ArticlePDF Available

Ensuring language acquisition for deaf children: What linguists can do

Authors:

Abstract

Parents of small deaf children need guidance on constructing home and school environments that affect normal language acquisition. They often turn to physicians and spiritual leaders and, increasingly, the internet. These sources can be underinformed about crucial issues, such as matters of brain plasticity connected to the risk of linguistic deprivation, and delay or disruption in the development of cognitive skills interwoven with linguistic ability. We have formed a team of specialists in education, linguistics, pediatric medicine, and psychology, and at times specialists in theology and in law have joined our group. We argue that deaf children should be taught a sign language in the early years. This does not preclude oral-aural training and assistive technology. With a strong first language (a sign language), the child can become bilingual (with the written form of the ambient spoken language and, perhaps, the spoken form), accruing the benefits of bilingualism. We have published in medical journals, addressing primary care physicians, in a journal with a spiritual-leader readership, and in a health-law journal. Articles in progress address medical educators and practitioners. Team members present findings at conferences, work on lobbying and legislative efforts with the National Association of the Deaf, and spread the word at conferences of target audiences. We share our work in Word format, so that anyone can easily appropriate it for our common interests. One of our articles has been downloaded over 27,000 times (as of April 2014), and we are asked to consult with committees in other countries as they draft national policies.
e31
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY
Ensuring language acquisition for deaf children: What linguists can do
TOM HUMPHRIES POORNA KUSHALNAGAR GAURAV MATHUR
University of California, Rochester Institute Gallaudet University
San Diego of Technology
DONNA JONAPOLI CAROL PADDEN CHRISTIAN RATHMANN
Swarthmore College University of California, University of Hamburg
San Diego
Parents of small deaf children need guidance on constructing home and school environments
that affect normal language acquisition. They often turn to physicians and spiritual leaders and, in-
creasingly, the internet. These sources can be underinformed about crucial issues, such as matters
of brain plasticity connected to the risk of linguistic deprivation, and delay or disruption in the de-
velopment of cognitive skills interwoven with linguistic ability. We have formed a team of spe-
cialists in education, linguistics, pediatric medicine, and psychology, and at times specialists in
theology and in law have joined our group. We argue that deaf children should be taught a sign
language in the early years. This does not preclude oral-aural training and assistive technology.
With a strong first language (a sign language), the child can become bilingual (with the written
form of the ambient spoken language and, perhaps, the spoken form), accruing the benefits of
bilingualism. We have published in medical journals, addressing primary care physicians, in a
journal with a spiritual-leader readership, and in a health-law journal. Articles in progress address
medical educators and practitioners. Team members present findings at conferences, work on lob-
bying and legislative efforts with the National Association of the Deaf, and spread the word at
conferences of target audiences. We share our work in Word format, so that anyone can easily ap-
propriate it for our common interests. One of our articles has been downloaded over 27,000 times
(as of April 2014), and we are asked to consult with committees in other countries as they draft na-
tional policies.
Keywords: deaf children’s rights, first language acquisition, brain plasticity, sign languages, ethics
and activism in scholarship
1. The policy problem. We argue that beliefs about spoken and sign languages
among underinformed professionals have serious consequences; parents are advised to
make decisions and construct home and school environments that affect normal lan-
guage acquisition among deaf children. In the United States, around 96% of deaf chil-
dren are born to hearing parents (Moores 2001) who have no family history of using a
sign language. The remaining 4% are born to deaf parents, and in most cases, though
not always, the primary language of the home is a sign language. Many hearing parents
are initially uninformed about fundamental language matters and turn to the medical
profession, the internet, their spiritual leaders, and/or their friends and family for advice
about the language choices they need to make for their children (Luterman 1979, Gre-
gory 1995, Porter & Edirippulige 2007). Too often, those they turn to are under- or mis-
informed about the language needs of deaf children (Meader & Zazove 2005). Parents
are often told that the best way for their child to acquire spoken language is to raise
them without sign language. In many cases, parents are advised that sign is to be chosen
only as a last resort (Petitto 1998, Johnston 2006), and that great effort should be de-
voted instead to the acquisition of speech. Given that these parents are hearing and un-
familiar with deaf people’s lives and sign languages, many opt for the more typical oral
and/or aural choice (speech and audition only).
Printed with the permission of Tom Humphries, Poorna Kushalnagar, Gaurav Mathur, Donna Jo Napoli,
Carol Padden, & Christian Rathmann. © 2014.
e32 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
In addition, over 80% of deaf children in developed countries receive cochlear im-
plants (CIs), and the percentage is increasing (Boyes Braem & Rathmann 2010). CI is
now the treatment of choice in the medical sciences for most children with sensori-
neural hearing loss (SNHL) (Niparko 2009), and sign language is seen as both a barrier
to learning speech and a symptom of treatment failure (Broesterhuizen & Leuven 2008).
The most frequent recommendation is to isolate deaf children from sign language envi-
ronments during the important years of first language acquisition (Wrigley 1997, Padden
& Humphries 2005, The Canadian Hearing Society 2005, Krausneker 2008).
However, CI has a variable rate of success with respect to long-term language devel-
opment. (Rather than interrupt the discussion with a long list here, we indicate these ref-
erences with two asterisks in the bibliography.) The factors involved in CI success are
not well understood, although age of the patient (Tomblin et al. 2005, Vermeire et al.
2005, Nicholas & Geers 2007, and many others), onset of deafness (Leung et al. 2005,
Green et al. 2007), coding strategies (Skinner et al. 2002), family socioeconomic-
education level (Svirsky et al. 2004, Szagun 2008), and surgical technique (Meshik et
al. 2010) are relevant. Even under optimal conditions, CI implantation does not guaran-
tee first language acquisition. Many implanted children who are born deaf or become
deaf in the first few years of life experience little to no success in language acquisition
with a CI, and only turn to sign language after the early critical period. Unfortunately,
this means these children run the risk of never having completely fluent use of either a
spoken or a sign language.
Further, not meeting the language needs of deaf children can mean harm to their
psycho-social health, putting them at risk for depression, behavioral problems, social
disorders, and juvenile delinquency (Northern & Downs 2002, Andrews et al. 2003,
Schick et al. 2006, Leigh 2009). They are more likely to engage in criminal behavior in
later life (Kleimenov & Shamkov 2005, Miller et al. 2005), to be the target of abuse of
various sorts (Sullivan & Knutson 2000, Knutson et al. 2004, Kvam 2004), and to rely
on the social services safety net. Long term, language access is critical for the partici-
pation of deaf people in preventive health and health care services (Iezzoni et al. 2004,
McKee, Barnett, et al. 2011, McKee, Schlehofer, et al. 2011), education (Oliva 2004),
mental health care (Steinberg et al. 1998), the workplace (Rashid et al. 2011, Haynes &
Linden 2012), and social relationships (Gerich & Fellinger 2012).
Additionally, failure to acquire language in the early years results in delay or disrup-
tion in the development of cognitive skills that interweave with linguistic ability. Such
children have trouble with verbal memory organization (Rönnberg 2003), mastery of
numeracy and literacy (MacSweeney 1998), and higher-order cognitive processing
such as executive function and theory of mind (Courtin 2000, 2010, Courtin & Melot
2005, Morgan & Kegl 2006, Schick et al. 2007, Courtin et al. 2008, Figueras et al.
2008, Marschark & Hauser 2008, Remmel & Peters 2009).
Globally, SNHL is one of the most common among those birth conditions labeled
‘defects’ by the medical profession. Profound SNHL occurs in two or three out of 1,000
newborns in North America (National Institutes of Health 2011) and is as high as three
out of 1,000, depending on the severity threshold used in a given study and whether uni-
lateral hearing loss is included (Spivak 2007, Kozak et al. 2009). In Germany, profound
SNHL occurs in one to three out of 1,000 newborns (Schnell-Inderst et al. 2006). In
Nigeria, a striking number of twenty-eight per 1,000 infants have permanent congenital
and early-onset hearing loss (Olusanya et al. 2008). Poverty, combined with many other
factors, produces higher levels of SNHL; lower socioeconomic areas around the world
are home to higher numbers of people with SNHL (for Canada, see Bowd 2005; for
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e33
India, see Reddy et al. 2006; for Malawi, see van Hasselt & van Kregten 2002; for Pa-
kistan, see Musani et al. 2011; for the United States, see many, especially Oghalai et al.
2002 and Prince et al. 2003). Most deaf and hard-of-hearing children live in developing
countries (Jauhiainen 2001, Tucci et al. 2010). Nevertheless, in developing countries,
an increasing percentage of deaf children do get CIs, and there is an outcry among the
medical profession for CI funding (Garg et al. 2011, Saunders & Barrs 2011). Postnatal
causes of SNHL include bacterial meningitis, beta-hemolytic streptococcal sepsis, tox-
ins, trauma, and late onset due to gene mutation (Paqarkar et al. 2006); by school age,
six to seven out of 1,000 children have permanent hearing loss, most of which is SNHL
(Bamford et al. 2007).
Given all of these frequency data and the trend toward speech-only training in med-
ical settings, it is clear that a significant number of children in the world with SNHL are
likely to be given CIs and kept away from sign language during their early years, and,
consequently, run a high risk of linguistic deprivation and related cognitive deficits.
2. The linguistic evidence that informs this policy problem. Before entering
into the linguistic evidence, it is important to recognize nonlinguistic debates concern-
ing language choices for deaf children. The Food and Drug Administration (FDA) ap-
proved the use of CIs in adults in 1984, in children aged two and above in 1990, and in
children aged twelve months and above in 2000. Almost this entire time, there has been
a controversy revolving around the question of whether CIs would remove a child from
Deaf communities and eventually threaten Deaf communities with extinction (Wine-
field 1987, Grant 2008). There has also been much discussion over ethical concerns of
CIs that go beyond linguistic issues and surgical-risk issues (Christiansen & Leigh
2002). Here, we set these debates aside not because they are wrong-minded, but be-
cause they obfuscate the linguistic issues, which, by themselves, are straightforward
and compelling.
With respect to the linguistic evidence, two points can be made, one involving recog-
nition of the fact that both the oral-aural and the manual-visual modalities of language
nourish the brain’s language mechanism, and the other involving the recognition of
changing plasticity in the brain with respect to first language acquisition.
2.1. Two modalities of language. First, language and the brain are flexible with
respect to modality. Both spoken and sign languages can nurture brain development, as
is shown by much research on the structure of particular spoken and sign languages and
on language universals (see a multitude of articles in many linguistics journals, includ-
ing Sign Language & Linguistics and Sign Language Studies, as well as more recently
in journals that do not focus on sign languages, such as Language; and see a variety of
comprehensive books, such as Sandler & Lillo-Martin 2006, Brentari 2010, Pfau et al.
2012), on language acquisition (Newport & Meier 1985, Meier & Newport 1990, Pe-
titto & Marentette 1991, Lillo-Martin 1999, among many others), on language process-
ing (Emmorey 2001, among many others), on neurolinguistics (Poizner et al. 1987,
Neville 1995, among many others), on language pathologies (Corina 1998, among
many others), and on second language learning (Newport 1990). (We have chosen to
cite seminal works, which laid the foundation for much following research.)
Too often in the relevant medical literature, we find the confused belief that language
is equivalent to speech despite a half-century of research on sign languages. For exam-
ple, consider this statement from Kral and O’Donoghue (2011:485): ‘Nonetheless, the
available evidence suggests that early intervention through sensory restoration offers
the best hope of mitigating the pernicious effects of hearing deprivation on multiple lev-
els of brain function’. The authors recognize that absence of hearing can lead to absence
of language, which can, in turn, lead to cognitive deficits, but they see ‘sensory restora-
tion’ (i.e. auditory restoration) as the only way to ensure language and to prevent cogni-
tive deficits that follow from absence of language input. This quotation is representative
of the basic misconception that equates language with speech. Published policy state-
ments about deaf children recommend early screening; early intervention; close and
continued monitoring of the child’s communicative, language, motor, cognitive, and
social-emotional development; and protection of infant and family rights through in-
formed choice, decision making, and consent (Early Hearing Detection and Interven-
tion Information & Resource Center 2004, Joint Committee on Infant Hearing 2007,
Department of Health and Human Services 2009, and so on). Frequently, such recom-
mendations discuss almost exclusively audio-verbal therapy (AVT) via habituation and
vocal output, although more recent policy statements emphasize cognitive language de-
velopment and the importance of nurturing and communicating with the child regard-
less of modality. Nevertheless, primary care physicians express a lack of confidence in
discussing follow-up procedures and intervention needs for deaf newborns because of
their lack of familiarity with deafness (Moeller et al. 2006), and thus immediately refer
the parents to audiologists, whose primary concern is auditory input, often with no or
only skeptical recommendations of looking into sign language options. Evidence that
there are at least two modalities that offer a normal pathway to language acquisition is
often disregarded, leading to a failure to understand and take advantage of the flexibil-
ity of the human brain.
2.2. First language acquisition and plasticity. The second relevant linguistic
point with respect to the policy problem is that first language acquisition takes place
most naturally and successfully in the first few years of life; if a child is not exposed to
accessible or learnable language on a regular and frequent basis before the age of
around five years old, that child is unlikely to ever use any language with native-like
fluency across the grammar (Lenneberg 1964, 1967, Mayberry 1994, 1998, Hall &
Johnston 2009, Hudson & Newport 2009). Over the years we see a gradual decline in
the ability to acquire a first language (note that a second language is a separate matter
with distinct considerations—our concern here is first language acquisition). Some
areas of the grammar seem to be resilient; that is, even in the absence of early input,
they can be mastered later in life (see Goldin-Meadow 2003, 2005), such as word order,
while other areas of language are more fragile and, without input in the very early years,
tend to never get mastered, such as complex morphology, as in verb agreement (Wood
2007, 2011). Evidence for this sensitive (or critical) period comes from children whose
language development is somehow special, and from children who have been neglected
and/or abused.
Aphasic, bilingual, and deaf individuals. Lenneberg (1967) reported that chil-
dren with acquired aphasia can recover completely, but adults cannot, concluding that
there must be a critical period for language acquisition. Later research on aphasia shows
variable recovery from aphasia with children (Woods & Carey 1979, for example), but
worse prognosis for adults (Martins 2004). Other works on aphasia likewise support a
critical period for first language acquisition (Alajouanine & Lhermitte 1965, and
Goorhuis-Brouwer 1976, a study written in Dutch and reported on in English in Snow
& Hoefnagel-Höhle 1978).
Similarly, evidence on bilingualism supports the existence of a sensitive period. In a
study of twenty-year-olds comparing monolinguals, early bilinguals (before the age of
e34 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
ten), and late bilinguals, early bilinguals and monolinguals displayed the same level of
proficiency in English and a greater proficiency than that of late bilinguals. Further, the
age of onset of bilingualism was negatively correlated to English proficiency across all
bilinguals (Luk et al. 2011).
Finally, and most important to us, studies of deaf children who did not receive acces-
sible language until after the critical period due to lack of hearing aids (Curtiss 1994,
Grimshaw et al. 1998) or because they were denied sign language (Mayberry & Fischer
1989, Emmorey & Corina 1990, Newport 1990, Emmorey 1991, Mayberry & Eichen
1991, Wood 2007, 2011, among many others) show reduced language facility. Deaf
children who were first exposed to an accessible language (i.e. a sign language) at vary-
ing ages show varying degrees of mastery of language as they age, with early learners
doing far better than late learners overall (Newport & Supalla 1987, Johnson & New-
port 1989, Newport 1990, 1991, Boyes Braem 1999, Galvan 1999, Helmuth 2001,
Newport et al. 2001, Singleton & Newport 2004, Morford & Hänel-Faulhaber 2011,
Wood 2011, Cormier et al. 2012, Skotara et al. 2012).
Neglected and/or abused individuals. Other evidence for the first critical period
comes from unfortunate incidents of neglect and abuse so severe that children did not
acquire any language by the end of the first critical period, and thus were linguistically
deprived and accordingly severely limited in their interactions with other humans and
in their cognitive functions. These include cases of children discovered growing up
‘wild’ without being surrounded by human language (Shattuck 1980) and cases of chil-
dren criminally abused (Curtiss 1977). Alarge-scale incidence of this is the case of chil-
dren left in profoundly understaffed orphanages in Romania, where in 1999 it is
estimated that there were at least 60,000 children languishing in state-run orphanages
(Cohn 2011). In 2000, the Bucharest Early Intervention Project placed some of the chil-
dren with foster families (Zeanah et al. 2003). They then studied the development of
three groups: children remaining in orphanages, children in foster homes, and a control
group of children with their original parents. Their research shows that early institu-
tional neglect led to cognitive and socio-emotional deficits and psychiatric disorders.
While foster intervention enhanced development, some areas of neural activity, cogni-
tion, and social-emotional functioning were resistant to recovery unless intervention
took place before the age of two years. Language development fell among those func-
tions. Studies of similar orphanages in China and Russia confirm these findings (Nelson
et al. 2007).
Further, a recent study of the Bucharest Early Intervention Project (Drury et al. 2012)
shows that the telomeres (the protective caps on the ends of chromosomes) of children
in the Romanian orphanages grew shorter the longer they remained there. In other
words, neglect has a biological effect; in particular, it changes the architecture of the
brain. This is pertinent evidence that without proper cognitive nourishment, cortical ac-
tivities are curtailed. In particular, the language mechanism no longer functions prop-
erly for fluid receptive and expressive language behavior.
2.3. Relevance to the policy problem. The combination of these two facts, that
cognitive ability can develop in either language modality and that there is a sensitive
period for first language acquisition (regardless of whether abuse or neglect is in-
volved), is of crucial relevance to the problem. While the first fact is by and large ig-
nored in the literature that favors CI, the second fact has long been accepted. Much
research has shown better auditory results with earlier implantation; this has been the
spur to implanting children before the age of two, and often before the age of one
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e35
(Yoshinaga-Itano et al. 1998, Yoshinaga-Itano et al. 2000, Waltzman & Roland 2005,
among many others). The crucial problem is that even with early implantation, the level
of aided hearing is less than optimal, which makes acquisition of a spoken language im-
perfect and difficult and, most of all, unpredictable (Santarelli et al. 2008). The problem
is magnified if the child’s environment is noisy and unclear. The bottom line is that
many children do not acquire a spoken language fully with a CI, and one cannot predict
with reliability which children fall into that group. Even work that is explicitly support-
ive of CI includes statements such as ‘there remains huge, unexplained, variation in
outcomes from implantation and the challenges of ensuring life-long use and benefit re-
main’ (Archbold & O’Donoghue 2009:457). For this reason, the failure of the relevant
medical professionals to recognize the viability of sign languages means that these chil-
dren run a risk of, and indeed often experience, linguistic deprivation. But sign lan-
guages are viable human languages, with all of the cognitive benefits attributed to
spoken languages. Further, sign languages are accessible to all deaf children, even to
the deaf-blind child, since there are tactile versions of sign languages (Mesch 2001). If
deaf children acquire a sign language during the early years of life, they will not risk
linguistic deprivation and the consequent cognitive deficits.
Many studies show that deaf children who sign achieve better in school than those
who do not, regardless of other factors (such as whether their parents are deaf or hear-
ing and whether they have assistive hearing devices and/or oral training) (Padden &
Ramsey 2000, Strong & Prinz 2000, Mayer & Akamatsu 2003, Paul 2003, Schick 2003,
Allen et al. 2007, Wilbur 2008). Indeed, ASL skill above other possible factors corre-
lates strongly with reading achievement (Chamberlain & Mayberry 2008).
Moreover, the deaf child who acquires a sign language and then learns the written and,
perhaps, spoken form of a spoken language is bilingual. Bilingualism has great benefits
for the deaf child in cognitive, social, and educational areas (Wilbur 2001, Christiansen
& Leigh 2002). In fact, both the sign language and the spoken language of bilingual deaf
children display more syntactic complexity than that of their monolingual peers (Klatter-
Folmer et al. 2006). In addition, the evidence that high proficiency in two or more
languages results in more creative thinking in problem solving, and better mental flexi-
bility and cognitive control that persists through late adulthood, is firm (Cummins & Gu-
lustan 1974, Prinz & Strong 1998, Bialystok et al. 2004, Baker 2006, Lightbown & Spada
2006, Bialystok et al. 2007, Kushalnagar, Hannay, & Hernandez 2010). All around the
world children are raised multilingually, and the bilingual-bicultural trend for deaf edu-
cation is a mega-trend (Munoz-Baell et al. 2008). Dual proficiency in a sign language,
such as American Sign Language, and in a spoken language, such as English, affords the
deaf child the benefit of adapting to both signing and nonsigning peer groups with greater
ease, resulting in better overall socio-emotional and behavioral development (Marschark
2009). Information of this sort will, we hope, disarm those who are strongly attached to
the promotion of CI-only choice.
3. Recommendations that follow from the evidence. One basic recommenda-
tion follows from the evidence.
Overall recommendation:All deaf newborns and newly deafened small chil-
dren should learn a sign language, regardless of whether they receive a CI or a
hearing aid.
Several more specific recommendations follow from this basic one.
(1) Medical education must be updated and include linguistic considerations. Med-
ical professionals should be trained in recent research about language acquisition, par-
e36 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
ticularly with respect to the issues of linguistic deprivation for those children at risk,
primarily deaf children. Medical schools, nursing schools, and schools of public health
should include this information in their curriculum.
(2) Delivery of medical care to deaf children should be coordinated across the rele-
vant health professionals, including audiologists, psychologists, surgeons, and rehabili-
tation teams. These teams should stay in constant contact with and respond to input
from parents, sign language teachers, and classroom teachers. This way, the risk of lin-
guistic deprivation can be caught early and responded to appropriately.
(3) Advice from medical professionals must be accurate and adequate. Parents of
deaf newborns and newly deafened small children should be advised to teach their child
sign language, regardless of whether the child also uses hearing aids or a CI. This
means the entire family should learn sign language; and since the biological health of
the language mechanism is at stake, this is properly a medical matter, so it is the med-
ical profession’s responsibility to tell the parents this.
When the entire family uses sign language at the dinner table, for example, the deaf
child has visual access and picks up on incidental information on a variety of topics.
Developmentally, the inclusion of the child in family dialogues promotes healthy psy-
chosocial and emotional functioning (Hauser et al. 2010). The deaf child is likely to feel
included in family conversations and is less frustrated, as is commonly reported in other
situations with communication barriers. This has been self-reported as having an im-
portant impact on the deaf youth’s quality of life, and the perception of being included
in family dialogues is associated with fewer reports of depression symptomatology
(Kushalnagar et al. 2011). Deaf children whose hearing parents and siblings, particu-
larly hearing mothers, sign with them demonstrate language expressiveness and theory
of mind on a par with hearing children of the same age (Spencer 1993, Schick et al.
2007).
(4) More research needs to be done on second language learning, especially in a sec-
ond modality. Second language learning is difficult for adults (Krashen 1981 and later
work by many), perhaps even more so when the new language is in a different modal-
ity. Hearing relatives of a deaf child are going to need help in learning a sign language.
Projects such as VL2 at Gallaudet University,1for example, should be adequately
funded.
(5) Deaf children should be brought into contact with deaf signing children and
adults frequently. The family of a deaf child should not feel the burden of being good
sign language models for the child. The important point is that family members engage
in frequent, direct language interaction with the deaf child, but the family must under-
stand that their own efforts will not be enough. Parents of deaf children should help
them find other deaf children to socialize with in a common language—a community of
others like themselves—without continual adult intervention in that communication.
Individual interpreters, who act as surrogate teachers or even parents in the classroom,
often have little contact with the deaf community. As a result, deaf students can be lim-
ited to dyadic groups for communication, which do not approach the richness and com-
plexity of language as used by a larger community. It appears the optimal way to ensure
the needed exposure is to participate in group discourse.
Given this, medical advisors must inform the family that the deaf child needs to be
brought into contact with a community of deaf signers so as to be exposed to consistent
and multiple models of signing on a regular and frequent basis. Families need to be-
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e37
1http://vl2.gallaudet.edu/
come informed about the local culture of Deaf people and help their child (and the
whole family) to participate in Deaf events. There are good publications out there to
help, like Lane et al. 1996, Padden & Humphries 2005, Bauman 2008, Bauman & Mur-
ray 2009, Marschark 2009, and Marschark & Spencer 2010, 2011, all of which provide
substantial references.
(6) Advice from others outside the hearing sciences and medical profession must be
better informed about pertinent language matters. These advisors include spiritual lead-
ers, particularly since the risk of depression or other psychosocial stress on the part of
deaf children and their parents may bring them to these leaders for guidance (Spahn et
al. 2003, Turner et al. 2007, Mellon 2009, Kushalnagar et al. 2011). So schools of the-
ology should include information on first language acquisition particularly as it pertains
to deaf children in their curriculum. Others in counseling professions need to be simi-
larly informed.
(7) Make sign language accessible to hearing parents and their deaf child. If a family
of a deaf child does not have easy access to a signing community, they must take a very
strong active role in providing their child with a sign language. First, the family must
try to learn a sign language in the best way possible, which may require driving a sub-
stantial distance to classes. If the local community is small, the family can enlist the
whole community in the effort to learn a sign language and to communicate with the
deaf child in that sign language. A community might want to advertise for and hire a
sign language teacher to come and stay in their community for an extended period of
time, teaching everyone who is willing to learn. There are also multiple online sites and
DVDs to help someone learn a sign language (see the websites of Dawn Sign Press in
the United States, Forest Books in the United Kingdom, or Karin Kestner Verlag in Ger-
many, for example2).
Second, the family should find out about camps for deaf children, where sign language
is used and deaf children learn about and get welcomed into Deaf culture. Many such
camps exist: in the United States they are scattered across the states; in Germany the Ger-
man Deaf YouthAssociation and German DeafAssociation of Hard-of-Hearing annually
organize camps for Deaf and hard-of-hearing children and youth. Some have scholar-
ships available. Some are for the entire family.There are various websites with up-to-date
information on such camps (in the US: Summer Camps for Deaf and Hard of Hearing
Children and Teens;3in Germany: Bundeselternverband gehörloser Kinder e.V.4).
Third, the family must be resourceful. Since it is important that others sign with the
deaf child, the family could start a sign language class with parents and children who
are not deaf. If the family has relatives in a city with a thriving Deaf community, visit-
ing or even arranging to spend time there may be a significant act that makes a world of
difference to the child’s development. The family might want to get online (using cur-
rent video technology: Skype, FaceTime, gChat, ooVoo, Facebook, etc.) with someone
who knows many people in the Deaf community to see if a Deaf family might like to
come visit them for extended periods. The deaf child in one’s home makes the home el-
igible to obtain a videophone setup from a video relay service. Alternatively, one can in-
stall videophone software in a home computer. With this setup, the family and the deaf
child can talk in sign language directly via video to deaf people whom they meet and
e38 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
2http://www.dawnsign.com/, http://www.forestbooks.com/, http://www.kestner.de/
3http://www.gallaudet.edu/clerc_center/information_and_resources/info_to_go/resources/summer_camps
.html
4http://www.gehoerlosekinder.de/
form stronger relationships. Sign language tutoring via videophone might even be
arranged. These setups often cost nothing to the family except an internet connection. If
the family has opportunities to live in an urban area that has a Deaf community, now
might be the time to realize those opportunities.
These family responsibilities can be costly in a number of ways beyond money and
time. Knoors and Marschark (2012) argue that using sign language can hinder family
dynamics and that learning a sign language can be beyond the abilities of some family
members, particularly older ones. We would suggest that, regardless of whether fam-
ily members learn a sign language, a deaf child born into a hearing family always im-
pacts family dynamics simply by virtue of the fact that the child is deaf. Further, every
deaf child is entitled to be recognized and accepted as deaf and to develop their own
identity as a deaf person. The United Nations Convention on the Rights of Persons with
Disabilities (2006) calls upon states to protect the rights of deaf children by ‘facilitating
the learning of sign language and the promotion of the linguistic identity of the deaf
community’ and by ensuring that their education ‘is delivered in the most appropriate
languages and modes and means of communication for the individual, and in environ-
ments which maximize academic and social development’.
Knoors and Marschark (2012) point out further that bilingual education for deaf chil-
dren has not had uniform success. However, the questions of how to ensure access to
language in the early years of life and how to educate deaf children are distinct. Many
and complex educational issues arise regardless of which kind of program a child enters
(whether one of the various kinds of mainstreaming programs or one of the various
kinds of bilingual/bicultural programs; see Ramsey 1997, Stinson & Liu 1999, Oliva
2004, Marschark 2009, and many others). We are confident that present and future ef-
forts (including more research) will lead to better-qualified teachers using more appro-
priate and efficacious methods and materials (see Humphries 2013). The fact remains,
however, that the cognitive factor that correlates best to literacy among deaf children is
a foundation in a first language. Much earlier work shows this, and the most recent find-
ings continue to confirm it: Davidson and colleagues (2014) show that children with
CIs who also sign perform better in standardized language testing than children with
CIs who do not have exposure to a sign language. (Again, we choose not to interrupt the
flow of the argument with a long list of earlier works, so instead mark the relevant ref-
erences with three asterisks in the bibliography.)
(8) Government sources must fund sign language instruction for these families.
Every human has a right to language (as we argue in Humphries et al. 2013). Therefore,
instruction in a sign language should be funded by federal and state governments for all
deaf children and their families. This funding should continue at least until the age of
twelve.
(9) The current risks associated with CIs need to be reduced. The risks of harm asso-
ciated with CIs should be more widely understood, and the current high risk of linguis-
tic consequences due to using CIs only as a response to deafness in the family needs to
be alleviated greatly by the use of sign language along with CIs. Cochlear implants run
a host of risks beyond linguistic deprivation. All surgeries come with risks, and surger-
ies involving the brain may be particularly troubling. With CI surgery, many complica-
tions arise, including injury to the facial nerve, necrosis and breakdown of the flap,
injury to hair follicles, improper electrode placement, postsurgery infection under the
flap and in the middle ear, and meningitis (Cohen & Roland 2006, McJunkin & Jeyaku-
mar 2010, Rubin & Papsin 2010, Thom et al. 2013). There is also a huge risk (40% to
74% of patients) of vertigo that can last for years (Steenerson et al. 2001, Walker 2008).
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e39
The apparatus can fail, requiring repeated surgery with all of the same associated risks
(Borkowski et al. 2002, Marlowe et al. 2010). Since many CI surgeries disable the
cochlea (O’Reilly et al. 2008), the implanted ear loses whatever residual hearing it had;
so if the CI does not offer language access to the child, then the surgery has, in fact, had
a result contrary to its very intention. The harms of cochlear implant surgery are in-
creasing as the popularity of binaural implantation goes up (Snow & Wackym 2009),
while the claimed benefits have yet to be established (and see the results in Tyler et al.
2010). Further, some deaf and hard-of-hearing children are implanted even when they
already recognize up to 30% of sentence material with or without a hearing aid (Tobin
1995), which is a better recognition rate than many children have post implantation.
These children actually might be losing ground with respect to speech skills. And, fi-
nally, hearing aids do not present the surgical risks of CIs and may well offer compara-
ble or better advantages with respect to speech development, depending on the
particular needs of individual children (Figueras et al. 2008). We therefore believe that
no child should be implanted unless implantation is accompanied by sign language, and
there is a very strong chance that the child will have excellent oral communication skills
as a result of the child’s curiosity and motivation for speaking, the child’s bias toward
auditory learning style, and the child’s neural response to implantation.
4. What we have been doing. We are a team with a core of a developmental psy-
chologist, a pediatrician, an education scholar, and a group of linguists. At various times
our team has been augmented by additional pediatricians, a philosopher, a theologist,
and a lawyer. Most, but not all, of the core have been active in all our projects.
We write articles aimed at ensuring the right of deaf children to have a language, and
thus to participate in human society (Kushalnagar, Mathur, et al. 2010, Blankmeyer
Burke et al. 2011, Humphries et al. 2012a,b, 2013). Some of us have presented our
work at national and international conferences. Most of our articles have been aimed at
medical professionals (see e.g. Humphries et al. 2014), particular the primary care
physician, although one was aimed at spiritual leaders, and a recent one (Humphries et
al. 2013) is aimed at lawyers and legislators. We hope to get our information into the
curriculum for health sciences courses in general, and into the core curriculum for med-
ical schools and nursing schools, and one of our papers in progress is aimed at trying to
bring that about. The fact that our team has specialists in a number of different areas
allows us to complement each other’s knowledge of both substance and the terminol-
ogy/culture of different fields. An article aimed at physicians, for example, has a differ-
ent style from one aimed at bioethicists, or one aimed at ministers, and so on.
While our arguments and recommendations may be obvious to the linguist reader,
they come as a surprise (sometimes a disturbing one) to many outside our field. Since
our first publication came out in 2010, we have been contacted by various groups and
individuals around the globe in their endeavors to protect deaf children. In 2012, we ex-
changed emails with a committee in Denmark as they reworked their national policy to-
ward the language and education needs of deaf children; we advised them on substance
as well as wording of the policy. In 2013, we exchanged emails with members of the
National Association of the Deaf (NAD) in the United States while they were working
on lobbying and legislative efforts. One of our team was asked to join the NAD Educa-
tion Policy Committee and is helping to prepare a position paper and other information
pieces (on the effects of language deprivation and accurate information on the risks of
CIs) for this organization. One of our 2012 articles (Humphries et al. 2012a) has been
downloaded over 27,000 times (as of April 2014), so we are steadily attracting attention
to the problem.
e40 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
We also reach out to others by attending meetings at which we have a chance of in-
fluencing interested individuals who might be able to advance our cause. For example,
at the annual Health Law Professors Conference hosted by the American Society of
Law, Medicine, & Ethics in June, 2013, we gathered contact information from profes-
sors who teach in health law and bioethics and then sent them copies of our articles with
a suggestion of how they might use them in their classes. When we spread the word
among colleagues in Deaf Studies, particularly from other countries, we give them Mi-
crosoft Word versions of our articles, rather than PDF versions, so that they can easily
copy and paste the material and use it in whatever way best suits them to advance our
shared goals.
Beyond the articles, we have published a decision-making tool for patients and their
physicians to share, called an option grid. These grids give a summary table that al-
lows comparisons between options a patient (or a patient’s family) faces by listing fre-
quently asked questions with very short answers supported by an evidence document
that gives references for further readings. Typically a patient reads the grid then meets
with the physician to discuss it and make a final decision. Option grids have been
shown to be effective in enhancing patients’ confidence as they make decisions even
about very complex matters and to increase their involvement in their own treatment
(Elwyn et al. 2013). Our option grid is labeled Language Options for Deaf Newborns. It
is available through the website http://www.optiongrid.org/, which is administered at
Dartmouth College by a collective of health professionals. We are open to suggestions
from readers and we are open to sharing our materials and our wisdom based on our ex-
periences thus far about what works and what does not, in setting up an activist team, in
working as a team, and in choosing sites for publication. We still have much to learn
and a lot of work ahead, but we are encouraged by the results to date and we have not
slowed down; if anything, our pace has picked up.
REFERENCES
Alajouanine, Théophile, and François Lhermitte. 1965. Acquired aphasia in children.
Brain 88.653–62.
Allen, Shannon; Doreen DeLuca; and Donna Jo Napoli. 2007. Societal responsibility
and linguistic rights: The case of deaf children. Journal of Research in Education
17.41–53.
Andrews, Jean F.; Irene W. Leigh; and Mary T. Weiner. 2003. Deaf people: Evolving
perspectives from psychology, education, and sociology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Archbold, Sue, and Gerard M. O’Donoghue. 2009. Cochlear implantation in children:
Current status. Paediatrics and Child Health 19.457–63.
Baker, Colin. 2006. Foundations of bilingual education and bilingualism. 4th edn. Cleve-
don: Multilingual Matters.
Bamford, John; Heather M. Fortnum; K. Bristow; J. Smith; G. Vamvakas; Linda
Davies; Rod S. Taylor; P. Watkin; Sarita Fonseca; Adrian Davis; and Sally E.
Hind. 2007. Current practice, accuracy, effectiveness, and cost-effectiveness of the
school-entry hearing screen. Health Technology Assessment 11.1–168.
Bauman, H-Dirksen L. (ed.) 2008. Open your eyes: Deaf studies talking. Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota Press.
Bauman, H-Dirksen L., and Joseph J. Murray. 2009. Reframing: From hearing loss
to Deaf gain. Deaf Studies Digital Journal 1. Online: http://dsdj.gallaudet.edu/index
.php?issue=1&section_id=2&entry_id=19.
Bialystok, Ellen; Fergus I. M. Craik; and Morris Freedman. 2007. Bilingualism as a
protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 45.459–64.
Bialystok, Ellen; Fergus I. M. Craik;Raymond Klein; and Mythili Viswanathan.
2004. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psy-
chology and Aging 19.290–303.
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e41
Blankmeyer Burke, Teresa; Poorna Kushalnagar; Gaurav Mathur; Donna Jo
Napoli; Christian Rathmann; and Kirk Vangilder. 2011. The language needs of
deaf and hard-of-hearing infants and children: Information for spiritual leaders and
communities. Journal of Religion, Disability & Health 15.272–95.
Borkowski, Gerd; Henning Hildmann; and Thomas Stark. 2002. Surgical aspects of
cochlear implantation in young and very young children. Cochlear implants: An update,
ed. by Takeshi Kubo, Y. Takahashi, and Takako Iwaki, 223–26. The Hague: Kugler.
Bowd, Alan D. 2005. Otitis media: Health and social consequences for aboriginal youth in
Canada’s north. International Journal of Circumpolar Health 64.5–15.
Boyes Braem, Penny. 1999. Rhythmic temporal patterns in the signing of deaf early and
late learners of Swiss German Sign Language. Language and Speech 42.177–208.
Boyes Braem, Penny, and Christian Rathmann. 2010. Transmission of sign languages
in Northern Europe. In Brentari, 19–45.
Brentari, Diane (ed.) 2010. Sign languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Broesterhuizen, Marcel, and K. U. Leuven. 2008. Worlds of difference: An ethical
analysis of choices in the field of deafness. Ethical Perspectives: Journal of the Euro-
pean Ethics Network 15.103–31.
**Burkholder, Rose A., and David B. Pisoni. 2006. Working memory capacity, verbal
rehearsal speed and scanning in deaf children with cochlear implants. In Spencer &
Marschark, 328–57.
***Chamberlain, Charlene, and Rachel I. Mayberry. 2008. American Sign Language
syntactic and narrative comprehension in skilled and less skilled readers: Bilingual and
bimodal evidence for the linguistic basis of reading. Applied Psycholinguistics 29.367–
88.
Christiansen, John B., and Irene W. Leigh. 2002. Cochlear implants in children: Ethics
and choices. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Cohen, Noel L., and J. Thomas Roland, Jr. 2006. Complications of cochlear implant sur-
gery. Cochlear implants, 2nd edn., ed. by Susan B. Waltzman and J. Thomas Roland,
Jr., 205–13. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers.
Cohn,Jonathan. 2011. The two year window: The new science of babies and brains—and
how it could revolutionize the fight against poverty. The New Republic, November 9,
2011. Online: http://www.newrepublic.com/article/economy/magazine/97268/the-two
-year-window, accessed September 12, 2012.
Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities. 2006. United Nations. On-
line: http://www.un.org/disabilities/convention/conventionfull.shtml.
Corina, David. 1998. The processing of sign language: Evidence from aphasia. Handbook
of neurolinguistics, ed. by Harry A. Whitaker and Brigitte Stemmer, 313–29. San
Diego: Academic Press.
Cormier, Kearsy; Adam Schembri; David Vinson; and Eleni Orfanidou. 2012. First
language acquisition differs from second language acquisition in prelingually deaf
signers: Evidence from sensitivity to grammaticality judgement in British Sign Lan-
guage. Cognition 124.50–65.
Courtin, Cyril. 2000. The impact of sign language on the cognitive development of deaf
children: The case of theories of mind. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education
5.266–76.
Courtin, Cyril. 2010. A critical period for the acquisition of the theory of mind? Clues
from homesigners. Deaf around the world: The impact of language, ed. by Gaurav
Mathur and Donna Jo Napoli, 184–93. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Courtin, Cyril, and Anne-Marie Melot. 2005. Metacognitive development of deaf chil-
dren: Lessons from the appearance—Reality and false belief tasks. Developmental Sci-
ence 8.16–25.
Courtin, Cyril; Anne-Marie Melot; and Denis Corroyer. 2008. Achieving efficient
learning: Why theory of mind is essential for deaf children … and their teachers. In
Marschark & Hauser, 102–30.
**Crouch, Robert A. 1999. Letting the deaf be deaf: Reconsidering the use of cochlear
implants in prelingually deaf children. Meaning and medicine: A reader in the philoso-
phy of health care, ed. by James Lindemann Nelson and Hilde Lindemann Nelson,
360–70. London: Routledge.
e42 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
Cummins, Jim, and M. Gulustan. 1974. Bilingual education and cognition. Alberta Jour-
nal of Educational Research 20.259–66.
Curtiss, Susan. 1977. Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern-day ‘wild child’. New
York: Academic Press.
Curtiss, Susan. 1994. Language as a cognitive system: Its independence and selective vul-
nerability. Noam Chomsky: Critical assessments, vol. 4, ed. by Carlos Otero, 211–55.
London: Routledge.
Davidson, Kathryn; Diane Lillo-Martin; and Deborah Chen Pichler. 2014. Spoken
English language development among native signing children with cochlear implants.
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 19.2.238–50.
Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, Early Hearing Detection & Intervention (EHDI) Program. 2009.
Organizational recommendations and screening guidelines. Online: http://www.cdc
.gov/NCBDDD/ehdi/ddscreen.htm, accessed September 5, 2009.
Drury, Stacy S.; Katherine P. Theall; M. M. Gleason; Anna T. Smyke; Immaculata
De Vivo; J. Y. Y. Wong; N. A. Fox; Charles H. Zeanah; and Charles A. Nelson.
2012.Telomere length and early severe social deprivation: Linking early adversity and
cellular aging. Molecular Psychiatry 17.719–27.
Early Hearing Detection and Intervention Information & Resource Center, Na-
tional Center for Hearing Assessment & Management. 2004. 2004 State EHDI
survey. Online: http://www.infanthearing.org/survey/2004statesurvey/index.html, ac-
cessed June 10, 2008.
Elwyn, Glyn; Amy Lloyd; Natalie Joseph-Williams; Emma Cording; Richard
Thomson; Marie-Anne Durand; and Adrian Edwards. 2013. Option grids: Shared
decision making made easier. Patient Education and Counseling 90.207–12.
Emmorey, Karen. 1991. Repetition priming with aspect and agreement morphology in
American Sign Language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 20.365–88.
Emmorey, Karen. 2001. Language, cognition and the brain: Insights from sign language
research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Emmorey, Karen, and David Corina. 1990. Lexical recognition in sign language: Effects
of phonetic structure and morphology. Perceptual and Motor Skills 71.1227–52.
Figueras, Berta; Lindsey Edwards; and Dawn Langdon. 2008. Executive function and
language in deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 13.362–77.
**Fink, Nancy E.; Nae-Yuh Wang; Jiovani Visaya; John K. Niparko; Alexandra
Quittner; Laurie S. Eisenberg; and Emily A. Tobey. 2007. Childhood development
after cochlear implantation (CDaCI) study: Design and baseline characteristics. Coch-
lear Implants International 8.92–116.
***Fischer, Susan. 1998. Critical periods for language acquisition: Consequences for deaf
education. Issues unresolved: New perspectives on language and deaf education, ed. by
Amatzia Weisel, 9–26. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Galvan, Dennis. 1999. Differences in the use of American Sign Language morphology by
deaf children: Implication for parents and teachers. American Annals of the Deaf 144.
320–24.
Garg, Suneela; Ritesh Singh; Shelly Chadha; and Arun Kuman Agarwal. 2011.
Cochlear implantation in India: A public health perspective. Indian Journal of Medical
Sciences 65.116–20.
**Geers, Ann E. 2006. The process and early outcomes of cochlear implantation by three
years of age. In Spencer & Marschark, 271–97.
**Geers, Ann E.; Emily Tobey; Jean Moog; and Chris Brenner. 2008. Long-term out-
comes of cochlear implantation in the preschool years: From elementary grades to high
school. International Journal of Audiology 47.21–30.
Gerich, Joachim, and Johannes Fellinger. 2012. Effects of social networks on the qual-
ity of life in an elder and middle-aged deaf community sample. Journal of Deaf Studies
and Deaf Education 17.102–15.
**Giraud, Anne-Lise, and Hyo-Jeong Lee. 2007. Predicting cochlear implant outcome
from brain organization in the deaf. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 25.381–90.
Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 2003. The resilience of language: What gesture creation in deaf
children can tell us about how all children learn language. NewYork: Psychology Press.
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e43
Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 2005. What language creation in the manual modality tells us
about the foundations of language. The Linguistic Review 22.199–225.
Goorhuis-Brouwer, Sieneke M. 1976. Enkele opmerkingen over afasie bij kinderen. Lo-
gopedie en Foniatrie 48.69–77.
Grant, Susan E. 2008. The silent debate: The controversy over the cochlear implant and
how it is changing the Deaf community. Los Angeles: University of Southern California
Press.
**Green, Kevin M. J.; Y. M. Bhatt; Deborah J. Mawman; Martin P. O’Driscoll;
Shakeel R. Saeed; Richard T. Ramsden; and M. W. Green. 2007. Predictors of au-
diological outcome following cochlear implantation in adults. Cochlear Implants Inter-
national 8.1–11.
Gregory, Susan. 1995. Deaf children and their families. New York: George Allen &
Unwin.
Grimshaw, Gina M.; Ana Adelstein; M. Philip Bryden; and G. E. MacKinnon. 1998.
First-language acquisition in adolescence: Evidence for a critical period for verbal lan-
guage development. Brain and Language 63.237–55.
Hall, James W., and Kristin N. Johnston. 2009. Diagnostic audiology, hearing instru-
ments and aural habilitation. In Snow & Wackym, 115–30.
Hauser, Peter C.; Amanda O’Hearn; Michael McKee; Anne Steider; and Denise
Thew. 2010. Deaf epistemology: Deafhood and deafness. American Annals of the Deaf
154.486–92.
Haynes, Scott, and Maureen Linden. 2012. Workplace accommodations and unmet
needs specific to individuals who are deaf or hard of hearing. Disability and Rehabili-
tation: Assistive Technology 7.408–15.
Helmuth, Laura. 2001. From the mouths (and hands) of babes. Science 293.1758–59.
***Hermans, Daan; Harry Knoors; Ellen Ormel; and Ludo Verhoeven. 2008. The
relationship between the reading and signing skills of deaf children in bilingual educa-
tion programs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 13.518–30.
**Hicks, Candace B., and Anne Marie Tharpe. 2002. Listening effort and fatigue in
school age children with and without hearing loss. Journal of Speech, Hearing, and
Language Research 45.573–84.
Hudson,Carla, and Elissa Newport. 2009. Getting it right by getting it wrong: When
learners change languages. Cognitive Psychology 59.30–66.
Humphries,Tom. 2013. Schooling in American Sign Language: A paradigm shift from a
deficit model to a bilingual model in deaf education. Berkeley Review of Education
4.7–33.
Humphries, Tom; Poorna Kushalnagar; Gaurav Mathur; Donna Jo Napoli; Carol
Padden; Christian Rathmann; and Scott Smith. 2012a. Language acquisition for
deaf children: Reducing the harms of zero tolerance to the use of alternative ap-
proaches. Harm Reduction Journal 9.16. Online: http://www.harmreductionjournal
.com/content/9/1/16. [German translation: Spracherwerb für gehörlose Kinder: Min-
derung der durch fehlende Toleranz entstehenden Schäden—hin zum Einsatz alterna-
tiver Ansätze. Das Zeichen 26.91.334–47, 2012.]
Humphries, Tom; Poorna Kushalnagar; Gaurav Mathur; Donna Jo Napoli; Carol
Padden; Christian Rathmann; and Scott Smith. 2012b. Cochlear implants and the
right to language: Ethical considerations, the ideal situation, and practical measures to-
ward reaching the ideal. Cochlear implant research updates, ed. by Cila Umat and
Rinze Anthony Tange, 193–213. Rijeka: InTech. Online: http://cdn.intechopen.com
/pdfs-wm/36348.pdf.
Humphries, Tom; Poorna Kushalnagar; Gaurav Mathur; Donna Jo Napoli; Carol
Padden; Christian Rathmann; and Scott Smith. 2014. Bilingualism: A pearl to
overcome certain perils of cochlear implants. Journal of Medical Speech-Language
Pathology 21.107–25.
Humphries, Tom; Raja Kushalnagar; Gaurav Mathur; Donna Jo Napoli; Carol
Padden; Christian Rathmann; and Scott Smith. 2013. The right to language. The
Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics (Special issue: Symposium: Human Rights and Dis-
ability) 41.872–84.
Iezzoni,Lisa I.; Bonnie L. O’Day;Mary Killeen; and Heather Harker. 2004. Com-
municating about health care: Observations from persons who are deaf or hard of hear-
ing. Annals of Internal Medicine 140.356–62.
e44 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
Jauhiainen,Tapani. 2001. Hearing impaired children in developing countries. Scandina-
vian Audiology 30.83–84.
Johnson, Jacqueline S., and Elissa L. Newport. 1989. Critical period effects in second
language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as
a second language. Cognitive Psychology 21.60–99.
Johnston,Trevor A. 2006. Response to comment. Sign Language Studies 6.225–43.
Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. 2007. Year 2007 position statement: Principles and
guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. Pediatrics 120.898–
921.
Klatter-Folmer, Jetske; Roeland van Hout; Esther Kolen; and Ludo Verhoeven.
2006. Language development in deaf children’s interactions with Deaf and hearing
adults: A Dutch longitudinal study. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11.
238–51.
Kleimenov, Mikhail, and Stanislav Shamkov. 2005. Criminal transportation of persons:
Trends and recommendations. Human traffic and transnational crime: Eurasian and
American perspectives, ed. by Sally Stoecker and Louise Shelley, 29–46. Lanham, MD:
Rowman & Littlefield.
Knoors, Harry, and Marc Marschark. 2012. Language planning for the 21st century:
Revisiting bilingual language policy for deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education 17.291–305.
Knutson, John F.; Christina R. Johnson; and Patricia M. Sullivan. 2004. Discipli-
nary choices of mothers of deaf children and mothers of normally hearing children.
Child Abuse & Neglect 28.925–37.
Kozak, Frederick K.; Juan C. Ospina; and Marcela Fandino. 2009. Investigation
of sensorineural hearing loss. Paper presented at Curso Internacional de Otorino-
laringologia Pediatrica. Online: http://www.cpnlac.org/memoriasacademicasinter
americano2009/files/Frederick%20K.%20Kozak%20-%20Investigation%20of%20
SNHL%20Monterey%20Talk%20Jul.pdf.
Kral, Andrej, and Gerard M. O’Donoghue. 2011. Profound deafness in childhood (Cor-
respondence: The authors reply). The New England Journal of Medicine 364.485.
Krashen, Stephen D. 1981. Second language acquisition and second language learning.
Oxford: Pergamon.
Krausneker, Verena. 2008. The protection and promotion of sign languages and the
rights of their users in Council of Europe member states: Needs analysis. Strasbourg:
Council of Europe. Online: http://www.coe.int/t/DG3/Disability/Source/Report_Sign
_languages_final.pdf.
Kushalnagar, Poorna; H. Julia Hannay; and Arturo E. Hernandez. 2010. Bilingual-
ism and attention: A study of balanced and unbalanced deaf users of American Sign
Language and English. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 15.263–73.
Kushalnagar, Poorna; Gaurav Mathur; Christopher J. Moreland; Donna Jo
Napoli; Wendy Osterling; Carol Padden; and Christian Rathmann. 2010. In-
fants and children with hearing loss need early language access. Journal of Clinical
Ethics 21.143–54.
Kushalnagar, Poorna; Tari D. Topolski; Brenda Schick; Todd Edwards; Anne
Skalicky; and Donald L. Patrick. 2011. Mode of communication, perceived level of
understanding and perceived quality of life in youth who are deaf or hard-of-hearing.
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16.512–23.
Kvam, Marit Hoem. 2004. Sexual abuse of deaf children: A retrospective analysis of the
prevalence and characteristics of childhood sexual abuse among Deaf adults in Norway.
Child Abuse & Neglect 28.241–51.
Lane, Harlan; Robert Hoffmeister; and Benjamin Bahan. 1996. A journey into the
Deaf-world. San Diego: Dawn Sign.
Leigh, Irene W. 2009. Identity and deafness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lenneberg, Eric. 1964. The capacity of language acquisition. The structure of language,
ed. by Jerry Fodor and Jerry Katz, 579–603. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Lenneberg, Eric. 1967. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.
**Leung, Janice; Nae-Yuh Wang; Jennifer D. Yeagle; Jill Chinnici; Stephen
Bowditch; Howard W. Francis; and John K. Niparko. 2005. Predictive models for
cochlear implantation in elderly candidates. Archives of Otolaryngology—Head and
Neck Surgery 131.1049–54.
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e45
Lightbown, Patsy, and Nina Spada. 2006. How languages are learned. 3rd edn. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Lillo-Martin, Diane. 1999. Modality effects and modularity in language acquisition: The
acquisition of American Sign Language. Handbook of child language acquisition, ed.
by William C. Ritchie and Tej K. Bhatia, 531–67. San Diego: Academic Press.
Luk, Gigi; Eric De Sa; and Ellen Bialystok. 2011. Is there a relation between onset age
of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition 14.588–95.
Luterman, David. 1979. Counseling parents of hearing-impaired children. Boston: Little,
Brown & Co.
***MacSweeney, Mairead. 1998. Cognition and deafness. Issues in deaf education, ed.
by Susan Gregory, Pamela Knight, Wendy McCracken, Stephen Powers, and Linda
Watson, 20–27. London: David Fulton.
Marlowe, Andrea L.; Jill E. Chinnici; Alejandro Rivas; John K. Niparko; and
Howard W. Francis. 2010. Revision cochlear implant surgery in children: The Johns
Hopkins experience. Otology & Neurotology 31.74–82.
Marschark, Marc. 2009. Raising and educating a deaf child: A comprehensive guide to
the choices, controversies, and decisions faced by parents and educators. Oxford: Ox-
ford University Press.
Marschark, Marc, and Peter C. Hauser (eds.) 2008. Deaf cognition: Foundations and
outcomes. New York: Oxford University Press.
**Marschark, Marc; Cathy Rhoten; and Megan Fabich. 2007. Effects of cochlear im-
plants on children’s reading and academic achievement. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education 12.269–82.
**Marschark, Marc; Thomastine Sarchet; Cathy Rhoten; and Megan Zupan. 2010.
Will cochlear implants close the reading achievement gap for deaf students? In
Marschark & Spencer 2010, 127–43.
Marschark, Marc, and Patricia E. Spencer (eds.) 2003. The Oxford handbook of deaf
studies, language, and education. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Marschark, Marc, and Patricia E. Spencer (eds.) 2010. The Oxford handbook of deaf
studies, language, and education, vol. 2. 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Marschark, Marc, and Patricia E. Spencer (eds.) 2011. The Oxford handbook of deaf
studies, language, and education, vol. 1. 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
**Martin, Daniela;Yael Bat-Chava; Anil Lalwani; and Susan B. Waltzman. 2010.
Peer relationships of deaf children with cochlear implants: Predictors of peer entry and
peer interaction success. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16.108–20.
Martins, Isabel P. 2004. Persistent acquired childhood aphasia. Neurogenic language dis-
orders in children, ed. by Franco Fabro, 231–51. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Mayberry, Rachel. 1994. The importance of childhood to language acquisition: Evidence
from American Sign Language. The development of speech perception: The transition
from speech sounds to words, ed. by Judith Goodman and Howard C. Nusbaum, 57–90.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mayberry, Rachel. 1998. The critical period for language acquisition and the deaf child’s
language comprehension: A psycholinguistic approach. Bulletin d’Audiophonologie:
Annales Scientifiques de L’Université de Franche-Comte 15.349–58.
Mayberry, Rachel, and Ellen B. Eichen. 1991. The long-lasting advantage of learning
sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition.
Journal of Memory and Language 30.486–512.
Mayberry, Rachel, and Susan Fischer. 1989. Looking through phonological shape to
lexical meaning: The bottleneck of non-native sign language processing. Memory and
Cognition 17.740–54.
***Mayer, Connie, and C. Tane Akamatsu. 2003. Bilingualism and literacy. In Mar-
schark & Spencer 2003, 136–50.
***McGuinness, Diane. 2006. Language development and learning to read: The scientific
study of how language development affects reading skill. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
McJunkin, Jonathan, and Anita Jeyakumar. 2010. Complications in pediatric cochlear
implants. American Journal of Otolaryngology 31.110–13.
McKee, Michael M.; Steve L. Barnett; Robert C. Block; and Thomas A. Pearson.
2011. Impact of communication on preventive services among deaf American Sign
Language users. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 41.75–79.
e46 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
McKee, Michael M.; Deirdre Schlehofer; Jessica Cuculick; Matthew Starr;
Scott Smith; and Nancy P. Chin. 2011. Perceptions of cardiovascular health in an
underserved community of deaf adults using American Sign Language. Disability and
Health Journal 4.192–97.
Meader, Helen E., and Philip Zazove. 2005. Health care interactions with Deaf culture.
Journal of the American Board of Family Practice 18.218–22.
Meier, Richard, and Elissa Newport. 1990. Out of the hands of babes: On a possible sign
advantage. Language 66.1–23.
Mellon, Nancy K. 2009. Parental response to the diagnosis of hearing loss. In Niparko,
147–49.
Mesch, Johanna. 2001. Tactile sign language: Turn taking and questions in signed con-
versations of deaf-blind people. Camarillo, CA: Signum.
Meshik, Xenia; Timothy A. Holden; Richard A. Chole; Timothy E. Hullar. 2010.
Optimal cochlear implant insertion vectors. Otology & Neurotology 31.1.58–63.
Miller, Katrina R.; McCay Vernon; and Michele E. Capella. 2005. Violent offenders
in a deaf prison population. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10.417–
25.
**Miyamato, Richard T.; Karen Iler Kirk; Amy M. Robbins; Susan Todd; Allison
Riley; and David B. Pisoni. 1997. Speech perception and speech intelligibility in chil-
dren with multichannel cochlear implants. Advances in Otorhinolaryngology 52.198–
203.
Moeller, Mary Pat; Karl R. White; and Lenore Shisler. 2006. Primary care physi-
cians’ knowledge, attitudes, and practices related to newborn hearing screening. Pedi-
atrics 118.1357–70.
Moores, Donald. 2001. Educating the deaf: Psychology, principles, and practices. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin.
Morford, Jill, and Barbara Hänel-Faulhaber. 2011. Homesigners as late learners:
Connecting the dots from delayed acquisition in childhood to sign language processing
in adulthood. Language and Linguistics Compass 5.535–37.
Morgan, Gary, and Judy Kegl. 2006. Nicaraguan Sign Language and theory of mind: The
issue of critical period and abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
47.811–19.
Munoz-Baell, Irma M.; Carlos Alvarez-Dardet; Maria Teresa Ruiz; R. Ortiz;
M. L. Esteban; and Emilio Ferreiro. 2008. Preventing disability through understand-
ing international megatrends in deaf bilingual education. Journal of Epidemiology and
Community Heath 62.131–37.
Musani, Mohammed Ayub; Faheem Ahmed Khan; Abdul Rauf; and Murtaza Ahsan.
2011. Frequency and causes of hearing impairment in tertiary care center. Journal of
Pakistan Medical Association 61.141–44.
National Institutes of Health. May 2011. It’s important to have your baby’s hearing
screened. (NIH publication no. 11-4968.) Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Deaf-
ness and Other Communication Disorders. Online: https://www.nidcd.nih.gov/health
/hearing/pages/screened.aspx.
Nelson, Charles; Nathan Fox; Charles Zeanah; and Dana Johnson. 2007. Caring
for orphaned, abandoned and maltreated children: Bucharest Early Intervention Project.
Presentation at the Better Care Network Discussion Day, Washington, DC, January 10,
2007. Online: http://www.unicef.bg/public/images/tinybrowser/upload/PPT%20BEIP
%20Group%20for%20website.pdf, accessed September 12, 2012.
Neville, Helen. 1995. Developmental specificity in neurocognitive development in hu-
mans. The cognitive neurosciences, ed. by Michael S. Gazzaniga, 219–31. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Newport, Elissa L. 1990. Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Sci-
ence 14.11–28.
Newport, Elissa L. 1991. Contrasting conceptions of the critical period for language. The
epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition, ed. by Susan Carey and Rochel
Gelman, 111–30. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Newport, Elissa L.; Daphne Bavelier; and Helen J. Neville. 2001. Critical thinking
about critical periods: Perspectives on a critical period for language acquisition. Lan-
guage, brain and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler, ed. by
Emmanuel Dupoux, 481–502. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e47
Newport, Elissa L., and Richard Meier. 1985. The acquisition of American Sign Lan-
guage. The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. 1, ed. by Dan Slobin,
881–938. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Newport, Elissa L., and Ted Supalla. 1987. Acritical period effect in the acquisition of a
primary language. Rochester: University of Rochester, ms.
**Nicholas, Johanna Grant, and Ann E. Geers. 2007. Will they catch up? The role of
age at cochlear implantation in the spoken language development of children with se-
vere to profound hearing loss. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50.
1048–62.
Niparko, John K. (ed.) 2009. Cochlear implants: Principles & practices. 2nd edn.
Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Northern, Jerry L., and Marion P. Downs. 2002. Hearing in children. 6th edn. Philadel-
phia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Oghalai, John S.; Lei Chen; Megan L. Brennan; Ross Tonini; and Spiros Manolidis.
2002. Neonatal hearing loss in the indigent. The Laryngoscope 112.281–86.
Oliva, Gina A. 2004. Alone in the mainstream: A Deaf woman remembers public school.
Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Olusanya, Bolajoko O.; Sheila L. Wirz; and Linda M. Luxon. 2008. Community-
based infant hearing screening for early detection of permanent hearing loss in Lagos,
Nigeria: A cross-sectional study. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 86.956–63.
**O’Reilly, Robert C.; Amanda J. Mangiardi; and H. Timothy Bunnell. 2008.
Cochlear implants. Access: Multiple avenues for deaf people, ed. by Doreen DeLuca,
Irene W. Leigh, Kristin A. Lindgren, and Donna Jo Napoli, 38–74. Washington, DC:
Gallaudet University Press.
Padden, Carol, and Tom Humphries. 2005. Inside Deaf culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
***Padden, Carol, and Claire Ramsey. 2000. American Sign Language and reading abil-
ity in deaf children. Language acquisition by eye, ed. by Charlene Chamberlain, Jill P.
Morford, and Rachel Mayberry, 165–89. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
**Papsin, Blake C.; Claudine Gysin; Nina Picton; Julian Nedzelski; and Robert V.
Harrison. 2000. Speech perception outcome measures in prelingually deaf children up
to four years after cochlear implantation. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngol-
ogy 109.38–42.
Paqarkar, Waheeda; Maria Bitner-Glindzicz; Jeffrey Knight; and Tony Siri-
manna. 2006. Late postnatal onset of hearing loss due to GJB2 mutations. Interna-
tional Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 70.1119–24.
***Paul, Peter V. 2003. Processes and components of reading. In Marschark & Spencer
2003, 97–109.
**Peterson, Nathaniel R.; David B. Pisoni; and Richard T. Miyamoto. 2010. Cochlear
implants and spoken language processing abilities: Review and assessment of the liter-
ature. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 28.237–50.
Petitto, Laura-Ann. 1998. On the biological, environmental and neurogenetic factors de-
termining early language acquisition: Evidence from signed and spoken languages.
Bulletin D’Audiophonologie 14.337–48.
Petitto, Laura-Ann, and Paula F. Marentette. 1991. Babbling in the manual mode:
Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science 251.1493–96.
Pfau, Roland; Marcus Steinbach; and Bencie Woll (eds.) 2012. Sign language: An in-
ternational handbook. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
**Pisoni, David B., and Miranda Cleary. 2003. Measures of working memory span and
verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing
24.106S–120S.
**Pisoni, David B., and Miranda Cleary. 2004. Learning, memory, and cognitive
processes in deaf children following cochlear implantation. Cochlear implants: Audi-
tory prostheses and electrical hearing, ed. by Fan-Gang Zeng, Arthur N. Popper, and
Richard R. Fay, 377–426. New York: Springer.
**Pisoni, David B.; Miranda Cleary; Ann E. Geers; and Emily A. Tobey. 2000. Indi-
vidual differences in effectiveness of cochlear implants in prelingually deaf children:
Some new process measures of performance. Volta Review 101.111–64.
e48 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
**Pisoni, David B.; Christopher M. Conway; William G. Kronenberger; David L.
Horn; Jennifer Karpicke; and Shirley C. Henning. 2008. Efficacy and effective-
ness of cochlear implants in deaf children. In Marschark & Hauser, 52–101.
**Pisoni, David B., and Ann E. Geers. 2001. Working memory in deaf children with
cochlear implants: Correlations between digit span and measures of spoken language
processing. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology 109.92–93.
Poizner, Howard; Edward Klima; and Ursula Bellugi. 1987. What the hands reveal
about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Porter, Ann, and Sisira Edirippulige. 2007. Parents of deaf children seeking hearing
loss-related information on the internet: The Australian experience. Journal of Deaf
Studies and Deaf Education 12.518–29.
Prince, Cheryl B.; Lloyd Miyashiro; Yusnita Weirather; and Patricia Heu. 2003.
Epidemiology of early hearing loss detection in Hawaii. Pediatrics 111.1202–6.
Prinz, Philip M., and Michael Strong. 1998. ASL proficiency and English literacy
within a bilingual deaf education model of instruction. Topics in Language Disorders
18.47–60.
Ramsey, Claire L. 1997. Deaf children in public schools: Placement, context and conse-
quences, vol. 3. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Rashid, Khadijat; Poorna Kushalnagar; and Raja Kushalnagar. 2011. How deaf
adult signers experience implants: Some preliminary conclusions. Cochlear implants:
Evolving perspectives, ed. by Raylene Paludneviciene and Irene Leigh, 45–59. Wash-
ington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Reddy, M. Vishnu Vardhan; L. Hema Bindu; P. Usha Rani; and P. P. Reddy. 2006. Post-
natal risk factors of congenital hearing impairment: Otitis media, head injuries, and
convulsions. International Journal of Human Genetics 6.191–93.
Remmel, Ethan, and Kimberly Peters. 2009. Theory of mind and language in children
with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 14.218–36.
**Robbins, Amy McConkey; Mario Svirsky; and Karen Iler Kirk. 1997. Children with
implants can speak, but can they communicate? Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Sur-
gery 117.115–60.
Rönnberg, Jerker. 2003. Working memory, neuroscience, and language: Evidence from
deaf and hard-of-hearing individuals. In Marschark & Spencer 2003, 478–90.
Rubin, Lorry G., and Blake Papsin. 2010. Cochlear implants in children: Surgical site in-
fections and prevention and treatment of acute otitis media and meningitis. Pediatrics
126.381–91.
Sandler, Wendy, and Diane Lillo-Martin. 2006. Sign language and linguistic univer-
sals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Santarelli, Rosamaria; Roberta De Filippi; Elisabetta Genovese; and Edoardo Ar-
slan. 2008. Cochlear implantation outcome in prelingually deafened young adults. Au-
diology and Neuro-Otology 13.257–65.
**Sarant, Julia Z.; Peter J. Blamey; Richard C. Dowell; Graeme M. Clark; and
W. P. R. Gibson. 2001. Variation in speech perception scores among children with
cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing 22.18–28.
Saunders, James, and David Barrs. 2011. Cochlear implantation in developing countries
as humanitarian service: Physician attitudes and recommendations for best practice.
Otolaryngology—Head and Neck Surgery 145.74–79.
***Schick, Brenda. 2003. The development of American Sign Language and manually
coded English systems. In Marschark & Spencer 2003, 219–31.
Schick, Brenda; Peter de Villiers; Jill de Villiers; and Robert Hoffmeister. 2007.
Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development 78.376–96.
Schick, Brenda; Marc Marschark; and Patricia Spencer. 2006. Advances in the sign
language development of deaf children. New York: Oxford University Press.
Schnell-Inderst, Petra; Silke Kunze; Franz Hessel; Eva Grill; Uwe Siebert; An-
dreas Nickisch; Hubertus Von V; and Jürgen Wasem. 2006. Hörscreening für
Neugeborene—Update. (Schriftenreihe health technology assessment 47.) Cologne:
Deutsches Institut für Medizinische Dokumentation und Information.
Shattuck, Roger. 1980. The forbidden experiment: The story of the wild boy of Aveyron.
New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e49
Singleton, Jenny, and Elissa Newport. 2004. When learners surpass their models: The
acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology
49.370–407.
Skinner, Margaret W.; Laura K. Holden; Lesley A. Whitford; Kerrie L. Plant;
Colleen Psarros; and Timothy A. Holden. 2002. Speech recognition with the nu-
cleus 24 SPEAK, ACE, and CIS speech coding strategies in newly implanted adults.
Ear and Hearing 23.207–23.
Skotara, Nils; Uta Salden; Monique Kügow; Barbara Hänel-Faulhaber; and
Brigitte Röder. 2012. The influence of language deprivation in early childhood on
L2 processing: An ERP comparison of deaf native signers and deaf signers with a de-
layed language acquisition. BMC Neuroscience 13.44.
Snow, Catherine, and Marian Hoefnagel-Höhle. 1978. The critical period for lan-
guage acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Child Development 49.
1114–28.
Snow, James B.,Jr., and P. Ashley Wackym (eds.) 2009. Ballengers otorhinolaryngology
17: Head and neck surgery. Shelton, CT: People’s Medical Publishing House.
Spahn, Claudia; Bernhard Richter; Thorsten Burger; Erwin Löhle; and Michael
Wirsching. 2003. A comparison between parents of children with cochlear implants
and parents of children with hearing aids regarding parental distress and treatment ex-
pectations. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 67.947–55.
Spencer, Patricia E. 1993. The expressive communication of hearing mothers and deaf in-
fants. American Annals of the Deaf 138.275–83.
Spencer, Patricia E., and Marc Marschark (eds.) 2006. Advances in the spoken language
development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Spivak, Lynn G. 2007. Neonatal hearing screening, follow-up, and diagnosis. Audiology di-
agnosis, 2nd edn., ed. by Ross J. Roeser, Michael Valente, and Holly Hosford-Dunn,
497–513. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers.
Steenerson, Ronald Leif; Gaye W. Cronin; and Lucinda B. Gary. 2001. Vertigo after
cochlear implantation. Otology & Neurotology 22.842–43.
Steinberg, Annie G.; Vicki Joy Sullivan; and Ruth C. Loew. 1998. Cultural and lin-
guistic barriers to mental health service access: The deaf consumer’s perspective.
American Journal of Psychiatry 155.982–84.
Stinson, Michael S., and Yufang Liu. 1999. Participation of deaf and hard-of-hearing
students in classes with hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education
4.191–202.
***Strong, Michael, and Philip Prinz. 2000. Is American Sign Language skill related to
English literacy? Language acquisition by eye, ed. by Charlene Chamberlain, Jill Mor-
ford, and Rachel Mayberry, 131–42. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sullivan, Patricia M., and John F. Knutson. 2000. Maltreatment and disabilities: A
population-based epidemiological study. Child Abuse & Neglect 24.1257–73.
Svirsky, Mario A.; Su Wooi Teoh; and Heidi S. Neuburger. 2004. Development of lan-
guage and speech perception in congenitally, profoundly deaf children as a function of
age at cochlear implantation. Audiology and Neuro-Otology 9.224–33.
Szagun, Gisela. 2008. The younger the better? Variability in language development of
young German-speaking children with cochlear implants. Proceedings of the Child
Language Seminar 2007—30th Anniversary, ed. by Theodoros Marinis, Angeliki Pa-
pangeli, and Vesna Stojanovik, 183–94. Reading: University of Reading Press.
The Canadian Hearing Society. 2005. Response of the Canadian Hearing Society to the
Standing Committee on Social Affairs, Science and Technology: Challenges facing
deaf, deafened, and hard of hearing individuals with mental health issues. Toronto: The
Canadian Hearing Society. Online: http://www.deafontario.ca/article-2011-03-13-CI
-by-snoddon.html.
Thom, Joshua J.; Matthew L. Carlson; Michael D. Olson; Brian A. Neff; Charles
W. Beatty; George W. Facer; and Colin L. W. Driscoll. 2013. The prevalence and
clinical course of facial nerve paresis following cochlear implant surgery. The Laryn-
goscope 123.1000–1004.
**Thoutenhoofd, Ernst D.; Sue Archbold; Sue Gregory; Mark E. Lutman;
Thomas Nikolopoulos; and Tracey H. Sach. 2005. Paediatric cochlear implanta-
tion: Evaluating outcomes. London: Whurr.
e50 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
**Tobey, Emily A.; Ann E. Geers; Chris Brenner; Dianne Altuna; and Gretchen
Gabbert. 2003. Factors associated with development of speech production skills in
children implanted by the age of five. Ear and Hearing 24.36S–46S.
Tobin, Henry (ed.) 1995. Rehabilitation research and development service: Practical hear-
ing aid selection and fitting. Baltimore: Department of Veterans Affairs.
Tomblin, J. Bruce; Brittan A. Barker; Linda J. Spencer; and Xuyang Zhang. 2005.
The effect of age at cochlear implant initial stimulation on expressive language growth
in infants and toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48.853–
67.
Tucci, Debara L.; Michael H. Merson; and Blake S. Wilson. 2010. A summary of the
literature on global hearing impairment: Current status and priorities for action. Otol-
ogy & Neurotology 31.31–41.
Turner, Oliver; Kirsten Windfuhr; and Navneet Kapur. 2007. Suicide in deaf popu-
lations: A literature review. Annals of General Psychiatry 6.26.
**Tyler, Richard S.; Holly F. B. Teagle; Danielle M. R. Kelsay; Bruce J.Gantz;
George G. Woodworth; and Aaron J. Parkinson. 2000. Speech perception by
prelingually deaf children after six years of cochlear implant use: Effects of age at im-
plantation. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology 109.82–84.
Tyler, Richard S.; Shelley A. Witt; Camille C. Dunn; Ann Perreau; Aaron J.
Parkinson; and Blake S. Wilson. 2010. An attempt to improve bilateral cochlear
implants by increasing the distance between electrodes and providing complemen-
tary information to the two ears. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology 21.
52–65.
**Uziel, Alain S.; Martine Sillon; Adrienne Vieu; Françoise Artieres; Jean-
Pierre Piron; Jean-Pierre Daures; and Michel Mondain. 2007. Ten year follow-
up of a consecutive series of children with multichannel cochlear implants. Otology &
Neurotology 28.615–28.
van Hasselt, Piet, and Eric van Kregten. 2002. Treatment of chronic suppurative otitis
media with ofloxacin in hydroxypropyl methylcellulose ear drops: A clinical/bacter-
iological study in a rural area of Malawi. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhino-
laryngology 63.49–56.
Vermeire, Katrien; Jan P. Brokx; Floris L. Wuyts; Ellen Cochet; Anouk Hofkens;
and Paul H. Van de Heyning. 2005. Quality-of-life benefit from cochlear implanta-
tion in the elderly. Otology & Neurotology 26.188–95.
Walker, Grace. 2008. A conversation with Grace Walker: Personal experiences with a
cochlear implant. Access: Multiple avenues for deaf people, ed. by Doreen DeLuca,
Irene W. Leigh, Kristin A. Lindgren, and Donna Jo Napoli, 140–45. Washington, DC:
Gallaudet University Press.
Waltzman, Susan B., and J. Thomas Roland, Jr. 2005. Cochlear implantation in chil-
dren younger than 12 months. Pediatrics 115.e487–e493.
***Wilbur, Ronnie. 2001. Sign language and successful bilingual development of deaf
children. Journal of the Institute for Social Research 56.1039–79.
***Wilbur, Ronnie. 2008. How to prevent educational failure. Signs & voices: Deaf cul-
ture, identity, language and arts, ed. by Kristin A. Lindgren, Doreen DeLuca, and
Donna Jo Napoli, 117–38. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Winefield, Richard. 1987. Never the twain shall meet: Bell, Gallaudet, and the communi-
cation debate. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Wood, Sandra. 2007. Degrees of resiliency in acquisition of language. Nanzan Linguistics
(Special issue 3) 1.315–30.
Wood, Sandra. 2011. Acquisition of topicalization in very late learners of LIBRAS: De-
grees of resilience in language. Deaf around the world: The impact of language, ed. by
Gaurav Mathur and Donna Jo Napoli, 164–83. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Woods, Bryan T., and Susan Carey. 1979. Language deficits after apparent clinical re-
covery from childhood aphasia. Annals of Neurology 6.405–9.
Wrigley, Owen. 1997. The politics of deafness: Family handbook on adult hearing loss.
Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
**Yoshinaga-Itano, Christine. 2006. Early identification, communication modality, and
the development of speech and spoken language skills: Patterns and considerations. In
Spencer & Marschark, 298–327.
LANGUAGE AND PUBLIC POLICY e51
Yoshinaga-Itano, Christine; Diane K. Coulter; and Vickie Thomson. 2000. The Col-
orado hearing screening program: Effects on speech and language for children with
hearing loss. Journal of Perinatology 20.s132–s142.
Yoshinaga-Itano, Christine; Allison L. Sedey; Diane K. Coulter; and Albert L.
Mehl. 1998. Language of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pedi-
atrics 102.1161–71.
Zeanah, Charles H.; Charles A. Nelson; Nathan A. Fox; Anna T. Smyke; Peter
Marshall; Susan W. Parker; and Sebastian Koga. 2003. Designing research to
study the effects of institutionalization on brain and behavioral development: The
Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Development and Psychopathology 15.885–907.
Napoli [Received 30 July 2013;
500 College Avenue accepted 22 January 2014]
Department of Linguistics
Swarthmore College
Swarthmore, PA 19081
[donnajonapoli@gmail.com]
[thumphri@gmail.com]
[poorna.kushalnagar@gmail.com]
[gaurav.mathur@gallaudet.edu]
[cpadden@ucsd.edu]
[christian.rathmann@sign-lang.uni-hamburg.de]
e52 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 90, NUMBER 2 (2014)
... Luego, son fundamentales las acciones pertinentes para la atención o estimulación temprana de los niños Sordos y niñas Sordas, en todos los ámbitos del desarrollo, y que consideren el acompañamiento a los padres y familia del niño Sordo o niña Sorda para apoyar en la aceptación de la sordera (Salamanca, 2010). Posteriormente, se debe guiar a las familias en la elección de programas educativos adecuados y oportunos, que favorezcan las características de cada uno, ello es, evaluando cuáles serían los aportes que estos programas educativos brindarán al desarrollo psicolingüístico del niño/a Sordo/a (Humphries, et al., 2014;Sánchez, 2015). ...
... Por desconocimiento, las familias tienden a pensar que la única y «mejor» forma de comunicación es la oral, creencias que se reafirman con comentarios o indicaciones del entorno social, que incluso pueden sugerir que el último recurso es la comunicación a través de lengua de señas. Como consecuencia de lo anterior, los niños Sordos y niñas Sordas desarrollan un sistema lingüístico limitado y sus familias descubren tardíamente los beneficios de una educación bilingüe (Humphries et al., 2014). Por lo tanto, el medio de comunicación inicial de los niños y niñas Sordos(as) al interior de la familia dependerá básicamente de: si los padres son Sordos u oyentes; si existe ya en la familia alguna otra persona Sorda, y de la actitud propia de los padres y familiares hacia el uso de la lengua de señas. ...
... En cambio, niños que no recibieron una lengua accesible hasta después del periodo crítico (Lennemberg, 1967) porque se les negó el uso de la lengua de señas muestran una reducción de la competencia lingüística y un daño psicolingüístico de largo plazo (Cheng y Mayberry, 2019;Emmorey, 1990;Emmorey y Corina, 1990;Mayberry y Fischer 1989). En estas poblaciones, se observa un claro fracaso en el sistema escolar, ya que no logran incorporar los contenidos educativos porque carecen de un medio de comunicación eficiente para interactuar con su entorno, dificultando su acceso a la participación y el aprendizaje (Humphries et al., 2014). ...
... In fact, it is at least plausible that language acquisition exhibits a critical role for human cognitive development 5 : a subsequent deficit in cognitive functions has been experimentally linked to a primary insufficient development of linguistic skills. [6][7][8] Moreover, according to Tattersall's theory, 9 10 language should be regarded as the fact that permitted the transition across different cognitive phenotypes during the evolution of human species. In philosophy, many authors highlighted the deep constitutive character of language for humans. ...
... The episode cannot be due to the direct physiological effects of a substance or medical condition and does not need to cause a clinically significant drop in functioning. AND Cyclothymic features: for a minimum of 6 months (lifetime) high and low mood (no more than two consecutive months without symptoms) and at least three criteria from the list: (1) decreased need for sleep (eg, feels rested after only 3 hours sleep), (2) increased energy, (3) inflated self-esteem or grandiosity, (4) increased goal directed activity, (5) restlessness, (6) more talkative than usual or pressure to keep talking, (7) unusual ideas, clear thinking, (8) troublesome behaviour, (9) inappropriate sense of humour. The episode cannot be due to the direct physiological effects of a substance or medical condition and does not need to cause a clinically significant drop in functioning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Language is usually considered the social vehicle of thought in intersubjective communications. However, the relationship between language and high-order cognition seems to evade this canonical and unidirectional description (ie, the notion of language as a simple means of thought communication). In recent years, clinical high at-risk mental state (CHARMS) criteria (evolved from the Ultra-High-Risk paradigm) and the introduction of the Clinical Staging system have been proposed to address the dynamicity of early psychopathology. At the same time, natural language processing (NLP) techniques have greatly evolved and have been successfully applied to investigate different neuropsychiatric conditions. The combination of at-risk mental state paradigm, clinical staging system and automated NLP methods, the latter applied on spoken language transcripts, could represent a useful and convenient approach to the problem of early psychopathological distress within a transdiagnostic risk paradigm. Methods and analysis Help-seeking young people presenting psychological distress (CHARMS+/− and Clinical Stage 1a or 1b; target sample size for both groups n=90) will be assessed through several psychometric tools and multiple speech analyses during an observational period of 1-year, in the context of an Italian multicentric study. Subjects will be enrolled in different contexts: Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health (DINOGMI), Section of Psychiatry, University of Genoa—IRCCS Ospedale Policlinico San Martino, Genoa, Italy; Mental Health Department—territorial mental services (ASL 3—Genoa), Genoa, Italy; and Mental Health Department—territorial mental services (AUSL—Piacenza), Piacenza, Italy. The conversion rate to full-blown psychopathology (CS 2) will be evaluated over 2 years of clinical observation, to further confirm the predictive and discriminative value of CHARMS criteria and to verify the possibility of enriching them with several linguistic features, derived from a fine-grained automated linguistic analysis of speech. Ethics and dissemination The methodology described in this study adheres to ethical principles as formulated in the Declaration of Helsinki and is compatible with International Conference on Harmonization (ICH)-good clinical practice. The research protocol was reviewed and approved by two different ethics committees (CER Liguria approval code: 591/2020—id.10993; Comitato Etico dell’Area Vasta Emilia Nord approval code: 2022/0071963). Participants will provide their written informed consent prior to study enrolment and parental consent will be needed in the case of participants aged less than 18 years old. Experimental results will be carefully shared through publication in peer-reviewed journals, to ensure proper data reproducibility. Trial registration number DOI:10.17605/OSF.IO/BQZTN.
... For example, while English is the de facto official language of the United Kingdom, British Sign Language (BSL) is the language predominantly used by deaf signers in the UK (Irish The fact that the majority of deaf children have reduced access to language input from birth could partially explain why many deaf children are generally labelled as "language delayed" (Kuntze 1998). This language deprivation in deaf children can bring a delay on their development of cognitive skills that are closely linked to their linguistic abilities (Humphries et al., 2014), as well as have detrimental consequences for successful reading acquisition. Previous research has shown that the speed of language acquisition in deaf children exposed early to sign language (e.g., in ASL) is similar to that of hearing children, showing the same language development milestones (e.g., Newport & Meier, 1986). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The role of phonological and orthographic access during word recognition, as well as its developmental trajectory in deaf readers is still a matter of debate. This thesis examined how phonological and orthographic information is used during written and fingerspelled word recognition by three groups of deaf readers: 1) adult readers of English, 2) adult and 3) young readers of Spanish. I also investigated whether the size of the orthographic and phonological effects was related to reading skill and other related variables: vocabulary, phonological awareness, speechreading and fingerspelling abilities. A sandwich masked priming paradigm was used to assess automatic phonological (pseudohomophone priming; Experiments 1-3) and orthographic (transposed-letter priming; Experiments 4–6) effects in all groups during recognition of single written words. To examine fingerspelling processing, pseudohomophone (Experiments 7–9) and transposed-letter (Experiments 10-12) effects were examined in lexical decision tasks with fingerspelled video stimuli. Phonological priming effects were found for adult deaf readers of English. Interestingly, for deaf readers of Spanish only those young readers with a small vocabulary size showed phonological priming. Conversely, orthographic masked priming was found in adult deaf readers of English and Spanish as well as young deaf readers with large vocabulary size. Reading ability was only correlated to the orthographic priming effect (in accuracy) in the adult deaf readers of English. Fingerspelled pseudohomophones took longer than control pseudowords to reject as words in the adult deaf readers of English and in the young deaf readers of Spanish with a small vocabulary, suggesting sensitivity to speech phonology in these groups. The findings suggest greater reliance on phonology by less skilled deaf readers of both Spanish and English. Additionally, they suggest greater reliance on phonology during both word and fingerspelling processing in deaf readers of a language with a deeper orthography (English), than by expert readers of a shallow orthography (Spanish).
... "The use of sign language in teaching English to Deaf students is important because English is not the first language of Deaf student". Humphries et al. (2014). Since Deaf student do not use English as their first language, when Deaf student learning English, they need several communication methods to help them communicate with their hearing pers in daily conversation. ...
Article
In language learning, the understanding towards the language usage which is appropriate with the grammatical rules is an important thing, because most of the students’ tasks are dealing with writing, especially writing a scientific paper. In the writing process, Deaf students sometimes struggles with the writing issues. One of their writing issues is the difficulties in understanding the concept of a language which is suitable with the grammar rules, word order, etc. The type of this research is Literature review research, and the method or structure used in this research is Narrative literature review with chronological order. This research mostly discussing the difficulties and challenges faced by Deaf student and the teacher in the teaching and learning English as a foreign language. The data gained in this research is using the review from 10 journal articles. Thus, the goal of this research is to identify the challenge of Deaf students in learning English, especially at the university level.
... Aunque en términos generales se reconoce que la adquisición tardía de una primera lengua impacta en el desarrollo de cualquier ser humano, sería conveniente que el autor nos describiera en qué consiste la carencia de lenguaje y en dónde radican estas limitaciones cognitivas, pues una lectura equivocada del discurso de Sánchez conduciría a falsas interpretaciones y expectativas sobre los alumnos sordos. De igual manera, habría que discutir sus descubrimientos con profundidad y establecer un diálogo entre sus hallazgos y los de otros especialistas que han abordado esta temática, como Humphries et al. (2014). ...
Book
Full-text available
La educación es un derecho fundamental para el ejercicio de otros derechos civiles, políticos, económicos, sociales y culturales. No obstante, muchas personas con discapacidad en México y el mundo son coartadas de este derecho como resultado de diversas barreras sociales y políticas. Entre los obstáculos que enfrentan, destacan la limitada atención que reciben por parte de las políticas y los programas educativos, la falta de información, las modalidades de formación segregatorias, así como las ideas y actitudes discriminatorias y excluyentes que aún persisten en gran parte de la población. El propósito de esta obra es reflexionar sobre la situación que guarda el derecho a la educación -como epítome de su indivisibilidad e interdependencia- de las personas con discapacidad en México. Así, esta obra aborda los alcances que ha tenido este derecho desde diferentes ángulos políticos e institucionales, poniendo énfasis en los estudiantes y el trabajo docente.
... 'Attitudes can explain how language variation is distributed, the level of knowledge that a person has over their own language and even predict behaviour (Ladegaard 2000;Wicker 1969), have a major influence on language and educational policies and often provide valuable information for language planners (Ruíz 1984). Sometimes this language planning can have disastrous consequences (Kaplan et al. 2011), as is often the case in deaf education (Humphries et al. 2014), as although deaf people in the UK have used sign language for centuries (Jackson, 1990;Kyle and Woll, 1985;Ladd, 2003), sign language is rarely used in education, even today. This is largely due to misconceptions about the language itself and its impact on educational attainment. ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
Having conducted a review of the impact of the Scottish national BSL plan on deaf education, in particular its issues, failures and successes, during Phase 1 of this project, the purpose of this report was to ascertain whether there is an appetite at government or local authority level for deaf children to be educated in either BSL-medium or bilingual schools-or whether parents of deaf children expect to see such provision-within the countries of Scotland and Wales, which have Welsh-and Gaelic-medium education provision respectively. Nineteen interviews with a total of 21 participants were carried out with Scottish and Welsh Government civil servants, national public body representatives, council officials, college and university representatives, families of deaf children, Teachers of the Deaf and third sector employees. There were marked differences in the conceptualisation of BSL as a language between top-, mid-and low-level, with the top-level tending to veer towards BSL as a communication tool and having a greater awareness of language policy and the right to language. At the mid-and low-level, there was however, a tendency to frame deaf children according to their audiological status, and that even though health-more specifically audiology-is outside education, it clearly exerts a huge force over the work of Teachers of the Deaf which would explain their attitudes towards BSL. Gaps in early years provision for deaf children also emerged as an important theme, with recognition that it is this period that is vital for language acquisition. The final theme identified through the interview stage was the availability or scarcity of resources in both Wales and Scotland for the teaching of BSL and in deaf education. We make 14 recommendations grouped under five headings: early years, language pedagogies, BSL teachers, Teachers of the Deaf and language policy. These include developing a new profession of BSL therapists to support efforts to develop BSL in deaf children in early years, the development of language pedagogies courses, the commissioning of mapping exercise of BSL teachers, the expansion of undergraduate and postgraduate courses to provide opportunities to develop fluency in BSL, initial teaching training courses that incorporate BSL, and training for qualified teachers,
... Different supports for helping families provide rich sign language input have been suggested. For example, Humphries and colleagues [174] call for government resources to fund sign language instruction for families of deaf children until the child is at least age 12. They also call for research on adult second-language learning in a second modality so that countries can improve the effectiveness of sign language instruction for hearing adults. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bilingual bimodalism is a great benefit to deaf children at home and in schooling. Deaf signing children perform better overall than non-signing deaf children, regardless of whether they use a cochlear implant. Raising a deaf child in a speech-only environment can carry cognitive and psycho-social risks that may have lifelong adverse effects. For children born deaf, or who become deaf in early childhood, we recommend comprehensible multimodal language exposure and engagement in joint activity with parents and friends to assure age-appropriate first-language acquisition. Accessible visual language input should begin as close to birth as possible. Hearing parents will need timely and extensive support; thus, we propose that, upon the birth of a deaf child and through the preschool years, among other things, the family needs an adult deaf presence in the home for several hours every day to be a linguistic model, to guide the family in taking sign language lessons, to show the family how to make spoken language accessible to their deaf child, and to be an encouraging liaison to deaf communities. While such a support program will be complicated and challenging to implement, it is far less costly than the harm of linguistic deprivation.
Article
Full-text available
This paper attempts to showcase a case study from an organization in Haryana, India which has eight deaf schools under its umbrella, where a systematic change in language policy and quality of deaf education has been achieved through various small steps over a period of three years. This positive development can be attributed to involvement of deaf experts and scholars at every step. There was a preliminary research conducted in 2016-17 to evaluate the existing language competency and based on the data, it was deduced that despite years of schooling, basic language competency (with reference to reading/writing skills) has not been developed in the deaf students. A team of deaf and education experts visited the eight schools and created a roadmap for development. As a result, the communication policy was changed from 'Total Communication' to 'Bilingualism', keeping in view the need of deaf students. Many innovative steps were taken right from early language exposure through the Early Intervention Project where deaf teachers provided age appropriate language input to deaf children between the age 0 to 6 years, sign language classes for parents and family members, introducing sign language as a subject, school empowerment at all levels and accessibility options to educational content and awareness material in the native sign language through use of technology. One critical aspect has been introduction of deaf resource people, from a single deaf teacher in 2015, the organization now have more than 35 deaf resource people across the eight centers. This paper explores the impact of introduction of deaf teachers on the school environment and overall quality of deaf education, through questionnaires to deaf students, parents and teachers. It can be said that the transformation is not complete yet and there are many more challenges to be addressed. However, a beginning has been made which is truly remarkable and unique and serves as a model for the other deaf schools.
Article
The purpose of this case study was to explore the communication needs of a child with deafness and cerebral palsy (DCP) and how these needs can be supported by interventions targeting the use of communication approaches and strategies. One semi-structured interview with the child’s parents and five observations of the child in different contexts before and after the interventions took place. A rating scale “Checklist of Identified Needs”, deriving from the observation data, the “Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS) Development Matters” assessment and a British Sign Language (BSL) receptive and expressive skills assessment were used to explore the child’s communication strategies before and after the intervention. The child completed two parallel school-based interventions lasting seven weeks, (i) one delivered daily which focused on BSL usage and Augmentative Alternative Communication (AAC) and (ii) one targeting communication strategies such as initiating and maintaining eye contact using a weekly “social story” and play-based sessions. Our results indicated that the combined use of sign and AAC technology to target key areas of communication use and a targeted approach using social stories to support communication strategies can be effective in enhancing communication skills of children with DCP. Implications for practice are discussed.
Book
This is the republication of a study originally entitled The Deaf Child and his Family which was a landmark in the study of early deafness. Dr Gregory's work, based on interviews with 122 mothers of deaf children under the age of six years, parallels that already done with hearing children and investigates with the same methodology the ways in which deaf children develop, change and are changed by their home and their wider environment. The book describes the everyday life of young deaf children and their families, looks at the deaf child's activities and their daily routine and considers the support and advice given to the parents during the child's early years. This republication, and the addition of a new introduction, coincides with the publication of Dr Gregory's follow-up longitudinal study of the same cohort of families in Deaf Young People and their Families.
Book
Thoroughly updated for its Second Edition, this book provides an in-depth discussion on prosthetic restoration of hearing via implantation. The text succinctly discusses the scientific principles behind cochlear implants, examines the latest technology, and offers practical advice on how to assess candidates, how to implant the devices, and what rehabilitation is most effective. The authors thoroughly examine the outcomes of cochlear implantation, the impact on the patient's quality of life, the benefits in relation to the costs, and the implications of cochlear implants for language and speech acquisition and childhood education.