reasoning, this issue.)
Dynamic Webs of Skill Development
Although it is possible to identify particular tasks and activities that operate within
particular domains of thinking, feeling, or acting in everyday life, most tasks in-
volve an integration of multiple task domains. When working with relatively
simple or bounded tasks, it is often a simple matter to identify the conceptual do-
main or domains on which a given task will draw. Over time, however, individuals
construct higher-order skills for purposes of adapting to novel tasks, events, and
problems. Higher-order skills reflect performances at higher stages of hierarchical
complexity. In so doing, higher-order skills emerge from the constructive differ-
entiation and inter-coordination of skill elements from diverse task domains. For
example, the skills involved in composing an effective Letter to the Editor go be-
yond basic writing skills. They involve identifying the perspectives of the editors
and potential readers; adjusting the language in order to be persuasive; ability to
fashion narratives in ways that describe how a given issue affects people; the use
of mathematics or statistics to support a point being made, and so on. Thus, in
everyday social activity—particularly when performing higher-order activities—
there are few if any “pure” conceptual domains of functioning. As a result, it is
often useful to speak of emergent or higher-order domains of action. For example,
consider the repertoire of skills required to function as a social activist or political
lobbyist. A social activist operates in many spheres of action—organizing sup-
porters, giving speeches, making contacts with influential people, writing letters,
and composing persuasive documents. Although actions in each of these areas
results from the coordination of skills from more “basic” domains, when dealing
with higher-order skills, it is not ordinarily practical or useful to analyze such
skills in terms of basic elements or domains. When attempting to understand the
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 337
domains of functioning relevant to the everyday operation of a social activist or
lobbyist, each of these broad categories of activity could be viewed as an emer-
gent higher-order domain. In this way, when understanding the nature of complex
psychological activity, that which may be considered to be a higher-order domain
must often be defined by the particularities of the context, task demands, and by
one’s analytic purposes.
From this view, when analyzing the development of any particular skill or
capacity, rather than attempting to identify one or more distinct domains into
which a task falls, it is often preferable to work backwards by performing a
task analysis (Commons, 2006; Commons, Miller, Goodheart, Danaher-Gilpin,
Locicero, and Ross, 2007; Fischer, 1980). A task analysis provides a specification
of the skill elements (i.e., the structure of what a person must do) that must be
brought together in performing any given task. In performing a task analysis,
one breaks a task into its basic elements and relations. In so doing, one “works
backwards” in order to identify the particular skill elements that task performance
requires, regardless of what domains these elements ultimately derive. Over time,
one can trace the pathways through which particular skills emerge and develop as
products of a person’s ongoing attempts to adapt to local tasks, demands, contexts,
and social goals.
Viewed in this way, it becomes clear that development takes place in a multidi-
rectional web of pathways (Fischer and Bidell, 2006) rather than a unidirectional
ladder—a metaphor depicted in Figure 1. Developing skills do not move in a fixed
order of steps in a single direction, but they develop in multiple directions along
multiple strands that weave in and out of each other in ontogenesis, the develop-
mental history of the person (or other organism). The developmental web portrays
variability in developing skills within individuals, not only between them. For de-
velopment in an individual person, different strands represent divergent pathways
in the development of skills for different tasks or conceptual domains. For exam-
ple, the development of addition and subtraction skills might occupy one strand,
skills for producing stories another, and skills for reading words still another. As
such, the developmental web provides a metaphor for understanding how different
skills develop through diverging and converging pathways toward or away from
different endpoints.
THE COORDINATION OF DOMAIN KNOWLEDGE IN ADULT
DEVELOPMENT: A POSTFORMAL CASE ILLUSTRATION
These principles will be illustrated with a case analysis of “Dr. K,” a highly
accomplished adult currently serving as the provost of a small Catholic col-
lege. In what follows, I will examine the ways in which (a) “Dr. K’s” aca-
demic leadership skills emerge and operate as a high-level inter-coordination
of multiple conceptual and behavioral domains; (b) the college capitalized on
his particular skill sets by reorganizing the position of Provost; and (c) how Dr.
K’s talents functioned to support the college’s issues at a particular time in its
history and development. In so doing, I will argue that the domains of knowl-
edge that are required to fulfill the post of Provost are relationally rather than
338 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
Figure 1. Development as a constructive web.
individually defined. That is, at very high levels of functioning, what constitutes
a “domain” of thinking, feeling, or acting is dependent not only on the individ-
ual actor, but on the relation of the individual to the needs of the sociocultural
context.
“Dr. K,” a 59-year-old male, began his career as a priest in a Catholic order. After
several years, Dr. K left the priesthood, eventually married, and had two children.
Dr. K received a doctorate in ministry (D.Min.) and taught classes in the Depart-
ment of Religious Studies at the college at which he is currently provost. Thereafter,
Dr. K returned to graduate school and obtained a second doctorate (Ph.D.), this
time in counseling psychology. Prior to being appointed provost, Dr. K published
a well acclaimed book and a series of scholarly articles. He was appointed the vice
president of “Spiritual Legacy and Mission Advancement”1—a position designed
to forge links between the school’s spiritual heritage and its academic mission. Af-
ter serving in that post successfully for several years, Dr. K was appointed provost
of the college. Dr. K’s appointment as provost of the college was predicated on a
variety of considerations; however, his exceptional interpersonal skills and capac-
ity to “bring people together” were paramount. This was judged necessary because
of the historically tumultuous relations between faculty and administration at the
college.
I interviewed Dr. K about his sense of the qualities that he brings to his office.
In so doing, I indicated that “I am interested in the structure and content of your
‘personal job description’—especially with reference to addressing the college’s
most pressing needs. I’m not so much interested in the ‘formal’ job description,
but instead your personal sense of the different areas of responsibility that you
have and how they are related.” I also asked Dr. K to describe areas in which he felt
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 339
Figure 2. The hierarchic coordination of domain-specific skills: The structure of Dr.
K’s representation of academic leadership.
he had great expertise as well as areas in which he felt he exhibited weaknesses.
Finally, at the end of the interview, I asked Dr. K to describe a principle that would
“tie all of this together.
As expected, Dr. K’s “personal job description” operates at an extremely high
level of complexity. In terms of the Model of Hierarchical Complexity and Fis-
cher’s (1980; Fischer and Bidell, 2006) skill theory, his articulation of his duties
functions at the respective levels of Metasystematic stage 12, or abstract prin-
ciples, which is the highest level in Fischer’s developmental system. Figure 2
provides a representation of the structure of Dr. K’s academic leadership skills.
As indicated there, Dr. K described his personal commitments in terms of a series
of interrelated abstract systems, each of which is related to his singular guiding
principle. Dr. K’s conception of his duties represents an integration of multi-
ple higher-order domains of cognitive, affective and behavioral functioning. Dr.
K identified six major themes—each of which can be considered a domain of
thought and action—organized in terms of a core principle. Dr. K’s articulates his
core principle in terms of the maxim “through knowledge to wisdom,” which, to
Dr. K, means:
through knowledge to wisdom . . . you have to get the knowledge, the information,
the data . . . listen, listen, listen . . . you have to learn constantly . . . so [that]
340 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
you can . . . decide. Maybe they are big decisions, little ones, or just directions
. . . based on the knowledge that you have gained. . . . The many ways as you
can integrate [knowledge] and exercise will . . . that is what I mean by wisdom.
. . . [Wise] decisions . . . have to always be with respect to the dignity of each
person and for the common good; or for the good of the common enterprise.
At another point in the interview, Dr. K elaborated on the ways in which “respect
for individuals” is informed by the need for deep empathy and active listening.
“The virtue of empathy is really really important for me. [Empathy means that we]
don’t just tolerate the other, but actively enter into the world of their frustrations
. . . it’s a big help.”
This principle weaves its way through the various domains of operation in
which Dr. K participates. The six basic domains of thinking include the need to (a)
actualize a vision for the college that will attract high quality students and faculty.
Toward this end, he described the need to (b) develop an innovative curriculum;
in order to develop such curriculum, there is a need to (c) reallocate faculty work-
load in order to make faculty scholarship and curriculum development possible.
Curriculum and faculty workload revision requires (d) extensive assessment of
existing programs, which requires effective interpersonal interaction and collabo-
ration between the provost and the faculty on the one hand, as well as with (e) the
admissions officer who requires innovative programs to market the college, and
(f) the president of the college whose imprimatur is necessary to effect change in
the existing system.
Each of these six themes articulated by Dr. K requires different yet overlapping
systems of skill and knowledge. It is not possible, on the basis of a reflective
interview, to assess the degree of development of Dr. K’s skills in each domain. For
the present purposes, it is important to note Dr. K’s “personal job description” is (a)
embodied by the inter-coordination of multiple particular domains of knowledge
and action, organized in terms of integrative abstract principles. One might suggest
that Dr. K’s effectiveness in his post—like that of any high-level administrator
or manager—relies on (b) the extent to which this particular configuration of
dynamically executed skills fits the unique needs of the institution at this phase in
its history, and (c) the ways in which the skills and proclivities of other key actors
(e.g., the president, faculty leaders) complement or conflict with Dr. K’s initiatives
and actions. For example, when Dr. K was appointed provost, the former position
of academic vice president was divided into two positions—provost and dean of
the college. The provost was charged with the running the everyday academic
affairs of the college; the dean of the college assumed different responsibilities.
This unusual action not only made it possible to render the massive workload of
the former position more manageable, but it also provided flexibility in defining
the duties of each position so that they fit the skills and proclivities of the two
incumbents.
Although it is likely that one could identify seeds of Dr. K’s leadership skills
earlier in his career, like all skills and abilities, Dr. K’s academic leadership skills
developed over a long period of time through the successive inter-coordination
of lower-level skills within different conceptual domains. Figure 3 provides a
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 341
developmental web describing some of the strands that contributed to the devel-
opment of Dr. K’s academic leadership skills. As indicated in Figure 3, Dr. K’s
leadership skills built upon the development and integration of at least four broad
domains of knowledge and action: academics, spirituality, social interaction, and
technical/business skills. Although each of these domains develops along partially
separate pathways, new skills emerge as novel paths split off from existing ones, or
when skills from multiple paths come together in multiple acts of integration. Dr.
K’s “knowledge about academic life” (an important knowledge domain given that
the current president and other vice presidents do not have academic backgrounds)
developed through graduate training, as well as the normal duties of an academic
(i.e., teaching, scholarship, and community service). Dr. K’s interpersonal skills
and commitment to “respect for others” emerged early from his activity in the
priesthood, and continued to develop over his career, including in his capacity
as a teacher, administrator, and counseling psychologist. Dr. K’s academic lead-
ership skills, as exemplified in Figures 2 and 3, result from the coordination of
these multiple paths over time. Nonetheless, Dr. K attests to several domains in
which he feels challenged as provost. These include aspects of his position that
require technical knowledge of a legal or fiscal kind, as well as skills for forging
connections with wealthy individuals and business leaders outside of the college.
These less developed skill domains are represented toward the bottom of Figure 3
in terms of unelaborated “legal” and “corporate” pathways.
POSTFORMAL THINKING AND THE FUTURE OF LEADERSHIP IN
HIGHER EDUCATION: COORDINATING DIVERSE DOMAINS
The concept of domain is a relational and dynamic concept. Any domain of
knowledge or action is defined with reference to an organism’s need to adapt
to shifting ecological demands. In modern human cultures, such demands shift
continuously with changes in technology, the global economy, demographics, and
a variety of other socioeconomic processes. In the past half-century, such changes
have spawned the production of novel domains of knowledge and action (Collis,
2001). The proliferation of novel domains poses special challenges for individuals
and groups in positions of leadership—especially leaders in the field of higher
education. Consider the following statement by the president of Pennsylvania
State University:
Institutions of higher learning, like business and government, cannot be com-
placent about the future. There was a time when universities were content to
adopt a “Field of Dreams” approach—if we build it, they will come. For most of
our history, we were omniscient elders who told our students what they needed,
when they could get it, and what they would pay for it. But those days are rapidly
becoming a distant memory. Changes in technology, demographics, competition,
and for public institutions, legislative expectations, are all coming together to
alter the way we operate. (Spanier, 2000, p. 19)
342 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
Figure 3. Pathways in the development of Dr. K’s academic leadership skills.
As a result of cultural shifts like those identified by Dr. Spanier, new domains
of knowledge and action have emerged. To adapt to these changes, educational
leaders face the tasks of recognizing shifts in sociocultural demands when they
occur; identifying novel domains of expertise that have evolved to meet those
demands, and, perhaps most importantly, finding ways of coordinating novel
domains of knowledge into the integrated fabric of the college or university.
Such acts of coordination become increasingly difficult in an age of increasing
specialization, fragmentation, and decentralization of intellectual and economic
activity like our own. Integrative multiple domains of knowledge in the service
of a common goal requires leadership skills that operate at postformal levels of
development.
In these concluding remarks, I will illustrate the ways in which societal shifts
have spawned the production of novel domains of knowledge and action in the
field of higher education as well as the need to coordinate across novel domains
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 343
of knowledge and expertise in order to ensure the continued productivity of the
academy. Toward this end, I will touch briefly upon three basic challenges that face
higher education. These include changes in shifting demographics, the revolution
of information technology, and the explosion of knowledge.
First, educational demographics have changed significantly in the past half
century. Whereas 25 percent of all high school graduates went on to college at the
midpoint of the 20th century, today in the United States, 75 percent of graduates
attend some form of higher education within two years of graduation (Caboni and
Adisu, 2004). This shift has produced unprecedented diversity in higher education;
what was once seen as the purview of the well-to-do has now become a virtual
necessity for entry into the workforce. Such diversity has placed novel demands
on higher education that have motivated novel domains of expertise. The rising
number of students has brought with it a heightened need for remedial education
(Caboni and Adisu, 2004). Most schools have racial and ethnic diversity offices
to address the needs of minority students (Gose, 2006). Social and legislative
changes have mandated resources for students with learning disabilities and other
special needs (Hadley, 2007). Each of these needs has engendered a new domain of
knowledge and expertise within the educational environment. While most colleges
and universities have established programs built around these domains of action,
it has proved more difficult to coordinate these novel programs into the cultural
fabric of the educational institution.
Second, the centrality of information technology in cultural and economic life
has spawned an explosion of academic departments needed to meet increasing
demands for workers trained in the many facets of information technology (La-
genberg and Spicer, 2001; Van Ginkel, 2003). Information technology, including
the Internet, continues to motivate dramatic changes in the delivery of education
(Maughan, 2001; Williams, 2007). Information technology evolves faster than the
academy’s capacity to master effective ways to use it. With the Internet, we have
witnessed a surge in distance learning, online classes, and computer-mediated
instructed within classrooms (Guri-Rosenblit, 2005). However, as with any tech-
nology, information technology is a tool; as a tool, its merits are to be found in
the ways in which it is used. As with many technologies, one runs the risk that
the tool may come to control the user. While Web-based technologies can support
teaching and learning in many ways—convenience; ease of access to multimedia
information; use of education-related software, and so on—more efficient delivery
of information is not the same as genuine learning. Some modes of learning are
well suited for information technology; others may continue to require face-to-
face interaction and the formation of student—teacher relationships (Reinsmith,
2006). The economic incentives for use of Web-based technologies are legion;
the challenge to educational leaders is to find novel ways to integrate the various
domains of information technology with the best of traditional education, as well
as with the core values that sustain higher education (Barnett, 2000).
A final issue concerns the explosion of knowledge (Adair and Vohra, 2003;
Barnett, 2000). With the passing of each decade, the quantity of knowledge grows
exponentially. Academic disciplines and areas of expertise are becoming increas-
ingly specialized over time. In this way, in addition to the production of novel
344 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
disciplines (e.g., such as information technology), novel academic domains are
emerging through a process of increasing specialization, resulting in increasingly
narrow domains of expertise. From an educational standpoint, what intellectual
values should prevail when confronting the explosion of knowledge (Rhinesmith,
2006)? Is more knowledge better knowledge? Or should colleges and universities
seek ways to empower students with conceptual tools for organizing relations
among fields and subfields of knowledge (Barnett, 2000; Boulton and Panizzon,
1998)? How should specialization of knowledge affect disciplinary structures? As
academics become increasingly specialized, what should be the fate of traditional
liberal arts curricula?
The task of leading an educational institution through cultural shifts is no easy
matter. Educational leadership—like leadership in any complex institution—will
require the capacity to coordinate multiple domains of knowledge and expertise
within a shifting sociocultural-economic climate. In the present context, the goal
of inter-coordination of emergent domain-specific activity functions as a key
leadership principle (Blackmore and Blackwell, 2006; Blaisdell, 1993; Lucas,
2007). Educational leadership will require more than capacity simply to juggle
competing interests; genuine leadership will require teams of postformal leaders
working together to inter-coordinate multiple domains of knowledge for purposes
of pursuing common educational goals.
NOTE
1. This title is an alias that preserves the original meaning of the position in question.
REFERENCES
Adair, J. G., and Vohra, N. 2003. The explosion of knowledge, references, and citations:
Psychology’s unique response to a crisis. American Psychologist 58(1): 15–23.
Barnett, R. 2000. University knowledge in an age of supercomplexity. Higher Education
40(4): 409–422.
Bekoff, M., Allen, C., and Burghardt, G. M. Eds. 2002. The cognitive animal: Empirical
and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Blackmore, P. L., and Blackwell, R. 2006. Strategic leadership in academic development.
Studies in Higher Education 31(3): 373–387.
Blaisdell, M. L. 1993. Academic integration: Going beyond disciplinary boundaries. New
Directions for Teaching and Learning 54: 57–69.
Boulton, A., and Panizzon, D. 1998. The knowledge explosion in science education: Bal-
ancing practical and theoretical knowledge. Journal of Research in Science Teaching
35(5): 475–481.
Caboni, T. C., and Adisu, M. P. J. E. 2004. A nation at risk after 20 years: Continuing
implications for higher education.Peabody Journal of Education 79(1): 164–176.
Case, R. 1992. The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s
thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
———, Okamoto, Y., Griffin, S., McKeough, A., Bleiker, C., Henderson, B., and Mara,
K. 1996. The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s
thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, serial 246,
61(1–2).
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 345
Collis, D. 2001. When industries change: The future of higher education. Continuing Higher
Education Review 65: 7–24.
Commons, M. L. 2006. Measuring an approximate g in animals and people. Integral
Review: A Transdisciplinary and Transcultural Journal for New Thought, Research,
and Praxis 3: 82–99. Online at http://integral-review.org.
Commons, M. L., Miller, P. M., Goodheart, E. A., Danaher-Gilpin, D., Locicero, A. and
Ross, S. N. 2007. Applying the model of hierarchical complexity. Unpublished manual,
Dare Institute. Available from commons@tiac.net.
———,Trudeau, E. J., Stein, S. A., Richards, F. A. and Krause, S. R. 1998. The existence of
developmental stages as shown by the hierarchical complexity of tasks. Developmental
Review 18: 237–278.
Dawson, T. 2004. Assessing intellectual development: Three approaches, one sequence.
Journal of Adult Development 11: 71–85.
———, and Gabrielian, S. 2003. Developing conceptions of authority and contract across
the life-span: Two perspectives. Developmental Review 23: 162–218.
Dawson, T., Xie, Y., and Wilson, M. 2003. Domain-general and domain-specific devel-
opmental assessments: Do they measure the same thing? Cognitive Development 18:
61–78.
Demetriou, A., and Efklides, A. 1994. Structure, development, and dynamics of mind: A
meta-Piagetian theory. In Mind, intelligence, and reasoning: Structure and develop-
ment, Eds. Demetriou, A., and Efklides, A., 75–109. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Guri-Rosenblit, S. 2005. Distance education and “e-learning”: Not the same thing. Higher
Education 49(4): 467–493.
Fischer, K. W. 1980. A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of
hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review 87: 477–531.
———, and Bidell, T. R. 2006. Dynamic development of action, thought, and emotion.
In Theoretical models of human development. Handbook of child psychology,Eds.
Damon, W., and Lerner, R. M., 313–399. New York: Wiley.
Fischer, K. W., Bullock, D., Rotenberg, E. J., and Raya, P. 1993. The dynamics of com-
petence: How context contributes directly to skill. In Development in context: Acting
and thinking in specific environments, Eds. Wozniak, R., and Fischer, K. W., 93–117.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Fowler, J. 1981. Stages of faith. San Francisco: Harper and Rowe.
Gose, B. 2006. The rise of the chief diversity officer.Chronicle of Higher Education 53(6):
55.
Griffin, S. 2005. Fostering the development of whole-number sense: Teaching mathematics
in the primary grades. In How students learn: History, mathematics and science in
the classroom, Eds. Donovan, M. S., and Bransford, J. D., 257–308. Washington, DC:
The National Academies Press.
Hadley, W. M. 2007. The necessity of academic accommodations for first-year
college students with learning disabilities. Journal of College Admission 195:
9–13.
Halford, G. 1993. Children’s understanding: The development of mental models. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Halford, G. 1999. The development of intelligence includes the capacity to processing
relations of greater complexity. In The development of intelligence Ed Sigel, I., 93–
213. Hove, England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.
Kim, J. M., and Turiel, E. 1996. Korean children’s concepts of adult and peer authority.
Social Development 5: 310–329.
346 MICHAEL F. MASCOLO
Kitchener, K. S., King, P. M., and De Luca, S. 2005. The development of reflective judgment
in adulthood. In Handbook of adult development and learning, Ed. Hoare, C., 73–98.
New York: Wiley.
Kohlberg, L. 1963. The development of children’s orientations toward a moral order 1:
Sequence in the development of moral thought.Vita Humana 6: 11–33.
———. 1984. The psychology of moral development. New York: Harper and Row.
Lagenberg, D. N. and Spicer, D. S. 2001. The Modern Campus. New Directions for Higher
Education, 115: 3–15.
Lamborn, S. D., Fischer, K. W., and Pipp, S. 1994. Constructive criticism and social lies: A
developmental sequence for understanding honesty and kindness in social interactions.
Developmental Psychology 30: 495–508.
Lucas, L. 2007. Research and teaching work within university education departments:
Fragmentation or integration? Journal of Further and Higher Education 31(1): 17–
29.
Laupa, M. and Elliot Turiel, E. 1986. Children’s Conceptions of Adult arid Peer Authority.
Child Development 57: 405–412.
Mascolo, M. F., and Fischer, K. W. 1998. The development of self through the coordination
of component systems. In Self-awareness: Its nature and development, Eds. Ferrari,
M., and Sternberg, R., 332–384. New York: Guilford.
———, and Fischer, K. W. 2005. Constructivist theories. In Cambridge encyclopedia of
child development, Eds. Hopkins, B., Barre, R. G., Michel, G. F., and Rochat, P.,
49–63. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
———, Fischer, K. W., and Li, J. 2003. Dynamic development of component systems of
emotions: Pride, shame, and guilt in China and the United States. In Handbook of
affective science, Eds. Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K., and Goldsmith, H. H., 295–408.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Maughan, G. R. 2001. Communication and information systems infrastructure. New Direc-
tions in Higher Education 115: 17–28.
McKeough, A., and Genereux, R. 2003. Transformation in narrative thought during ado-
lescence. The structure and content of story compositions. Journal of Educational
Psychology 95: 537–552.
Perry, W. G. 1970. Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years.New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Piaget, J. 1970. Structuralism. Trans. C. Maschler. New York: Basic Books.
———. 1971. The theory of stages in cognitive development. In Measurement and Piaget,
Eds. Green, D. R., Ford, M. P., and Flamer, G. B., 1–11. New York: McGraw-Hill.
———. 1983. Piaget’s theory. In History, theory, and methods, Ed. W. Kessen, 103–126.
New York: Wiley.
Reinsmith, W. A. 2006. The forest, not the tree(s): The plight of the generalist. Liberal
Education 92(1): 56–60.
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. S., and Taylor, T. J. 2001. Apes, language, and the
human mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Spanier, G., 2000. Five challenges facing American education. Executive Speeches 14:
19–24.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., and Call, J. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The
origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5): 675–735.
Turiel, E. 1983. The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention.Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
———. 2002. The culture of morality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
THE CONCEPT OF DOMAIN 347
———, and Wainryb, C. 1998. Concepts of freedoms and rights in a traditional hierarchi-
cally organized society. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 16: 375–395.
Van Ginkel, H. 2003. The university of the twenty-first century: From blueprint to reality.
Higher Education in Europe 28(1): 83–86.
Wainryb, C. 1995. Reasoning about social conflicts in different cultures. Druze and Jewish
children in Israel. Child Development 66: 390–401.
———, and Turiel, E. 1994. Dominance, subordination, and concepts of personal entitle-
ments in cutural contexts. Child Development 65: 1701–1722.
Williams, P. 2007. Valid knowledge: The economy and the academy. Higher Education
54(4): 511–523.
... Several factors influenced my choice of a method to use for developing a developmentally informed pre-engagement diagnosis instrument. First, there is growing consensus between researchers (Commons, Richard, & Kruse, 1998;Dawson, 2003;Fischer & Bidell, 2006;Mascolo, 2008;Mascolo & Fischer, 2010;Wolfsont, Ross, Miller, Commons, & Chernoff, 2008) that development is best characterized as development across multiple domains of activity. Second, it appears that development asynchronously occurs across these various domains. ...
... While used in the psychological literature, there is little consensus as to the meaning of the term (Mascolo, 2008;Wolfsont et al., 2008). The following orientation is useful to situate this portion of my results: ...
... It is thus necessary to postulate the concept of domain to refer to the particular conceptual, behavioral or affective area within which activity operates. (Mascolo, 2008, p. 330) For this research, I adopted Mascolo's (2008) definition of a domain which defines a domain as "the particular conceptual, behavioral, or affective area within which skilled activity operates" (p. 330). ...
Research
This research addressed the problem that currently there is no adult developmentally informed pre-engagement diagnosis instrument for change efforts available. Jaques’s (1996, 2002; Jaques & Clement, 1994) Requisite Organization theory was used as design approach for the creation of such a tool. This study aimed to describe what is involved in creating an instrument for assessing the individual stage of performance on select change effort tasks and for designing a set of guidelines to support consultants and change agents using the diagnostic results. Based on an action learning approach, an attempt was made to answer a set of research questions that focused on developing a developmentally informed pre-engagement diagnosis for change efforts and relevant use guidelines. To answer these questions, first, a task analysis was conducted to identify essential task domains. Next, it was necessary to learn how to score complexity based on the Model of Hierarchical Complexity (MHC). Third, it was essential to investigate what is involved in writing context specific vignettes. Finally, it was necessary to consider how RO theory could be used to develop a set of preliminary use guidelines. Detailed descriptions are provided for each step of the process used for creating a partial prototype of a developmentally informed pre-engagement diagnosis instrument and a set of preliminary use guidelines. Included are descriptions of the task analysis process, the domain setting process, the vignette, the hierarchical task item development process, and the use guidelines development process. Difficulties encountered and insights gained from each of these process steps are presented. Finally, process specific suggestions are provided for researchers interested in forwarding this research agenda. This research is significant in that it is the first attempt to develop an instrument for evaluating an individual’s stage of performance for essential task domains to match in-house resources to change efforts. Further, this study illustrates the complexity and resources required to develop a developmentally informed instrument based on the MHC. Finally, the finding put forth in this study will support further research aimed at understanding the influence of task domain stage of performance on change efforts.
... One of the most well-known and used general models for assessing the complexity in learning outcomes is the Structure of Observed Learning Outcomes (SOLO) taxonomy that was first introduced in The SOLO Taxonomystructure of the observed learning outcome, a book by Biggs and Collis (1982). The word 'structure' in the title signifies that it is not the knowledge content but the knowledge structure that is being assessed, regardless of subject matter or domain (Commons 2008a). 'Learning Outcome' means the learning result, i.e. the student output. ...
... Examples of theories of the intellectual development of students are Baxter Magolda (2000), Belenky et al. (1986), King and Kitchener (1994), and Perry (1970). The theories and models advanced in this field were for a long time limited to their respective domains, but a series of validation studies has shown that the hierarchical complexity scoring system assesses a unidimensional developmental trait (Commons 2008a;Dawson 2002Dawson , 2003Dawson , 2004Dawson, Xie, and Wilson 2003), which enables analysis of complexity in any information and domain. Currently, there are three theories/models that make this claim: the Fischer skill theory (Fischer and Bidell 2006), the Lectical Assessment System (Dawson and Stein 2008) and MHC. ...
Article
An important aspect of higher education is to educate students who can manage complex relationships and solve complex problems. Teachers need to be able to evaluate course content with regard to complexity, as well as evaluate students’ ability to assimilate complex content and express it in the form of a learning outcome. One model for evaluating complexity is the Structure of Observed Learning Outcomes (SOLO) taxonomy. The aim of this analysis is to address the limitations of the SOLO taxonomy in detecting the more subtle differences of the learning outcomes and to clarify the concept of learning modes. This is done by analysing the SOLO taxonomy by means of the model of hierarchical complexity (MHC). The two models are compared by examining their respective theoretical background, the definitions and descriptions of the stages of each model, as well as through evaluating examples illustrating the SOLO levels using MHC. The two models can be viewed as compatible, with MHC also being able to put the SOLO taxonomy in an adult development context, thereby emphasising the importance of developing the students’ access to complex thinking.
... The second contribution is illustration of a dimension of increasing complexity and the expanding and evolving notion of leadership development, which becomes even more apparent when the results are related to identity development theories and constructivist adult development theories. In the adult development field, the notion of domain means the area or topic that the individual is understanding (Mascolo, 2008). The six ways of understanding can be seen as the first outline of a new developmental trajectory in the leadership development domain. ...
Article
Full-text available
Leadership development is a multifaceted phenomenon with a multitude of definitions and meanings requiring closer exploration. The aim of this study was to identify and investigate qualitatively different ways of understanding leadership development and categorize them from a complexity perspective. We conducted 21 semi-structured interviews with professionals and managers. Analysis using a phenomenographic approach revealed six categories and different ways of understanding leadership development: (1) one’s own development, (2) fulfilling a leadership role, (3) personal development, (4) leader and organizational development, (5) collective leadership development, and (6) human development. The categories were arranged hierarchically according to increasing complexity. Our contribution recognizes more nuanced interpretations than previously identified and highlights underlying structures of complexity. The results help to empirically ground and elaborate current theories and distinctions within the field of leadership development research where similar patterns can be observed. They may assist researchers in making both their own and other’s assumptions on leadership development explicit, as well as informing the practice of tailoring leadership development activities to better match individuals and organizational contexts.
... In addition to the introduction of support from the MHC to the SST context, another useful concept in relation to the MHC is domain, which refers to the subject area in which tasks are being defined, such as logicalmathematical, moral reasoning, or social perspective taking (Mascolo, 2008). A consequence of the MHC only defining content-free OHC of the structure is that an individual is not necessarily at a certain developmental stage, according to the Piagetian concept of structures d=ensemble [structure of the whole]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Organizations can be seen as social systems with hierarchical structures and roles at different levels of complexity with correspondingly different complexity of tasks. This article applies the perspectives of two theories from the field of adult development, namely, the model of hierarchical complexity (MHC) and ego development theory (EDT) to analyze stratified systems theory (SST). Although the theories are not regarded as strictly comparable and commensurable on account of differences in basic assumptions and methods of the theories, the analysis leads to the conclusion that descriptions of role complexity and individual capabilities in SST, to some extent, correspond to descriptions of developmental levels according to the MHC and EDT. Both comparisons support the notion that task and leadership complexity increases with organizational level, and thereby demonstrates support for the existence of qualitatively different levels of leadership. However, based on the methodological choices of the study, it is beyond the scope of the article to validate the key concepts, constructs in SST, as well as provide support or nonsupport for the proposed value of application in practice. Furthermore, we point out the lack of a more thorough analysis and comparison between the theories built on rich empirical material. Nevertheless, we conclude that the MHC, EDT and SST are fruitful lenses that can further the understanding of organizations as social systems with hierarchical structures.
... MHC kan således vara ett redskap för att förstå hur utveckling av kunskaper och förmågor sker över tid. Denna utveckling är dock inte någon linjär och enkel process utan snarare dynamisk och icke-linjär, och varierar väsentligt mellan personer i samma ålder och även inom en och samma person beroende på livsdomän och uppgift (Commons, 2008a; Fischer & Pruyne, 2003). Varje steg innebär en förbättrad kapacitet att se relationer och göra samband mellan saker och idéer. ...
Article
Full-text available
Vi utbildar studenter för att de ska ha möjlighet att lösa mycket komplexa samhällsproblem,men hur vet vi att de har utvecklat de former av tänkande och handlande som krävs? Forskning visar att vuxna tänker, talar och handlar utifrån olika nivåer av komplexitet, och att utbildning i hög grad påverkar människors förmågor. Model of Hierarchical Complexity(MHC) är en teori som beskriver hur komplext information sätts samman och hur komplext personer resonerar i en fråga, vanligtvis på någon av nivåerna konkret, abstrakt, formellt, systematiskt eller metasystematisk. Syftet med denna artikel är att introducera MHC och visa på dess relevans som verktyg inom högre utbildning. Med hjälp av teorin är det möjligt att analysera både hur komplex en uppgift är och hur studenter klarar av att lösa den, vilket speglar förståelse inom ett ämne. Med modellen som mått på komplexitet tydliggörs svårighetsgraden i det som ska läras och på vilken nivå studenterna klarar att ta till sig kunskapsinnehållet. Avslutningsvis diskuteras hur studenter kan stödjas att utvecklasina förmågor till komplext resonerande och därmed skapa kvalitet i både lärande och undervisningOne of the aims of higher education is to teach students to solve complex problems, but what is the complexity of problems and the reasoning of students? The Model of Hierarchical Complexity (MHC) is a theory applicable to all domains in which information is organized and accounts for increases in behavioral complexity which includes cognitive or reasoning complexity. The paper is a theoretical introduction to MHC as a tool for teaching in higher education. The model clarifies and shows the gap between the complexity in the subject and the students understanding of the same subject. We also discuss how to support the development of more complex reasoning in students.
... Structures of thought and action and their level of developmental complexity are represented using the conventions of dynamic skill theory (Fischer, 1980;Fischer & Bidell, 2006). Dynamic skill theory (Fischer & Bidell, 2006;Mascolo & Fischer, 2010) identifies a set of 13 different hierarchical levels of skill as they are deployed in particular conceptual domains and social contexts (Mascolo, 2008). Skills develop through four broad tiers (reflexes, sensorimotor acts, representations, and abstractions), each of which is composed of four levels (single sets, mappings, systems, and systems of systems) and an indefinite number of intermediate steps. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent decades, the developmental sciences have undergone a relational turn. Epigenetic (Gottlieb & Lickliter, 2007), embodied (Thompson, 2007), relational (Lerner & Overton, 2008) and systems (Kelso, 2003) approaches are transforming the ways in which we think about the nature and origins of psychological structures. At their most basic level, relational and systems approaches analyze the developmental origins of order and variability not in terms of sets of separable causal forces but instead in analyses of relations between causal systems. From this view, genes and environment, biology and culture, cognition and emotion, self and other, and so forth are inseparable as causal processes in the development of action and experience. Drawing on these principles, this paper contains an outline of an embodied coactive systems framework for understanding how individual psychological structures develop as a product of socially distributed coactions that occur among elements of the extended person-environment system. Based on these principles, a system for the Developmental Analysis of Joint Action is described. This system provides a set of conceptual and empirical tools for making precise assessments of dynamic structure of jointly constructed patterns of thinking, feeling and acting. By tracking developmental changes in joint action, the system allows researchers to track the origins of higher order psychological structures through particular sequences of coconstructive activity. The holistic analytic system is illustrated through microdevelopmental analyses of (1) the joint construction of shoe-tying skill between a 5-year-old boy and a caregiver, and (2) socioemotional organization developing representations of self in a young adult over the course of a single session of psychotherapy.
Book
Die vorliegende Studie enthält eine umfassende Aufarbeitung der kognitionspsychologischen Aspekte von Sprachbewusstheit, einen Vorschlag zur Neukonzeptualisierung derselben, eine exemplarische entwicklungspsychologische Einordnung der Schwierigkeit bzw. Abstraktheit von Sprachbetrachtungsinhalten sowie eine Interventionsstudie, die über die das/dass-Schreibung und die Kommasetzung zwischen (Teil-)Sätzen operationalisiert wurde. Die quasi-experimentelle Interventionsstudie mit Prä-Post-Follow-up-Messung (Teilung der Gruppen nach Alter 12/16 und Treatment metakognitiv/nicht metakognitiv) an 167 Schüler*innen bzw. acht Gymnasialklassen (Intervention jeweils sechs Schulstunden) wurde mit einem multi-level model mit random intercept ausgewertet. Die Ergebnisse deuten darauf hin, dass (1) weitgehend situationsentbundene und handlungsentlastete Sprachbetrachtung bei konsequent zielorientierter Unterrichtsplanung (komplexitätsreduzierte, Heuristik-dominierte, terminologiearme Vermittlung) mit starkem bis mittlerem Effekt auf Performanzebene wirksam ist und dass (2) sogar eine solche vereinfachende Form der Sprachbetrachtung die höchste Effizienz erst auf Sekundarstufe II erreicht (Wirksamkeit auch auf Sekundarstufe I gegeben). Das zusätzlich metakognitiv ausgerichtete Treatment (3) erbrachte keine größeren Lernerfolge. Die Arbeit schließt mit Implikationen für Schulcurricula und die schulische Praxis.
Thesis
Full-text available
This dissertation is concerned with a systematic organization of the epistemological dimension of human knowledge in terms of viewpoints and methods. In particular, it will be explored to what extent the well-known organizing principle of integrative levels that presents a developmental hierarchy of complexity and integration can be applied for a basic classification of viewpoints or epistemic outlooks. The central thesis pursued in this investigation is that an adequate analysis of such epistemic contexts requires tools that allow to compare and evaluate divergent or even conflicting frames of reference according to context-transcending standards and criteria. This task demands a theoretical and methodological foundation that avoids the limitation of radical contextualism and its inherent threat of a fragmentation of knowledge due to the alleged incommensurability of the underlying frames of reference. Based on Jürgen Habermas’s Theory of Communicative Action and his methodology of hermeneutic reconstructionism, it will be argued that epistemic pluralism does not necessarily imply epistemic relativism and that a systematic organization of the multiplicity of perspectives can benefit from already existing models of cognitive development as reconstructed in research fields like psychology, social sciences, and humanities. The proposed cognitive-developmental approach to knowledge organization aims to contribute to a multi-perspective knowledge organization by offering both analytical tools for cross-cultural comparisons of knowledge organization systems (e.g., Seven Epitomes and Dewey Decimal Classification) and organizing principles for context representation that help to improve the expressiveness of existing documentary languages (e.g., Integrative Levels Classification). Additionally, the appendix includes an extensive compilation of conceptions and models of Integrative Levels of Knowing from a broad multidisciplinary field.
Chapter
Commons (2008a) has developed the most comprehensive model of postformal thought in the Neo-Piagetian literature. In 2008, he presented his model with colleagues in a special issue of the journal World Futures. The present chapter is largely based on the articles in the issue. After reviewing each article, I provide commentary. Commons (2008a) model involves four postformal stages, and mine consists of one stage with five substages. The major difference in the models is that Commons appears to have missed a step involving hierarchization after the first step in his series. In the following, I show how the descriptions that Commons provides for his four postformal steps and the examples given to elaborate them suggest that a hierarchical step is needed in his model and that it fits very well into his series. Moreover, I present several models that are comparable to those of Commons and colleagues. However, these models avoid some of the issues deriving from the Commons’ model that I point out in the comparison of the two models. In particular, I describe a model of management styles derived from the present model.
Chapter
This chapter continues to compare the Neo-Piagetian theories of Case and Fischer, but beyond cognitive/socioaffective parallels. I concentrate especially on language development, which leads to a section on narrative development. As with the prior chapters, in reviewing their approaches, I point out where the present model either handles the examples better or deals with their gaps and inconsistencies. The chapter includes a brief discussion of transition mechanisms and then moves on to cortical organization in relation to the steps in cognitive development in their models. In other chapters, of the book, I present my perspective on the latter topic.
Article
Full-text available
In this Monograph, we present a new theory of children's conceptual development and the empirical research on which that theory is based. The main construct in the theory is the notion of a central conceptual structure. These structures are denned as networks of semantic nodes and relations that represent children's core knowledge in a domain and that can be applied to the full range of tasks that the domain entails. Major transformations are hypothesized to take place in these structures as children enter each new stage of their development. Once formed, the new structures are hypothesized to exert a powerful influence on all subsequent knowledge acquisition. The process by which they exert this effect is believed to be a dynamic one, in which general conceptual insights and more specific task understandings become reciprocally coupled, each exerting a bootstrapping effect on the other. In the first chapter, the general theoretical framework that underlies this conception is spelled out in broad strokes and compared to other contemporary views of conceptual development. In subsequent chapters, more detailed models of children's central conceptual structures are presented for three different domains: number, space, and social interaction. These models are then tested using a mixture of new and previously designed cognitive tasks, which are administered to children from four different age groups (4, 6, 8, and 10 years), three different social classes (high, medium, and low), and four different countries (the United States, Canada, Japan, and China). The results of a 6-year program of instructional research are also summarized and used to clarify and refine the theory. Analytic tools that are employed include computer simulation, item analysis, and linked growth curve analysis. Statistical techniques include latent structure analysis, factor analysis, and Guttman scaling. Problems with previous versions of the theory and implications of the present version are discussed in the final chapter.
Article
Full-text available
The nature of academic work is changing rapidly, with moves towards professionalisation taking place against a background of fragmentation. Indeed, some aspects of professionalisation may have a fragmenting effect. It is suggested that there remains considerable value in the idea of an integrated faculty role. Noting that leaders in staff development face similar pressures to professionalise, the writers consider what expertise is required for the leadership in academic development role, and how role holders and those aspiring to the role may best develop their professional capabilities. They argue for an integrated conception of academic development, and a correspondingly integrated view of the developer’s professional identity and role. It is suggested that this will put leaders in academic development into a position that is more congruent with faculty self‐perceptions, and enable them to support those in faculty roles more effectively.
Article
To provide an assessment of the dynamics of development of complex sociomoral concepts, a 10-step scale for assessing development of understanding relations between honesty and kindness was administered under multiple assessment conditions to 113 youths who were 9–20 yrs old. The sequence proved to be both scalable and reliable, even while level of understanding varied greatly as a function of contextual support and practice. Development moved through 3 age periods: With high support and practice, youths aged 9–22 yrs demonstrated abstract concepts of honesty and kindness, youths aged 13–25 yrs demonstrated simple abstract relations, and youths aged 16–20 yrs demonstrated complex abstract relations. Independent of age and verbal intelligence, understanding related to prosocial problem solving but not to nonsocial problem solving.
Chapter
This chapter provides an overview of a meta-Piagetian theory. The theory aspires to accommodate the architecture and development of human intelligence, mind, and reasoning. For reasons of convenience, from this point onwards, the chapter uses the term “mind” to refer to all of these three aspects of human knowing. As a theory about the architecture and the dynamics of the mind, it involves propositions, assertions, and hypotheses about (a) the structural organization of human knowledge acquisition and problem solving devices and capabilities, (b) the condition of these devices and capabilities at different ages, and (c) the causes and mechanisms which are responsible for their change along with age. The theory has originated in the Piagetian tradition. In addition, testing, in the psychometric tradition is based on the assumption that thought activity is organized along a number of specifiable ability dimensions which can be used to differentiate individuals. In turn, it is assumed that individual differences are signs indicating the boundaries between cognitive structures. Furthermore, the chapter provides the propositions of the theory about the structure of human mind.
Article
A thought-provoking examination of how explanations of social and moral development inform our understandings of morality and culture. A common theme in the latter part of the twentieth century has been to lament the moral state of American society and the decline of morality among youth. A sharp turn toward an extreme form of individualism and a lack of concern for community involvement and civic participation are often blamed for the moral crisis. Turiel challenges these views, drawing on a large body of research from developmental psychology, anthropology, sociology as well as social events, political movements, and journalistic accounts of social and political struggles. Turiel shows that generation after generation has lamented the decline of society and blamed young people. Using historical accounts, he persuasively argues that such characterizations of moral decline entail stereotyping, nostalgia for times past, and a failure to recognize the moral viewpoint of those who challenge traditions.
Article
This article raises some questions about the current policy context of debates on the link between research and teaching in academic work, specifically within university education departments. It draws selectively from a research project which investigated education academics' perceptions and experiences in Scotland and England but focuses exclusively here on the Scottish institutions. Research activity as it is commonly defined and articulated, particularly as part of the selectivity mission driving the Research Assessment Exercise (RAE) in the UK, is increasingly perceived as the preserve of elite research‐intensive universities and has served to fragment and differentiate research and teaching work within university departments. Using a communities of practice model, this article focuses on investigating the possibilities of creating a community of scholarly knowledge‐building practice within these education departments, which can help to develop the research potential of academic staff previously not research‐active as well as the student teachers being taught within these departments. Evidence from the project shows that at least within university education departments there is enormous potential to invigorate and innovate research and teaching work and for this to have beneficial implications for education professionals across the education sector.