ArticlePDF Available

A taste of Kandinsky: Assessing the influence of the artistic visual presentation of food on the dining experience

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background Researchers have demonstrated that a variety of visual factors, such as the colour and balance of the elements on a plate, can influence a diner’s perception of, and response to, food. Here, we report on a study designed to assess whether placing the culinary elements of a dish in an art-inspired manner would modify the diner’s expectations and hence their experience of food. The dish, a salad, was arranged in one of three different presentations: One simply plated (with all of the elements of the salad tossed together), another with the elements arranged to look like one of Kandinsky’s paintings, and a third arrangement in which the elements were organized in a neat (but non-artistic) manner. The participants answered two questionnaires, one presented prior to and the other after eating the dish, to evaluate their expectations and actual sensory experience. Results Prior to consumption, the art-inspired presentation resulted in the food being considered as more artistic, more complex, and more liked than either of the other presentations. The participants were also willing to pay more for the Kandinsky-inspired plating. Interestingly, after consumption, the results revealed higher tastiness ratings for the art-inspired presentation. Conclusions These results support the idea that presenting food in an aesthetically pleasing manner can enhance the experience of a dish. In particular, the use of artistic (visual) influences can enhance a diner’s rating of the flavour of a dish. These results are consistent with previous findings, suggesting that visual display of a food can influence both a person’s expectations and their subsequent experience of a dish, and with the common assumption that we eat with our eyes first.
Content may be subject to copyright.
A taste of Kandinsky: assessing the influence
of the artistic visual presentation of food
on the dining experience
Michel et al.
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
R E S E A R C H Open Access
A taste of Kandinsky: assessing the influence
of the artistic visual presentation of food
on the dining experience
Charles Michel
*
, Carlos Velasco, Elia Gatti and Charles Spence
Abstract
Background: Researchers have demonstrated that a variety of visual factors, such as the colour and balance of
the elements on a plate, can influence a diners perception of, and response to, food. Here, we report on a study
designed to assess whether placing the culinary elements of a dish in an art-inspired manner would modify the
diners expectations and hence their experience of food. The dish, a salad, was arranged in one of three different
presentations: One simply plated (with all of the elements of the salad tossed together), another with the elements
arranged to look like one of Kandinskys paintings, and a third arrangement in which the elements were organized
in a neat (but non-artistic) manner. The participants answered two questionnaires, one presented prior to and the
other after eating the dish, to evaluate their expectations and actual sensory experience.
Results: Prior to consumption, the art-inspired presentation resulted in the food being considered as more artistic,
more complex, and more liked than either of the other presentations. The participants were also willing to pay
more for the Kandinsky-inspired plating. Interestingly, after consumption, the results revealed higher tastiness ratings
for the art-inspired presentation.
Conclusions: These results support the idea that presenting food in an aesthetically pleasing manner can enhance
the experience of a dish. In particular, the use of artistic (visual) influences can enhance a diners rating of the
flavour of a dish. These results are consistent with previous findings, suggesting that visual display of a food can
influence both a persons expectations and their subsequent experience of a dish, and with the common
assumption that we eat with our eyes first.
Keywords: Food, Art, Perception, Multisensory, Experience, Plating
Background
I try to interpret the artists message and to make it
mine, to translate it in my life and in the dishes.([1],
Massimo Bottura, Chef at Osteria Francescana).
People perceive and appreciate food in a manner that is
multisensory [2-4]; that is, information from the different
senses is integrated at both the perceptual and semantic
levels in order to give rise to specific multisensory experi-
ences. Just imagine, for instance, a typical meal and the
variety of factors that play a role in modulating the diners
overall experience [5-7]. These include, amongst other
things, the presence of other people [8], the atmosphere
or the environment in which the food is consumed [9,10],
the cutlery with which we happen to be eating [11,12],
and the plateware from which we are eating [13-15].
What people see also exerts a substantial influence over
their perception of food and drink [16]. Visual cues such
as colour [17] and texture [18] have been shown to exert a
significant influence on the perceived flavour and accept-
ance of foods [19], and techniques typically belonging to
the realm of painting and visual communication design
have been theorized to be useful and resourceful tools
when it comes to designing food experiences [20]. A foods
visual features not only affect the perception of the food
* Correspondence: charles.michel@psy.ox.ac.uk
Crossmodal Research Laboratory, Department of Experimental Psychology,
University of Oxford, South Parks Road, OX1 3UD, Oxford, UK
© 2014 Michel et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain
Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article,
unless otherwise stated.
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
itself but also play a crucial role in driving our food-
related expectations [21] and guiding our food choices [5].
Delwiche [22] recently reiterated an oft-made claim that
people eat first with their eyes (see Apicius [23] for one of
the earliest documented claims of this type). Although the
complex visual arrangements of the various elements in a
dish play an irrefutably important role in determining a
diners overall perception, there are still not many insights
from the scientific literature on this matter available to cu-
linary practitioners that would help them enhance the ex-
perience of their guests. In one of the few studies to have
been published in this area, Debra Zellner and her col-
leagues assessed the influence of the balance and complex-
ity of the elements in a dish on the perceived attractiveness,
willingness to try, and liking [24]. Their results revealed that
manipulating the interaction between complexity (increased
by the addition of colour) and balance exerted a significant
effect on the perceived attractiveness of the presentation
and their participantswillingness to try the food. That said,
they did not find any effect of these variables on their par-
ticipantsliking for the foodsflavour.
In a follow-up study, Zellner and her colleagues went on
to demonstrate that people prefer food when it is presented
in a neat, as compared to a messy, arrangement [25]. The
neat visual presentation also exerted a positive influence on
their participantswillingness to pay and their judgments of
perceived quality. While the results of this previous re-
search represent an interesting contribution to the study of
how the visual arrangement of food can influence peoples
perception, there is still a need for researchers to further as-
sess the influence that aesthetic dishes (the plating typically
found in fine dining restaurants
a
) exert on dinners. When
taken together with Zellner et al.s studies, the present
study helps to highlight different aspects of how the visual
presentation of a dish can change the way the diner/con-
sumer will perceive the food.
Specifically, in the present study, we assessed any influence
of an abstract-art based dish design on peoples food expecta-
tions and on their subsequent experience. We compared
peoples experience of a dish presented in a simple manner,
with a dish whose presentation had been inspired by one of
Kandinskys paintings, and a dish in which the elements were
arranged neatly, but without any artistic pretensions.
Methods
Participants
Sixty participants (mean age of 27.7 years, SD = 7.2; ran-
ging from 18 to 58 years), 30 males and 30 females took
part in the study. Upon arrival at the laboratory, the par-
ticipants had to fill in a consent form and a questionnaire
in order to assess the existence of any sensory dysfunc-
tions, allergies, or food intolerances. A small number of
the participants reported being allergic to, or disliking, cer-
tain ingredients, none of which were used in the present
study. The experiment was approved by the Ethics Com-
mittee of the Department of Experimental Psychology at
the University of Oxford. The participants were compen-
sated with five British pounds for their time.
Apparatus and materials
The stimuli consisted of the same set of ingredients pre-
sented in one of three different visual arrangements. Import-
antly, the visual arrangements characterizing the three
conditions contained the exact same quantity of exactly the
same ingredients. The regularpresentation condition con-
sisted of a mix of the ingredients, which were simply placed
in the middle of the plate. In the neatpresentation condi-
tion, the ingredients together with the sauces were placed
side by side without touching each another. Lastly, for the
art-inspiredcondition, the ingredients were placed on the
plate in a very specific manner, inspired by one of Wassily
Kandinskys abstract paintings [26]. The painting that served
as the inspiration for this dish was Painting number 201
(see Figure 1), and was arbitrarily chosen by the authors
b
.
It was described as nonobjective paintingby the artist
himself, a landscape of colour free of descriptive devices
[27]. Kandinskys theories on colour and harmony could
supposedly be applicable to any matter, or medium [28].
Before being placed on the plate, the vegetables and
condiments were prepared in exactly the same manner for
all three presentations. While the sauces were specifically
laid out on the plate for the neat and art-inspired presen-
tations, they were mixed with all the elements of the salad
for the regular presentation. The plate on which the food
was served consisted of a white rectangle of cardboard (di-
mensions of 270 × 180 mm).
The food consisted of a relatively complex salad with 17
distinct components made up of a total of 30 ingredients.
They included three types of elements: vegetables, sauces
(purees and a reduction), and condiments. The 17 compo-
nents of the dish were as follows:
Vegetables: seared Portobello slice, shimeji
mushrooms (briefly boiled with a sweet vinegar
marinade), cooked and raw broccoli sprouts, a variety
of endive salad, raw red and yellow pepper cut into
fine brunoises, one slice of raw red pepper, three slices
of red pepper skin fine julienne, half a slice of raw
yellow pepper, raw cauliflower sprouts, five slices of
mange-tout fine julienne, and half a mange-tout.
Sauces: beet purée, carrot purée, cauliflower and
lemongrass crème, mushroom essence with squid
ink, and, finally, pepperoncino oil.
Condiments: Spanish olive oil, and Maldon sea salt.
A more detailed description of how to prepare each of
the elements can be found in the culinary worksheet pre-
sented in Additional file 1.
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 2 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
Procedure
A between-participants experimental design was used. The
experimental setting, which was the same for all partici-
pants, was designed to replicate a typical restaurant table
(see Figure 2) in a dark room, isolated by means of a cur-
tain. On the table and over a white tablecloth were placed a
fork, a knife, a paper napkin and a glass of water. The only
lighting in the room, a small lamp, was directed at the dish.
The three conditions were randomized across the various
testing times (between 10:00 and 17:00 hrs) and gender was
balanced for each condition. The experiment lasted for ap-
proximately twenty minutes. Upon completing the consent
form, the participants were seated at the table and told the
procedure by the experimenter. The participants were also
instructed that they would be presented with a plate of
food, a salad, and asked to eat it. Before they could start
eating, they were asked to complete a questionnaire con-
cerning the visual aspects of the salad. Moreover, the partic-
ipants were informed that after completion of the first
questionnaire, they would be allowed to eat as much of the
salad as they liked and that after they had finished they
would be given another questionnaire to complete. While
the experimenter explained this procedure, the dish was
plated in an adjacent room. None of the participants were
aware of the existence of different visual presentations and
no further information was given concerning the aims of
the study or the food they were about to eat and its prepar-
ation. When the dish was ready, it was placed on the table
in front of the participant as shown on Figure 1, together
with the first questionnaire. The participants were left alone
while eating the food and completing the questionnaires.
All of the questions were presented using 10-point
Likert scales. The first questionnaire was designed to as-
sess the visual appeal of the dish and the participants
Figure 1 The Kandinsky painting used as the inspiration for the dish (A), and the three different visual arrangements presented (B, C, D).
Note that the three arrangements consisted of the same quantity of the same ingredients.
Figure 2 Setting in which the experiment was conducted.
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 3 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
expectations. The second questionnaire assessed the per-
ception of intensity of different taste attributes (saltiness,
bitterness, sourness, and sweetness) and again the same
questions as asked in the first questionnaire (liking, tasti-
ness and willingness to pay), this time testing the actual
experience of the food rather than merely the participants
expectations about it. For a complete list of the questions
before and after consumption, see Table 1.
While similar questions were asked before and after
consumption, we assumed that the preliminary judgments
were based solely on the visual attributes of the food,
while the latter judgments would provide information
about the eating experience and the impression the food
left in the mind of the participant.
Results
The effect of the three visual arrangements on participants
responses to each of the questions in the two question-
naires (pre- and post-consumption) was analyzed using a
mixed model (to fit the data), including participants as
a random factor (in order to account for any between-
participants variability). Furthermore, we included the fol-
lowing as fixed factors in the model: gender, age, whether
the participant considered his/herself to be a foodieor not,
how much they enjoyed eating vegetables, and how inter-
ested they were in the visual arts, in order to control for the
effect of such variables on the results of each question.
Three of the variables tested (willingness to pay for the
dish, liking, and tastiness) were assessed before and after
consumption. The participantsratings concerning these
three variables were analyzed by pooling together the data,
including consumption of the food (that is: whether the
data belonged to the questionnaire presented before or
after consumption) as a further factor in the model. Note
that the effect of food consumption could vary depending
on the dish presented. The significant factors resulting
from the analysis performed on the three variables are
shown in Table 2.
Post-hoc t-tests (Bonferroni corrected, alpha = .05/3, df =
19) were used in order to assess the difference between the
ratings given by the participants for each presentation. In
particular, statistically significant effects between the type of
presentation were found for five of the items examined (see
Figure 3): The art-inspired dish was considered as being
presented in a more complexand artisticmanner, and
was liked more than either of the other two presentations.
The expected tastiness of the food was also affected by the
presentation, with the art-inspired dish associated with sig-
nificantly higher ratings as compared to both the neat and
the regular presentations. The participants were also keen
to pay twice as much for the artistically presented dish than
for the dishes in the other presentations. It is worth men-
tioning that the ratings concerning the regular and the neat
presentations did not reveal any significant differences for
any of the questions.
Only one variable was significantly affected by the con-
sumption of the dish. The results revealed an increase of
18% in the tastiness ratings for the art-inspired presenta-
tion (6.8 ± 1.8 before consumption, 8.3 ± 1.5 after con-
sumption, this difference was statistically significant: t =
2.7, P< .01), while the ratings decreased slightly in the
regular presentation (6.0 ± 1.8 before consumption, 5.6 ±
2.2 after consumption). The latter difference was, however,
not statistically significant (t = -.5, P= .5) as shown by the
interaction plot (see Figure 4; coefficient value of the inter-
action: -.74, P < .05). For the neat presentation, the con-
sumption did not have any effect on the tastiness ratings.
Eating the food led to an increase in ratings of the tasti-
ness of the food in the case of the art-inspired dish, likely
showing that the aesthetic value of this visual arrangement
Table 1 Questionnaires used in this study
Questionnaire 1. It was presented simultaneously with the dish, and
aimed to measure the visual appeal of the food and the participants
expectations
Questionnaire 2. It was presented after the participant had finished
eating, aiming to measure different cues of the experience the
participant had eating the dish and other general impressions
How complex does this dish appear to be? How salty was the food?
How much do you like the presentation of the food? How bitter was the food?
How much would you be willing to pay for this dish (in British Pounds)? How sour was the food?
Please rate how artistically arranged you think this plate is? How sweet was the food?
How tasty does this dish look? How much did you like this dish?
How healthy do you think this dish is? How much would you be willing to pay for the dish (in British pounds)?
How tasty did you find the dish?
How full are you after eating this plate?
Do you generally enjoy eating vegetables?
How many ingredients do you think the dish contained?
Do you consider yourself to be a foodie?
How interested are you in the visual arts?
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 4 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
made the food more enjoyable to eat. The food might also
have been tastier than expected; even though the different
perceived tastes (in terms of the rated saltiness, sweetness,
bitterness, and sourness of the food) did not differ signifi-
cantly between the three conditions.
The participantsappetite level was assessed before dis-
playing the food, using a 10-point Likert scale. No signifi-
cant differences were found between the three groups
(Art-inspiredgroup: M = 5.05, SD = 2.03; Regulargroup:
M = 5.05, SD = 1.90; Neatgroup: M = 5.85, SD = 1.87). A
Kolmogorov-Smirnov test revealed that the data from
the three groups was normally distributed (P= .2 for the
Art-inspiredgroup, P= .058 for the Regulargroup, and
P= .082 for the Neatgroup).
Discussion
We compared an art-inspired food presentation to a condi-
tion where the same ingredients were arranged in a more
regular manner, or else in a neat (and hence effortful) but
non-artistic manner. Before the participants had tasted the
food, the artistic plating was liked more than both the regu-
lar and neat food presentations, as well as being recognized
as more artistic and complex than either of the other two
presentations, even though the participants were not in-
formed that the dish was supposed to mimic a work of art.
After eating, participants rated the food presented in the
art-inspired as being more flavourful.
Art-infused food design
The fact that the participants in the present study liked the
art-inspired dish more presumably reflects that they were
actually able to recognize an artistic pattern in the food in-
tuitively. The debate concerning what can be considered as
arthas, for a long time, involved philosophers, aesthetes,
psychologists, and, more recently, cognitive neuroscientists
[29,30]. It is reasonable to assume that since art involves,
at least in part, the ability to communicate feelings and
sensations [30,31], the art-inspired presentation of the
food could have been an edible rendition of the message
originally intended by Kandinsky on the canvas Painting
number 201. Indeed, the differences between ratings of
liking,artistic valueand complexitycould be attributed
to an absolute aesthetic value that would have been trans-
ferred from the painting to the food design. According to
another point of view, however, one could simply argue
that art is that which viewers categorize as such [32,33].
The concept of an identifiable pattern is not an un-
usual idea in the field of art [34], and this could have led
the participants to define the dish as being more artis-
tic because patterns (of colour and shape) were easy to
Table 2 The left part of the table shows the significant factors in the mixed models highlighting the significant effects
of the fixed variables, the right part shows the post-hoc test (Bonferroni corrected) significance values, the relevant
means and the standard deviations, values in bold indicate significant results in the comparisons
Question Significant
factors
Coefficient
value
t -value Pvalues Means ± standard deviations
for each presentation
Art versus
Regular
Art versus
Neat
Neat versus
Regular
Complexity Presentation -.97 -2.83 .0064 Art-inspired (7.5 ± 1.95) t = 2.28 t = 2.51 t = .65
Pvalue < .05 Pvalue < .01 Pvalue = .51Regular (5.7 ± 1.7)
Neat (5.2 ± 2.5)
Artistic
presentation
Presentation -.99 -2.61 .01 Art-inspired (7.9 ± 2.3) t = 3.04 t = 2.4 t = -.71
Pvalue < .01 Pvalue < .05 Pvalue = .47Regular (5.7 ± 2.2)
Neat (6.2 ± 2.12)
Liking Presentation -1.18 -3.36 .0014 Art-inspired (8.0 ± 1.8) t = 5.5 t =5.3 t = .01
Pvalue < .001 Pvalue < .01 Pvalue = .90Regular (5.6 ± 2.0)
Neat (5.5 ± 2.3)
Willingness
to pay
Presentation -116.9 -2.30 .02 Art-inspired (425 ± 511) t = 3 t = 3.2 t = 0.3
Pvalue < .01 Pvalue < .01 Pvalue = .74Regular (208 ± 283)
Neat (214 ± 268)
Tastiness Presentation -.70 -2.12 .03 Art-inspired (7.5 ± 1.8) t = 3.9 t = 4.5 t = .73
Pvalue < .01 Pvalue < .01 Pvalue = .46Regular (5.8 ± 2)
Neat (5.4 ± 2.2)
Consumption 1.8 2.40 .01
Consumption:
presentation
-.74 -2.07 .04
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 5 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
identify. If this were to have been the case, the identified
patterns could also have influenced the participantslik-
ing judgments. Although participantspersonal prefer-
ences (or taste) in art could have influenced our results,
it should be noted that the display of art has been shown
to activate reward systems in the human brain [35]. Re-
sults reported by Ramachandran [36] show that people
may experience some sort of reward when processing
visually complex stimuli (as, indeed, the art-inspired
presentation was perceived by our participants). Further-
more, in one experiment, both art experts and novices
were found to rate more complex artistic stimuli as be-
ing more interesting [31]. Indeed, the way the diners
interest is cultivated in high-end restaurants through
highly complex food preparations and presentations,
amongst other factors, is probably a key aspect of de-
signing pleasurable food experiences.
A different perspective on the effect of plating on par-
ticipantsresponses to the food would be to consider the
Art-Infusionphenomenon as advanced by Hagtvedt and
Patrick [37]. According to this theory, consumers evalu-
ate products more favourably when they are associated
with art. In this case, the art-inspired dish might have
implicitly suggested a connotation of higher value (or ef-
fort) through the visual display, value that might have
helped to deliver a more pleasurable eating experience.
Our participants were willing to pay more for the art-
istic presentation of the dish, both before and after tast-
ing it (note that the consumption of the food did not
modify peoples price estimation). These results are
consistent with previous research suggesting that the
aesthetic presentation of food can result in people
being willing to pay more for it [25]. It is important to
assess any potential explanation as to why people may
Figure 3 Bar graphs show the mean values and the standard deviations of the ratings for each of the variables that showed a statistically
significant effect of the presentation (complexity, liking, artistically arrangement, tastiness, and willingness to pay). Statistically significant
differences are highlighted (*P < .05) between the art-inspired presentation, the regular plating, and the neat presentation of the food.
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 6 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
pay more for an art-inspired dish (for example, a plate
of salad inspired by Kandinsky); for instance, we might
consider how the effortinvolved in preparing a dish
can be appreciated by a diner and, thus, change the
perceived value of the dish. Neatness and complexity
might be some of the elements that people are ready to
pay more for as well; as the philosopher Denis Dutton
puts it, the value of an artwork is rooted in the assump-
tions about the human performance underlying its cre-
ation [38].
A taste of Kandinsky
When the participants in the present study were asked
to rate the expected tastiness of the dishes before they
had sampled the food, no difference was reported between
the three conditions. Even if the visual properties of the art-
inspired condition received higher ratings than the other
two conditions, participants were not expecting it to taste
better. Interestingly, after consumption, the art-inspired pre-
sentation was rated as significantly tastier (up to 18% more)
than the other two, even though they were composed of the
same quantity of the same ingredients (see Figure 4). A
higher rating for the experienced tastiness
c
[39,40] of the
Kandinsky-inspired dish clearly shows that plating can have
an important effect on flavour perception. This observation
is consistent with previous findings [22], confirming that
what we see can indeed influence what we taste. In addition
to the arguments discussed in the previous sections, we
would argue that such percept could be the result of more
enjoyment elicited by the act of consuming an aesthetically
pleasing product, whose creation requires a more skilful and
effortful act. This result supports the theory that cultivating
uniqueness in plating and presentation could be central to
delivering pleasurable food experiences.
Art or novelty?
The higher ratings given to the art-inspired presentation
of food could also be an effect of novelty; one might won-
der if any salad that is not just mixed and placed on a plate
would seem to be more artistic. However, the elements of
a salad placed in any form would not necessarily seem
more artistic, given the risk that the plating might end up
being considered as messy, and therefore less appealing.
Indeed, Zellner and her colleagues have shown that people
are more willing to try and tend to like a neat presentation
more rather than a messy one [25].
Different food designs could be used in future research
on plating, to understand how artistry and novelty are
processed and evaluated by diners, and how this can im-
pact on the eating experience. For instance, a novel and
artistic plating could be compared to a novel but non-
artistic one.
Limitations of present research and directions for future
research
There are a number of limitations with the present study
that should be borne in mind: First, it is important to note
that mixed culinary elements on a plate (for example, two
different sauces, or one sauce and one garnish) may have
merged to create a new flavour. In this sense, the way in
which the three dishes are arranged may, in fact, have led
to their having different flavours (physically - as opposed
to any effect that they have due to the psychological im-
pact of the dish). The participants were not asked to eat
all of the food, but rather to eat as much as they wanted.
Interestingly, those participants in the art-inspired and
regular presentations tended to eat all of the food, while
they left more when presented the neat presentation; some
of the participants would try the various elements in the
dish without necessarily eating them all. It would be inter-
esting to know the extent to which seeing the various
components visually integrated, rather than separated, af-
fected consumption behaviour and probably how flavour-
ful the food ended up being perceived.
It is also important to note that the present study was
conducted in a laboratory setting, a most unusual place in
which to eat, granted, and with most of the participants
being students [41]. This might explain the large standard
deviation found for how much participants were ready to
pay for the dish (with a few of the participants being will-
ing to pay much more than the average for the dish). Al-
though the experimental set-up attempted to replicate a
restaurant table, the contextual variables of the laboratory
setting may, for instance, have influenced how much the
participants were willing to pay (presumably being lower
than would have been expected had the same dish been
served in a restaurant context)
d
. Moreover, another pos-
sible bias in the results regards the time of day at which
the participants consumed the food. Indeed, it would be
Figure 4 Interaction plot showing the effect of the consumption
of the dish on the tastiness ratings for the three presentations.
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 7 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
likely that higher ratings would have been obtained from
hungry participants and lower ratings for participants per-
forming the experiment in the early afternoon (for ex-
ample, right after lunch). However, since conditions were
randomly distributed across the time of the day, it is rea-
sonable to assume that the groups had an equal number
of hungryand fullparticipants. As a consequence, this
might have affected the variance of the data within groups,
leaving untouched the differences between groups.
Thefactthatourparticipantsreceived monetary compen-
sation for taking part in this study might also have influenced
their experience, as it is unusual experience for most of us to
be paid to eat. It would also be intriguing to see whether
preparing/presenting the dish in different contexts (for
example, science lab/gastronomy event/restaurant) would
change the way in which people respond to it as well
e
.
Future research may, in turn, take into account the differ-
ences that arise in the perceived attributes of an art-inspired
food when exactly the same dish is served in one context
versus another [9,42,43]. One question that still needs to be
addressed is whether it is possible to spot different visual
patterns in the dish, and if so, which ones in particular may
lead people to consider a dish as being artistic or not. It
would also be interesting to test how more specific cues bor-
rowed from the visual arts interact (here we are thinking of
balance, symmetry, or colour associations). Future studies
may also attempt to understand how knowing (before tast-
ing) the story about the dish and its inspiration can impact
the perception of the food. In addition, there is a need to de-
velop objective measures of the resulting complexity of food
presentations, since the various aspects of a visual arrange-
ment can affect the resulting complexity of an image, in
terms of its various components and their interaction.
Conclusions
The results of the study reported here provide evidence for
the idea that there are differences in the expectations and
consumption experience of a dish as a result of the various
elements having been artistically arranged on the plate.
Diners intuitively attribute an artistic value to the food, find
it more complex and like it more when the culinary ele-
ments are arranged to look like an abstract-art painting.
More importantly, people are ready to pay more for the food
when it is presented in an aesthetically pleasing manner,
both before and after trying it. Interestingly, consuming the
artistically arranged dish enhanced peoples assessment of
the palatability of the food.
Taking these results into account, it could be assumed
that the diners hedonic and sensory perception of a dish
is influenced by the expectations that have been estab-
lished by visual cues. Complexity and neatness could be
key aspects to produce an aesthetic display of ingredi-
ents. Furthermore, the positive values set by visual cues
seem to be transferred to the perceived flavour of foods.
Here, we argue that using artistic inspiration in the de-
sign of the culinary experience, even when used impli-
citly, can indeed enhance the enjoyment of food.
The visual appeal of food has been, and will always be, an
important matter to entice the appetite, ultimately enhan-
cing the flavours of culinary creations. While chefs rely
mostly on their intuition and expertise to plate their dishes
[7], we suggest that studying food presentations under the
lens of psychology and sensory science could give precious
insights to the so far empirical, art of plating.
Color is forever a part of our food, a visual element to
which human eyes, minds, emotions and palates are
sensitive. Perhaps through eons of time, man has come
to build up strong and intuitive associations between
what he sees and what he eats. A good meal, to say
the least, is always a beautiful sight to behold.
(Birren, 1963 [44]).
Endnotes
a
Chefs in high-end restaurants tend to put a lot of
thought in the design of the visual appearance of their
dishes to enhance the experience of flavour [7], making
this an opportunity for scientist to observe the context-
ual effects of plating on flavour perception.
b
We wanted to prove the effect of an aesthetic food
display on flavour perception. We thought that rather
than designing a food display that would look niceto
us, we would choose a visual display widely established
as being aesthetic, as the work of Kandinsky is recog-
nised to be, and transform it into a dish of food.
c
We assume that the term tastinesscould be inter-
preted in the sense of flavourfulor delicious.
d
In fact, we used the same questionnaire to assess the
perception of the art-inspired dish in the Food and Wine
Matching: A South American Perspectiveevent held at the
University of London, UK, 7 February 2013, as part of the
London Enology Series. The results of this event were
consistent with those obtained in the main experiment re-
ported here. Compared to the main study, statistically sig-
nificant differences (P< .05) were found regarding peoples
willingness to pay for the dish (£3.53 ± 3.1 in the main ex-
periment, £7.60 ± 3.3 in the event in London), as well as
the estimated number of ingredients needed to create the
dish (11.75 ± 5.9 in the main experiment versus 16.89 ± 5.9
in the event in London). Such differences may be attribut-
able to the different public that tasted the food (mostly
students in the laboratory, people interested in wine and
food in this event), but also to the different experimental
settings in which the two studies were conducted. Indeed,
in the London event, the participants were aware of the
facts concerning the preparation of the dish, such as the
painting that had inspired it and the cook preparing the
food. This knowledge could have biased their judgment
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 8 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
on the actual difficulty of preparing the dish, resulting in
their giving a higher estimate of the price and number of
ingredients in comparison with the main experimental set-
ting. (Of course, it could also be that food simply costs
more in London than in Oxford where the main study
was conducted.) This evidence suggests the importance of
the context in which people eat, as well as the importance
of the information regarding the food that they are eating,
in their evaluation of a dish.
e
In the original culinary version of this art-inspired
dish, the chef offers truffle-oil scented paintbrushes to
eat the salad. While people are comfortable eating with
the use of a paintbrush in a large social setting, prelim-
inary experiments have revealed that many of our
participants were somewhat reluctant to use such a
modernistcutlery when we tested them individually in
the laboratory.
Additional file
Additional file 1: Culinary worksheet for the Taste of Kandinskydish.
Competing interests
The authors confirm that there are no conflicts of interest.
Authorscontributions
CM created the design of the Salad with a taste of Kandinskydish used in
this study. CV, EG, and CM worked on the experimental design of the study,
under the supervision of CS. CM, CV, and EG conducted the experiment at
the Crossmodal Research Laboratory, Oxford University. CM effectuated all
the culinary preparations. EG and CV analysed the data. CM, CV, EG, and CS
were all involved in the writing of the manuscript. All authors read and
approved the final version of the manuscript.
Authorsinformation
CV DPhil candidate at the department of Experimental Psychology, University
of Oxford.
CM is a classically trained professional cook and researcher. He is the first
Chef in Residence at the Crossmodal Research Laboratory.
EG is a researcher working at Politecnico di Milano, interested in understanding
sensory experience.
CS is a Professor of Experimental Psychology, and Head of the Crossmodal
Research Laboratory at the Department of Experimental Psychology,
University of Oxford.
Acknowledgements
CS is funded by the AHRC grant Rethinking the Senseswithin the Science
in Culture theme. CM would like to thank 'Comes Cake' for the picture used
as cover image. The authors confirm that they received no external funding
for this research.
Received: 30 January 2014 Accepted: 2 May 2014
Published: 20 June 2014
References
1. Barba EB: My cuisine is tradition in evolution. [http://www.swide.com/
food-travel/chef-interview/michelin-starred-chef-an-interview-with-massimo-
bottura/2013/4/23]
2. Spence C: Multisensory flavour perception. Curr Biol 2013, 23:365369.
3. Stevenson RJ: The Psychology Of Flavour. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009.
4. Verhagen JV, Engelen L: The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal
food perception: Sensory integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006, 30:613650.
5. Köster EP: Diversity in the determinants of food choice: A psychological
perspective. Food Qual Prefer 2009, 20:7082.
6. Meiselman HL: Dimensions of the meal: The science, culture, business, and art
of eating. Gaithersburg, MA: Aspen Publishers; 2000.
7. Spence C, Piqueras-Fiszman B: The Perfect Meal: The Multisensory Science of
Food and Dining. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell; in press.
8. Herman CP, Roth DA, Polivy J: Effects of the presence of others on food
intake: A normative interpretation. Psychol Bull 2003, 129:873886.
9. Bell R, Meiselman HL, Pierson BJ, Reeve WG: Effects of adding an Italian
theme to a restaurant on perceived ethnicity, acceptability, and
selection of foods. Appetite 1994, 22:1124.
10. Wansink B, Van Ittersum K: Fast food restaurant lighting and music can
reduce calorie intake and increase satisfaction. Psychol Rep 2012, 111:15.
11. Harrar V, Spence C: The taste of cutlery. Flavour 2013, 2:21.
12. Spence C, Harrar V, Piqueras-Fiszman B: Assessing the impact of the
tableware and other contextual variables on multisensory flavour
perception. Flavour 2012, 1:112.
13. Piqueras-Fiszman B, Alcaide J, Roura E, Spence C: Is it the plate or is it the
food? Assessing the influence of the color (black or white) and shape of
the plate on the perception of the food placed on it. Food Qual Prefer
2012, 24:205208.
14. Piqueras-Fiszman B, Spence C: The influence of the color of the cup on
consumersperception of a hot beverage. J Sens Stud 2012, 27:324331.
15. Stewart P, Goss E: Plate shape and colour interact to influence taste and
quality judgments. Flavour 2013, 2:27.
16. Delwiche J: The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavour.
Food Qual Prefer 2004, 15:137146.
17. Spence C, Levitan CA, Shankar MU, Zampini M: Does colour influence taste
perception in humans? Chemosens Percept 2010, 3:6884.
18. Okajima K, Spence C: Effects of visual texture on taste perception.
i-Perception 2011, 2:966.
19. Imram N: The role of visual cues in consumer perception and acceptance
of a food product. Nutr Food Sci 1999, 99:224230.
20. Watz B: The entirety of the meal: A designer's perspective. J Foodservice
2008, 19:96104.
21. Yeomans MR, Chambers L, Blumenthal H, Blake A: The role of expectancy
in sensory and hedonic evaluation: The case of smoked salmon
ice-cream. Food Qual Prefer 2008, 19:565573.
22. Delwiche JF: You eat with your eyes first. Physiol Behav 2012, 107:502504.
23. Apicius: Cooking and Dining in Imperial Rome. (c. 1
st
Century). Translated
by Vehling JD. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1936.
24. Zellner DA, Lankford M, Ambrose L, Locher P: Art on the plate: Effect of
balance and color on attractiveness of, willingness to try and liking for
food. Food Qual Prefer 2010, 21:575578.
25. Zellner DA, Siemers E, Teran V, Conroy R, Lankford M, Agrafiotis A, Ambrose
L, Locher P: Neatness counts. How plating affects liking for the taste of
food. Appetite 2011, 57:642648.
26. Schapiro M: The nature of abstract art. In Modern art, 19th and 20th
Centuries: Selected papers. New York: George Braziller; 1937:7798.
27. Gallery label text, MoMA museum. [http://www.moma.org/collection/
browse_results.php?object_id=79452]
28. Kandinsky W: Concerning the Spiritual in Art, Especially in Painting
(1914). Translated by Sadler MTH. New York: Dover Publications; 1977.
29. Carey J: What Good Are the Arts? London: Faber & Faber; 2005.
30. Zeki S: Inner Vision: An Exploration of Art and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford
University Press; 2000.
31. Silvia PJ: Emotional responses to art: From collation and arousal to cognition
and emotion. Rev Gen Psychol 2005, 9:342357.
32. Bourdieu P, Darbel A: The Love of Art: European Art Museums and Their
Public. Oxford: Blackwell; 1997.
33. Dewey J: Having an experience.InThe Later Works, 19251953: Art as
Experience. Edited by Boydston JA, Dewey J. Carbondale: Southern Illinois
University Press; 1989:4263.
34. Behrens RR: Art, design and Gestalt theory. Leonardo 1998, 31:299303.
35. Lacey S, Hagtvedt H, Patrick VM, Anderson A, Stilla R, Deshpande G, Hu X,
Sato JR, Reddy S, Sathian K: Art for reward's sake: Visual art recruits the
ventral striatum. NeuroImage 2011, 55:420433.
36. Ramachandran VS: The Emerging Mind. London: Profile Books; 2003.
37. Hagtvedt H, Patrick VM: Art infusion: The influence of visual art on the
perception and evaluation of consumer products. J Mark Res 2008,
45:379389.
38. Dutton D: The Art Instinct, Beauty, Pleasure, and Human Evolution. New York:
Bloomsbury Press; 2009.
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 9 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
39. Spence C, Smith B, Auvray M: Confusing tastes and flavours.InThe Senses.
Edited by Matthen M, Stokes D. Oxford: Oxford University Press; in press.
40. Rozin P: Tastesmell confusionsand the duality of the olfactory sense.
Percept Psychophys 1982, 31:397401.
41. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A: The weirdest people in the world?
Behav Brain Sci 2010, 33:61135.
42. Green DM, Butts JS: Factors affecting acceptability of meals served in the air.
JAmDietAssoc1945, 21:415419.
43. Meiselman HL, Johnson JL, Reeve W, Crouch JE: Demonstrations of the
influence of the eating environment on food acceptance. Appetite 2000,
35:231237.
44. Birren F: Color and human appetite. Food Technol 1963, 17:4547.
doi:10.1186/2044-7248-3-7
Cite this article as: Michel et al.:A taste of Kandinsky: assessing the
influence of the artistic visual presentation of food on the dining
experience. Flavour 2014 3:7.
Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central
and take full advantage of:
Convenient online submission
Thorough peer review
No space constraints or color figure charges
Immediate publication on acceptance
Inclusion in PubMed, CAS, Scopus and Google Scholar
Research which is freely available for redistribution
Submit your manuscript at
www.biomedcentral.com/submit
Michel et al. Flavour 2014, 3:7 Page 10 of 10
http://www.flavourjournal.com/content/3/1/7
... In addition, although the specific effect of packaging appeal on product liking has not been explicitly investigated yet, some studies have explored how enhancing food appeal through design makes the food more palatable (Di Cicco et al., 2020;Michel et al., 2014;Techawachirakul et al., 2023;Zellner et al., 2010Zellner et al., , 2011Zellner et al., , 2014. For example, Michel et al. (2014) showed that plating inspired by a work of art increased the tastiness ratings of the food served on it, and Zellner et al. (2014) demonstrated that consumers reported liking the same food more when the plating was rated as more attractive. ...
... In addition, although the specific effect of packaging appeal on product liking has not been explicitly investigated yet, some studies have explored how enhancing food appeal through design makes the food more palatable (Di Cicco et al., 2020;Michel et al., 2014;Techawachirakul et al., 2023;Zellner et al., 2010Zellner et al., , 2011Zellner et al., , 2014. For example, Michel et al. (2014) showed that plating inspired by a work of art increased the tastiness ratings of the food served on it, and Zellner et al. (2014) demonstrated that consumers reported liking the same food more when the plating was rated as more attractive. Furthermore, the literature suggests that appealing packaging increases the hedonic value of the product it contains (Zhao et al., 2019), positively influences brand preference by enhancing attitudes towards the product (Wang, 2013), and increases purchase intention (Bigoin-Gagnan and Lacoste-Badie, 2018). ...
... This research shows that implied motion imagery can increase product liking by enhancing design appeal. Previous studies have examined how liking is influenced by the impact of aesthetics (Michel et al., 2014;Zellner et al., 2010Zellner et al., , 2011Zellner et al., , 2014 or certain specific packaging cues (Di Cicco et al., 2020;Gil-P� erez et al., 2020), but the specific effect of packaging design appeal was yet to be assessed (for a discussion of aesthetics as a topic in psychology and neuroscience, see Skov and Nadal, 2020). This finding is consistent with other research suggesting that attractive packaging draws attention and can increase the hedonic value of the products it contains (Stoll et al., 2008), as well as the perceived product quality (Wang, 2013). ...
Article
Purpose This paper aims to investigate how implying movement in food packaging imagery may affect product liking. Furthermore, the underlying mechanism is investigated by studying the effect of implied motion visuals on design appeal and naturalness perception. Design/methodology/approach Two packages of pineapple juice were designed in which the implied motion depicted in their imagery was manipulated, and a tasting experiment was conducted in which two samples of the same juice were evaluated. Findings The results show that the effect of packaging imagery on product liking occurs indirectly through both design appeal and the product naturalness perception. The results of a parallel multiple-mediator analysis show that (1) depicting implied motion made the package be perceived as more appealing, (2) the product corresponding to the package depicting implied motion was perceived as being more natural, and (3) both effects equally contributed to the positive effect of visuals depicting implied motion on product liking. Originality/value Overall, these findings widen our understanding of the effects of packaging design on product liking and may help both designers and manufacturers design more appropriate packaging for their products.
... Dentro desse panorama, muitos pesquisadores das ciências que envolvem os alimentos vêm estudando os alimentos e as características de sua percepção usando como ferramentas análises e grupos de foco. As pesquisas vão desde apresentações de pratos de maneira artística e suas influências na escolha, como o registrado por Michel et al. (2014), até a aplicação de estímulos sonoros durante uma degustação e os efeitos que eles causam, assim feito por Leonor (2014) que estudou a nostalgia causada pelos sons em uma refeição e Campinho (2018) que detectou variações de sensações de doçura e acidez com a mudança de músicas ambientes. ...
... The last area of this study is presentation. Presentation refers to the aesthetically attractive element of food [53,54]. Food presentation is related to the first impression of food, which makes consumers pay attention to the food [14,15]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This work aimed to explore the food quality attributes of in-flight meals and to examine the antecedents of positive emotion and continuous usage of these meals. As a subdimension, this study uses multiple attributes: menu diversity, familiarity, temperature, nutrition, and presentation. Another purpose of this work is to examine the moderating effect of menu diversity on the relationship between nutrition and continuance usage. A survey via clickworker was used to collect the data for this work. There were 317 valid observations for statistical inference. This study used a structural equation model to test the hypotheses, and the Hayes process model macro 1 was adopted to test the moderating effect. The results showed that all independent variables other than familiarity significantly accounted for positive emotion. Moreover, all of these attributes had a positive impact on continuous usage. This work unveiled a significant moderating effect of menu diversity on the relationship between nutrition and continuance usage. This research elucidates the literature by clarifying the influential attributes of emotion and continuous usage intention in the domain of in-flight meal products and discussing practical implications.
Article
The rapidly increasing number of elderly people in the world highlights the need for the development of innovative foods with modified textures that do not expose the elderly to the risks associated with food consumption (risk of aspiration, suffocation, and chocking). Providing specific food such as edible gel for the elderly population and the study of their properties is a challenge for the scientific community. There are some available gels in the supermarkets destined for the sports population, with specific texture and technological properties that could be used and extrapolated for senior people. To explore this potential, five types of sport commercial gels purchased from a local Slovak market were characterized in order to evaluate their technological properties and to know if these types of gels are suitable for the senior population. The energy gels were evaluated using acceptance testing, involving 75 seniors who evaluated important organoleptic attributes by a combination of hedonic and intensity scales. The same consumer panel then profiled the gels using the Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS) technique. The prevalence of food neophobia was measured with the Food Neophobia Scale (FNS) and also using neuroimaging and biometric methods. The results suggest that there are significant differences in the perception of edible gels, as confirmed by measurements via electroencephalography (EEG) and Facereading. We conclude by suggesting the potential of specific foods such as edible gels for the elderly population as our findings also confirm that the composition of these specific and sustainable foods may elicit different perceptions. This highlights the need to use biometric and neuroimaging methods in food research in order to create more optimal formulations for specific populations.
Chapter
The “eatertainment” phenomenon has become widespread as one of the latest trends for many restaurant brands. The main goal of the present work is to examine the relationship between the visual presentation of food and its influence on the multidimensional notion of brand experience. The research was exploratory and three focus group discussions were held. Findings revealed that food presentation enhances various dimensions of brand experience, although its impact varies: it significantly influences sensory brand experiences, mainly the visual and taste perceptions; it is less prominent in affective brand experiences, where other factors play a more significant role; it also shapes behavioral brand experiences, encouraging photography, online sharing, and culinary exploration; and its impact on intellectual brand experiences varies, with some seeing it inspiring and others merely aesthetic. However, plating is just one aspect of a holistic brand experience, necessitating a multifaceted strategy for contemporary diners.
Article
Full-text available
In the research, 8 dishes from Turkish Cuisine (İmambayıldı and zeytinyağlı enginar (artichoke with olive oil) for the olive oil theme, Adana kebab and Hünkâr beğendi for the main course theme, fırında sütlaç (baked rice pudding) and pumpkin dessert for the dessert theme, çay (Turkish tea) and Turkish coffee for the beverage theme) were produced separately using Adobe Firefly 3 and Dall-E 3 Artificial Intelligence (AI) applications, and the same number of real food photographs were added to them. Thirty-one volunteer professional food stylists and photographers participated in the study. Consequently, a total of 24 food images were created and evaluated by professionals according to aesthetic criteria. The participants indicated that there is no significant difference between food photos produced according to AI 1 application and real food photos. There was no significant difference between half of the photos produced with the AI 2 application and real food photos. There is a significant difference between 5 food images between the two AI applications. Participants emphasize low cost, fast and flexible production as strengths in the use of AI applications in food styling and photography. Weaknesses include surreal image production and aesthetic concerns. The potential opportunities include the acquisition of new perspectives, innovation and creativity. Conversely, the potential threats to be considered include ethical and moral concerns and copyright issues, as well as the risk of over-dependence on AI tools and the possibility of job losses.
Article
Full-text available
Bu çalışma, nörogastronomi kavramını ve gastronomi alanındaki uygulamalarını inceleyerek, nörogastronominin tüketici algısı üzerindeki etkilerini ve ulusal literatürdeki nörogastronomi çalışmalarının bibliyometrik analizini yapmayı amaçlamaktadır. Araştırma kapsamında, Ulusal Tez Merkezi, TR Dizin ve Dergipark'ta yayımlanmış nörogastronomi temalı çalışmalar incelenmiştir. Tarih sınırlaması yapılmaksızın, 2 yüksek lisans tezi, 2 doktora tezi ve 9 makale olmak üzere toplam 13 çalışma araştırmaya dahil edilmiştir. Bu çalışmalar, nörogastronominin tüketici algısı üzerindeki etkisini gösteren örneklerle analiz edilmiştir. İncelenen literatürler, nörogastronominin tüketici algısı üzerindeki etkilerini çeşitli örnekler üzerinden göstermektedir. Lezzet, aroma, tat, koku ve dil temaları sıkça rastlanan konular arasında yer almaktadır. Nörogastronomi, yalnızca koklama ve tatma duyularını değil, aynı zamanda tüm duyuları, algıyı ve hafızayı da içeren geniş bir kavramdır. Çalışma, nörogastronominin gelişimini, tat ve lezzet algısını oluşturan unsurları, koku algısını, işitsellik ve görselliği, tat ve lezzet algısını etkileyen parametreleri analiz etmektedir. Nörogastronominin gastronomi turizmine ve yiyecek içecek işletmelerine yönelik potansiyel etkileri değerlendirilmiştir. Bu bağlamda, nörogastronominin, gastronomi alanında yenilikçi uygulamalara ve tüketici deneyimini zenginleştirmeye yönelik katkılar sağlayabileceği sonucuna varılmıştır. Abstract This study aims to examine the concept of neurogastronomy and its applications in the field of gastronomy, as well as to analyze the impact of neurogastronomy on consumer perception and perform a bibliometric analysis of national literature on neurogastronomy. The research included neurogastronomy-themed studies published in the National Thesis Center, TR Index, and Dergipark. A total of 13 studies, including 2 master's theses, 2 doctoral theses, and 9 articles, were examined without any date limitation. These studies were analyzed with examples demonstrating the impact of neurogastronomy on consumer perception. The reviewed literature indicates that neurogastronomy affects consumer perception through various examples. Themes of taste, aroma, flavor, smell, and tongue frequently appear. Neurogastronomy is a broad concept encompassing not only the senses of smell and taste but also all the senses, perception, and memory. This study analyzes the development of neurogastronomy, the elements that constitute taste and flavor perception, smell perception, and the auditory and visual aspects. It evaluates the potential impacts of neurogastronomy on gastronomy tourism and food and beverage establishments. The conclusion is that neurogastronomy can contribute to innovative applications in the field of gastronomy and enrich consumer experiences.
Article
Full-text available
Background Research has demonstrated that factors external to the food source can influence consumers’ perceptions of food. Contextual factors including cutlery or tableware (for example, size and composition), the atmosphere (for example, noise levels and odours), and packaging (for example, shape and colour) have all been shown to influence the perceptual experience. Plateware has also been shown to influence taste perception since ratings of a dessert (strawberry mousse) were modified by plate colour but not by plate shape. In the current study, which used a 2 × 2 between-subjects design, the effect of plate colour (black versus white) and plate shape (round versus square) on taste perception is re-examined. Through sweetness, intensity, quality, and liking ratings of cheesecake, the current study extends the previous investigation to include an examination of the plate colour by plate shape interaction while using plates with more angular corners.
Article
Flavour is arguably the most fascinating aspect of eating and drinking. It utilises a complex variety of senses and processes, that incredibly work together to generate a unified, and hopefully pleasurable, experience. The processes involved are not just those involved in tasting at the time of eating, but also memory and learning processes - we obviously shun those foods of which we have a negative memory, and favour those we enjoy. Our understanding of the science of flavour has improved in recent years, benefiting psychology, cuisine, food science, oenology, and dietetics. This book describes what is known about the psychology and biology of flavour. The book is divided into two parts. The first explores what we know about the flavour system; including the role of learning and memory in flavour perception and hedonics; the way in which all the senses that contribute to flavour interact, and our ability to perceive flavour as a whole and as a series of parts. The later chapters examine a range of theoretical issues concerning the flavour system. This includes a look at multisensory processing, and the way in which the mind and brain bind information from discrete sensory systems. It also examines the broader implications of studying flavour for societal problems such as obesity.
Article
Food color affects taste perception. However, the possible effects of the visual texture of a foodstuff on taste and flavor, without associated changes to color, are currently unknown. We conducted a series of experiments designed to investigate how the visual texture and appearance of food influences its perceived taste and flavor by developing an Augmented Reality system. Participants observed a video of tomato ketchup as a food stimulus on a white dish placed behind a flat LC-display on which was mounted a video camera. The luminance distribution of the ketchup in the dynamic video was continuously and quantitatively modified by tracking specified colors in real-time. We changed the skewness of the luminance histogram of each frame in the video keeping the xy-chromaticity values intact. Participants watched themselves dip a spoon into the ketchup from the video feed (which could be altered), but then ate it with their eyes closed. They reported before and after tasting the ketchup on the perceived consistency (a liquid to solid continuum) the food looked and felt and how tasty it looked or felt. The experimental results suggest that visual texture, independent of color, affects the taste and flavor as well as the appearance of foods.
Article