ArticlePDF Available

Advancing the clinical science of creativity

Frontiers
Frontiers in Psychology
Authors:
  • McLean Hospital / Harvard Medical School

Abstract

Can the therapeutic benefits of creativity explain its documented association with psychopathology (Andreasen, 1987; Ludwig, 1995)? Past research seems to have devoted most of its attention to another hypothesis in order to explain this relationship: that features of some disorders may be beneficial for creative cognition (especially in the arts)—for example, the racing thoughts, energy, and openness characteristic of hypomania in bipolar disorder (Johnson et al., 2012), or the rumination observed in depression (Verhaeghen et al., 2005). Other explanations, however, should not be ignored or considered mutually exclusive. Creative work may sometimes exacerbate psychopathology. For example, Kaufman and Baer (2002) suggested that poets may be especially susceptible to mental illness because poetry requires emotional expression and introspection, and unlike prose, may not provide adequate opportunities for making meaning out of one's experience. Conversely, and leaving aside third variable explanations (which also deserve further research), we explore the hypothesis that psychopathology may motivate individuals to engage in creative activities as a way to alleviate their suffering and enhance their well-being. To date, two main empirical literatures have examined this claim. First, reviews of art therapy trials have found that such interventions typically lead to small but statistically significant improvements on a range of psychological measures (Slayton et al., 2010; Forgeard and Eichner, 2014; Maujean et al., 2014). Second, studies examining the benefits of “everyday creativity” suggest that engaging in day-to-day creative activities may both reflect and foster psychological health (Richards, 2007). In keeping with this, findings of a recent experience-sampling study showed that young adult participants were more likely to be engaged in creative activities than other activities when they reported feeling happy and active (Silvia et al., 2014). In spite of these efforts, important gaps exist in our understanding of the therapeutic benefits of creativity. The first and foremost of these gaps is the following: to the best of our knowledge, little empirical evidence has demonstrated that creative thinking per se is one of the specific active ingredients accounting for the benefits of creative activities. To date, past research has investigated the role of other potential mechanisms including adaptive emotion regulation, flow, meaning-making, or growth from adversity in order to explain the benefits of creative activities (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Drake and Winner, 2012; Forgeard et al., 2014). Thus, it remains unclear whether the benefits of creative activities are due to creative thinking, or to other factors. We propose that the time is ripe to collect such evidence in order to provide a richer understanding of the nature of the therapeutic benefits of creative thinking. We outline a research agenda to advance the clinical science of creativity from a cognitive-behavioral perspective.
OPINION ARTICLE
published: 19 June 2014
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00613
Advancing the clinical science of creativity
Marie J. C. Forgeard*and Jeanette G. Elstein
Department of Psychology, Positive Psychology Center, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
*Correspondence: mariefd@psych.upenn.edu
Edited by:
Anna Abraham, Kuwait University, Kuwait
Reviewed by:
Mahesh Menon, University of Toronto, Canada
Anna Abraham, Kuwait University, Kuwait
Keywords: creativity, psychopathology, clinical psychology, psychotherapy, flexibility
Can the therapeutic benefits of creativ-
ity explain its documented association
with psychopathology (Andreasen, 1987;
Ludwig, 1995)? Past research seems to
have devoted most of its attention to
another hypothesis in order to explain this
relationship: that features of some disor-
ders may be beneficial for creative cogni-
tion (especially in the arts)—for example,
the racing thoughts, energy, and open-
ness characteristic of hypomania in bipo-
lar disorder (Johnson et al., 2012), or
the rumination observed in depression
(Verhaeghen et al., 2005). Other expla-
nations, however, should not be ignored
or considered mutually exclusive. Creative
work may sometimes exacerbate psy-
chopathology. For example, Kaufman and
Baer (2002) suggested that poets may
be especially susceptible to mental illness
because poetry requires emotional expres-
sion and introspection, and unlike prose,
may not provide adequate opportunities
for making meaning out of one’s experi-
ence. Conversely, and leaving aside third
variable explanations (which also deserve
further research), we explore the hypoth-
esis that psychopathology may motivate
individuals to engage in creative activi-
ties as a way to alleviate their suffering
and enhance their well-being. To date, two
main empirical literatures have examined
this claim. First, reviews of art therapy
trials have found that such interventions
typically lead to small but statistically sig-
nificant improvements on a range of psy-
chological measures (Slayton et al., 2010;
Forgeard and Eichner, 2014; Maujean
et al., 2014). Second, studies examining
the benefits of “everyday creativity” sug-
gest that engaging in day-to-day creative
activities may both reflect and foster
psychological health (Richards, 2007). In
keeping with this, findings of a recent
experience-sampling study showed that
young adult participants were more likely
to be engaged in creative activities than
other activities when they reported feeling
happy and active (Silvia et al., 2014).
In spite of these efforts, important gaps
exist in our understanding of the thera-
peutic benefits of creativity. The first and
foremost of these gaps is the following:
to the best of our knowledge, little empiri-
cal evidence has demonstrated that creative
thinking per se is one of the specific active
ingredients accounting for the benefits of
creative activities. To date, past research
has investigated the role of other poten-
tial mechanisms including adaptive emo-
tion regulation, flow, meaning-making,
or growth from adversity in order to
explain the benefits of creative activi-
ties (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Drake and
Winner, 2012; Forgeard et al., 2014). Thus,
it remains unclear whether the benefits of
creative activities are due to creative think-
ing, or to other factors. We propose that
the time is ripe to collect such evidence
in order to provide a richer understand-
ingofthenatureofthetherapeuticbenets
of creative thinking. We outline a research
agenda to advance the clinical science of
creativity from a cognitive-behavioral per-
spective.
CREATIVE THINKING AS A
TRANSDIAGNOSTIC PROCESS
Clinical scientists are developing a growing
interest in understanding transdiagnos-
tic processes (i.e., processes shared across
disorders) that can account for overlap
in symptoms and high rates of comor-
bidity between psychological disorders, as
well as recovery or resilience. These pro-
cesses (whether pathological or adaptive)
can help develop parsimonious theories
of disorder and health, as well as prag-
matic treatments (Mansell et al., 2009;
Forgeard et al., 2011). The research agenda
we present here is based on the following
hypothesis: Creative thinking constitutes
an important yet understudied transdiag-
nostic process that can be defined, oper-
ationalized, assessed, and (if found to be
adaptive) enhanced. Creativity refers to
the generation of ideas or products that
are both novel (i.e., original, unusual) and
useful (i.e., valuable, helpful) (Stein, 1953;
Runco and Jaeger, 2012). Creative think-
ing can be subjective (i.e., novel and
useful to the self) and/or in compari-
son to others (i.e., novel and useful to
all) (Kaufman and Beghetto, 2009). It is
also not reserved to prototypical creative
domains (e.g., the arts and the sciences),
but is present to varying degrees in almost
all areas of life—excelling at work, solving
thorny interpersonal problems, managing
painful emotions, or cooking dinner, are
all tasks that may benefit from effective
creative thinking. Related to this, creative
thinking takes place not only in “creative
therapies” (e.g., art therapy), but to some
degree also in all forms of psychotherapy.
How does creativity relate to other pro-
cesses already studied by clinical scientists?
Creative thinking is by definition closely
relatedtoprospection,denedasthe
mental representation of possible futures
(Seligman et al., 2013). Past research sug-
gests that maladaptive patterns in future-
oriented thinking play a key role in
psychopathology (Miloyan et al., 2013).
For example, both anxious and depressed
individuals tend to overestimate future
www.frontiersin.org June 2014 | Volume 5 | Article 613 |1
Forgeard and Elstein Clinical science of creativity
negative outcomes, and depressed individ-
uals also tend to underestimate future pos-
itive outcomes (e.g., MacLeod and Byrne,
1996; Miranda and Mennin, 2007). How
might generating novel and useful ideas
influence the extent to which individ-
uals think about and prepare for the
future in a constructive manner? Creativity
may contribute to adaptive prospection by
enhancing another closely related process:
psychological flexibility, defined as the
ability to effectively adapt one’s cognitions,
emotions, and behaviors to the situation
at hand (Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010).
Psychological flexibility does not necessar-
ily require creative thinking—individuals
may build a repertoire of options by
learning from others or from the envi-
ronment (as opposed to inventing them
anew). We propose, however, that creative
thinking probably enhances and strength-
ens psychological flexibility by allowing
individuals to generate new and effec-
tive cognitive, emotional, and behavioral
strategies on their own. Creative thinking
may therefore help counteract a number
of detrimental transdiagnostic processes
reflecting maladaptive prospection and
inflexibility, including repetitive negative
thinking, as well as interpretational and
expectancy biases (Harvey et al., 2004)by
helping individuals adopt adaptive inter-
pretations and coping styles (Fresco et al.,
2006).
EXAMINING CREATIVE THINKING AS
AN ACTIVE INGREDIENT
What comes next for clinical scientists
interested in examining whether and
how creative thinking promotes flex-
ibility and decreases psychopathology?
Treatment outcome researchers should
continue to build empirical support for
the efficacy of interventions thought to
rely on creative thinking (e.g., art ther-
apy) (Kaplan, 2000; Gilroy, 2006; Maujean
et al., 2014). Randomized controlled tri-
als (RCTs) remain the gold standard for
this purpose and are necessary to establish
that an intervention is empirically sup-
ported, among other criteria (Chambless
and Hollon, 1998). Of course, preliminary
investigations such as single case designs,
or uncontrolled trials, often provide useful
insights.
Aside from outcome research, rigor-
ous process research is needed in order
to test whether creative thinking itself
(as opposed, or in addition to, other
mechanisms) is one of the active ingre-
dients accounting for positive outcomes.
Process research uses appropriate research
designs and mediation analyses in order
to test causal mechanisms responsible for
the effects of an intervention (Kazdin,
2007). In addition to assessing the con-
tribution of creative thinking to out-
comes, researchers should also further
assess the mediating role of mechanisms
examined in prior scholarship (includ-
ing adaptive emotion regulation, flow,
meaning-making, or growth from adver-
sity, as mentioned above), as well as
additional mechanisms such as psycholog-
ical flexibility (Kashdan and Rottenberg,
2010), behavioral activation (Jacobson
et al., 2001), or self-efficacy (Bandura,
1997), among others.
Future research should examine the
extent to which creative thinking per se
contributes to these processes, and in turn,
to psychological adjustment. Such research
is needed to establish whether creative
thinking holds special benefits for well-
being compared to other thinking styles.
This assessment in no way diminishes
the value of previous findings, but rather
highlights the importance and value of
addressing this question in future research.
Similarly, little research has investigated
whether and how creative thinking abil-
ities contribute to the effects of other
forms of therapy. For example, cognitive
therapy for depression encourages indi-
viduals to generate alternative explana-
tions for automatic thoughts and to assess
cognitions for accuracy and usefulness
(Beck et al., 1979)—a process which could
recruit and/or develop creative thinking
abilities.
It is not just on the client’s end that cre-
ative thinking may enhance outcomes—
therapists too need to be creative thinkers.
Concerns have been raised about the
extent to which manualized treatments
can help clients whose symptoms are
more complex than those included in
RCTs (Westen et al., 2004). Yet, although
manuals are required to operational-
ize and demonstrate the efficacy of a
treatment, most researchers and clini-
cians tend to agree that good man-
uals leave space for “flexibility within
fidelity” in order to effectively tailor
treatment to clients’ specific concerns
and learning styles (Kendall and Beidas,
2007). Therapists’ creative thinking abil-
ities therefore probably enable them to
flexibly invent new ways to faithfully
implement treatments (Deacon, 2000).
Within the context of cognitive-behavioral
therapy, such creative thinking may be
manifested in astute behavioral experi-
ments to test negative cognitions, individ-
ualized exposures for anxiety disorders, or
compelling metaphors to foster motiva-
tion and change (Peterman et al., in press).
CONCLUSION
Researchers interested in advancing the
clinical science of creativity have exciting
tasks ahead of them: to continue building
empirical support for the value of creative
therapies using outcome research, and to
investigate the role of creative thinking as a
transdiagnostic process that may promote
adaptive future-thinking and psychologi-
cal flexibility using process research. These
endeavors will enrich our understanding
of the relationship between creativity, psy-
chopathology, and health by investigating
the circumstances under which creative
thinking is or is not beneficial, and by
identifying the metacognitive strategies
that help individuals tell the difference
(Kaufman and Beghetto, 2013). In partic-
ular, it is likely that original thinking may
only be beneficial in moderate amounts or
in certain situations, though more research
is needed to test this claim. Related to
this, researchers have called for investigat-
ing the boundary conditions under which
any positive psychological trait or pro-
cess may become detrimental, as seem-
ingly linear relationships may in fact be
nonmonotonic when examining their full
range of expression (Grant and Schwartz,
2011). The optimal “dose” of originality
and flexibility may therefore vary accord-
ing to the situation at hand. For example,
a person might benefit from considering a
wide array of options in order to repair a
romantic relationship after a fight. A sim-
ple “I am sorry” may not be as effective
as an apology expressed in a clever and
constructive way. Past a certain point how-
ever, the search for novel and flexible solu-
tions may lead to impulsivity or instability
(Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010). In this
case, organizing a last-minute unusual and
extravagant date or writing an entire book
Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology June 2014 | Volume 5 | Article 613 |2
Forgeard and Elstein Clinical science of creativity
of poems to apologize could be perceived
as “too much of a good thing.
In addition, future research should fur-
ther examine how various forms of cre-
ativity relate to well-being, given that past
research in this area has mainly explored
the effects of artistic creativity. For exam-
ple, past research suggests that artists suf-
fer from psychopathology at a greater rate
than scientists (Ludwig, 1995). These find-
ings could be influenced by self-selection
effects, and/or by the possibility that the
creative process has differential benefits
for artists vs. scientists. Related to this,
the extent to which creative thinking ben-
efits well-being may depend on whether
the creative work at hand focuses on
one’s personal situation or mental state
(a case perhaps more typical of the arts)
or on an external problem (a case per-
haps more typical of the sciences). Thus,
future research should further investigate
whether and how creative work affects
well-being in fields other than the arts.
In light of past research in this area,
as well as the promise of addressing exist-
ing remaining questions highlighted here,
we believe that the study of the thera-
peutic benefits of creativity will continue
to make important contributions to clini-
cal science by further investigating one of
the possible causal mechanisms account-
ing for the relationship between creativity,
psychopathology, recovery, and resilience.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
We thank Ann Marie Roepke for her
comments on an earlier draft of this
manuscript.
REFERENCES
Andreasen, N. (1987). Creativity and mental illness:
prevalence in writers and their first-degree rela-
tives. Am.J.Psychiatry144, 1288–1292.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of
Control.NewYork,NY:Freeman.
Beck,A.T.,Rush,A.J.,Shaw,B.E.,andEmery,G.
(1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression.NewYork,
NY: Guilford Press.
Chambless, D., and Hollon, S. (1998). Defining empir-
ically supported therapies. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
66, 7–18. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.1.7
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the
Psychology of Discovery and Invention.NewYork,
NY: HarperCollins Publishers.
Deacon, S. A. (2000). Using divergent-thinking exer-
cises within supervision to enhance therapist
creativity. J. Fam. Psychother. 11, 67–73. doi:
10.1300/J085v11n02_06
Drake, J. E., and Winner, E. (2012). Confronting sad-
ness through art-making: distraction is more ben-
eficial than venting. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 6,
255–261. doi: 10.1037/a0026909
Forgeard, M. J., Haigh, E. A., Beck, A. T.,
Davidson, R. J., Henn, F. A., Maier, S. F.,
et al. (2011). Beyond depression: towards a
process-based approach to research, diagnosis,
and treatment. Clin. Psychol. 18, 275–299. doi:
10.1111/j.1468-2850.2011.01259.x
Forgeard, M. J. C., and Eichner, K. V. (2014).
“Creativity as a target and tool for positive inter-
ventions, in Handbook of Positive Psychological
Interventions, Chapter 7, eds A. C. Parks and S. M.
Schueller (Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell).
Forgeard, M. J. C., Mecklenburg, A. C., Lacasse, J.
J., and Jayawickreme, E. (2014). “Bringing the
whole universe to order: creativity, healing, and
posttraumatic growth,” in New Ideas About an
Old Topic: Creativity and Mental Illness,edJ.
Kaufman(NewYork,NY:CambridgeUniversity
Press), 321–342.
Fresco,D.M.,Williams,N.L.,andNugent,N.R.
(2006). Flexibility and negative affect: examining
the associations of explanatory flexibility and cop-
ing flexibility to each other and to depression
and anxiety. Cogn. Ther. Res. 30, 201–210. doi:
10.1007/s10608-006-9019-8
Gilroy, A. (2006). Art Therapy, Research, and Evidence-
Based Practice. London: Sage Publications.
Grant, A. M., and Schwartz, B. (2011). Too much of
a good thing: the challenge and opportunity of
the inverted U. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 61–76. doi:
10.1177/1745691610393523
Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E. R., Mansell, W., and
Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive Behavioural Processes
Across Psychological Disorders: A Trandiagnostic
Approach to Research and Treatment. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Jacobson, N. S., Martell, C. R., and Dimidjian, S.
(2001). Behavioral activation treatment for depres-
sion: returning to contextual roots. Clin. Psychol.
Sci. Pract. 8, 255–270. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.8.
3.255
Johnson, S. L., Murray, G., Fredrickson, B.,
Youngstrom, E. A., Hinshaw, S., Malbrancq Bass,
J., et al. (2012). Creativity and bipolar disorder:
touched by fire or burning with questions? Clin.
Psychol. Rev. 32, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.
10.001
Kaplan, F. F. (2000). Art, Science, and Art Therapy.
London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Kashdan, T. B., and Rottenberg, J. (2010).
Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect
of health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 865–878. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001
Kaufman, J. C., and Baer, J. (2002). I bask in dreams
of suicide: mental illness, poetry, and women.
Rev. Gen. Psychol. 6, 271–286. doi: 10.1037/1089-
2680.6.3.271
Kaufman, J. C., and Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond
big and little: the four c model of creativity. Rev.
Gen. Psychol. 13, 1–12. doi: 10.1037/a0013688
Kaufman, J. C., and Beghetto, R. A. (2013). In
praise of Clark Kent: creative metacognition and
the importance of teaching kids when (not)
to be creative. Roeper Rev. 35, 155–165. doi:
10.1080/02783193.2013.799413
Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mecha-
nisms of change in psychotherapy research.
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3, 1–27. doi:
10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432
Kendall, P. C., and Beidas, R. S. (2007). Smoothing
the trail for dissemination of evidence-based prac-
tices for youth: flexibility within fidelity. Prof.
Psychol. Res. Pract. 38, 13–20. doi: 10.1037/0735-
7028.38.1.13
Ludwig, A. (1995). The Price of Greatness: Resolving the
Creativity and Madness Controversy.NewYork,NY:
Guilford Press.
MacLeod, A. K., and Byrne, A. (1996). Anxiety,
depression, and the anticipation of future positive
and negative experiences. J. Abnorm. Psychol.
105, 286–289. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.
2.286
Mansell, W., Harvey, A., Watkins, E., and Shafran,
R. (2009). Conceptual foundations of the
transdiagnostic approach to CBT. J. Cognit.
Psychother. 23, 6–19. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.
23.1.6
Maujean, A., Pepping, C. A., and Kendall, E. (2014). A
systematic review of randomized controlled stud-
ies of art therapy. Art Therapy 31, 37–44. doi:
10.1080/07421656.2014.873696
Miloyan, B., Pachana, N. A., and Suddendorf, T.
(2013). The future is here: a review of foresight
systems in anxiety and depression. Cogn. Emot. 28,
795–810. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.863179
Miranda, R., and Mennin, D. S. (2007). Depression,
generalized anxiety disorder, and certainty in pes-
simistic predictions about the future. Cogn. Ther.
Res. 31, 71–82. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9063-4
Peterman, J. S., Read, K. L., Wei, C., and Kendall,
P. C. (in press). The art of exposure: putting
science into practice. Cogn.Behav.Pract.doi:
10.1016/j.cbpra.2014.02.003
Richards, R. (2007). “Everyday creativity: our hid-
den potential, in Everyday Creativity and New
Views of Human Nature: Psychological, Social, and
Spiritual Perspectives,edR.Richards(Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association), 25–53.
doi: 10.1037/11595-001
Runco, M. A., and Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard
definition of creativity. Creativity Res. J. 24, 92–96.
doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.650092
Seligman,M.E.P.,Railton,P.,Baumeister,R.F.,
and Sripada, C. (2013). Navigating into the future
or driven by the past. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8,
119–141. doi: 10.1177/1745691612474317
Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., Eddington,
K. M., Levin-Aspenson, H., and Kwapil, T.
R. (2014). Everyday creativity in daily life: an
experience-sampling study of “little c” creativ-
ity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 8, 183–188. doi:
10.1037/a0035722
Slayton, S., D’Archer, J., and Kaplan, F. (2010).
Outcome studies on the efficacy of art therapy: a
review of findings. Art Th era py 27, 108–118. doi:
10.1080/07421656.2010.10129660
Stein, M. I. (1953). Creativity and culture. J. Psychol.
36, 31–322. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1953.9712897
Verhaeghen, P., Joormann, J., and Khan, R. (2005).
Why we sing the blues: the relation between
self-reflective rumination, mood, and creativ-
ity. Emotion 5, 226–232. doi: 10.1037/1528-
3542.5.2.226
www.frontiersin.org June 2014 | Volume 5 | Article 613 |3
Forgeard and Elstein Clinical science of creativity
Westen, D., Novotny, C. M., and Thompson-Brenner,
H. (2004). The empirical status of empirically
supported psychotherapies: assumptions, findings,
and reporting in controlled clinical trials. Psychol.
Bull. 130, 631–663. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.
4.631
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare
that the research was conducted in the absence
of any commercial or financial relationships
that could be construed as a potential conflict of
interest.
Received: 29 March 2014; accepted: 30 May 2014;
published online: 19 June 2014.
Citation: Forgeard MJC and Elstein JG (2014)
Advancing the clinical science of creativity. Front.
Psychol. 5:613. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00613
This article was submitted to Psychopathology, a section
of the journal Frontiers in Psychology.
Copyright © 2014 Forgeard and Elstein. This is an
open-access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is
permitted, provided the original author(s) or licen-
sor are credited and that the original publication
in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduc-
tion is permitted which does not comply with these
terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology June 2014 | Volume 5 | Article 613 |4
... Perhaps unsurprisingly, one of the early fields that has started to approach creativity as a tool by which to promote positive outcomes has been clinical science-notably art therapy ( Forgeard & Elstein, 2014;Gavron & Mayseless, 2018;Han et al., 2017;Thayer & Bloomfield, 2021). One of the most replicated findings in the field is that creative activities tend to foster positive emotions, thereby contributing to individual wellbeing (Cheung et al., 2019;Conner et al., 2018;Gavron & Mayseless, 2018;Karwowski et al., 2021). ...
... But they suggest a link between creative behaviour and resilience-a link that is further supported by previous research documenting the therapeutic effect of creative activities in clinical settings (Forgeard, 2019;Forgeard & Elstein, 2014). For instance, creative activities seem to provide assets which help adults with cancer diagnoses to cope to a moderate to strong extent against anxiety, depression, and fatigue symptoms (Jiang et al., 2020;Tang et al., 2019;Xu et al., 2020). ...
... Little evidence has been produced to indicate whether, when, and how creative activities can contribute to young children's resilience. Indeed, creativity researchers have only recently begun to ask whether creativity might serve resilience, and had previously studied this question almost exclusively within adult populations Forgeard, • 29 2015; Forgeard & Elstein, 2014). In fact, even in the context of COVID-19, only four articles have specifically explored the relationship between creativity and resilience among populations of adolescents and young adults, since the onset of the pandemic (De Lorenzo et al. 2023). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis investigates the contribution of parent-child creative activities to young children's resilience, a topic that has received little attention in creativity research. Using a pragmatic methodology that combines quantitative and qualitative approaches, this multi-method doctoral project addresses three research questions: (a) Do parent-child creative activities contribute to young children's resilience factors? (b) What processes could explain the effects of parent-child creative activities in promoting young children's resilience factors? (c) What are the similarities and differences between two Western countries' (Great Britain and France)cultures that could account for the effects of creative activities on young children's resilience factors? This thesis opens with a bibliometric network analysis review (n = 454), which highlights the scarcity of studies addressing resilience abilities in early childhood. It identifies that creativity research strongly tends (a) to be biased towards the study of creativity as a dependent variable rather than as a predictor; (b) tends to rarely address the outcomes of creative activities among the population of children aged between 3 and 6 years; and (c) tends to rarely study the effects of creative activities on resilience-related outcomes in general, and particularly among young children. Then, a systematic review (n = 26) provides evidence that creative activities may contribute to other resilience outcomes: positive parent-child relationships, positive emotions, reduced cortisol levels, and increased emotion regulation abilities. A meta-analysis (k = 9) further shows that creative activities strongly contribute to two other resilience factors: inhibition and school readiness. Moreover, it identifies a moderate effect size on working memory, another important factor in resilience promotion. Aiming to understand more specifically dyadic and cross-cultural parent-child behaviours in creative activities, the thesis then reports the findings from a reflexive thematic analysis (n = 14). This study shows how and why parents in Great Britain and in France engage in dyadic creative activities with their children: according to them, creative activities are enjoyable, contribute to parent-child relationship quality, and the development of the child’s competence. While this cross-cultural qualitative study highlights similarities between the British and the French parent-child dyads, cross-sectional data (n = 169) identifies differences. Based on a scale designed for the purposes of this research (the Parent-Child Creative Activities Checklist), imagination activities (e.g., pretend play, story creation, Lego) and craft activities (e.g., drawing, painting, making) are associated with a positive parent-child relationship and low parent-child conflict in the British sample, but not in the French sample. To understand why, cross-cultural comparisons were conducted to analyse moment-by-moment parent-child interactions during creative activities, using audio recordings provided by the parents (n = 17). A main difference lies in that British dyads appear more collaborative and more focused on idea elaboration than the French. This thesis concludes with a theoretical contribution and summary of this thesis’s findings, aiming to inform interventional research. Thus, the Parent-Child Creative Partnership model posits that, when parents adopt attitudes that support children’s basic psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness (for instance by helping their children elaborate their ideas and by valorising their resulting creative productions), dyadic creative activities promote positive parent-child attachment, children's sense of self-efficacy, and self-image. Hence, this thesis shows how, when, and for whom dyadic parent-child creative activities contribute to important assets for immediate early childhood well-being, and future resilience.
... Creativity is not limited to the broad spectrum of mental health conditions [13], for example in creating coping mechanisms for alleviating adverse conditions [36]. Creativity is clearly present in daily life, such as performing at work, dealing with interpersonal issues, managing painful emotions, or cooking dinner [37]. Groundbreaking ideas relevant for societal innovations may eventually emerge through such daily endeavors, for instance through discussions at work. ...
... Forgeard and Elstein [37] highlighted that creative thinking constitutes an important yet understudied process, but which ultimately can be defined, operationalized, assessed, and (if adaptive) refined and improved. We concur with these authors. ...
... Inherent in these points are the creation and enhancement of adaptive prospection through psychological flexibility; that is, the ability to effectively adapt one's cognitions, emotions, and behaviors to a situation at hand [79,80]. Also, other potential mechanisms benefiting creative activities, including adaptive emotion regulation, flow, meaning-making, or growth from adversity could be targeted [37]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background This paper discusses a paradox in mental health. It manifests as a relationship between adverse “bad” effects (suffering, clinical costs, loss of productivity) in individuals and populations and advantageous “good” aspects of mental disorders. These beneficial aspects (scientific, artistic and political accomplishments) emanate at the societal level through the frequently unprecedented creativity of people suffering from mental disorders and their relatives. Such gains can contribute to societal innovation and problem-solving. Especially in times of accelerated social-ecological change, approaches are needed that facilitate best-possible mental health care but also recognize creative ideas conducive to beneficial clinical and social-ecological innovations as soon as possible. Discussion This paper emphasizes the need to account for creativity as a crucial component in evolving mental health systems and societies. It highlights the need for wide-ranging approaches and discusses how research targeting multiple facets (e.g., brain level, cognitive neuroscience, psychiatry, neurology, socio-cultural, economic and other factors) might further our understanding of the creativity-mental disorder link and its importance for innovating mental health systems and societies. Conclusion Our discussion clarifies that considerable research will be needed to obtain a better understanding of how creativity associated with mental disorders may help to create more sustainable societies on a fast-changing planet through innovative ideas. Given the current-state-of-the-art of research and healthcare management, our discussion is currently speculative. However, it provides a basis for how pros and cons might be studied in the future through transdisciplinary research and collaborations across sectors of society.
... In fact, in school research, divergent thinking affects stresscoping ability (Carson et al., 1994). As a result, creativity enables adaptive future thinking in the face of stressful situations (Forgeard & Elstein, 2014). It is associated with psychological characteristics such as high-level ego autonomy, which helps individuals respond to life's stressors in a positive way (Cropley, 1990). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to investigate the dual mediation role of creativity and academic coping in network efficiency and academic performance, using Folkman and Lazarus’s transactional model of stress. Data were obtained from 39 students taking business administration at a Korean university. Social network analysis was performed to check the ego network of students. The double mediation model was applied to analyze the path from network efficiency to academic performance. The results showed that network efficiency, in relation to the structural hole in social network parameters, was double mediated by creativity and academic coping, which had a positive effect on academic performance. However, the effect of double mediation through avoidance was insignificant. Based on the research results, the theoretical and practical implications for the dual mediation effect of creativity and academic coping were presented.
... THE PRESENT REVIEW Prior to this bibliometric review, little evidence has been produced to indicate whether, when, and how creative activities can contribute to young children's resilience. Indeed, creativity researchers have only recently begun to ask whether creativity might serve resilience, and had previously studied this question almost exclusively within adult populations (Forgeard, 2015(Forgeard, , 2018Forgeard & Elstein, 2014). Over the 200 studies included in their review, Forgeard and Kaufman (2016) found that only 12 (i.e., 6%) addressed processes or outcomes that could relate to resilience as proxied through domains including health, wellbeing, and problem-solving. ...
Article
Full-text available
Creativity researchers are increasingly interested in understanding when, how, and for whom creativity can be beneficial. Previous reviews have demonstrated that creativity research largely ignores the study of its impact on factors that promote health, and well-being among populations of adults. It is unclear, in fact, whether this gap in research also extends to creativity research among young children. This paper addresses this issue. Early childhood is a crucial stage for the cognitive development of young children who remain highly sensitive to stress, and adversity. It is therefore essential to identify and promote factors that are beneficial to early childhood resilience, thereby contributing to documenting more of the effects of creative activities on positive outcomes. This paper presents a review with a bibliometric analysis of 1000 randomly selected articles from the Web of Science, without bias towards any specific peer-reviewed journal. The analysis of 454 included articles shows that approximately 80% of the included studies focus on creativity as an outcome (replicating previous findings with a larger sample), with only 3.78% investigating creative activities as predictors among young children. In this small percentage, most of the studies addressed creative activities in young children related to resilience outcomes.
... Further cross-cultural comparisons of different populations of adults (in Israel, the United States, Italy, and China) showed that creative behaviour was positively and strongly associated with cross-sectional measure of resilient coping (the ability to positively adapt to stress, to perceive oneself as having a degree of personal control to deal with difficult situations, and grow); this, in turn, predicted emotional wellbeing among individuals in all four countries (Orkibi et al., 2021). This provides support to early views about how creativity would serve resilience, defined as the ability to navigate one's wellbeing in the presence of adversity (Forgeard & Elstein, 2014;Ungar, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
This commentary invites creativity researchers to address an area that, to date, has received little attention: the effects of dyadic creative activities on early childhood resilience. There is, indeed, a growing body of work on how creative behaviour can contribute to resilience in older children, adolescents, and adults. There is less research on this topic for populations of children aged 3 to 6 years. Yet, young children are particularly dependent upon the bonds they form with their caregivers, notably their parents. The quality of the ties they maintain with them can promote, or on the contrary hinder, their resilience. After presenting the need to foster resilience among young children through dyadic creative activities, the commentary proposes audio recording as a method of investigating this phenomenon. It presents perspectives on the analysis of momentary processes. It concludes with perspectives creative activities at home that researchers can propose to parents and children to address their effects on young children's resilience.
Article
Drawing on enrichment theory and the identity‐based integrative crafting model, the present paper explores the impact of leisure crafting on creativity and meaning at work using both the compensation and the spillover perspectives. We hypothesized that leisure crafting relates to employee creativity, particularly when employees experience low work engagement; and that leisure crafting predicts meaning at work via employee creativity, particularly for employees with low work engagement. We also expected that cognitive developmental and affective leisure‐to‐work enrichment acts as the mediator in the link between leisure crafting and creativity. Study 1, a three‐wave survey study with 1‐week time intervals among 191 employees confirmed that the indirect effect of leisure crafting on meaning at work via creativity is stronger among employees reporting low work engagement. Study 2, a follow‐up study of a similar design among 421 employees revealed that leisure crafting leads to creativity via cognitive developmental resources and that leisure crafting leads to creativity via affective resources for employees who report low levels of work engagement. Our findings highlight that leisure crafting possesses the inherent capacity to enhance meaning at work through employee creativity (spillover), especially for those employees who experience a lack of fulfillment at work (compensation). We also refine work‐life enrichment theories by uncovering that leisure crafting may enrich work via different pathways for different employees.
Article
Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a mental health condition that can occur following exposure to a traumatic experience. An estimated 12 million U.S. adults are presently affected by this disorder. Current treatments include psychological therapies (e.g., exposure-based interventions) and pharmacological treatments (e.g., selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs)). However, a significant proportion of patients receiving standard-of-care therapies for PTSD remain symptomatic, and new approaches for this and other trauma-related mental health conditions are greatly needed. Psychedelic compounds that alter cognition, perception, and mood are currently being examined for their efficacy in treating PTSD despite their current status as Drug Enforcement Administration (DEA)- scheduled substances. Initial clinical trials have demonstrated the potential value of psychedelicassisted therapy to treat PTSD and other psychiatric disorders. In this comprehensive review, we summarize the state of the science of PTSD clinical care, including current treatments and their shortcomings. We review clinical studies of psychedelic interventions to treat PTSD, trauma-related disorders, and common comorbidities. The classic psychedelics psilocybin, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), and N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT) and DMT-containing ayahuasca, as well as the entactogen 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and the dissociative anesthetic ketamine, are reviewed. For each drug, we present the history of use, psychological and somatic effects, pharmacology, and safety profile. The rationale and proposed mechanisms for use in treating PTSD and traumarelated disorders are discussed. This review concludes with an in-depth consideration of future directions for the psychiatric applications of psychedelics to maximize therapeutic benefit and minimize risk in individuals and communities impacted by trauma-related conditions.
Article
Full-text available
This review article examines current knowledge about the efficacy of art therapy based on the findings of 8 randomized controlled trials (RCTs) conducted with adult populations from 2008–2013 that met a high standard of rigor. Of these studies, all but one reported beneficial effects of art therapy. Review findings suggest that art therapy may benefit a range of individuals, including older adults, war veterans, and prison inmates. However, there is a need for further research using RCTs to examine more conclusively art therapy outcomes and the specific populations in which art therapy interventions offer greatest benefit.
Book
This title proposes an insightful and original approach to understanding these disorders, one that focuses on what they have in common. Instead of examining in isolation, for example, obsessive compulsive disorders, insomnia, schizophrenia, it asks - what do patients with these disorders have in common? It takes each cognitive and behavioural process - attention, memory, reasoning, thought, behaviour, and examines whether it is a transdiagnostic process - i.e., serves to maintain a broad range of psychological disorders. Having shown how these disorders share several important processes, it then describes the practical implications of such an approach to diagnosis and treatment. Importantly it explores why the different psychological disorders can present so differently, despite being maintained by the same cognitive and behavioural processes. It also provides an account of the high rates of comorbidity observed among the different disorders.
Article
Aristotle proposed that to achieve happiness and success, people should cultivate virtues at mean or intermediate levels between deficiencies and excesses. In stark contrast to this assertion that virtues have costs at high levels, a wealth of psychological research has focused on demonstrating the well-being and performance benefits of positive traits, states, and experiences. This focus has obscured the prevalence and importance of nonmonotonic inverted-U-shaped effects, whereby positive phenomena reach inflection points at which their effects turn negative. We trace the evidence for nonmonotonic effects in psychology and provide recommendations for conceptual and empirical progress. We conclude that for psychology in general and positive psychology in particular, Aristotle’s idea of the mean may serve as a useful guide for developing both a descriptive and a prescriptive account of happiness and success.
Chapter
Are creative people more likely to be mentally ill? This basic question has been debated for thousands of years, with the 'mad genius' concept advanced by such luminaries as Aristotle. There are many studies that argue the answer is 'yes', and several prominent scholars who argue strongly for a connection. There are also those who argue equally strongly that the core studies and scholarship underlying the mad genius myth are fundamentally flawed. This book re-examines the common view that a high level of individual creativity often correlates with a heightened risk of mental illness. It reverses conventional wisdom that links creativity with mental illness, arguing that the two traits are not associated. With contributions from some of the most exciting voices in the fields of psychology, neuroscience, physics, psychiatry, and management, this is a dynamic and cutting-edge volume that will inspire new ideas and studies on this fascinating topic.
Article
Creativity, which is generally defined as the generation of novel and useful ideas or products, is essential to human progress and flourishing. As such, many intervention approaches have been designed to promote creativity and its associated benefits. The goal of this chapter is to provide a review of existing research examining the conceptual basis and effectiveness of creativity interventions. In doing so, this chapter focuses on two main kinds of interventions. First, we discuss interventions that aim to increase creativity for its own sake and highlight one method that has been investigated in business and educational settings, the Creative Problem Solving (CPS) approach. Second, we discuss interventions that aim to increase creativity in order to increase psychological well-being, and provide an overview of scholarship describing the practice and effectiveness of art therapy. This chapter ends by describing future directions for building and testing brief creativity-based positive interventions.