ArticlePDF Available

Eye movements disrupt episodic future thinking

Authors:
  • https://sites.google.com/view/stefania-devito/home

Abstract and Figures

Remembering the past and imagining the future both rely on complex mental imagery. We considered the possibility that constructing a future scene might tap a component of mental imagery that is not as critical for remembering past scenes. Whereas visual imagery plays an important role in remembering the past, we predicted that spatial imagery plays a crucial role in imagining the future. For the purpose of teasing apart the different components underpinning scene construction in the two experiences of recalling episodic memories and shaping novel future events, we used a paradigm that might selectively affect one of these components (i.e., the spatial). Participants performed concurrent eye movements while remembering the past and imagining the future. These concurrent eye movements selectively interfere with spatial imagery, while sparing visual imagery. Eye movements prevented participants from imagining complex and detailed future scenes, but had no comparable effect on the recollection of past scenes. Similarities between remembering the past and imagining the future are coupled with some differences. The present findings uncover another fundamental divergence between the two processes.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by: [Stefania de Vito]
On: 24 June 2014, At: 11:23
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Memory
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pmem20
Eye movements disrupt episodic future thinking
Stefania de Vitoab, Antimo Buonocorec, Jean-François Bonnefond & Sergio Della
Salae
a Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière, INSERM,
Paris, France
b Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Suor Orsola Benincasa University, Naples,
Italy
c Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, Center for Mind/Brain
Sciences, Trento University, Trento, Italy
d CLLE, Maison de la Recherche, Toulouse, France
e Human Cognitive Neuroscience, Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh,
UK
Published online: 17 Jun 2014.
To cite this article: Stefania de Vito, Antimo Buonocore, Jean-François Bonnefon & Sergio Della Sala (2014): Eye
movements disrupt episodic future thinking, Memory, DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2014.927888
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.927888
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors
make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability
for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions
and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of
the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of
information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands,
costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or
indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution
in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Eye movements disrupt episodic future thinking
Stefania de Vito
1,2
, Antimo Buonocore
3
, Jean-François Bonnefon
4
, and
Sergio Della Sala
5
1
Centre de Recherche de lInstitut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière, INSERM, Paris,
France
2
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Suor Orsola Benincasa University, Naples, Italy
3
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, Center for Mind/Brain Sciences,
Trento University, Trento, Italy
4
CLLE, Maison de la Recherche, Toulouse, France
5
Human Cognitive Neuroscience, Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
(Received 27 February 2014; accepted 20 May 2014)
Remembering the past and imagining the future both rely on complex mental imagery. We considered
the possibility that constructing a future scene might tap a component of mental imagery that is not as
critical for remembering past scenes. Whereas visual imagery plays an important role in remembering the
past, we predicted that spatial imagery plays a crucial role in imagining the future. For the purpose of
teasing apart the different components underpinning scene construction in the two experiences of
recalling episodic memories and shaping novel future events, we used a paradigm that might selectively
affect one of these components (i.e., the spatial). Participants performed concurrent eye movements
while remembering the past and imagining the future. These concurrent eye movements selectively
interfere with spatial imagery, while sparing visual imagery. Eye movements prevented participants from
imagining complex and detailed future scenes, but had no comparable effect on the recollection of past
scenes. Similarities between remembering the past and imagining the future are coupled with some
differences. The present findings uncover another fundamental divergence between the two processes.
Keywords:Episodic future thinking; Prospection; Eye movements; Visual imagery; Spatial mental
imagery.
There is not only one time:
There are many ribbons that slide parallel
often in the opposite sense
(Eugenio Montale)
People can withdraw from the current moment and
mentally project themselves to an alternative time
and place, as they remember their past or imagine
their future (Szpunar, 2011). Importantly, the cog-
nitive and neuralprocesses involved in remembering
thepastseemtobethesameasthecognitiveand
neural processes involved in imagining the future
(episodic future thinking,Atance&ONeill, 2001;
Buckner & Carroll, 2007; de Vito & Della Sala,
2011). In this article, we show that even though both
remembering the past and imagining the future
depend on mental imagery, they involve a different
mix of spatial and visual mental imagery.
The neural activity underpinning the remem-
brance of the past is strikingly similar to the
Address correspondence to: Stefania de Vito, Centre de Recherche de lInstitut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière, INSERM,
U 1127, 47-83 Boulevard de lHôpital, F-75013 Paris, France. E-mail: stefaniadv29@libero.it
© 2014 Taylor & Francis
Memory, 2014
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.927888
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
neural activity underpinning episodic future
thinking (Buckner & Carroll, 2007; Irish &
Piguet, 2013; Schacter et al., 2012). The set of
cognitive processes sustaining these two functions
is thought to overlap considerably (Addis, Wong, &
Schacter, 2007;DArgembeau & Van der Linden,
2004; Levine, Svoboda, Hay, Winocur, & Mos-
covitch, 2002; Manning, Denkova, & Unterber-
ger, 2013). Nonetheless, relevant neuroimaging
(Addis et al., 2007; Okuda et al., 2003), behavi-
oural (Addis, Musicaro, Pan, & Schacter, 2010;
DArgembeau & Van der Linden, 2004; Gamboz,
Brandimonte, & de Vito, 2010) and neuropsycho-
logical studies (Berryhill, Picasso, Arnold, Dro-
wos, & Olson, 2010; de Vito et al., 2012) have
reported marked differences or dissociations
(Irish, Addis, Hodges, & Piguet, 2012) between
the ability of remembering the past and that of
imagining the future.
Both functions reflect the construction of
mental scenes that involve complex mental
imagery (Hassabis & Maguire, 2007; Schacter &
Addis, 2007). However, mental imagery is a mul-
tifarious cognitive tool, which relies on at least
two components, a spatial component and a
depictive (Kosslyn & Thompson, 2000), also
labelled visual, component (Reisberg & Heuer,
2002). We need to carefully consider the possib-
ility that these various components might be
differentially involved in remembering the past
and in imagining the future.
While it seems reasonably clear that remem-
bering the past crucially requires visual imagery
(Rubin, Schrauf, & Greenberg, 2003), much
remains to be elucidated about the mix of visual
and spatial imagery required to imagine the
future (de Vito, 2012).
One possible means to address this issue is to
explore the disruptive effect of voluntary eye
movements on episodic future thinking. Volun-
tary eye movements are known to disrupt mental
imagery (Postle, Idzikowski, Della Sala, Logie, &
Baddeley, 2006).
Early studies (e.g., Perky, 1910) indicated a
tight link between eye movements and mental
images. Neisser (1967) then argued that eye
movements are necessary to the construction of
a visual image, and Baddeley (1986) posited that
voluntary eye movements interfere with visuo-
spatial mental imagery. This hypothesis is sup-
ported by experimental data (Hale, Myerson,
Rhee, Weiss, & Abrams, 1996; Laeng & Teodor-
escu, 2002; Lawrence, Myerson, Oonk, &
Abrams, 2001; Pearson & Sahraie, 2003; Postle
et al., 2006). Further studies showed that eye
movements (specifically, endogenously generated
smooth pursuit) reduce the vividness and emo-
tional impact of personal recollections (Andrade,
Kavanagh, & Baddeley, 1997). This effect extends
to upsetting visual images of feared future events
(Engelhard, van den Hout, Janssen, & van der
Beek, 2010).
A close examination of the literature, however,
suggests that only spatial mental imagery has
been shown to be disrupted by concurrent eye
movements. The visual component of mental
imagery has rarely been investigated under sim-
ultaneous eye movements (see, e.g., Exp 4 in
Postle et al., 2006). Pearson and Sahraie (2003)
demonstrated that concurrent voluntary eye
movements reduce spatial memory task signifi-
cantly more than equivalent covert attention
shifts or limb movements. Indeed, all experiments
showing impairment of mental imagery due to
concurrent eye movements employed spatially
loaded tasks (Lawrence et al., 2001; Pearson &
Sahraie 2003; Postle et al., 2006). Even more
tellingly, Postle et al. (2006, Study 4), as well as de
Vito, Buonocore, Bonnefon, and Della Sala
(2014), presented participants with tasks tapping
either the visual or spatial component of mental
imagery, and showed that voluntary eye move-
ments impaired performance on the latter, but
not on the former. Postle et al. (2006) asked
participants for the delayed recognition of the
shape of previously provided targets, and for the
delayed recognition of the location of previous
targets. After the presentation of the target,
participants underwent a distraction periodin
which they were asked either to continuously
move their eyes or to read some words appearing
on the screen. The results indicated that saccadic
distraction affects spatial working memory per-
formance, but not performance on a non-spatial
task that is equally difficult. On the contrary,
word reading disrupts working memory for
shape, but not for locations. de Vito et al. (2014)
observed that an additional task of concurrent
eye movements perturbed performance for the
iconic version of a spatial imagery test (Brooks
matrices) but not for the iconic versions of visual
imagery tests (Animal tails task and Curvy Let-
ter task).
Accordingly, the question of whether remem-
bering the past and imagining the future depend
on the same mix of mental imagery can be
explored by investigating the disruptive effects
of voluntary eye movements.
2 DE VITO ET AL.
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
To this end, we instructed participants to per-
form cue-dependent memory tasks as well as
future-thinking tasks, concurrently with two dif-
ferent secondary tasks: eye movement and hand
tapping. This latter condition was added because
previous studies suggested that it could interfere
with mental imagery tasks (e.g., Farmer, Berman, &
Fletcher, 1986; Logie & Marchetti, 1991;Moscov-
itch, 1994; Salway & Logie, 1995;Smyth,Pearson,
& Pendleton, 1988). Its inclusion thus ensured that
our results would not simply reflect the unspecific
effect of any attention-demanding concurrent task.
We posit that spatial imagery serves the ability
of future thinking to a greater extent than the
ability of remembering past events. Indeed, rel-
ative to autobiographical memory, future thinking
requires a more extensive constructive process
(Schacter et al., 2012). Thus, concurrent voluntary
eye movements are likely to disrupt future think-
ing, while sparing episodic memory.
METHOD
Participants
Fourteen young adults (9 men) entered this
experiment. Participants were right-handed stu-
dents recruited at the University of Edinburgh.
Their average age was 31.14 years (SD = 2.24).
None was under psychoactive pharmacological
treatment or had a history of neurological or
psychiatric disorder. Participants did not receive
any compensation. Before starting the testing
session, participants signed an informed consent
form. The study procedures were approved by
the local ethical committee and were carried out
in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Materials
Participants were initially briefed that they
would be required to mentally re-experience and
pre-experience 12 autobiographical episodes
(6 occurred in the past; 6 occurring in the future
and never occurred in the past), in response to
randomly presented cue words. This technique of
open-ended cueing paradigms has been largely
used in most of the previous future-thinking
studies on healthy participants and has been
proved to be sensitive to future-imagining deficits
in patients (Squire et al., 2011). Participants were
given an explicit temporal frame with each future/
past cue (i.e., next few years/last few years).
Aiming at eliciting past and future events, two
sets of eight words, matched for familiarity,
frequency, imageability and concreteness, were
selected (Burani, Barca, & Arduino, 2001). Within
each list, the words were randomly assigned to
the two temporal directions and rotated in the
past/future conditions. Then, each participant was
given one of these two sets. The experimenter
further explained that the events had to be
remembered or imagined in as much detail as
possible. Participants also were encouraged to
produce temporally and contextually specific
events and to vividly imagine novel and plausible
future episodes, given their current plans. When
constructing future episodes, participants were
explicitly instructed not to describe a memory or
any part of it, or something they planned to do,
but rather to imagine something completely new.
This procedure follows those of DArgembeau
and van der Linden (2004) and of Addis et al.
(2007). The order of presentation of temporal
connotation (i.e., past and future) was
counterbalanced.
The cues were read, one at a time, by the
experimenter with the instructions about the tem-
poral direction (remember or imagine). Once an
episode had been recalled or imagined, it was
recounted by the participant. There was no time
limit. Participants were allowed to keep on verb-
ally illustrating the event until they thought that
nothing else could be added. We maintained
constantly the prompting procedure for the parti-
cipants in all the experimental conditions. When
participants stopped talking, the experimenter
asked only once whether there was any further
detail that they would have liked to add.
Recollections and simulations were digitally
recorded to enable later transcriptions and sub-
sequent scoring of participantsresponses.
After the transcription, a trained rater, who
was blind to the hypothesis of the study, used the
standardised scoring procedure developed by
Levine et al. (2002) to systematically parse the
details generated in the past and future events.
This allowed the rater first to segment the main
event (i.e., the most talked about, with a brief
time frame) into details and then to distinguish
between (1) internal details (i.e., episodic
information pertaining to the main event, specific
to time and place) and (2) external details
(general knowledge related to the event). A
second rater, trained for this purpose, then scored
the protocols of 10 participants. The inter-rater
EYE MOVEMENTS DISRUPT EPISODIC FUTURE THINKING 3
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
reliability (r) between the original rater and the
new rater was .96, p< .001 for the number of
internal details and r= .98, p< .001 for the
number of external details.
Internal details were further categorised into:
(1) event (happenings, individuals present, phys-
ical/emotional actions and reactions and weather),
(2) place (information about the environment
where the event occurred), (3) time (date, season,
month, day of the week and time of day), (4)
perceptual (sensory information and body posi-
tion) and (5) emotion (emotional state and
thoughts). External details were categorised into:
(1) external event (specific details from other
incidents, from all of the above categories,
external to the main event), (2) semantic (general
knowledge or facts, ongoing events and extended
states of being), (3) repetition (unsolicited repe-
tition of details) and (4) other (metacognitive
statements and editorialising).
Procedure
Testing was carried out in one single session. All
participants were tested individually and sat facing
the same experimenter in a quiet testing environ-
ment. They performed the task in three experi-
mental conditions. Participants were required to
mentally re-experience and pre-experience four
autobiographical episodes (two past and two future)
in each condition. The controlcondition was a
free-viewing condition not involving a concurrent
task. In the hand-tappingcondition participants
performed the main task while concurrently tap-
ping a square on a board with their right hand. The
tapping task was paced by a metronome, which
was set on Largo, at 40 bpm, to equate the speed
of the dot-moving on the screen in the eye
movementcondition. The participantshands
were covered so that movements were performed
without visual processing.
Finally, in the critical eye movementcondi-
tion, the task was performed concurrently with
lateral, continuous and voluntary eye movements.
Red fixation and green target stimuli on a white
background were presented on a 17-inch cathode
ray tube monitor (1024 × 768 pixels) at 100 Hz.
Participants were seated with their head in a chin
rest and their eyes horizontally and vertically
aligned with the centre of the screen at a distance
of 80 cm. Eye movements were recorded with
the EyeLink 1000 system (detection algorithm:
pupil and corneal reflex, 1000-Hz sampling).
A five-point horizontalvertical calibration was
run at the start of the experiment. Each trial
began with a drift correction and a tone accom-
panying the onset of a 0.5° red dot presented on
the left side of the screen at an eccentricity of 8.5°
of visual angle. The experimenter started each
trial by pressing the space bar. As soon as the
experimenter started the trial, the dot became
green and moved continuously from left to right
at a frequency of 0.6 Hz, spanning a total distance
of 17° of visual angle. Participants were required
to fixate the dot until it was red and then to
follow it with their gaze as soon as became green
and started moving. The experimenter stopped
the trial when a single episode was recounted.
Participants were also asked to perform a base-
line condition in which they were instructed solely
to follow the dot for 60 seconds.
The tapping task was voluntary and exogen-
ously driven to match the fundamental character-
istics of the eye movement task and was
introduced to tease apart the disruptive effect of
a motor task and the disruptive effect that might
be specific for eye movements. Thus, the tapping
task was used to ensure that future thinking
was not simply disrupted by any additional motor
task.
The order of presentation of the three concur-
rent tasks was fully counterbalanced across the
participants.
RESULTS
The performance in the future-thinking task, in
each condition, is shown in Figure 1.A3
(Concurrent task: Eye movements vs. Tapping
vs. Control) × 2 (Direction of time travel: Past vs.
Future) analysis of variance (ANOVA), with all
variables as within-subjects factors, was carried
out on the mean number of internal details.
The results revealed main effects of both
Concurrent task, F(1, 13) = 13.96, MSE =
445.36, p< .001, and Direction of time travel,
F(1, 13) = 14.2, MSE = 966.96, p< .005. These
indicated that participants generated a greater
number of internal details during the control
(M= 27.64, SD = 13.72) and the hand-tapping
(M= 26.76, SD = 13.99) conditions than in the
eye movement condition (M= 20.33, SD = 12.72),
and that, overall, more internal details were
uttered for the past events (M= 28.3, SD =
13.94) than for future events (M= 21.52,
SD = 10). A post hoc power analysis using the
4 DE VITO ET AL.
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
software package, GPower (Faul & Erdfelder,
1992), was conducted on these results. The
sample size of 14 was used for the statistical
power analyses. The alpha level used for this
analysis was p< .05. The post hoc analyses
revealed that the statistical power for this study
was .949 for detecting the effect size (g2
p
¼:522)
related to the main effect of Direction of time
travel and .947 for detecting the effect size
(g2
p
¼:518) related to the main effect of Concur-
rent task. Thus, there was more than adequate
power (i.e., power = .94) at our effect size levels.
A significant interaction between Concurrent
task and Direction of time travel, F(1, 13) = 4.78,
MSE = 195.77, p< 0.05, indicated that a lower
number of internal details was produced in future
episodes in the eye movement condition (M=
13.92, SD = 5.35) than during the hand-tapping
condition (M= 25.28, SD = 12.56), t(13) = 4.96,
MSE = 2.28, p< .001, and the control condition
(M= 25.35, SD = 13.75), t(13) = 4.24, MSE = 2.69,
p< .005. No significant difference in the number
of internal details was observed between the
hand-tapping condition and the control condition
in the future conditions.
Furthermore, no significant difference in the
number of internal details produced when
remembering past events was observed between
the eye movement, the tapping and the control
conditions.
A separate 3 (Concurrent task: Eye move-
ments vs. Tapping vs. Control) × 2 (Direction of
time travel: Past vs. Future) ANOVA was con-
ducted on the mean number of external details
and showed a main effect of the Concurrent task,
F(1, 13) = 4.12, MSE = 25.64, p< .05, and
Direction of time travel, F(1, 13) = 6.16, MSE =
21.5, p< .05. These indicate that a greater
number of external snippets was uttered during
the eye movement condition (M= 3.58, SD =
4.19), if compared to the tapping (M= 2.53,
SD = 3.25) and the control conditions (M= 1.67,
SD = 1.61), and that more external details were
generated in future (M= 3.1, SD = 3.83) than in
past episodes (M= 2.09, SD = 2.5).
The interaction between Concurrent task and
Direction of time travel was also significant,
F(1, 13) = 17.53, MSE = 44.86, p< .001, indicating
that the number of external details generated in
future episodes was greater in the eye movement
condition (M= 5.53, SD = 4.96) than in the
control condition (M= 1.67, SD = 1.89), t(13) =
3.2, MSE = 1.2, p< .01 and the hand-tapping con-
dition (M= 2.1, SD = 2.92), t(13) = 4.13, MSE =
3.1, p< .005.
No significant difference in the number of
external details produced when remembering
past events was observed between the eye move-
ment, the tapping and the control conditions.
Finally, the subcomponents of internal details
(i.e., events, place, time, perceptual details and
emotions) were separately analysed. We com-
pared, both in past and future episodes, the
number of details for each subcomponent in the
three conditions (Eye movements vs. Tapping vs.
Control), by means of ttests. The Bonferroni
adjustment was applied, α= .016, to correct for
multiple comparisons. No differences were found
in any of the subcomponents in past episodes. On
the contrary, for what concerns future episodes,
Figure 1. Performance (expressed in mean number of internal details produced) on past and future tasks in all the conditions.
Error bars show standard error of the mean.
EYE MOVEMENTS DISRUPT EPISODIC FUTURE THINKING 5
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
the following significant differences were found
in the components of the events, place and
emotions.
Events. The number of details was significantly
lower in the eye movement conditions (M= 4.03,
SD = 1.64), when compared to the tapping
condition (M= 9.35, SD = 4.88), t= 4.29, MSE =
1.23, p<.005, and the control condition (M=
9.25, SD = 4.68), t= 4.33, MSE = 1.2, p<.005.
Place. The number of details was significantly
lower in the eye movement conditions (M= 2.00,
SD = 1.00), when compared to the tapping con-
dition (M= 3.21, SD = 1.08), t= 6.04, MSE = .2,
p<.001, and the control condition (M= 3.6,
SD = 1.94), t= 3.82, MSE = .41, p<.005.
Emotions. The number of details was signifi-
cantly lower in the eye movement conditions
(M= 1.39, SD = 1.86), when compared to the
control condition (M= 2.85, SD = 2.44), t= 4.09,
MSE = .35, p<.005.
Eye-tracking data can also be useful to inter-
pret our results. In the eye movement condition,
we recorded the mean errordistance (in mm),
between the position of a participants gaze and
the position of the dot on the screen. The higher
the conflict between eye movements and the main
task, the greater this error distance should be. We
decided to remove the unique outlier who scored
3 SDs above the mean of the group for these
analyses. The error distance during future think-
ing (M
mm
= 22.87, SD = 14.74) was significantly
greater than that observed in the baseline condi-
tion (M
mm
= 10.30, SD = 4.67), t(12) = 2.82, MSE =
4.44, p< .05. Similarly, the error distance during
episodic memory (M
mm
= 19.55, SD = 10.6) was
significantly greater than that observed in the
baseline condition, t(12) = 2.95, MSE = 3.12, p<
.05. No significant differences were observed
between the error distance during episodic mem-
ory and future-thinking conditions.
DISCUSSION
Event construction plays a pivotal role in remem-
bering the past (Bartlett, 1932; Tulving, 2002).
Moreover, the two most influential hypotheses on
the cognitive underpinning of episodic future
thinking contend that scene construction plays a
crucial role also in foresight, although they inter-
pret this role in different ways (Hassabis &
Maguire, 2007; Schacter & Addis, 2007). The
constructive simulation hypothesis(Schacter &
Addis, 2007) contends that foresight relies on
episodic memory. To contemplate the infinite
array of elaborated simulations of possible
upcoming episodes, people need to mentally
manipulate and rearrange the items that unfolded
in the past. Thus, according to this theory, the
ability to make mental excursions in the future
was a driving force in the evolution of the scene
construction at the bases of episodic memory
(Tulving, 2002). On the other hand, the scene
construction hypothesis(Hassabis & Maguire,
2007) maintains that the mental scene construc-
tion underpins the creation of settings where both
past and future events can unfold, and suggests
that real memories are reconstructed along very
similar lines to the way imagined events are
constructed(Hassabis, Kumaran, & Maguire,
2007, p. 14371).
Our findings stand partly in contrast with the
scene construction hypothesis which states that
episodic memory and future thinking rely on the
same mechanism of scene construction. We sug-
gest that the construction of new future imaginary
experiences requires spatial imagery, while the
(re)construction of personal past episodes does
not, at least not to the same extent.
Despite strong evidence that the two abilities
of forecasting and remembering personal past
share a number of similarities (Schacter & Addis,
2007; for a review see Irish & Piguet, 2013), it is
feasible that constructing a future scene might
require crucial components, which are not funda-
mental in (re)constructing a personal memory.
When reconstructing a personal past event, we
need to look for a unique combination of details.
On the contrary, future thinking is more open-
ended and generative(Suddendorf & Corballis,
2007, p. 302) so that the number of future
scenarios that might be envisaged is potentially
infinite.
Therefore, future thinking might rely on some
components of mental imagery more than on
others. For the purpose of teasing apart the
different components underpinning scene con-
struction in the two experiences of recalling
episodic memories and shaping novel future
events, we used a paradigm that disrupted one
of these components (spatial) relatively sparing
the other one (visual). Our results showed that
voluntary eye movements disrupt future thinking,
while having no effect on the recollection of past
episodic memories. In particular, this effect is
6 DE VITO ET AL.
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
selective for the subcomponents of the internal
details strictly pertaining to the event, the place
and the emotions, whereas the perceptual and the
temporal subcomponents are relatively spared.
The effect of concurrent eye movements was not
selective for the spatial component of future
event representations. This is not surprising.
In fact, a difficulty in manipulating the spatial
components of a recollection may reasonably
determine a difficulty in manipulating the
events, which have intrinsic spatio-temporal
features. As to the emotions, we expected a
reduced emotional impact of future events ima-
gined during concurrent eye movements (Engel-
hard et al., 2010).
In parallel with the failure to produce internal
details, we observed an increment of external
details, when participants generated future epi-
sodes during the eye movement condition. These
data are similar to those reported on heterogen-
eous populations of patients (Addis, Sacchetti,
Allyc, Budson, & Schacter, 2009; de Vito et al.,
2012; Gamboz, de Vito, et al., 2010; Irish et al.,
2012). It has been shown that, when a cognitive
function that is crucial for future thinking is
impaired, people are prone to rely on alternative
strategies that help them to enrich their narratives
(Addis et al., 2009). Thus, it is probable that,
since the generation of future internal details was
hindered, participants tended to fill the gap in
their recollections by providing a greater amount
of tangential, off-target snippets.
The performance in visually tracking a dot
proved similar in future thinking and autobio-
graphical memory task, thus excluding that our
findings could be simply due to a trade-off effect.
Furthermore, although our sample size is rela-
tively small, the findings clearly reveal a strong
effect of eye movements on future thinking.
A number of studies suggested that eye move-
ments during imagery allow to correctly position
each part of a scene (e.g., de Vito et al., 2014;
Postle et al., 2006), while having no effect on the
visual component of mental imagery. The exact
nature of the spatial process that distinguishes the
disruption exerted by eye movement seems to
rely on the fact that commands to the eyes for
each fixation are stored along with the visual
representation and are used as spatial index in a
motor-based coordinate system for the proper
arrangement of parts of an image(Laeng &
Teodorescu, 2002, p. 207). Laeng, Bloem,
DAscenzo, and Tommasi (2014) observed that,
when asked to retrieve the image, participants
were likely to fixate the same regions of space as
those fixated during the perceptual scrutiny of the
shape. Johansson and Johansson (2014) also
showed that constraining eye movements to a
central fixation cross or to an incongruent loca-
tion (i.e., incongruent with the original location of
the to-be-remembered object) more readily affec-
ted memory for the spatial arrangement between
two objects than memory concerning the visual
orientation of an object. Consequently, concur-
rent eye movements might selectively interfere
with spatial imagery, while sparing visual
imagery. These two types of imagery are said to
play a different role in autobiographical memory.
Visual imagery plays a key role (see for instance
Johnson & Raye, 1981). In particular, one com-
ponent of visual imagery, i.e., long-term visual
memory, is fundamental for autobiographical
memory (Greenberg & Rubin, 2003). The preval-
ence of visual imagery in autobiographical mem-
ories has long been acknowledged (Brewer, 1988;
Galton, 1883). More recently, visual images have
been identified as the main representational
format of episodic memories(Conway, 2009,
p. 2308). Indeed, a loss of the ability to generate
visual images may determine a retrograde amnesia
(e.g., Conway, 2005). On the contrary, spatial
deficits do not produce profound global autobio-
graphical amnesia (Barr, Goldberg, Wasserstein, &
Novelly, 1990). According to Conway (2009), the
information contained in the visual images is con-
figural, i.e., the objects represented in the visual
images are already in relation to each other. This
pre-existing configuration may be sufficient when
retrieving an episodic memory, but may not be
enough when distinct details are to be collected
from distinct memories and recombined together,
as in the case of future thinking. In this case, the
objects that are contained in a visual image need to
be differently configured. We may suggest that,
when foreseeing, it is crucial to break down the pre-
existing relations between the objects present in the
autobiographical memories in order to alternatively
arrange each part. Spatial imagery may be very
important in serving this dynamic process. Indeed,
our results confirm that voluntary eye movements
do not hamper the recollection of autobiographical
memories. Eye movements might blur the image
(Andrade et al., 1997), though leaving it still
accessible. On the contrary, crucially, we demon-
strated that eye movements prevent participants
from imagining complex and detailed future scenes.
In sum, it has been widely acknowledged that
episodic memory is particularly important for the
EYE MOVEMENTS DISRUPT EPISODIC FUTURE THINKING 7
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
ability of future thinking. It has been also suggested
that the images derived from experience serve to
provide specific content to more abstract scripts to
be projected in the future (Conway, 2009). How-
ever, our findings suggest that such a specific
evidence may not be projected into the future as it
is, but rather that it needs to be further processed.
To the purpose of imagining novel future events,
people need to spatially reorganise the pre-existent
configuration of mental images of the past. There-
fore, whereas episodic memory is dominated by
visual imagery, episodic future thinking is likely to
be dominated by spatial imagery. This implies that
past and future are not constructed in the same
manner. Future directions, which may unfold from
the present work, should include neuropsychologi-
cal studies aiming at investigating whether or not
patients with specific impairments of spatial
imagery may be still capable to project themselves
into novel future scenarios.
REFERENCES
Addis, D. R., Musicaro, R., Pan, L., & Schacter, D. L.
(2010). Episodic simulation of past and future events
in older adults: Evidence from an experimental
recombination task. Psychology and Aging,25,
369376. doi:10.1037/a0017280
Addis,D.R.,Sacchetti,D.C.,Allyc,B.A.,Budson,A.E.,
& Schacter, D. L. (2009). Episodic simulation of future
events is impaired in mild Alzheimers disease. Neuro
psychologia,47, 26602671. Retrieved from http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.018
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007).
Remembering the past and imagining the future:
Common and distinct neural substrates during
event construction and elaboration. Neuropsycholo-
gia,45, 13631377. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2006.10.016
Andrade, J., Kavanagh, D., & Baddeley, A. (1997).
Eye-movements and visual imagery: A working
memory approach to the treatment of post-trau-
matic stress disorder. British Journal of Clinical
Psychology,36, 209223. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.
1997.tb01408.x
Atance, C. M., & ONeill, D. K. (2001). Episodic future
thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,5, 533539.
doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Barr, W. B., Goldberg, E., Wasserstein, J., & Novelly,
R. A. (1990). Retrograde amnesia following unilat-
eral temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia,28,
243255. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(90)90018-J
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Berryhill, M. E., Picasso, L., Arnold, R., Drowos, D., &
Olson, I. R. (2010). Similarities and differences
between parietal and frontal patients in autobio-
graphical and constructed experience tasks. Neurop-
sychologia,48, 13851393. Retrieved from http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.004
Brewer, W. F. (1988). Memory for randomly sampled
autobiographical events. In U. Neisser & E. Wino-
grad (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological
and traditional approaches to the study of memory (pp.
2190). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection
and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,11(2),
4957. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004
Burani, C., Barca, L., & Arduino, L. S. (2001). Una
base di dati sui valori di età di acquisizione,
frequenza, familiarità, immaginabilità, concretezza,
e altre variabili lessicali e sublessicali per 626 nomi
dellItaliano [A basis of data on the bases of the age
of acquisition, frequency, familiarity, imageability,
concreteness, and other lexical and sub-lexical vari-
ables for 626 Italian names]. Giornale Italiano di
Psicologia,4, 839856. doi:10.1421/379
Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of
Memory and Language,53, 594628. Retrieved from
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005
Conway, M. A. (2009). Episodic memories. Neuropsycho-
logia,47, 23052313. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2009.02.003
DArgembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2004).
Phenomenal characteristics associated with project-
ing oneself back into the past and forward into
the future: Influence of valence and temporal dis
tance. Consciousness and Cognition,13, 844858.
doi:10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.007
de Vito, S. (2012). The beyond within. Cortex,48, 781
784. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.09.008
de Vito, S., Buonocore, A., Bonnefon, J. F., & Della
Sala, S. (2014). Eye movements disrupt spatial but
not visual mental imagery. Cognitive Processing,15,
17. doi:10.1007/s10339-014-0617-1
de Vito, S., & Della Sala, S. (2011). Predicting the
future. Cortex,47, 10181022. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.
2011.02.020
de Vito, S., Gamboz, N., Brandimonte, M. A., Barone,
P., Amboni, M., & Della Sala, S. (2012). Future
thinking in Parkinsons disease: An executive func-
tion? Neuropsychologia,50, 14941501. Retrieved
from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2012.03.001
Engelhard, I. M., van den Hout, M. A., Janssen, W. C., &
van der Beek, J. (2010). Eye movements reduce
vividness and emotionality of flashforwards.
Behaviour Research and Therapy,48, 442447.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.01.003
Farmer, E. W., Berman, J. V. F., & Fletcher, Y. L.
(1986). Evidence for a visuo-spatial scratch-pad in
working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental
Psychology,38, 675688. doi:10.1080/146407486084
01620
Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (1992). G-Power: A priori,
post-hoc, and compromise power analyses for MS-
DOS [Computer program]. Bonn: Bonn University.
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its
development (1st ed.). London: Macmillan.
8 DE VITO ET AL.
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
Gamboz, N., Brandimonte, M. A., & de Vito, S. (2010).
The role of past in the simulation of autobiograph-
ical future episodes. Experimental Psychology,57,
419428. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000052
Gamboz, N., de Vito, S., Brandimonte, M. A., Pappa-
lardo, S., Galeone, F., Iavarone, A., & Della Sala, S.
(2010b). Episodic future thinking in amnesic mild
cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia,48, 2091
2097. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2010.03.030
Greenberg, D. L., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). The
neuropsychology of autobiographical memory. Cor-
tex,39, 687728. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70860-8
Hale, S., Myerson, J., Rhee, S. H., Weiss, C. S., &
Abrams, R. A. (1996). Selective interference with
the maintenance of location information in working
memory. Neuropsychology,10,228240. doi:10.1037/
0894-4105.10.2.228
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007).
Using imagination to understand the neural basis
of episodic memory. The Journal of Neuroscience,
27, 1436514374. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4549-
07.2007
Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing
episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cog-
nitive Sciences,11, 299306. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.
05.001
Irish, M., Addis, D. R., Hodges, J. R., & Piguet, O.
(2012). Considering the role of semantic memory in
episodic future thinking: Evidence from semantic
dementia. Brain,135, 21782191. doi:10.1093/brain/
aws119
Irish, M., & Piguet, O. (2013). The pivotal role of
semantic memory in remembering the past and
imagining the future. Frontiers in Behavioral Neu-
roscience,7,111. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00027
Johansson, R., & Johansson, M. (2014). Look
here, eye movements play a functional role in
memory retrieval. Psychological Science,25, 236
242. doi:10.1177/0956797613498260
Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality
monitoring. Psychological Review,88(1), 6785.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.67
Kosslyn, S. M., & Thompson, W. L. (2000). Shared
mechanisms in visual imagery and visual perception:
Insights from cognitive neuroscience. In M. S.
Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences
(2nd ed., pp. 975985). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Laeng, B., Bloem, I. M., DAscenzo, S., & Tommasi, L.
(2014). Scrutinizing visual images: The role of gaze
in mental imagery and memory. Cognition,131,
263283. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
cognition.2014.01.003.
Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D.-S. (2002). Eye scanpaths
during visual imagery reenact those of perception of
the same visual scene. Cognitive Science,26, 207
231. doi:10.1016/S0364-0213(01)00065-928
Lawrence, B. M., Myerson, J., Oonk, H. M., & Abrams,
R. A. (2001). The effects of eye and limb move-
ments on working memory. Memory,9, 433444.
doi:10.1080/09658210143000047
Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., &
Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiograph-
ical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic
retrieval. Psychology and Aging,17, 677689.
doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677
Logie, R. H., & Marchetti, C. (1991). Visuo-spatial
working memory: Visual, spatial or central execut-
ive? In R. H. Logie & M. Denis (Eds.), Mental
images in human cognition (pp. 105115). Amster-
dam: Elsevier.
Manning, L., Denkova, E., & Unterberger, L. (2013).
Autobiographical significance in past and future
public semantic memory: A case-study. Cortex,49,
20072020. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.11.007
Moscovitch, M. (1994). Cognitive resources and dual-
task interference effects at retrieval in normal
people: The role of the frontal lobes and medial
temporal cortex. Neuropsychology,8, 524534.
doi:10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.524
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York,
NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K.,
Suzuki, K., Yamadori, A. (2003). Thinking of the
future and past: The roles of the frontal pole and the
medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage,19, 13691380.
Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119
(03)00179-4
Pearson, D. G., & Sahraie, A. (2003). Oculomotor
control and the maintenance of spatially and tem-
porally distributed events in visuo-spatial working
memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology A,56, 10891111. doi:10.1080/02724
980343000044
Perky, C. W. (1910). An experimental study of imagina-
tion. American Journal of Psychology,21, 422452.
Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1413350
Postle,B.R.,Idzikowski,C.,DellaSala,S.,Logie,R.H.,&
Baddeley, A. D. (2006). The selective disruption of
spatial working memory by eye movements. The-
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,59(1),
100120. doi:10.1080/17470210500151410
Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2002). Visuospatial imagery.
In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), Handbook of
visuospatial thinking (pp. 4571). New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., & Greenberg, D. L.
(2003). Belief and recollection of autobiographical
memories. Memory & Cognition,31, 887890.
Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/article/
10.3758/BF03196443
Salway, A. F. S., & Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuospatial
working memory, movement control and executive
demands. British Journal of Psychology,86, 253
269. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02560.x
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). Constructive
memory: The ghosts of past and future. Nature,445
(7123), 27. doi:10.1038/445027a
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V.
C., Spreng, R. N., & Szpunar, K. K. (2012). The
future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and
the brain. Neuron,76, 677694. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.
2012.11.001
Smyth, M. M., Pearson, N. A., & Pendleton, L. R. (1988).
Movement and working memory: Patterns and posi-
tions in space. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psycho-
logy,40,497514. doi:10.1080/02724988843000041
EYE MOVEMENTS DISRUPT EPISODIC FUTURE THINKING 9
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
Squire, L. R., van der Horst, A. S., McDuff, S. G. R.,
Frascino, J. C., Hopkins, R.O., & Mauldin, K. N.
(2011). Role of the hippocampus in remember the
past and imagining the future. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Science,108, 1385813863.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1014391107
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of
foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to
humans? Behavioural and Brain Sciences,30, 299351.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X0700203831
Szpunar, K. K. (2011). On subjective time. Cortex,47,
409411. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.008
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to
brain. Annual Review of Psychology,53(1), 125.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114
10 DE VITO ET AL.
Downloaded by [Stefania de Vito] at 11:23 24 June 2014
... Previous research has attributed eye movements during future thinking to visual imagery (Conti & Irish, 2021;de Vito et al., 2015;El Haj & Lenoble, 2018;Wynn et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
We evaluated the relationship between eye movements and future thinking. More specifically, we evaluated whether maintained fixation could influence cognitive characteristics of future thinking. We invited participants to imagine future events in two conditions: while freely exploring a white wall and while fixating a cross on the wall. Results demonstrated fewer and longer fixations, as well as fewer and shorter saccades during maintained fixation condition than in the free gaze condition. Shorter total amplitude of saccades was also observed during the maintained fixation condition than during the free-gaze condition. Regarding the cognitive characteristics of future thinking, fewer spatiotemporal details and less visual imagery, slower retrieval time, and shorter descriptions were observed for future thinking during maintained fixation than during free-gaze condition. These results demonstrate that maintaining fixation results in an effortful construction of future scenarios. We suggest that maintained fixation limits the cognitive resources that are required for future thinking.
... Participants were instructed to use the full range of the rating scale for each episode's vividness (1 to 4) to the best of their abilities. Due to the importance of visual imagery in EFT (De Vito et al. 2015), all participants were scanned with eyes closed, and all instructions were provided via audio (including presentation of the cue sentences for EFT and episodic memory conditions as well as instruction for resting state and rating scales). Therefore, any potential effects of visual imagery were not confounded by requirements to visually attend to external task-relevant stimuli (Daselaar et al. 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Rationale Episodic future thinking (EFT) is a cognitive function that allows individuals to imagine novel experiences that may happen in the future. Prior studies show that EFT is impaired in different groups of substance users. However, there is no evidence regarding the neurobiological mechanisms of EFT in cannabis users. Objectives We aimed to compare brain activations of regular cannabis users and non-using controls during an EFT fMRI task. Exploratory analyses were also conducted to investigate the association between EFT and cannabis use variables (e.g., duration of use, age onset, frequency of use). Methods Twenty current cannabis users and 22 drug-naïve controls underwent an fMRI scanning session while completing a task involving envisioning future-related events and retrieval of past memories as a control condition. The EFT fMRI task was adapted from the autobiographical interview and composed of 20 auditory cue sentences (10 cues for past and 10 cues for future events). Participants were asked to recall a past or generate a future event, in response to the cues, and then rate their vividness after each response. Results We found that cannabis users compared to non-user controls had lower activation within the cerebellum, medial and superior temporal gyrus, lateral occipital cortex, and occipital fusiform gyrus while envisioning future events. Cannabis users rated the vividness of past events significantly lower than non-users (P < 0.005). There were marginal group differences for rating the vividness of future events (P = 0.052). Significant correlations were also found between the medial and superior temporal gyrus activities and behavioral measures of EFT and episodic memory. Conclusions Cannabis users, compared to drug-naïve controls, have lower brain activation in EFT relevant regions. Thus, any attempts to improve aberrant EFT performance in cannabis users may benefit from EFT training.
... The value of non-verbal dimensions has been noted by Berntsen & Bohn's (2010) who proposed physical/bodily reaction as a worthwhile descriptor for FT events. Some existing research provides support to this by linking eye movements (de Vito et al. 2015;El Haj and Lenoble 2018) or activation of pupil dilation (El Haj and Moustafa 2020) with FT events. ...
Article
Full-text available
Future thinking (FT) is the cognitive ability that enables humans to mentally imagine and pre-experience future events. Despite a sharp increase in basic research on future thinking in the last decade, we note the lack of transfer to Human Factors and Ergonomics (HF&E), although the field is increasingly concerned by future artefacts and although theory building on future-oriented HF&E has begun (e.g. prospective ergonomics). This article gives an overview of key findings regarding future thinking, namely i) underlying cognitive mechanisms, ii) functions and obstacles, iii) common descriptors of future thinking, iv) recommendations as to the possible improvement of future thinking during user research and, v) methods for the assessment of future thinking abilities. This synthesis can support HF&E practitioners in fine-tuning their future-oriented methods.
... Imagining such scenarios places a greater burden on mental construction processes than remembering past scenarios that can be captured by eye movements. For example, one experiment compared past and future event generation when making concurrent eye movements and found that these movements disrupted the ability to produce internal details only when imagining the future (de Vito, Buonocore, Bonnefon, & Della Sala, 2015). Similarly, other studies have found that excessive eye movements can hamper the ability to form clear visualised images (Andrade, Kavanagh, & Baddeley, 1997) and extract information from a mental construction (Ferreira, Apel, & Henderson, 2008). ...
Article
Autobiographical experiences can be mentally constructed as generalised events or as spatial scenes. We investigated the commonalities and distinctions in using episodic and visual imagery processes to imagine autobiographical scenarios as events or scenes. Participants described personal scenarios framed as future events or spatial scenes. We analyzed the number and type of episodic details within the descriptions. To measure imagery processing, we monitored eye-movements and examined the impact of viewing a imagery disrupting stimulus (Dynamic Visual Noise; DVN) when these descriptions were made. We found that events were described with more generalised details and scenes with more perceptual details. DVN reduced the number of episodic details generated for all descriptions and eye fixation rates negatively correlated with the number of these details that were generated. This suggests that different content is used to imagine event- or scene-based experiences and imagery contributes similarly to the episodic specificity of these imaginations.
... There is additionally evidence that voluntary smooth pursuit may disrupt mental imagery (Postle, Idzikowski, Sala, Logie, & Baddeley, 2006) and especially spatial imagery (de Vito, Buonocore, Bonnefon, & Della Sala, 2014). As for future thinking, a study found that eye movements disrupt future but not past thinking (de Vito, Buonocore, et al., 2015). Therefore, it is possible that our participants made more saccades during past than during future thinking because eye movements when constructing the future would be disruptive. ...
Article
Full-text available
We investigated eye movement during past and future thinking. Participants were invited to retrieve past events and to imagine future events while their scan path was recorded by an eye-tracker. Past thinking triggered more fixation (p < .05), and saccade counts (p < .05) than future thinking. Past and future thinking triggered a similar duration of fixations and saccades, as well as a similar amplitude of saccades. Interestingly, participants rated past thinking as more vivid than future thinking (p < .01). Therefore, the vividness of past thinking seems to be accompanied by an increased number of fixations and saccades. Fixations and saccades in past thinking can be interpreted as an attempt by the visual system to find (through saccades) and activate (through fixations) stored memory representations. The same interpretation can be applied to future thinking as this ability requires activation of past experiences. However, future thinking triggers fewer fixations and saccades than past thinking: this may be due to its decreased demand on visual imagery, but could also be related to a potentially deleterious effect of eye movements on spatial imagery required for future thinking.
Article
Episodic future thinking (EFT) refers to the critical ability that enables people to construct and pre-experience the vivid mental imagery about future events, which impacts on the decision-making for individuals and group. Although EFT is generally believed to have a visual nature by theorists, little neuroscience evidence has been provided to verify this assumption. Here, by employing the approach of connectome-based predictive modeling (CPM) and graph-theoretical analysis, we analyzed resting-state functional brain image from 191 participants to predict their variability of EFT ability (leave-one-out cross-validation), and validated the results by applying different parcellation schemas and feature selection thresholds. At the connectome strength level, CPM-based analysis revealed that EFT ability could be predicted by the connectome strength of visual network. Besides, at the network level, graph-theoretical analysis showed that EFT ability could be predicted by the network efficiency of visual network. Moreover, these findings were replicated using different parcellation schemas and feature selection thresholds. These results robustly and collectively supported that the visual network might be the neural substrates underlying EFT ability from a comprehensive perspective of resting-state functional connectivity strength and the neural network. This study provides indications on how the function of visual network supports EFT ability, and enhances our understanding of the EFT ability from a neural basis perspective.
Article
Recent work indicates that eye movements support the retrieval of episodic memories by reactivating the spatiotemporal context in which they were encoded. Although similar mechanisms have been thought to support simulation of future episodes, there is currently no evidence favoring this proposal. In the present study, we investigated the role of eye movements in episodic simulation by comparing the gaze patterns of individual participants imagining future scene and event scenarios to across-participant gaze templates for those same scenarios, reflecting their shared features (i.e., schemas). Our results provide novel evidence that eye movements during episodic simulation in the face of distracting visual noise are (1) schema-specific and (2) predictive of simulation success. Together, these findings suggest that eye movements support episodic simulation via reinstatement of scene and event schemas, and more broadly, that interactions between the memory and oculomotor effector systems may underlie critical cognitive processes including constructive episodic simulation.
Article
Much of the rich internal world constructed by humans is derived from, and experienced through, visual mental imagery. Despite growing appreciation of visual exploration in guiding episodic memory processes, extant theories of prospection have yet to accommodate the precise role of visual mental imagery in the service of future-oriented thinking. We propose that the construction of future events relies on the assimilation of perceptual details originally experienced, and subsequently reinstantiated, predominantly in the visual domain. Individual differences in the capacity to summon discrete aspects of visual imagery can therefore account for the diversity of content generated by humans during future simulation. Our integrative framework provides a novel testbed to query alterations in future thinking in health and disease.
Article
Full-text available
To examine the relationship between visual imagery and autobiographical memory, eye position and pupil size were recorded while participants first searched for memories and then reconstructed the retrieved memories (Experiment 1), or only searched for memories (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, we observed that, although recollective experience was not associated with the number of fixations per minute, memories that took longer to retrieve were linked to increased pupil size. In Experiment 2, we observed that directly retrieved memories were recalled more quickly and were accompanied by smaller pupils than generatively retrieved memories. After correcting for response time, retrieval mode also produced an effect, showing that decreased pupil size is not simply due to directly retrieved memories being recalled more quickly. These findings provide compelling evidence that objective measures, such as pupil size, can be used alongside subjective measures, such as self-reports, to distinguish between directly retrieved and generatively retrieved memories.
Article
Examining eye movement patterns when encoding and retrieving visually complex memories is useful to understand the link between visuo-perceptual processes and how associated details are represented within these memories. Here, we used images of real-world scenes (e.g., a couple grocery shopping) to examine how encoding and retrieval eye movements are linked to the details used to describe complex images during these two phases of memory. Given that memories are often elaborated upon during retrieval, we also examined whether eye-movements at retrieval related to details that were the same as those described when encoding the image (reinstated details) as well as details about the image event that were not initially described at encoding (newly generated details). Testing young healthy participants, we found that retrieval eye movements, specifically eye fixation rate, predicted reinstated details, but not newly generated details. This suggests that visuo-perceptual processes are preferentially engaged at retrieval to reactivate perceived information. At encoding, we found a relationship between eye movements and detail generation that changed over time. This relationship was positive early on in the encoding phase but changed to a negative relationship later in the phase, indicating that a unique relationship exists between activating visuo-perceptual processes during early encoding versus late encoding. Overall, our results provide new insights into how visuo-perceptual processes contribute to different components of complex memory.
Article
Full-text available
Semantic dementia is a progressive neurodegenerative condition characterized by the profound and amodal loss of semantic memory in the context of relatively preserved episodic memory. In contrast, patients with Alzheimer's disease typically display impairments in episodic memory, but with semantic deficits of a much lesser magnitude than in semantic dementia. Our understanding of episodic memory retrieval in these cohorts has greatly increased over the last decade, however, we know relatively little regarding the ability of these patients to imagine and describe possible future events, and whether episodic future thinking is mediated by divergent neural substrates contingent on dementia subtype. Here, we explored episodic future thinking in patients with semantic dementia (n = 11) and Alzheimer's disease (n = 11), in comparison with healthy control participants (n = 10). Participants completed a battery of tests designed to probe episodic and semantic thinking across past and future conditions, as well as standardized tests of episodic and semantic memory. Further, all participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging. Despite their relatively intact episodic retrieval for recent past events, the semantic dementia cohort showed significant impairments for episodic future thinking. In contrast, the group with Alzheimer's disease showed parallel deficits across past and future episodic conditions. Voxel-based morphometry analyses confirmed that atrophy in the left inferior temporal gyrus and bilateral temporal poles, regions strongly implicated in semantic memory, correlated significantly with deficits in episodic future thinking in semantic dementia. Conversely, episodic future thinking performance in Alzheimer's disease correlated with atrophy in regions associated with episodic memory, namely the posterior cingulate, parahippocampal gyrus and frontal pole. These distinct neuroanatomical substrates contingent on dementia group were further qualified by correlational analyses that confirmed the relation between semantic memory deficits and episodic future thinking in semantic dementia, in contrast with the role of episodic memory deficits and episodic future thinking in Alzheimer's disease. Our findings demonstrate that semantic knowledge is critical for the construction of novel future events, providing the necessary scaffolding into which episodic details can be integrated. Further research is necessary to elucidate the precise contribution of semantic memory to future thinking, and to explore how deficits in self-projection manifest on behavioural and social levels in different dementia subtypes.
Article
Full-text available
Processes of word recognition and production are affected by many lexical and sublexical variables, in both adults and children, in both unimpaired and impaired language conditions. In the present paper, values are reported for 626 Italian nouns relative to the following variables: Word age of acquisition, familiarity, imageability, concreteness, frequency in adult and children written corpora, spoken frequency, number of orthographic neighbors, mean bigram frequency, length in syllables and in letters. A classification of the initial phoneme is also included. Correlations among variables are discussed. Current studies dealing with the effects of these variables in lexical processing are reviewed. With respect to some recent studies which investigated the role of some of these variables in picture naming for Italian (Nisi, Longoni e Snodgrass, 2000; Lotto, Dell'Acqua e Job, in press), the present work deals specifically with properties of words, and extends the data base by including variables specifically relevant to word processing, with main reference to word naming. The present lexical data base, available in electronic format, has a specific utility for research on lexical processing, and provides a useful tool for the study, assessment and rehabilitation of Italian patients with disturbances of lexical processing
Article
Full-text available
It has long been known that eye movements are functionally involved in the generation and maintenance of mental images. Indeed, a number of studies demonstrated that voluntary eye movements interfere with mental imagery tasks (e.g., Laeng and Teodorescu in Cogn Sci 26:207-231, 2002). However, mental imagery is conceived as a multifarious cognitive function with at least two components, a spatial component and a visual component. The present study investigated the question of whether eye movements disrupt mental imagery in general or only its spatial component. We present data on healthy young adults, who performed visual and spatial imagery tasks concurrently with a smooth pursuit. In line with previous literature, results revealed that eye movements had a strong disruptive effect on spatial imagery. Moreover, we crucially demonstrated that eye movements had no disruptive effect when participants visualized the depictive aspects of an object. Therefore, we suggest that eye movements serve to a greater extent the spatial than the visual component of mental imagery.
Chapter
This chapter is divided into two parts. The first describes the effect of Pat Rabbitt's influence in encouraging the first author to use the increasingly sophisticated methods of ageing research to answer questions about the fundamental characteristics of working memory, together with reflections on why so little of this work reached publication. The second part presents a brief review of the literature on working memory and ageing, followed by an account of more recent work attempting to apply the traditional method of experimental dissociation to research on normal ageing and Alzheimer's disease. The discussion suggests that even such simple methods can throw light on both the processes of ageing and the understanding of working memory.
Chapter
Working memory comprises a central executive, responsible for reasoning, decision making, and coordinating the activities of two subsidiary systems. One of the subsystems, the articulatory loop, is responsible for retention of verbal material, while the other subsystem, the visuo-spatial scratch pad, is responsible for retention of visual or spatial information. A visual temporary memory system could be responsible for retaining the imaged pattern of numbers in specific locations in a matrix. In contrast, if the subject is genuinely required to retain a sequence of movements, rather than a static pattern, they would be more likely to rely on a spatial temporary memory system. This chapter reports the experiment that was designed to investigate this possibility. In this experiment, subjects were required to retain information from one of two kinds of visually presented displays, either color hues or the sequential order in which a series of squares was presented at different locations on the screen.
Article
Research on episodic memory has established that spontaneous eye movements occur to spaces associated with retrieved information even if those spaces are blank at the time of retrieval. Although it has been claimed that such looks to "nothing" can function as facilitatory retrieval cues, there is currently no conclusive evidence for such an effect. In the present study, we addressed this fundamental issue using four direct eye manipulations in the retrieval phase of an episodic memory task: (a) free viewing on a blank screen, (b) maintaining central fixation, (c) looking inside a square congruent with the location of the to-be-recalled objects, and (d) looking inside a square incongruent with the location of the to-be-recalled objects. Our results provide novel evidence of an active and facilitatory role of gaze position during memory retrieval and demonstrate that memory for the spatial relationship between objects is more readily affected than memory for intrinsic object features.