ArticlePDF Available

Developmental Consequences of Fetal Exposure to Drugs: What We Know and What We Still Must Learn

Authors:

Abstract

Most drugs of abuse easily cross the placenta and can affect fetal brain development. In utero exposures to drugs thus can have long-lasting implications for brain structure and function. These effects on the developing nervous system, before homeostatic regulatory mechanisms are properly calibrated, often differ from their effects on mature systems. In this review, we describe current knowledge on how alcohol, nicotine, cocaine, amphetamine, ecstasy and opiates (among other drugs) produce alterations in neurodevelopmental trajectory. We focus both on animal models and available clinical and imaging data from longitudinal human cohorts. Early studies of fetal exposures focused on classic teratological methods that are insufficient for revealing more subtle effects that are nevertheless very behaviorally relevant. Modern mechanistic approaches have informed us greatly as to how to potentially ameliorate the induced deficits in brain formation and function, but conclude that better delineation of sensitive periods, dose-response relationships, and long-term longitudinal studies assessing future risk of offspring to exhibit learning disabilities, mental health disorders, and limited neural adaptations are crucial to limit the societal impact of these exposures.Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews accepted article preview online, 18 June 2014; doi:10.1038/npp.2014.147.
Developmental Consequences of Fetal Exposure to
Drugs: What We Know and What We Still Must Learn
Emily J Ross
1
, Devon L Graham
2
, Kelli M Money
3
and Gregg D Stanwood*
,2,4
1
Chemical & Physical Biology Program, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA;
2
Department of Pharmacology, Vanderbilt
University, Nashville, TN, USA;
3
Neuroscience Graduate Program, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA;
4
The Vanderbilt
Kennedy Center for Research on Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
Most drugs of abuse easily cross the placenta and can affect fetal brain development. In utero exposures to drugs thus can
have long-lasting implications for brain structure and function. These effects on the developing nervous system, before
homeostatic regulatory mechanisms are properly calibrated, often differ from their effects on mature systems. In this review,
we describe current knowledge on how alcohol, nicotine, cocaine, amphetamine, Ecstasy, and opiates (among other drugs)
produce alterations in neurodevelopmental trajectory. We focus both on animal models and available clinical and imaging
data from cross-sectional and longitudinal human studies. Early studies of fetal exposures focused on classic teratological
methods that are insufficient for revealing more subtle effects that are nevertheless very behaviorally relevant. Modern
mechanistic approaches have informed us greatly as to how to potentially ameliorate the induced deficits in brain formation
and function, but conclude that better delineation of sensitive periods, dose–response relationships, and long-term
longitudinal studies assessing future risk of offspring to exhibit learning disabilities, mental health disorders, and limited neural
adaptations are crucial to limit the societal impact of these exposures.
Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews (2015) 40, 61–87; doi:10.1038/npp.2014.147; published online 30 July 2014
INTRODUCTION
Substance use disorders among pregnant women continues
to be a major public health concern, posing risk to the
child’s development, and imposing socioeconomic burdens
on society by increasing needs for medical and social
services. Given the crucial roles for the major protein
targets of drugs of abuse in shaping brain development
(Bhide, 2009; Bonnin and Levitt, 2011; Frederick and
Stanwood, 2009; Hohmann, 2003; Kater and Lipton, 1995;
Lauder, 1993; Money and Stanwood, 2013; Stanwood and
Levitt, 2004), it should not be surprising that fetal drug
exposures have been linked to a wide variety of brain
deficits. In this review, we will focus on: (1) what is
currently known about the likely pattern of substance use
among pregnant or women of childbearing age; (2) the
cellular and molecular pathways by which prenatal drug
exposure may influence structural and functional brain
development; (3) current studies on outcomes of exposed
individuals across various areas of functioning (neurobiol-
ogy, physical growth, intelligence, executive functioning,
behavior, and psychopathology); (4) reviews of current
experimental animal models, and (5) current limitations in
understanding and potential avenues for future research.
While beyond the scope of the current review, untreated
drug abuse/addiction also typically coincides with poor
nutrition and prenatal care, which increases the risk of
obstetric complications and disrupted developmental pro-
cesses in the fetus. Thus, beyond the specific developmental
biology of individual drugs, there are additional common
factors that can produce deficits in neurodevelopmental
trajectories.
Recent data suggest that nearly 25 million Americans aged
12 or older are current illicit drug users; this estimate
represents 9.2 percent of the population. Illicit drugs include
marijuana/hashish, cocaine (including crack), heroin, hallu-
cinogens, inhalants, or prescription-type psychotherapeutics
used non-medically (Substance Abuse and Mental Health
Services Administration, 2013). Current illicit drug use
among pregnant women aged 15–44, has remained constant
at 5.9% despite efforts of prevention and education programs
(Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administra-
tion, 2013). In fact, the pattern of rates of current illicit drug
use among young adolescent females has grown to where
they are now more likely than males in their age group to be
current non-medical users of psychoactive drugs.
*Correspondence: Dr GD Stanwood, Department of Pharmacology,
Vanderbilt University, 23rd Avenue South, 476 RRB, Nashville TN 37232-
6600, USA, Tel: +1 615 936 3861, Fax: +1 615 936 2202,
E-mail: gregg.stanwood@vanderbilt.edu
Received 3 March 2014; revised 29 May 2014; accepted 2 June 2014;
accepted article preview online 18 June 2014
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS (2015) 40, 61– 87
&
2015 American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. All rights reserved 0893-133X/15
...............................................................................................................................................................
www.neuropsychopharmacologyreviews.org 61
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
Legal drugs such as alcohol and nicotine also represent a
significant hurdle regarding unintended effects on the fetus.
The 2012 NSDUH also found that among pregnant women
aged 15–44, the prevalence of reported current alcohol and
cigarette usage rates has not changed substantially in the
past decade. Although the US population as a whole is
smoking less, the past month cigarette use among pregnant
women aged 15–44 has held at B16–17% over the past
2 years (as compared with 18% in 2002–2003). An annual
average of B8.5% also reports current alcohol use during
pregnancy. As described below, legality of a drug does not
necessarily correlate with its safety profile, and as research-
ers and clinicians, we need better therapies and educational
strategies for pregnant and/or breastfeeding women.
Different drugs and biological processes are often
modeled in different animal species, each with its own
unique developmental timeline (Figure 1) and advantages/
disadvantages. This greatly complicates translatability to
humans. Rodents (mice, rats) and lagomorphs (rabbits) are
born relatively early, as compared with humans, and thus
those offspring must be treated with drugs postnatally if the
aim is to model third trimester human exposure (Figure 1).
However, this omits the transplacental transfer that is
inherent in human fetal drug exposure. Issues of accurate
dose levels, route of administration, and pharmacokinetics/
bioavailability are also often underappreciated in animal
models.
Moreover, drugs can alter fetal development through a
wide variety of mechanisms (Figure 2). For example, if the
drug crosses the placenta (and the vast majority of drugs of
abuse do cross), then it can directly act on its molecular
target in the fetus. Drugs can also act directly on the uterus
and/or placenta. These effects would include altering
placental secretory activity or uteroplacental blood flow,
for example. Finally, the drug can produce effects on the
mother’s physiology that may secondarily influence the
fetus, such as increased secretion of stress hormones or
altered maternal health behaviors attributable to the
mother’s addiction. And although it is beyond our scope
to review it here, it has recently become clear that paternal
exposures, to drugs such as cocaine, during spermatogen-
esis, can also influence offspring brain development and
neurobehavioral development through epigenetic mechan-
isms, at least in animal models (Killinger et al, 2012;
Rodgers et al, 2013; Vassoler et al, 2013). This topic is thus
extremely complicated; nevertheless, we will do our best to
review both animal models and clinical and imaging data
from longitudinal human cohorts following fetal exposure
to specific drugs of abuse.
PSYCHOSTIMULANTS
Methamphetamine
Methamphetamine (METH) is metabolized in rat and
human livers to the main metabolites, amphetamine,
and 4-hydroxymethamphetamine (Caldwell et al, 1972)
(prenatal amphetamine exposure discussed below). Due to
the addition of a methyl group, METH has a higher
lipophilicity than amphetamine, allowing more rapid
transport of the drug across the blood–brain barrier (Barr
et al, 2006). METH has a high potential for abuse and
Prenatal weeks
Prenatal days
Human
(G270)
Macaque
monkey
(G165)
Rabbit
(G31)
Mouse
(G18.5)
Rat
(G21.5)
1st trimester
1st trimester 2nd trimester
2nd trimester
3rd trimester
3rd trimester
24 28
0
012
1310 17
0
123
1512 19 1 2 3
2
2
3
3
4
912 24 38
7 8 15 18 23
13 18
Postnatal weeks
Figure 1. Major neurodevelopmental events across species. Schematic
diagram that aligns human brain development with several animal models
(monkey, rabbit, rat, and mouse) often used in studies of fetal drug
exposure. Note in particular that the rodent equivalent of third trimester
fetal development occurs postnatally.
Prenatal
substance
use
Additional risk markers
(maternal
and paternal age,
education, stressors)
Polypharmacy
exposure
(alcohol, tobacco,
marijuana)
Mental state
• Anxiety
• Depression
• Insomnia
• Memory loss
• Hallucinations
• Abnormal behavior
Increased risk
• Blood, heart, and skin
infections
• Arrhythmias
• Infectious diseases
• Seizures
• Stroke
• Hypothermia
Umbilical cord
• Drugs are passed
directly to fetus
• Tissue can be used to
detect drugs
Mother’s blood
• Increased levels of CO
2
,
CO, and blood pressure
• Anemia
• Pre-eclampsia
Breastfeeding
• Continued exposure
• Decreased prolactin
release and supply
Amniotic fluid
• Possible
accumulation of
intact drugs
Lungs
• Pulmonary edema
• Breathing problems
Uterus
• Premature birth
• Contractions
Placenta
• Vasoconstriction
• Placental insufficiency
• Placental abruption
Figure 2. Biological targets of fetal drug exposures. Drugs of abuse not
only target the developing fetal brain directly, but can exert effects
through a variety of organs with the mother, including the uterus,
placenta, heart, lungs, and brain.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
62
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
addiction compared with other amphetamine-type stimu-
lants, affecting the serotonin (5-HT) plasma membrane
transporter (SERT) and activating the psychological reward
system by triggering a cascade of the massive release of
dopamine (DA) in the brain. This release of DA occurs
by multiple mechanisms including: binding to reuptake
transporter (DAT), the displacement of vesicles and
inhibition of monoamine oxidase, and enhancing DAT-
mediated reverse transport of DA transport across the
plasma membrane (Scott et al, 2007). These targets of
METH appear by mid-gestation in the fetal brain (Bhide,
2009; Frederick and Stanwood, 2009; Malanga and
Kosofsky, 1999; Money and Stanwood, 2013).
The use of METH in the United States and in other parts
of the world still remains a serious public health concern.
According to the United Nations, 1.3% of the general
population in Central and North America use ampheta-
mine-type stimulants (UNODC, 2013). There are few studies
that have surveyed the prevalence of METH use among
pregnant women. The only large-scale investigation to
report METH prevalence for pregnant women has been the
Infant Development, Environment, and Lifestyle (IDEAL)
study. They found that B5% of women self-reported METH
use during their pregnancy (Arria et al, 2006) in areas of the
United States where METH use was of high concern.
Furthermore, over 40% of the women enrolled in the IDEAL
study continued using the drug during their third trimester,
and approximately half did not significantly change their
personal use during the course of the pregnancy (Della
Grotta et al, 2010).
Despite the fact that METH has been utilized by pregnant
women for many years, relatively little is known about
the effects of METH during early infancy and even less is
known about the long-term effects. The most common
effects noted in newborns indicate that METH use is
associated with growth restriction, decreased weight, length,
and head circumference (Little et al, 1988; Nguyen et al,
2010; Smith et al, 2003; Smith et al, 2006b). Echoencepha-
lography has revealed congenital anomalies in exposed
neonates include cardiac anomalies, cranial abnormalities,
and abnormal brain development closely resembling those
in ill, asphyxiated infants (Dixon and Bejar, 1989). Mild
withdrawal symptoms have also been noted in newborns
(Oro and Dixon, 1987; Smith et al, 2003), although these
symptoms are not as common. In animal models, increased
DNA oxidation and postnatal functional deficits in motor
coordination were documented in neonatal METH-exposed
mice (Jeng et al, 2005).
Several recent studies have examined the effects of METH
exposure on childhood growth patterns and behavior after
birth. Focusing on physical growth patterns, Zabaneh et al
(2012) found children exposed prenatally to METH have a
modest decrease in height during the first 3 years of life
with no observed difference in weight, head circumference,
or weight-for-length trajectories. Prenatal METH exposure
also is associated with significantly reduced caudate nucleus
volume and cortical thickness increases in perisylvian and
orbital–frontal cortices (Derauf et al, 2012; Zabaneh et al,
2012). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) suggests lower
diffusion and higher fractional anisotropy in METH-
exposed children at 3–4 years of age, indicating that fetal
METH may alter white matter tracts (Cloak et al, 2009).
In a magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) study,
METH-exposed 3–4 year-old children exhibited higher total
creatine, N-acetyl compounds, and glutamate þglutamine
concentrations in the frontal white matter, but lower
myoinositol and myoinositol/creatine in the thalamus
(Chang et al, 2009). The higher metabolite concentrations
of N-acetyl compounds, total creatine, and glutamate þ
glutamine suggest a higher neuronal density or cellular
compactness in the white matter, whereas lower myoinositol
suggests lower glial content in the thalamus. Furthermore,
the METH-exposed children performed significantly worse
on a visual motor integration task, which correlated with
lower myoinositol in the thalamus (Chang et al, 2009).
In another magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study on
children ranging from 3 to 16 years of age, exposed
children scored lower on measures of visual motor
integration, attention, verbal memory, and long-term spatial
memory. There were no differences among the groups in
motor skills, short delay spatial memory, or measures of
nonverbal intelligence. Despite comparable whole brain
volumes in each group, the METH-exposed children
appeared to have subtle but significant decreases in size
or volume in certain brain regions, including the putamen,
globus pallidus, and hippocampus. These reductions
correlated with poorer sustained attention and delayed
verbal memory (Chang et al, 2004). It is important to note
that although the study had an impressive age range, sample
sizes were limited.
Concentrating on long-term motor skills after birth,
Smith et al (2011) observed a subtle METH exposure effect
on fine motor performance at 1 year, with the poorest
performance observed in the most heavily exposed children.
However, by 3 years of age, no differences in fine motor
performance were observed. These findings suggest METH
exposure has modest motor effects in the first year of life,
but that this may be mostly resolved by 3 years of age. The
IDEAL study found that prenatal METH exposure was
associated with child externalizing behavioral problems at
5 years and parenting stress and psychological symptoms
experienced by primary caregivers were associated with
increased child behavioral problems; indicating these
effected children may have more difficulties negotiating
the increasing complex academic and social demands of
school-age children (LaGasse et al, 2012; Twomey et al,
2013).
Animal models may give us insight and provide correla-
tion to these studies examining the effects of prenatal
METH exposure and if these effects will remain into
adulthood. Results from mouse exposure to METH during
a time period equivalent to the third trimester of human
fetal gestation (see Figure 1) impaired weight gain, reduced
novel location recognition, and impaired novel object
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
63
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
recognition in both male and female mice during adoles-
cence. In rats, neonatal exposure to METH produces deficits
in latency and memory in a Morris water maze (Vorhees
et al, 2000). Both prenatal and neonatal exposure also
caused impaired spatial learning (Slamberova et al, 2005;
Williams et al, 2003b; Williams et al, 2003d; Williams et al,
2002) and neonatal exposure altered the adrenal response to
a forced swim stressor, suggesting that the adrenal output
during learning may contribute to the spatial learning
deficits (Williams et al, 2003a). Rats exposed to METH
in utero showed changes in the mesolimbic dopaminergic
system and were more sensitive to the administration of
the acute dose of METH in adulthood (Bubenikova-
Valesova et al, 2009). This indicates that offspring exposed
to METH in utero could be more sensitive to METH
and potentially to other psychostimulants (Bubenikova-
Valesova et al, 2009).
Although relatively little is known about the effects of
METH during early infancy or the following long-term
effects, recently there have been studies attempting to
examine children exposed to METH in utero. Further
studies are important to aid in prevention programs and
treatment for these individuals. Comparing the effects of
prenatal METH exposure on infant and childhood growth
between the United States and New Zealand demonstrated
that the effects of prenatal METH differed across countries
(Abar et al, 2013). In the study, the prenatal METH-exposed
children in New Zealand fared better than exposed children
in the United States. In addition, according to the United
Nations, 1.3% of the general population in Central and
North America use amphetamine-type stimulants (UNODC,
2013). This reported use is at higher levels than the global
average of 0.7% of the general population (UNODC, 2013).
These studies suggest that the United States needs improved
prevention programs, better pre- and postnatal treatments,
and caregiver support for this exposed population.
Amphetamine
Amphetamines (alpha-methyl-phenethylamine) (AMPH),
commonly known as ‘Speed’, continue to be widely used
by women of childbearing age. The users are either legally
prescribed AMPHs for medical reasons or are nonmedical
users. For example, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) affects B5% of adults (Fayyad et al, 2007) in the
United States. Although ADHD is less common in females
than males, a significant percentage of women of child-
bearing age are thus likely prescribed AMPH or other
stimulants as a treatment for ADHD. The prevalence of
women of childbearing age using non-medical AMPH is
currently unknown, and even less is known about the
frequency of use during pregnancy. Further studies are
required to determine the true frequency of illegal AMPH-
exposed pregnant and lactating women, so that appropriate
care can be provided for mother and her family.
After prenatal exposure, AMPH has been detected in
human umbilical cord (Jones et al, 2009), plasma (Dearlove
and Betteridge, 1992), and placenta as early as the first
trimester (Joya et al, 2010). The cellular actions of AMPH
are nearly identical to METH (see section above), including
increasing the levels of norepinephrine (NE), DA, and 5-HT
in the synaptic cleft via transporter reuptake inhibition.
This increased availability allows the monoamines to act
upon post-synaptic receptors. The use of AMPHs during
pregnancy increases the risk of adverse effects on the
outcome of pregnancy, such as placental hemorrhage
(Figure 2). This is mediated, at least in part, by stimulation
of contractions in the uterus and by targeting NET and
SERT in the human placenta (Cordeaux et al, 2008;
Ramamoorthy et al, 1995). These actions have been
hypothesized to contribute to preterm labor associated
with AMPH exposure (Cordeaux et al, 2008). To date, fetal
AMPH exposure has not been proven to be directly
teratogenic; however, in primary human cell cultures,
AMPH reduced folic acid uptake and this could potentially
lead to placental and/or fetal toxicity (Keating et al, 2009).
Animal studies revealed at a prepubertal age, an enhance-
ment of D1 receptors in the dorsal striatum and nucleus
accumbens (NAc) and a decrement of the D3 receptors
in NAc, olfactory tubercle, and the islands of Calleja. In
contrast, at a postpubertal age, the authors instead
measured an increase in the levels of DAT in the NAc and
striatum, and a decrease in D2 receptor expression in the
NAc shell. In addition, acute AMPH induces a marked
decrease in locomotor activity in rats following prenatal
AMPH exposure (Flores et al, 2011). These developmental
and behavioral changes in animal models associated with
in utero AMPH exposure provide insights to the mechan-
isms causing changes in affective, behavioral, and cognitive
outcomes in exposed children.
A meta-analysis of 10 studies of low-to-moderate risk
of bias submits that AMPH exposure in pregnancy is
associated with higher odds of preterm birth, low birth
weight, and small size for gestational age (Ladhani et al,
2011). Birth weight as a continuous variable was also found
to be significantly lower among exposed women. Gesta-
tional age, however, was not found to be significantly
different (Ladhani et al, 2011). The most extensive follow-
up data on affective, behavioral, and cognitive outcomes
following prenatal AMPHs are provided by Swedish
researchers who tracked a cohort of 65 AMPH-exposed
children from birth to age 14. They reported in children
with continuous AMPH exposure throughout gestation, a
variety of adverse physical, cognitive, emotional, and social
effects, including increased prevalence of ADHD, aggres-
sion, and learning difficulties attributed to deficits in
attention, memory, and motivation (Eriksson et al, 1994;
Eriksson et al, 2000). Furthermore, a relationship between
head circumference at birth and at 1 year of age predicted
language and mathematics proficiency at 14 years of age
(Eriksson et al, 2000). As with its parent drug METH,
further studies on prenatal AMPH exposure is needed
to better understand effects from prescribed AMPHs or
non-medical users (Oei et al, 2012).
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
64
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine
3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), commonly
known as Ecstasy, is a derivative of AMPH that has both
stimulant and hallucinogenic properties. It acts as a powerful
releasing agent of 5-HT, NE, and DA and also acts as a
reuptake inhibitor of their high-affinity transporters (de la
Torre et al, 2004; Rudnick and Wall, 1992). MDMA is actively
transported through the plasma membrane and can then
inhibit the vesicular monoamine transporter, resulting in
increased concentrations of 5-HT, NE, and DA in the
cytoplasm. MDMA can also directly bind a number of
receptors with moderate affinity, including a2-adrenergic
(adrenaline) and 5-HT2A receptors (de la Torre et al, 2004).
A single MDMA injection to pregnant rat dams results in
MDMA transfer into the fetal compartment (Campbell et al,
2006). However, emerging data also suggests key roles for
maternal and placental 5-HT in regulating brain development
(Bonnin et al, 2011; Bonnin and Levitt, 2011) (Figure 2).
In the United States, data from the 2012 National Survey
on Drug Use and Health estimate that about 6.2 percent
of individuals aged 12 or older had used Ecstasy at least
once in their lifetime. In addition, B900 000 individuals
tried Ecstasy for the first time in 2012. Although data from
pregnant women was not presented, the prevalence of at
least once in lifetime Ecstasy use in 2012 saw a significant
increase from past-year use, and past-year use for eighth
through twelfth graders remained stable (UNODC, 2013).
To date, there are only a few studies examining prenatal
exposure to MDMA in humans, despite concerns about
its potential harmful effects to the fetus (Ho et al, 2001;
McElhatton et al, 1999; Singer et al, 2012; van Tonningen-
van Driel et al, 1999). Very little is known about the
characteristics of pregnant women who use Ecstasy. Using
data collected regarding women contacting the Motherrisk
Helpline in Toronto (Ho et al, 2001), MDMA users tended
to be younger, single, Caucasian, binge drinkers, and had
a higher prevalence of psychiatric symptoms. Most of these
users discontinued MDMA once pregnancy was known,
but because the recruitment was based on contact initiated
by the mothers, the sample likely overrepresented more
‘motivated’ women. The few available studies demonstrate
that pregnant MDMA users exhibit a clustering of repro-
ductive risk factors that contribute to neurobehavioral and
teratological outcomes. A retrospective report of 136 babies
exposed to Ecstasy in utero noted premature births,
a significantly increased risk of congenital defects, cardio-
vascular anomalies, and musculoskeletal anomalies
(McElhatton et al, 1999). Another study in the Netherlands
reported congenital cardiac malformation and spontaneous
abortions (van Tonningen-van Driel et al, 1999). A recent
neurobehavioral outcome study suggests that prenatal
MDMA exposure predicts poorer infant mental and motor
development at 4 and 12 months of age in a dose-dependent
manner (Singer et al, 2012).
Only a handful of animal models of pre- and perinatal
MDMA exposure have been developed as well. A rat study
of prenatal MDMA exposure (E14–E20) described increases
in dopaminergic fibers in the prefrontal cortex, striatum,
and NAc—areas critical in novelty responses, reward,
attention, and motor behavior (Thompson et al, 2009b). It
also appears to increase NE fiber density in the prelimbic
region of the prefrontal cortex and in the hippocampus
(Thompson et al, 2012).
Further studies suggest that developmental MDMA,
particularly during a time analogous to the early third
trimester, can produce persistent reductions in DA and
5-HT metabolites (Koprich et al, 2003a; Koprich et al,
2003b). Meyer et al (2004) determined that MDMA
exposure from P1 to P4 can decrease 5-HT levels in the
hippocampus on P25 and P60, and suggested that MDMA
treatment stimulates apoptotic cell death during early
postnatal development. Significant reductions in 5-HT fiber
density were observed in the cerebral cortex with a
hyperinnervation in the caudate–putamen and NAc in 9–
month-old animals, demonstrating enduring effects (Meyer
et al, 2004). The early hippocampal changes were not
observed in adulthood, suggesting a recovery of damaged
serotonergic fibers following neonatal exposure. The Meyer
lab further demonstrated that developmental MDMA
administration can lead to long-lasting functional abnorm-
alities including enhanced sensitivity to the drug later in life
(Piper et al, 2009; Piper and Meyer, 2006).
Other studies have shown few effects of MDMA exposure
in developing animal models. This could possibly be
because of time period of treatment—embryonic versus
postnatal—or doses (once again, please see Figure 1 for a
description of human and animal development timelines).
For example, when rats were exposed to MDMA twice a day
from E12 to E15, no differences were seen in hippocampal
levels of 5-HT, DA, NE, or metabolites when assayed on P21
(Winslow and Insel, 1990). In addition, no changes in 5-HT
or DA levels in various brain regions were observed in rat
offspring whose mothers were exposed to 20 mg/kg MDMA
once per day every other day beginning at E6 until birth
(Aguirre et al, 1998).
As adults, prenatal MDMA-exposed rats exhibit increased
activity, risk-taking behavior, and altered spatial learning;
however, it did not affect their feeding or food reward,
or alter cocaine self-administration behaviors or locomotor
responses (Thompson et al, 2009b). Neonatal administra-
tion of MDMA also produces impaired path integration
learning (Broening et al, 2001; Vorhees et al, 2004; Williams
et al, 2003c). Neonatal MDMA exposure alters the release of
DA and 5-HT at P70 in the striatum and hippocampus, and
reduces sucrose preference (an index of reward systems) at
P70 (Galineau et al, 2005).
The detailed mechanisms through which prenatal/peri-
natal MDMA exposure produces alterations in neurodeve-
lopmental trajectory are thus not fully understood. Heavier
use has been associated with impaired motor and intellec-
tual development in infants, but whether there are
long-lasting changes in neurobehavioral outcomes is not
yet known. A major concern is possible serotonergic
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
65
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
dysfunction produced by repeated and/or high doses of the
drug. A recent animal report suggests that that the simul-
taneous abuse of alcohol and Ecstasy during pregnancy,
even for short periods of time, may cause much more
significant abnormalities in neurocognitive development than
either drug alone (Canales and Ferrer-Donato, 2014). Given
the widespread recreational use of MDMA (Ecstasy), pregnant
women should be cautioned about possible developmental
effects in offspring. Animal models need to be designed to
include relevant doses and human developmental periods to
best ascertain the developmental effects of MDMA (for an
excellent review, see Skelton et al (2008)).
Cocaine
Currently the percentage of women of childbearing age
consuming cocaine is unknown and less information is
known about the current frequency of use during preg-
nancy. According to the latest figures from (UNODC, 2013),
cocaine was the third largest abused illicit drug (Substance
Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2013)
within the past year. Among women ages 12 and older,
including women of childbearing age, 0.3% were current
users of cocaine (Substance Abuse and Mental Health
Services Administration, 2013). Thus, cocaine use in women
continues to be a major public health concern. It has been
noted that prenatal cocaine exposure alone accounts for
over 26 million dollars per year in special education services
in the United States (Levine et al, 2008).
Studies of cocaine in animal models, using a variety of
species, have demonstrated cocaine easily crosses both the
placenta and blood–brain barrier and can have teratogenic
effects on the developing fetus (Mayes, 1994). The negative
outcomes and effects from neonatal exposure to cocaine
can result from the pathophysiology originating from
three possible pathways; first, cocaine directly inhibits
the reuptake of multiple monoamines at the presynaptic
junction, leading to increased concentrations of DA, 5-HT,
and NE within the synaptic cleft. Also, neonatal cocaine
exposure has been attributed to indirect vasoconstrictive
effects and higher levels of activation in the catecholami-
nergic system (Nassogne et al, 1998). In addition to the
neurochemical and vascoconstrictive effects of cocaine on
fetal development, cocaine may also act by altering genetic
programming (Lester and Padbury, 2009).
Clinical reports of the impact of prenatal cocaine have
been varied; some suggest global and severe physical
malformations, others document specific deficits in physi-
cal, cognitive, and emotional development, and yet other
studies indicate no effects. It is important to note that the
magnitude of these effects depend upon the dosage,
gestational timing, duration of exposure, and/or postnatal
care. Some of the disparities in the human literature arose
from the problem that many of the early studies were poorly
controlled and produced media hysteria around so-called
‘crack babies’. Nevertheless, well-controlled studies have
demonstrated that prenatal cocaine exposure does in fact
affect fetal physical growth, and results in an increase of
premature birth, and generalized growth retardation—
including decreased birth weight, shorter body length, and
smaller head circumference (Bigsby et al, 2011; Covington
et al, 2002; Gouin et al, 2011; Mayes et al, 2003). Abnormal
infant behavioral outcomes have also been documented;
these outcomes include abnormalities related to lower
arousal, poorer quality of movement and self-regulation,
higher excitability, jitteriness, and more non-optimal
reflexes (Lester et al, 2002; Richardson et al, 2008; Singer
et al, 2000; Tronick et al, 2005). Studies have revealed that
the behavioral outcomes observed at birth continue and
sometimes worsen after 12 months of age (Bigsby et al,
2011; Chiriboga et al, 2007; Mayes et al, 2003; Richardson
et al, 2008).
Growth restriction among prenatal cocaine exposure
children has been documented to continue well past infancy
(Minnes et al, 2006) and may persist in children as old as 10
years of age (Covington et al, 2002; Richardson et al, 2013).
While consistency exists in the reports of reduced fetal
growth and development, some follow-up studies at later
ages have suggested catch-up growth. Shankaran et al
(2011) found that children exposed to prenatal cocaine were
similar in weight to non-exposed children at 6 years of age.
Interestingly, children who had been exposed to high levels
of prenatal cocaine, and were born full-term, were reported
to have a higher body mass index and blood pressure
changes. These variable outcomes of prenatal cocaine
exposure on physical growth and development are in part
the result of important covariates such as amount of drug
use and timing throughout pregnancy. Other possible
variables are disturbances in physiological regulation, such
as the respiratory sinus arrhythmia and cardiac develop-
ment (Finger et al, 2014). Studies in animal models also
provide evidence for a programming effect resulting in
detrimental long-term changes to the heart induced by
fetal cocaine exposure (Meyer and Zhang, 2009). Overall,
additional long-term follow-up studies of children with
neonatal cocaine exposure and its effects on health
outcomes are needed to help identify mechanisms that
result in abnormal growth and development.
Although prenatal cocaine exposure alone does not
appear to lower global intelligence, there is consistent
evidence of poor cognitive performance in language skills,
behavior, and executive functioning. Longitudinal studies
have reported that prenatal cocaine is associated with
impaired language development through early adolescence
(Bandstra et al, 2011; Lewis et al, 2007; Lewis et al, 2011).
However, some studies suggest a general improvement in
receptive language starting in adolescence through age 17
(Betancourt et al, 2011). Furthermore, adoption or foster
care appears to enrich the children’s linguistic environment
and helps to protect children against some language delays
(Lewis et al, 2011).
Long-term studies have described varying effects on
executive function in exposed children. Executive function
is a set of mental processes for the management of cognitive
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
66
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
operations that include attention, behavior, cognition,
working memory, and information/problem solving.
Richardson et al (2013) found that first trimester cocaine
exposure has been associated with less sociability, more
withdrawn behavioral problems, more anxious/depressed
behaviors and symptoms in the children. These behaviors
may be precursors of later psychiatric problems. By
caregiver reports, higher cocaine use was associated with
disruptive behaviors including aggression and delinquent
behavior at 9 years of age (McLaughlin et al, 2011).
Moreover, caregiver reports indicate overall issues of
executive function in 12 year-old children with higher
amounts of prenatal exposure; in particular, females had
greater problems with initiation activities, working mem-
ory, and organization (Minnes et al, 2014). Carmody et al
(2011) further documented cocaine exposure affected
the attention and inhibitory control performance of
males, but not females, in children at ages 6, 9, and 11.
The neurobiological mechanisms leading to gender-depen-
dent differences in disrupted executive function following
prenatal cocaine are not currently understood; however,
this is an important consideration for future investigation.
These longitudinal studies have described varying effects
on executive function in exposed children; however, it is
also important to know the structure, function, and
pharmacology of the brain that is possibly being altered
by the drug exposure. Neuroimaging—for example, struc-
tural MRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
and DTI—is valuable in examining the underlying neural
circuitry and connectivity. Recent reports describe long-
term structural alterations in brain regions such as cortical
and limbic regions (Rando et al, 2013). Lower fractional
anisotropy in the right arcuate axons and higher mean
diffusivity in the splenium of the corpus callosum has also
been observed (Lebel et al, 2013). Cortical thickness of the
right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex appears to be thinner in
adolescents following prenatal cocaine (Liu et al, 2013), and
cocaine-exposed adolescents have reduced ventral prefron-
tal cortex activation in response to increased memory load.
Reduced structural connectivity between the ventral pre-
frontal cortex and the amygdala was also observed through
DTI measurements (Li et al, 2013).
Overall, fetal cocaine exposure can affect fetal and long-
term growth patterns, as well as cause language deficits,
behavior defects, and executive functioning abnormalities.
Animal models, across multiple species, give us further
insight on the pathophysiological mechanisms that are
possibly occurring during the developmental changes and
are not accessible to study in vivo. Animal models confirm
that prenatal cocaine exposure results in specific and long-
lasting behavioral, cellular, and molecular changes (Bhide,
2009; Dow-Edwards, 2011; Harvey, 2004; Lidow et al, 2003;
Mactutus et al, 1994; Malanga and Kosofsky, 1999; Mayes,
2002; Stanwood and Levitt, 2004). Moreover, animal models
of prenatal exposure have disrupted cortical neurogenesis
and migration during and after birth, reduction of neuronal
numbers and density in the cortex, and differences in
dopaminergic function (Crandall et al, 2004; Hamilton et al,
2010; McCarthy and Bhide, 2012; McCarthy et al, 2012;
Ren et al, 2004). Specific anatomical defects include aberrant
growth of dendrites of cortical projection and interneurons,
suggesting disruption of local circuitry, and behavioral
abnormalities that involve learning and stereotypic motor
behavior (Mayes, 2002; Stanwood et al, 2001b; Thompson
et al, 2005). Data suggest that the equivalent of the second
trimester may be the most ‘sensitive period’ for the actions
of fetal cocaine (Stanwood et al, 2001a). Alterations in the
fetal development of the monoaminergic system can affect
short- and long-term attention and cognitive development.
Perhaps a point of origin for the disturbances from the
effects of cocaine is the reduction of the coupling of the D1
receptor to its G protein-coupled receptor (Jones et al, 2000;
Stanwood et al, 2005; Zhen et al, 2001). This uncoupling is
sustained into adulthood and is specific for D1 receptors.
Behavioral changes in response to cocaine exposure have
also been reported in multiple animal models, including
deficits in attention and emotional reactivity (Gabriel et al,
2003; Garavan et al, 2000; Harvey et al, 2001; Morrow et al,
2002a, b; Stanwood and Levitt, 2003; Thompson et al, 2005;
2009a). These findings correspond with the human clinical
literature, which reports disturbances in both attention
and emotion regulation in children exposed prenatally to
cocaine. Gestational cocaine also increases the sensitivity to
the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine in male rats and
modestly affected females (Dow-Edwards et al, 2014),
indicating alterations in the development of reward circuits.
This brief discussion of prenatal exposure to cocaine
clearly emphasizes the complexity of determining the
pathophysiological mechanisms and associated risk factors.
Given the consistently large number of affected individuals,
there is a constant need for furthering research and medical,
mental health, and educational services for this impacted
population.
OPIOIDS
Opiate receptors are G protein-coupled receptors and fall
into three groups: mu, delta, and kappa. In situ hybridiza-
tion of the adult rat brain identified mRNA for all three
receptors throughout the central nervous system as well as
numerous peripheral tissues (Wittert et al, 1996). Opioid
receptor expression (Barg and Simantov, 1989; Lenoir et al,
1984) and endogenous opioid concentrations in the fetus
and neonate differ from that in adults (Barg and Simantov,
1989). Thus, administration of opiates in utero may have
more distinctive effects compared with adult exposure
(Pertschuk et al, 1977).
Illicit opiate use has been steadily increasing in the past
decade and a large part of this increase is in the 18–25 age
bracket, which includes women of reproductive age
(Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Admin-
istration, 2013). The probability of preeclampsia, premature
labor and rupture of membranes, placental insufficiency,
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
67
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
abruptio placentae, intrauterine growth retardation, and
intrauterine death increases greatly with illicit opiate use
during pregnancy (Bashore et al, 1981; Hulse et al, 1998;
Kaltenbach et al, 1998). Even with a successful labor and
delivery, neonates often have low birthweight and smaller
head circumference as well as experience symptoms of
opiate withdrawal (Binder and Vavrinkova, 2008; Hunt et al,
2008; Kandall et al, 1976). Some clinical studies have also
suggested an increased prevalence of heart defects, auto-
nomic dysregulation (Paul et al, 2014), nystagmus (Gupta
et al, 2012), and strabismus (Gill et al, 2003) in children
exposed prenatally to opiates. At the pre- and elementary
school ages, these children show motor and cognitive
impairments (Bunikowski et al, 1998; Guo et al, 1994; Hunt
et al, 2008), inattention (Hickey et al, 1995; Ornoy et al,
1996), hyperactivity (Ornoy et al, 1996), and an increase in
ADHD when exposed prenatally to heroin (Ornoy et al,
2001). The damage of prenatal opiate exposure is debilitat-
ing and long lasting, and physicians must continue to track
cohorts of exposed children to further understand the
impact into adulthood.
Similar to human clinical studies, rodents exposed to
heroin or morphine have a lower birthweight (Eriksson and
Ronnback, 1989; Lu et al, 2012; Zagon and McLaughlin,
1977b, c) and impaired learning and memory (Steingart
et al, 2000a; Wang and Han, 2009). Numerous structural
and functional alterations have been found that could
underlie the effects of prenatal opiates on cognition,
including perturbations in dendritic length, synaptic
plasticity, neuronal proliferation, and cholinergic function.
Prenatal heroin or morphine exposure decreases dendritic
branch length in layer II/III pyramidal neurons in
somatosensory cortex, which is thought to be specific to
opiate receptor activation, as it can be blocked by co-
administration of an opiate receptor antagonist (Lu et al,
2012; Ricalde and Hammer, 1990). Long-term potentiation,
long-term depression, and proteins associated with synaptic
transmission are all attenuated with perinatal morphine
exposure (Villarreal et al, 2008; Yang et al, 2006). Decreased
proliferation in the developing striatum (Harlan and Song,
1994) and increased apoptosis in dopaminergic cell cultures
and the hippocampus have been observed with perinatal
heroin or morphine exposure (Oliveira et al, 2003; Oliveira
et al, 2002; Svensson et al, 2008; Wang and Han, 2009).
The Yanai lab has provided substantial evidence to
suggest that the deficits in spatial learning and memory
may be tied to hippocampal cholinergic alterations. They
found increased hippocampal levels of a cholinergic
receptor (muscarinic M1 receptor) and the choline trans-
porter with perinatal heroin (Steingart et al, 2000a; Steingart
et al, 2000b). In addition, they observed altered levels and
activity of protein kinase C—a signaling protein down-
stream of the M1 receptor- and enhanced inositol
phosphate induction by cholinergic agonists (Steingart
et al, 2000a; Steingart et al, 2000b; Yaniv et al, 2004).
Whether a cholinergic deficit is found in cohorts of opiate-
exposed children is unknown.
Perinatal heroin and morphine exposure also disrupt
maturation of the opiate receptor system. Postnatal
morphine exposure decreases mu opioid receptor binding
in the striatum, NAc, amygdala, hypothalamus, and spinal
cord (Hammer et al, 1991; Kirby, 1983; Tempel, 1991).
Perinatal morphine exposure also induces morphine
tolerance (Chiang et al, 2010; Eriksson and Ronnback,
1989; Hovious and Peters, 1984), although an increased
sensitivity to morphine analgesia has been reported in
female offspring (Arjune and Bodnar, 1989). Aroyewun and
Barr (1982) proposed that postnatal morphine also accel-
erates the maturation of some aspects of opiate-dependent
behaviors, such as opiate antagonist-induced anorexia, that
normally only occurs after P14. Postnatal morphine
exposure accelerated the appearance of this behavior to
P10 and 12 (Aroyewun and Barr, 1983), but the observed
hypophagia could also have been induced by the precipita-
tion of opiate withdrawal. In addition, perinatal morphine
or heroin has also been shown to alter sexual behavior
(Vathy and Katay, 1992), NE turnover and release (De Vries
et al, 1991), neuroendocrine function (Litto et al, 1983), and
several other important structures and processes that this
review cannot cover (for a comprehensive review of the
developmental effects of illicit opiates see Slamberova
(2012)).
Opiate Maintenance Therapies
The American Academy of Pediatrics and American College
of Obstetricians and Gynecologists recommend opioid
maintenance therapy as the first line of treatment for
opioid dependence during pregnancy (ACOG Committee,
2012). Untreated illicit opiate use is associated with poor
prenatal care, nutrition, and fetal health, which is improved
with opioid maintenance therapy (Binder and Vavrinkova,
2008; Kandall et al, 1976; Maas et al, 1990). Initiation of
an effective opioid maintenance therapy facilitates better
prenatal care, decreased illicit use of opiates and other
drugs, and a continuous dosing regimen that prevents
maternal/fetal withdrawal. Opiate withdrawal is not recom-
mended during pregnancy unless the mother refuses opiate
maintenance therapy due to increased likelihood of relapse
(ACOG Committee, 2012). Animal studies have demon-
strated in utero withdrawal to increase fetal activity and
perinatal mortality (Kirby and Holtzmann, 1982; Kuwahara
and Sparber, 1981; Lichtblau and Sparber, 1981).
Opioid maintenance therapies are not without substantial
risk, as they can cross the placenta and alter development.
Withdrawal from illicit or prescribed opiates incites Neo-
natal Abstinence Syndrome in neonates, which often requires
treatment with morphine, diluted tincture of opium, or
methadone with or without diazepam (Finnegan et al,1975;
Jansson et al,2009). However, incidence, peak severity
score, duration, and length of required hospital stay due to
Neonatal Abstinence Syndrome symptoms are less severe in
neonates born to women following medically controlled
maintenance therapies compared near-term mothers still
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
68
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
using illicitly (Binder and Vavrinkova, 2008; Kandall et al,
1976; Maas et al, 1990).
Methadone
Methadone is considered the current gold standard opioid
maintenance therapy for pregnant women. It is a long-
acting full mu opioid receptor agonist that is distributed as
a daily dose only at licensed methadone clinics; allowing a
tightly regulated, appropriately titered dosing regimen, and
decreased abuse. Even though methadone reduces symp-
toms compared with illicit opiate use, clinical cohorts have
demonstrated that prenatal methadone exposure can lead to
increased premature birth (Cleary et al, 2012; Fajemirokun-
Odudeyi et al, 2006; Lejeune et al, 2006), decreased
birthweight (Cleary et al, 2012; Hulse et al, 1997; Kandall
et al, 1976; Sarfi et al, 2009; van Baar et al, 1994), and
smaller head circumference (Brown et al, 1998; Hans, 1989;
Rosen and Johnson, 1985; van Baar et al, 1994; Welle-Strand
et al, 2013; Wilson et al, 1981). Additional reports indicate
an increased incidence of respiratory insufficiency at birth
(Maas et al, 1990), altered corrected QT interval on
electrocardiogram during the first postnatal week (Parikh
et al, 2011), postnatal hyperphagia (Martinez et al, 1999),
disrupted auditory event related potentials (Paul et al,
2014), and myelination deficits (Walhovd et al, 2012).
However, the prevalence of cognitive impairments pro-
duced by prenatal methadone has been questioned because
some studies have not observed differences in cognitive
development (de Cubas and Field, 1993; Hans, 1989; Rosen
and Johnson, 1985) or performance (Bauman and Levine,
1986; Soepatmi, 1994; van Baar, 1990; van Baar and de
Graaff, 1994; van Baar et al, 1994). These variations may be
in part due to socioeconomic status and other variables
(Hans, 1989).
In clinical studies of opiate maintenance therapies, we are
aware of the prenatal opiate exposure dose, which allows
researchers to mimic physiologically relevant plasma
concentrations of the drug. Most animal studies do not
measure plasma concentrations, but variations in dose and
method of delivery are common issues in prenatal exposure
studies. Despite these differences, fairly consistent altera-
tions have been seen with prenatal methadone exposure
in animals. Similar to models of perinatal heroin and
morphine exposure, perinatal methadone exposure leads to
decreased birthweight (Enters et al, 1991; Ford and Rhines,
1979; McLaughlin et al, 1978; Zagon and McLaughlin, 1977a;
1983), mu opioid receptor binding (Hou et al, 2004),
hyperactivity (Hutchings et al, 1992) and reduced perfor-
mance on learning and memory tasks (Vargas et al, 1975;
Zagon and McLaughlin, 1979; Zagon et al, 1979). Decreased
brain and cerebellar weight and brain DNA content (Ford
and Rhines, 1979; McLaughlin et al, 1978; Zagon and
McLaughlin, 1977a; 1983) as well as cortical thickness (Ford
and Rhines, 1979) have also been seen. Zagon and
McLaughlin (1982) found reduced cerebellar area and
decreased number and density of internal granular neurons
in the cerebellum with perinatal methadone exposure.
A delay of developmentally timed behaviors was reported
by the same group (Zagon and McLaughlin, 1978).
Consistent with the cardiac abnormalities in neonates
by Parikh and colleagues, rodent perinatal methadone
exposure delays cellular development of the heart evident
by altered levels of polyamines (Slotkin et al, 1982).
Alterations in the levels and/or activity of multiple
neurotransmitter systems occur with perinatal methadone
exposure. Studies have shown decreased hippocampal NE
(Robinson et al, 1997), and disrupted striatal cholinergic
activity compared with controls (Guo et al, 1990; Robinson,
2002; Robinson et al, 1996; Robinson and Wallace, 2001).
McGinty and Ford found decreased DA in the forebrain
at P1 and P20 in methadone-exposed rodents (McGinty
and Ford, 1980). Prenatal only methadone exposure may
decrease striatal DA turnover whereas combined pre- and
postnatal exposure appears to increase it (Robinson et al,
1996). However, postnatal methadone exposure was
required to decrease DA turnover in the frontal cortex
(Robinson et al, 1997), which paints a complex picture of
altered DA neurotransmission in perinatal methadone
exposure. The 5-HT system shows irregularities in the
transport system in the cortex and hippocampus
(De Montis et al, 1983) and increased basal levels in the
parietal cortex with perinatal methadone (Robinson et al,
1997). Additional disruptions in monoamine oxidase B
levels (Tsang et al, 1986), METH sensitization (Wong et al,
2014), and startle response (Hutchings et al, 1993;
Zmitrovich et al, 1994) also have implications for cognition
and addiction vulnerability.
Buprenorphine
Buprenorphine is a partial mu opioid receptor agonist and
a kappa opioid receptor antagonist that is given as an
outpatient opioid maintenance therapy. Buprenorphine is
not currently the recommended prenatal opioid mainte-
nance therapy in the United States, although it is widely
used elsewhere. A recent review of double-blind rando-
mized control trials found insufficient evidence to recom-
mend buprenorphine over methadone during pregnancy
(Minozzi et al, 2013). This recommendation was largely
based on a slightly superior retention of pregnant women
in maintenance therapy with methadone, although bupre-
norphine may produce less severe neonatal abstinence
syndrome (Minozzi et al, 2013). The American College of
Obstetricians and Gynecologists has urged that buprenor-
phine be considered first-line treatment, but methadone
is likely still the gold standard due to slightly higher
adherence, more tightly controlled dosing, and insufficient
evidence that buprenorphine is superior than methadone
treatment (ACOG Committee, 2012). Observation of clinical
cohorts has suggested that prenatal buprenorphine may
produce fewer neurobehavioral problems (Coyle et al,
2012), higher birthweight, and larger head circumference
compared with methadone (Welle-Strand et al, 2013).
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
69
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
Several studies have found no difference in growth patterns
between buprenorphine-exposed and non-exposed neonates
(Bakstad et al, 2009; Fischer et al, 2000; Schindler et al,
2003; Sundelin Wahlsten and Sarman, 2013). Yet, hyper-
activity, visual/motor impairment, and memory problems
(Sundelin Wahlsten and Sarman, 2013) as well as an
increase in premature birth compared with non-exposed
neonates have been observed. Therefore, although there are
still developmental perturbations observed with prenatal
buprenorphine, the risk to the fetus may be less than with
prenatal methadone, and we encourage physicians to
consider buprenorphine as a first-line maintenance therapy
with pregnant patients.
Fewer animal studies have been published for perinatal
buprenorphine than methadone and heroin/morphine, but
perinatal buprenorphine appears to produce alterations
common to opiate exposure. Like other opiates, perinatal
buprenorphine produces morphine tolerance (Chiang et al,
2010; Robinson and Wallace, 2001), delayed acquisition of
developmentally timed behaviors (Robinson and Wallace,
2001), and increased sensitization to METH (Chiang et al,
2013). Chiang et al (2013) also found decreased D1 receptor
mRNA, basal cyclic AMP, and D1 receptor induced adenylyl
cyclase activity in the NAc of buprenorphine-exposed
offspring, suggesting disrupted signal transduction may
underlie the increased METH sensitization. Although mu
opioid receptor binding is decreased at birth, it corrects by
postnatal day 7 (Belcheva et al, 1998; Belcheva et al, 1994;
Hou et al, 2004). Perinatal buprenorphine does not appear
to cause hyperactivity in animal models to date, although
this has been reported in some clinical studies (Hutchings
et al, 1996; Sundelin Wahlsten and Sarman, 2013). Effects
on striatal acetylcholine levels are dose dependent with high
doses causing a decrease at P4 and 21 but low doses
producing an increase at P21 (Guo et al, 1990; Robinson,
2002). Altered myelination, which has been observed in
clinical studies of opiate-exposed neonates (Walhovd et al,
2012), also occurs, producing increased myelinated axon
caliber with disproportionately thin myelin sheaths poten-
tially due to changes in myelin basic proteins and myelin-
associated glycosylation (Sanchez et al, 2008). Additional
observations in animal models of perinatal buprenorphine
exposure have been reviewed comprehensively by others
(Farid et al, 2008).
Naltrexone
Naltrexone is a long-acting, nonselective opioid receptor
antagonist used more commonly outside of the United
States to prevent relapse in opiate addicts due to its ability
to block the euphoric effects of opioid agonists, low
tolerance and abuse potential, and modest adverse effects
(Jones et al, 2013). Newly developed depot formulations
allow for dosing every 4–6 weeks, improving patient
adherence and making this an increasingly preferred treat-
ment option. Inadvertent exposure due to conception after
naltrexone depot implant placement in Australia, Portugal,
and the United Kingdom has been cited, showing unre-
markable neonatal and obstetric features compared with
national averages as well as improved outcomes relative to
methadone maintenance during pregnancy (Hulse and
O’Neil, 2002; Hulse et al, 2003; Hulse et al, 2004; Hulse
et al, 2001). However, these studies did not address the
complications of pain management during labor and
delivery or effects on postnatal development.
Animal studies suggest that caution should be taken with
naltrexone exposure during pregnancy. Endogenous opioid
growth factor and the zeta receptor (this was subsequently
renamed the ‘opioid growth factor receptor’) are found in
both rodents and humans developmentally and decrease
cell proliferation (Zagon et al, 1992; Zagon et al, 1999). At
supratherapeutic dosing, animal models show increased
birth weight and brain and cerebellar weight likely through
attenuation of endogenous opioid growth inhibition with no
remarkable effects on gestation (McLaughlin et al, 1997a, b;
Zagon and McLaughlin, 1984; Zagon et al, 1998). Zagon et al
(1998) supported this hypothesis by showing decreased
cerebellar proliferation with perinatal naltrexone (Zagon
and McLaughlin, 1987). Perinatal naltrexone also alters
opioid receptor expression and function (Bardo et al, 1982;
1983; Medina Jimenez et al, 1997). High perinatal doses
(50 mg/kg) also increase dendritic arbor and spine growth
in the cerebellum and hippocampus (Hauser et al, 1987;
1989). In contrast, low doses of perinatal naltrexone appear
to produce decreases in dendritic length, proliferation, and
overall growth (Farid et al, 2012; Hauser et al, 1989; Zagon
and McLaughlin, 1984, 1987). Although these data advocate
that naltrexone exposure during pregnancy may not be
harmless, dosing discrepancies with human studies con-
found interpretations. Postnatal human studies with accu-
rate measurements of dose and timing of exposures along
with neurobehavioral endpoints are needed.
Prenatal exposure to opiates thus causes long-lasting
alterations in growth, cognition, and motor and visual
abilities. Although evidence suggests that any treatment for
opiate addiction improves maternal and fetal outcomes,
each form of medical treatment brings their own risks. The
gold standard, methadone, has good patient retention but
causes a more pronounced neonatal abstinence syndrome
whereas buprenorphine has higher abuse potential but
causes a shorter, less severe neonatal abstinence syndrome.
Both of these mu opioid receptor agonists may alter
developmental trajectory and negatively impact cognition.
The increasing use of opioid antagonist (naltrexone) depot
formulations to treat opioid addiction will bring more cases
of fetal exposure to naltrexone and an increased risk of
opioid overdose and relapse during pregnancy.
CANNABIS/delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol
Cannabis, also known as marijuana or, in its more
concentrated form, hashish, is an illicit drug derived from
the Cannabis sativa plant and is commonly smoked or, less
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
70
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
frequently, ingested orally. For the purposes of this review,
we will focus on the more commonly used form, marijuana,
in its most popular route of administration, smoking.
Marijuana is the most commonly abused form of illicit drug
in the US, with over 42% of the population aged 12 years
and over trying the drug within their lifetime. Over 38% of
US females aged 12 and over will try the drug at some point
in their lives, with 5% of females using the drug within the
past month (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
Administration, 2013). These statistics include women of
childbearing age, and nearly 5% of pregnant women report
smoking marijuana within the first trimester of pregnancy,
although these numbers decrease in the second (2.9%) and
third trimesters (1.4%) (Substance Abuse and Mental
Health Services Administration, 2009). Given that cannabis
has been recently legalized in Washington and Colorado
and decriminalized in other states, with many others likely
to follow suit, prenatal exposure to it is of growing concern.
Marijuana elicits a sedative-like effect, primarily due to its
active ingredient, delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC).
THC binds to the cannabinoid-1 (CB-1) receptor to induce
this effect (for review, see Trezza et al (2008b)). Transcript
for the CB-1 receptor is present in the second embryonic
week in rodents (Buckley et al, 1998) and in the second
trimester in humans (Wang et al, 2003), indicating that
exposure to marijuana in the womb might have develop-
mental consequences. Furthermore, THC can cross the
placenta with reasonable efficiency (Hutchings et al, 1989),
although unlike many other drugs of abuse, the placenta
appears to limit fetal exposure to marijuana, as fetal
THC concentrations have been documented to be lower
than maternal concentrations in studies of various animal
species (Behnke and Smith, 2013). Endogenous cannabinoids
(endocannabinoids) are crucial for proper development
in utero, and exogenous cannabinoids such as marijuana
alter fetal growth trajectories (El Marroun et al, 2009), an
effect that can have long-term consequences (for review,
see Keimpema et al (2011)). Marijuana users can
become dependent on the drug. Moreover, the THC
content of marijuana has greatly increased over the years
(Mehmedic et al, 2010), causing additional concern for fetal
exposure.
Despite the overwhelming use among pregnant women,
there are relatively few clinical studies looking at the effects
of marijuana on the offspring. Studies have shown that
fetuses exposed to marijuana during gestation exhibited
stunted growth outcomes, as assessed by fetal weight and
foot length (Hurd et al, 2005). Shorter gestation lengths,
decreased birth weights, and deficits in other growth
measures have been reported (Cornelius et al, 1995;
Fergusson et al, 2002; Fried et al, 1999; Fried et al, 1984;
Linn et al, 1983), although others have shown little to no
effect on birth outcomes (Day et al, 1991; Witter and Niebyl,
1990). While the gestational and newborn growth outcomes
remain equivocal, postnatal neurobehavioral outcomes are
only slightly less so. Our understanding of the role of
prenatal marijuana exposure derives primarily from two
long-term longitudinal studies: the Ottawa Prenatal Pro-
spective Study (OPPS) and the Maternal Health Practices
and Child Development Study (MHPCD). The OPPS
study, initiated in 1978, consists of primarily ‘low-risk’
middle class subjects, while the MHPCD, begun in 1982
in Pittsburgh, focuses on ‘higher risk’ subjects of lower
socioeconomic status. As outlined below, evidence from
these studies and others suggests that prenatal marijuana
exposure has deleterious effects on exposed children in
particular domains, although these results are not definitive
(for review, see Fried and Smith (2001) and (Campolongo
et al (2009)).
Newborns exposed to marijuana exhibit sleep distur-
bances (Scher et al, 1988), a problem that persists through
age 3 (Dahl et al, 1995), as well as a shorter, high-pitched
cry (Lester and Dreher, 1989). Altered responses to visual
stimuli and increased startles and tremors were noted in
newborns exposed to marijuana (Fried, 1982; Fried and
Makin, 1987; Fried et al, 1987), with some such symptoms
lasting for at least 30 days after birth (Fried et al, 1987),
although others, also using the Brazelton Neonatal
Behavioral Assessment Scale, have shown no significant
differences on these outcomes (Tennes et al, 1985). Diffe-
rential results were also seen in the MHPCD and OPPS
cohorts using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development.
Decreased mental scores in marijuana-exposed children at
9 months of age were found in the Pittsburgh group, an
effect attributed to exposure during the third trimester,
although at 19 months, no significant differences were seen
relative to controls (Richardson et al, 1995). Meanwhile,
others have shown no significant cognitive deficits in
children ages 1–3 years that were prenatally exposed to
marijuana (Astley and Little, 1990; Fried and Watkinson,
1988; 1990). However, exposed children at 3 years of age
from the higher risk MHPCD study demonstrated attenu-
ated cognitive development, including decreased short-term
memory, verbal, and visual skills (Day et al, 1994). These
effects correlated with exposure during the first and second
trimester yet that can in some populations be ameliorated
by environmental enrichment (Day et al, 1994).
As children approach school age, additional detrimental
effects of prenatal marijuana exposure become apparent.
These deficits are not generalized to overall cognition but
are specific to higher order function, specifically, executive
function. Impairments in verbal and memory tasks become
apparent in children at age 4, despite exhibiting no such
alterations when tested at younger ages (Fried and
Watkinson, 1990). At 5–6 years of age, exposed children
showed no overall deficits in cognition or language skills
(Fried et al, 1992a), but 6–year-olds showed attention
deficits, elevated impulsivity, and hyperactivity (Fried et al,
1992b; Leech et al, 1999). Deficits in short-term memory
and verbal reasoning were apparent at this age as well
(Goldschmidt et al, 2008). Over time, attention deficits
remain and can escalate to increased delinquency and
externalizing behaviors (Goldschmidt et al, 2000). Interest-
ingly, a study on marijuana-exposed fetuses found elevated
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
71
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
levels of D2 receptor transcript in the amygdala of males
(Wang et al, 2004), suggesting potential for altered
emotional regulation. Alterations in visuospatial memory
are also noted in early adolescence (Fried and Smith, 2001).
Deficits in these more complex domains were seen at ages
9–12 (Fried and Watkinson, 2000; Fried et al, 1998),
although other data indicate that deficits in visual and
abstract reasoning can be detected as early as 3 years of age
(Griffith et al, 1994). Perhaps most striking is that even in
their early 20 s, exposed individuals still have deficits in
visuospatial working memory and impulsivity (Smith et al,
2004, 2006a). Adolescents performed more poorly in some
areas of academic testing (Fried and Smith, 2001; Fried et al,
2003; Goldschmidt et al, 2012). These data indicate that
prenatal marijuana exposure has significant effects on
multiple neurobehavioral outcomes—deficits that are en-
during, particularly at the level of executive function.
Exposed children also exhibit signs of neuropsychiatric
disorders and may be more susceptible to substance abuse
(for review, see Jutras-Aswad et al (2009)). For example,
children from the MHPCD cohort presented with signs of
depression as early as 10 years of age (Gray et al, 2005).
Moreover, decreased levels of D2 receptor transcript were
found in the NAc of exposed fetuses, potentially linking
susceptibility to drug use to prenatal marijuana exposure
(DiNieri et al, 2011). Prenatally exposed children were also
more likely to experiment with marijuana at an earlier age
as well as smoke the drug more frequently (Day et al, 2006;
Goldschmidt et al, 2012). Given that drug use and psy-
chiatric disorders are burdens not only to the individual but
also to society as a whole, in total these results indicate the
prenatal marijuana exposure has persistent deleterious
effects that can have considerable consequences.
Animal models of prenatal marijuana exposure support
these findings. Exposed offspring showed reduced body
weight at birth (Abel et al, 1981; Abel et al, 1980). As with
humans, young rats exposed to THC throughout pre- and
postnatal development had increased ultrasonic vocaliza-
tions (cries) when isolated, indicative of increased anxiety
and potentially analogous to increased irritability in human
infants (Trezza et al, 2008a). In this same study, rats showed
deficits in social behaviors during adolescence and
elevated anxiety in adulthood. Increased locomotor activity
has been demonstrated as well (Borgen et al, 1971; Mereu
et al, 2003; Rubio et al, 1995), although others have shown
either no changes in locomotion or significant hypoactivity
(for review, see Navarro et al (1995); Schneider (2009)).
As previously mentioned, marijuana exposure during
human prenatal development appears to affect ‘higher
order’ facets of learning and memory. Similar results have
been found in animal models as well. Using either the CB-1
receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 (Mereu et al, 2003) or THC
(Campolongo et al, 2007), researchers found that develop-
mental exposure resulted in impairments in passive
avoidance, indicative of diminished memory retention.
Furthermore, the THC-treated group also was unable to
discriminate between a novel and a familiar rat in the social
discrimination test. These effects are not limited to a
particular phase of life, as in utero-exposed rats showed
deficits in learning and memory tasks throughout the
lifespan (Silva et al, 2012). These data and others
(Campolongo et al, 2009; Schneider, 2009) indicate that
marijuana exposure during pregnancy impacts learning and
memory. Marijuana exposure during development may also
result in increased likelihood of drug experimentation later
in life. Exposed rats exhibited decreased levels of D2
receptor expression and binding in the NAc, a key reward
center, and increased sensitivity to the rewarding properties
of opiates (DiNieri et al, 2011). Others have shown that
exposure to THC during a period equivalent to mid-
gestation in humans resulted in increased heroin-seeking
behavior in offspring (Spano et al, 2007). Following a
similar exposure paradigm, rats administered AMPH in
adulthood were less sensitive to its locomotor-stimulating
effects (Silva et al, 2012), indicative of aberrant responsive-
ness to psychostimulants. While the preclinical data support
the clinical findings, further research is needed to under-
stand the mechanism and circuitry underlying these
deficits.
TOBACCO/NICOTINE
According to the 2010 Pregnancy Risk Assessment Mon-
itoring System data from the CDC, 12.3% of pregnant
women in the US continue to smoke throughout pregnancy
(Tong et al, 2013), and these numbers might be higher
worldwide (Bloch et al, 2008). Over 7000 different chemicals
are found in cigarette smoke, many of which are toxic and/
or carcinogenic (U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services, 2010). Nicotine, the primary psychoactive compo-
nent of tobacco, and its metabolite, cotinine, readily pass
through the placenta, and fetal concentrations of these
compounds are significantly greater than those achieved by
the mother (Lambers and Clark, 1996). Nicotine is an
agonist of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR),
which is expressed in the first trimester in humans and by
the second gestational week in rodents (see review Dwyer
et al (2008); Dwyer et al (2009)). Acting through this
receptor, developmental exposure to nicotine disrupts the
cholinergic system, a key modulator of brain development,
and alters synaptogenesis, neuronal migration, neurotrans-
mitter release, and a host of other molecular and functional
endpoints (see review of Slotkin 2004). However, nicotine is
only 1 of more than 4000 compounds to which the fetus is
exposed through maternal smoking (Behnke and Smith,
2013).
Maternal smoking is not the only exposure to nicotine for
the fetus. Maternal exposure to secondhand smoke (SHS)
is also highly detrimental to proper fetal development and
can cause long-term neurobehavioral alterations on its own
(Chen et al, 2013). In addition, nicotine replacement
therapies (ie, lozenges, patches, gum, etc.) are prescribed
more and more to reduce fetal exposure to the additional
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
72
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
compounds found in cigarette smoke (Coleman, 2008).
However, nicotine alone, based on animal studies, is highly
toxic in and of itself, and these devices should not be
considered ‘safe’ for pregnant women to use, although the
risks are significantly less compared with cigarette smoking
alone (for review, see Bruin et al (2010)). Along those same
lines, electronic cigarettes (e-cigarettes) have become in-
creasingly popular as they eliminate SHS by delivering
nicotine as an inhalable vapor. These devices should be used
with caution, as they expose not only the user, but also
passerby to aerosolized nicotine (Czogala et al, 2013).
Similarly, maternal use of smokeless (chewing) tobacco
not only decreases birth weight (England et al, 2003), a
risk factor for long-term neurobehavioral consequences
(Rosenthal et al, 2011), but also results in higher Lipsitz
scores, a neurobehavioral test used to measure drug with-
drawal symptoms such as tremor, increased muscle tone and
reflexes, and irritability in newborns (Hurt et al, 2005).
There is a wealth of data examining the effects of prenatal
exposure to tobacco in both humans and animals, and a
number of very detailed reviews exist on the consequences
of prenatal tobacco exposure (Abbott and Winzer-Serhan,
2012; Bublitz and Stroud, 2012; Cornelius and Day, 2009;
Pauly and Slotkin, 2008; Slotkin, 2008). For this review, we
will provide an overview of some of the findings pertaining
to neurobehavioral outcomes. At birth, exposed infants tend
to be smaller in body weight, height, and head circumfer-
ence, effects attributable to third and possibly second
trimester exposure (Espy et al, 2011; Himes et al, 2013).
There is also a greater likelihood of exposed infants to be
admitted into the neonatal intensive care unit relative to
unexposed controls (Adams et al, 2002). Newborns exposed
in utero to nicotine are more irritable and have poorer
attention than unexposed infants, and they exhibit hyper-
tonicity, increased tremors and startle responses, and
deficient speech processing as well (Espy et al, 2011; Fried
et al, 1987; Key et al, 2007; Mansi et al, 2007; Reijneveld
et al, 2002; Stroud et al, 2009a). Within the first month of
life, exposed infants show signs of poorer self-regulation
and require more handling by caregivers (Stroud et al,
2009b). Newborns also demonstrate an attenuated response
to auditory stimuli, an effect that can contribute to language
and learning impairments later in life (Kable et al, 2009;
Mansi et al, 2007).
Following birth, the consequences of prenatal nicotine
exposure are persistent, with some outcomes being
consistently shown by multiple independent reports.
Children age 6 and younger had decreased receptive
language skills (Fried et al, 1992a; Fried and Watkinson,
1990; Lewis et al, 2007), which can contribute to language
comprehension deficits. Exposed children also show poorer
academic achievement or cognitive scores than peers
(Agrawal et al, 2010; Fried et al, 1992a; Fried and
Watkinson, 1990). These effects are not limited to young
children. Older adolescents also exhibit cognitive deficits, as
16–18 year olds, exposed to tobacco in utero and were
current smokers themselves, showed deficits in sensory
processing (ie, visuospatial memory) when abstinent from
nicotine (Jacobsen et al, 2006). Interestingly, this effect was
absent when subjects were allowed to smoke freely,
indicating that the deficit was withdrawal-induced and that
smoking was a means to compensate for this deficit.
Furthermore, there was no difference between subjects
exposed to tobacco throughout pregnancy or during only
the first trimester, emphasizing the necessity of smoking
cessation before pregnancy. In a test of working memory
(N-back test) combined with fMRI, there was differential
activation of various brain regions following correct
responses in exposed versus unexposed adolescents, in-
dicating abnormal function or circuitry following exposure
(Bennett et al, 2013). Moreover, pallidum volume was
smaller in adolescents following prenatal tobacco exposure,
and increased impulsivity was correlated with thalamic
volume following prenatal tobacco (Liu et al, 2013).
Attention is also a key area where a great number of
deficits are seen following gestational tobacco exposure.
Studies have shown that children as young as 6 years old
show attention deficits, including diagnosis with ADHD, an
effect seen well into the late teens (Bennett et al, 2009;
Cornelius et al, 2011; Cornelius et al, 2007; Jacobsen et al,
2007; Kotimaa et al, 2003; Langley et al, 2007; Lindblad and
Hjern, 2010). Both second (Cornelius et al, 2007) and third
(Cornelius et al, 2011) trimester exposures have been
correlated to attention problems. Interestingly, individuals
with self-reported ADHD or attention problems were more
likely to smoke than their peers (Kollins et al, 2005), further
corroborating a link between attention disorders and
nicotine use. While gestational exposure to nicotine appears
to be a primary indicator for attention deficits, confounding
factors such as maternal diagnosis of ADHD, environmental
factors, socioeconomic status, and exposure to other drugs
are often not accounted for in these studies (Agrawal et al,
2010; Ball et al, 2010; Langley et al, 2012). Thus, more
thorough research to eliminate these factors as confounding
variables is necessary.
Conduct and behavioral disorders are other domains
frequently associated with prenatal tobacco exposure.
Children as young as 18–24 months show increases in exter-
nalizing behaviors (Stene-Larsen et al, 2009; Wakschlag
et al, 2006a) as well as internalizing and total problem
scores following prenatal tobacco exposure (Carter et al,
2008). A study of 3-year-olds exposed gestationally to
tobacco determined that increases in oppositional behavior
were correlated to third trimester exposure (Day et al,
2000). Similar behavioral issues, including aggression,
oppositional defiance, and delinquency, have been noted
in older children (6–16 years old) (Bennett et al, 2009;
Cornelius et al, 2011; Cornelius et al, 2007; Indredavik et al,
2007; Langley et al, 2007; Wakschlag et al, 2006b; Weitzman
et al, 1992). It would appear that no level of tobacco
exposure may be safe, as children aged 7–15 years, born of
non-smoking mothers with SHS exposure during preg-
nancy, showed increased externalizing behavior (Gatzke-
Kopp and Beauchaine, 2007). Moreover, children with only
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
73
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
prenatal exposure to tobacco were more likely to exhibit
abnormal behaviors relative to children exposed only
during the postnatal period (Ruckinger et al, 2010), thus
emphasizing that gestation is a critical period in the
development of behavioral disorders.
In addition to the aforementioned deficits, prenatally
exposed children are more likely to develop substance use
disorders. Lotfipour et al (2009) found that exposed
children were more likely to experiment with drugs, an
effect correlated to thinning of the orbitofrontal cortex. This
same group also demonstrated that prenatal tobacco
exposure, in combination with a genetic polymorphism of
the a6 subunit of the nAChR, resulted in increased likelihood
of smoking and drug use (Lotfipour et al, 2010). The
combined environmental and genetic conditions altered
brain development as well, as affected children had larger
striatal volumes. Others have shown that exposed children
begin to smoke and become regular users earlier than
unexposed peers (Agrawal et al, 2010).
While preclinical models allow for more specific and
controlled studies, variables such as species differences,
pharmacokinetics, routes of administration, and the timing
of the developmental exposure make comparisons or
conclusions difficult. These studies, however, are invaluable
in defining critical periods and insight into the underlying
molecular mechanisms and circuitry following develop-
mental nicotine exposure. For instance, tobacco smoke
exposure in non-human primates during gestation and
lactation resulted in neuronal cell loss, increased glia, and
decreases in cell size (Slotkin et al, 2006). Following nicotine
exposure in utero, changes to spine density, dendritic
length, and dendritic branching were observed in juvenile
(Muhammad et al, 2012) and adult (Mychasiuk et al, 2013)
rats. Such changes can alter the trajectory of development
and contribute to the functional deficits seen later in life
(Pauly and Slotkin, 2008).
Changes in body weight are also found in animal models
of developmental nicotine exposure. When pregnant dams
were exposed to cigarette smoke throughout gestation,
fetuses were significantly smaller than controls (Bassi et al,
1984; Esposito et al, 2008). Differences in body weight
following other routes of administration or only postnatal
exposure (equivalent to third trimester human exposure,
see Figure 1) have been documented as well, although
results are variable (see (Abbott and Winzer-Serhan (2012))
for review). In line with the clinical data, deficits in learning
and memory and sensory processing have also been
detected. Rodents exposed in utero to nicotine performed
poorly on tests of learning and memory, including the radial
arm maze (Levin et al, 1993; Sorenson et al, 1991) and two-
way active avoidance (Vaglenova et al, 2008). Similarly,
mice exposed to nicotine via injection took longer to reach
criterion on the radial arm maze and had increased
latencies to reach the platform on the Morris water maze
(Yanai et al, 1992). Exposed mice exhibited deficits in
sensory processing, as evidenced by hypersensitive passive
avoidance (Heath et al, 2010). These researchers determined
that postnatal exposure—the equivalent of the human third
trimester—was the key critical period for this effect. A
study using a mouse model of inhaled cigarette smoke
throughout gestation through weaning (simulating expo-
sure throughout human pregnancy) resulted in deficits in
spatial and reference memory as assessed via Morris water
maze but not in route-based (egocentric) learning in the
Cincinnati water maze (Amos-Kroohs et al, 2013). These
findings bolster the clinical evidence that prenatal nicotine
exposure alters long-term learning and memory function.
Clinical data on developmental nicotine exposure fre-
quently refers to hyperactivity and attention deficits, effects
that are found in the preclinical literature as well. Schneider
et al (2011) found that rats exposed to nicotine during
gestation performed more poorly in the 5-choice serial
reaction time task in measures of sustained attention and
impulsivity (Schneider et al, 2011). These data correspond to
the clinical literature, specifically as attention deficits have
been correlated to second trimester exposure (Cornelius
et al, 2007). A similar exposure regimen in mice found that
males and females exposed to nicotine were significantly
more hyperactive than controls, especially during the dark
(awake) cycle (Zhu et al, 2012). Moreover, nicotine-exposed
mice responded to methylphenidate (MPH), a pharmacolo-
gical treatment for ADHD, with attenuated locomotor
activity. This low oral dose of MPH had no effect on control
mice, just as humans without ADHD do not respond to
MPH. Perhaps even more amazingly, the authors then
demonstrated that the prenatal nicotine-induced hyperac-
tivity can propagate transgenerationally via the maternal line
of descent (Zhu et al, 2014). These data suggest that in utero
exposure to substances even in previous generations may
have a role in the increased incidence of neurodevelop-
mental disorders in modern society.
Finally, while conduct disorders per se are difficult to
measure in animal models, comorbid conditions, such as
anxiety, can be modeled. In fact, evidence exists that
prenatal nicotine increases anxiety in rodents (Huang et al,
2007; Santiago and Huffman, 2013; Vaglenova et al, 2008),
although also see Amos-Kroohs et al (2013). Exposed
animals are more likely to self-administer nicotine later in
life (Chistyakov et al, 2010; Levin et al, 2006). Others have
shown that exposed rodents are more sensitive to the
rewarding (Harrod et al, 2012) and locomotor-stimulating
(Amos-Kroohs et al, 2013) properties of METH. Based on
these data, further research into the long-term alterations
resulting from developmental nicotine exposure, specifically
into aggression, abnormal social behavior, and substance
use disorders, is needed.
ALCOHOL
Alcohol consumption during pregnancy can result in a
long-recognized fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), which
includes morphogenic effects on limb and facial develop-
ment, reduced brain and birth weight, and cognitive delays
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
74
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
and impairments (Behnke and Smith, 2013; Foltran et al,
2011; Jones, 1986). However, even moderate exposure can
impact brain development and these effects persist at least
into young adulthood. While FAS represents the most severe
manifestation of heavy maternal alcohol consumption during
pregnancy, the term ‘fetal alcohol spectrum disorders’
(FASDs) has been applied to characterize a broad range of
deficits present in individuals with or without facial
dysmorphology who were exposed to alcohol prenatally
(Foltran et al, 2011; Hoyme et al, 2005; Nash et al, 2008; Riley
and McGee, 2005). Thus, moderate alcohol exposure during
fetal development produces significant neurobehavioral
consequences (Behnke and Smith, 2013; Day and
Richardson, 1991; 2004; Fried and Watkinson, 1988;
Jacobson and Jacobson, 1999; Jacobson et al, 1993; Nash
et al, 2013), and FASDs currently represent the leading cause
of mental retardation in North America, ahead of Down
syndrome and cerebral palsy (Nash et al, 2008). As described
in a 1996 Institute of Medicine Report to Congress—‘Of all
the substances of abuse (including cocaine, heroin, and
marijuana), alcohol produces by far the most serious
neurobehavioral effects in the fetus.’ (Institute of Medicine,
1996). It has been estimated that at least 1 per 100 (1% of live
births) are affected by FAS or FASDs (Sampson et al, 1997).
Twelve percent of pregnant women self-report alcohol use
within the past month (Substance Abuse and Mental Health
Services Administration, 2008). A recent Australian study
found that women who binged only before pregnancy were
more likely to continue (55%) rather than reduce drinking
(29%) once pregnant (Anderson et al, 2014). In 2002, the
estimated societal cost for each individual with FAS was $2.0
million ($1.6 million for medical treatment, special educa-
tion, and residential care for persons with mental retardation,
and $0.4 million for lost productivity) (Lupton et al, 2004).
The cost of FASD in the United States was last estimated at
$4–8 billion annually (Popova et al, 2011). For comparison,
the annual budget of the entire National Institute on Alcohol
Abuse and Alcoholism is substantially less than 500 million.
In addition to total quantity, patterns of alcohol drinking
can also modulate the severity of resultant deficits—for
example ‘moderate’ drinking has much more impact on
child development when the mother consumes several
drinks in a single day than when she drinks the same
quantity in doses of one to two drinks per-day over several
days (Jacobson and Jacobson, 1999). Imaging studies reveal
gray matter volume in the left cingulate gyrus, bilateral
middle frontal gyri, right middle temporal gyrus, and right
caudate nucleus following low-to-moderate exposure (De
Guio et al, 2014; Eckstrand et al, 2012; Treit et al, 2013).
Fetal alcohol has also been associated with a host of
cognitive and behavioral impairments, including deficits in
attention, memory, verbal fluency, executive functioning,
reaction time, and motor learning (Coles et al, 2002; Howell
et al, 2006; Mattson et al, 1998; Richardson et al, 2002; Riley
and McGee, 2005; Willford et al, 2004). Maternal and fetal
alcohol dehydrogenase polymorphisms also modulate risk
(Warren and Li, 2005).
Ethanol has diverse cellular targets, and produces wide-
ranging effects depending on the dose, duration, and timing
of exposure. Current evidence suggests that alcohol pro-
duces many of its damaging effects by exerting specific
actions on molecules that regulate key developmental
processes (eg, L1 cell adhesion molecule, alcohol dehydro-
genase, catalase), interfering with trophic factors that
regulate neurogenesis and cell survival, and/or inducing
excessive cell death via oxidative stress or activation of
caspase-3 proteases (Conti et al, 2009; Farber et al, 2010;
Goodlett et al, 2005; Ungerer et al, 2013). Several studies
have identified effects of low amounts of alcohol exposure
on the development of serotonergic neurons (Sari and Zhou,
2004; Zhou et al, 2003; Zhou et al, 2005), and suggested that
these types of defects in the ontogeny of a specific
neurotransmitter system may underlie the more ‘subtle’
deficits observed in human children diagnose with Alcohol
Related Neurodevelopmental Disorder, rather than full-
blown FAS (Zhou et al, 2003). Ethanol exposure disrupts
the proliferation of glia and neuronal precursors in the
developing central nervous system (Luo and Miller, 1998;
Miller, 2007; Miller and Hu, 2009; Powrozek and Miller,
2009) and can dramatically alter behavior (Hellemans et al,
2010; Valenzuela et al, 2012). Transcriptional analyses of
gene products regulated by fetal ethanol exposure in both
mouse and human cells also highlight genes related to neural
development, such as cell proliferation, neuronal migration,
and differentiation (Hashimoto-Torii et al, 2011; Hicks et al,
2010). Emerging data also implicate microRNAs and
epigenetic mechanisms (Balaraman et al, 2013; Mantha
et al, 2014; Pappalardo-Carter et al, 2013; Ungerer et al,
2013). For example, fetal alcohol results in alterations in
DNA methylation and microRNA expression and function
(Pappalardo-Carter et al, 2013). An intriguing hypothesis
suggests that fetal alcohol may induce a dual state of central
nervous system insulin resistance and oxidative stress and
proposes that adverse effects may be prevented or treated by
treatment with peroxisome-proliferated activated receptor
agonists (de la Monte and Wands, 2010).
Alcohol exposure continues to have potent effect on brain
development postnatally in rodents, demonstrated by
studies showing dramatic effects on the number of layer
2/3 cortical pyramidal neurons, on the branching patterns
of their basilar dendrites (Granato et al, 2003; Granato and
Van Pelt, 2003), and on dendritic excitability (Granato et al,
2012). The same group has gone on to also implicate
distinct alterations in cerebral cortical and striatal inter-
neuron distribution and morphology (De Giorgio et al,
2012; Granato, 2006). Rodent postnatal alcohol exposure
also leads to a persistent disruption in ocular dominance
plasticity, which can be treated, at least in part, with
overexpression of serum response factor or inhibition of a
phosphodiesterase (Medina et al, 2006; Paul et al, 2010).
Once again, these time periods are most similar to late
prenatal development in humans (Figure 1).
Finally, although we focus here on fetal exposures, of
great additional interest is the suggestion that adolescents
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
75
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
respond differentially to the acute cognitive, sensorimotor,
and behavioral effects of alcohol consumption than adults
(Spear, 2014; Spear and Varlinskaya, 2005). For example,
studies from Brunell and Spear (2006) indicate that
adolescents exhibit bimodal behavioral responses to alcohol,
with both hypo- and hyperresponsive systems (Doremus
et al, 2003; Long et al, 1992; Varlinskaya and Spear, 2002).
These findings have important implications for the risks
associated with recreational drinking in adolescents and
young adults.
Taken together, alcohol exposure during development
induces behavioral, cognitive, and neural alterations that
are specific, replicable, persistent and highly dependent on
the timing of the exposure. Given the staggering socio-
economic toll of prenatal alcohol exposures, new strategies
to prevent exposures must be considered, and better
mechanistic understandings are needed to facilitate poten-
tial ameliorative therapies.
CAFFEINE
There have been few studies about the effects of the
naturally occurring adenosine receptor antagonist caffeine,
which is ubiquitously consumed in coffee, tea, and cocoa-
containing foods. Caffeine is likely the psychostimulant
substance most consumed worldwide. Although considered
a fairly innocuous drug, a recent animal study has provided
compelling mechanistic data suggesting that developmental
caffeine exposure transiently antagonizes adenosine recep-
tor signaling resulting in discrete but persistent alterations
in hippocampal structure and function (Silva et al, 2013).
An accompanying editorial expressed concern that ‘We
must now heed the wake-up call sounded by Silva et al
(2013) that exposure to psychoactive substances, even a
seemingly innocuous, socially well-integrated substance
like caffeine, before or soon after birth can alter brain
development’ (Kabir et al, 2013). Another team has
suggested that embryonic caffeine exposure produces an
increase in body weight (B10%), alters cardiac function
and morphology, and produces long-lasting alterations in
DNA methylation (Buscariollo et al, 2014). A third group
has focused on the neuroendocrine consequences and found
that prenatal caffeine ingestion induces an increased
susceptibility to metabolic syndrome with alterations of
glucose and lipid metabolic phenotypes that then propagate
transgenerationally (Katayama et al, 1987; Xu et al, 2012a;
Xu et al, 2012b).
These recent studies join a larger body of work also
suggestive of caffeine-induced changes in developmental
trajectories (for review, see Porciuncula et al (2013)).
Gressens et al (2001) have documented caffeine-induced
acceleration of the evagination of neuroepithelium into
telencephalic vesicles (Sahir et al, 2000) and upregulation of
sonic hedgehog (Sahir et al, 2004). Gestational caffeine also
downregulates central adenosine receptors (Leon et al, 2005;
Lorenzo et al, 2010), reduces NMDA antagonist-induced
locomotor activity (da Silva et al, 2005), and affects
respiratory control (Bodineau et al, 2003; Saadani-Makki
et al, 2004). Cognitive and motor deficits have also been
reported in adult rodents exposed in caffeine during
gestation and/or early postnatal life (Bjorklund et al, 2008;
Soellner et al, 2009), although some impairments may be
paradigm specific (Soellner et al, 2009).
The metabolism of caffeine and other methylxanthines is
impaired in pregnant women, fetuses and neonates, leading
to reduced clearance and potentially higher circulating
levels (Aden, 2011). Several clinical studies have focused on
concerns regarding caffeine-induced fetal growth restriction
and have led to suggestions that women should reduce
caffeine intake before conception and throughout preg-
nancy (Bracken et al, 2003; CARE Study Group, 2008).
Other studies have failed to find an association (reviewed in
Brent et al, 2011). Regarding more subtle changes in
developmental trajectories, a recent study from the
Netherlands did not find significant effects of prenatal
caffeine on neurobehavioral functions at 5–6 years of age
(Loomans et al, 2012), although data on caffeine intake was
obtained only once during the B16th week of gestation.
In contrast, a Norwegian prospective study has reported a
small but significant association between maternal
caffeine intake and inattention/overactivity based on the
Child Behavior Check List (Bekkhus et al, 2010). Given the
fairly dramatic range in caffeine metabolism rates among
individual women (based on CYP1A expression and
activity), it seems crucial that future studies employ
pharmacogenomics strategies and caffeine metabolite
measurements, rather than simple self-report of intake.
And when combined with a still-evolving literature
implicating adenosine A1 and A2A receptors in establishing
neuronal homeostasis, tuning the ability of synapses to
undergo plasticity, and in modulating neurotoxicity (Dias
et al, 2013), these data suggest that expanded study of this
topic is sorely needed.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS AND CLINICAL
IMPLICATIONS
In summary, fetal exposure to drugs of abuse often produces
long-lasting changes in brain structure and function (see
Figure 3). These (mal)adaptations are regardless of whether
the drugs are legal or illicit; it is in fact arguable that alcohol
and nicotine produce more dramatic deficits than ‘harder’
drugs like cocaine. Adolescence encompasses another
sensitive period of development; given that this is when
illicit drug use typically begins, detailed research on how
drugs of abuse alter relatively late-developing processes in
the brain is also essential. Importantly, the molecular targets
of psychoactive drugs sometimes have quite different actions
in early development, as compared with their adult roles as
synaptic modulators. In some cases, transgenerational
effects via epigenetic changes have been documented. In
addition, studies have repeatedly pointed out the crucial
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
76
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
covariates of postnatal environment and socioeconomic
status. The developing brain is extremely plastic and
adaptable; we continue to discover new and better strategies
for intervention.
Surprisingly, research in this area is still in its formative
stages, and more precise and mechanistic work will be
necessary to characterize the extent of neurobehavioral
alterations and how developmental timing, dosages, and
genetics affect these processes. When costs for special
education, long-term medical management, and lost pro-
ductivity across the lifespan are considered, the socio-
economic impact is truly staggering. Moreover, societal
responses continue to be misguided. For example, the state
of Tennessee passed legislation in 2014 allowing women to
be charged with assault if they have a pregnancy complica-
tion after using narcotics illegally. The desire to guard fetal
health and outcomes is understandable; however, pregnant
women who fear prosecution and the potential loss of their
children will rarely seek essential prenatal and medical care.
We already discovered the negative consequences of such
practices during the peak of maternal cocaine exposures
in South Carolina from 1989–1994, before their prosecution
of these women was forced to cease (Annas, 2001; Budetti,
1993; Ferguson v. City of Charleston, 2001; McKnight v.
State of South Carolina, 2008; Poland et al, 1993). Physicians
and scientists need to better engage and educate the public
on these complex issues.
We do not claim to have all the answers, or even all of the
questions. But as a field, we must do a better job of bridging
the translational gap. Basic scientists must design their
animal models with consideration to (a) whether the route
of administration (to the dam or the pup) is the same as in
human users, (b) whether pharmacokinetics and biotrans-
formation of the drug differ between humans and the
animal model, (c) whether the model encompasses the
entirety of fetal development, and (d) how well the outcome
measures being tested in the animals conform to relevant
outcome measures in the clinical literature. Given that
women rarely, if ever, begin using an abused drug during
the third trimester of their pregnancy, postnatal-only
administration in rodents will usually be inadequate
(Figure 1). Given the great prevalence of polysubstance
use in drug-abusing women (and men), more models of
prenatal polydrug exposure seem warranted. Interactions
between fetal drug exposure and other environmental
variables, including toxicants and stressors, are also crucial
(Graf et al, 2013). Clinicians must also make better use of
the preclinical findings and predictions about the kinds of
deficits to be expected in people who have been exposed to
substances prenatally (Figure 3). Particular focus should be
Figure 3. Schematic summary of effects of distinct drug classes on offspring development. A wide variety of significant structural and neurobehavioral
deficits are induced by fetal exposures to abused substances. As described in the text, the drug class, timing, dose, and pattern of intake all substantially
determine the long-term effects on the developing child.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
77
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
paid when diverse types of models (species, dosing, timing,
etc.) produce congruent effects on functional domains (even
if the precise cellular mechanisms differ)—these are the
maladaptations that will likely be most prevalent in the
human population. The ability of animal data to potentially
predict future health issues should also not be overlooked—
for example, aging studies in animal models predict that
prenatal exposure to cocaine or nicotine (Abreu-Villaca
et al, 2004; Lawrence et al, 2008; Lloyd et al, 2006; Meyer
et al, 2009) may produce higher risks or expression
of cell death and/or neurodegenerative diseases later in life.
While only time will tell, we will likely be facing additional
societal and medical burdens as drug-exposed children
continue to age—better animal models will allow us to
predict and treat later-in-life disorders in these populations
as they emerge.
FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE
We are currently supported by RO1MH086629 (GDS),
R21DA035588 (GDS), the Vanderbilt Conte Center (GDS
and EJR), the Peter F. McManus Charitable Trust (DLG),
and the Vanderbilt Brain Institute. The authors declare no
conflicts of interest.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are grateful to innumerable colleagues who have helped
to shape our views on these topics, including Drs Pat Levitt,
Jeremy Veenstra-Vanderweele, Gale Richardson, Charles
Vorhees, Randy Blakely, Barbara Thompson, and Pradeep
Bhide. We also thank the reviewers who provided
substantial feedback and very helpful suggestions on an
earlier draft of this review. We are currently supported by
RO1MH086629 (GDS), the Vanderbilt Conte Center (GDS
and EJR), the McManus Foundation (DLG), and the
Vanderbilt Brain Institute.
REFERENCES
Abar B, Lagasse LL, Wouldes T, Derauf C, Newman E, Shah R et al (2013). Cross-
national comparison of prenatal methamphetamine exposure on infant and early
child physical growth: a natural experiment. Prev Sci 1: 1–10.
Abbott LC, Winzer-Serhan UH (2012). Smoking during pregnancy: lessons learned
from epidemiological studies and experimental studies using animal models. Crit
Rev Toxicol 42: 279–303. Thorough review of the most recent literature
regarding developmental tobacco exposure.
Abel EL, Bush R, Dintcheff BA, Ernst CA (1981). Critical periods for marihuana-
induced intrauterine growth retardation in the rat. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 3:
351–354.
Abel EL, Dintcheff BA, Day N (1980). Effects of marihuana on pregnant rats and
their offspring. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 71: 71–74.
Abreu-Villaca Y, Seidler FJ, Slotkin TA (2004). Does prenatal nicotine exposure
sensitize the brain to nicotine-induced neurotoxicity in adolescence? Neurop-
sychopharmacology 29: 1440–1450.
ACOG Committee (2012). ACOG Committee Opinion No. 524: opioid abuse,
dependence, and addiction in pregnancy. Obstet Gynecol 119: 1070–1076.
Adams EK, Miller VP, Ernst C, Nishimura BK, Melvin C, Merritt R (2002). Neonatal
health care costs related to smoking during pregnancy. Health Econ 11:
193–206.
Aden U (2011). Methylxanthines during pregnancy and early postnatal life. Handb
Exp Pharmacol 200: 373–389.
Agrawal A, Scherrer JF, Grant JD, Sartor CE, Pergadia ML, Duncan AE et al (2010).
The effects of maternal smoking during pregnancy on offspring outcomes. Prev
Med 50: 13–18.
Aguirre N, Barrionuevo M, Lasheras B, Del Rio J (1998). The role of dopaminergic
systems in the perinatal sensitivity to 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine-
induced neurotoxicity in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 286: 1159–1165.
Amos-Kroohs RM, Williams MT, Braun AA, Graham DL, Webb CL, Birtles TS et al
(2013). Neurobehavioral phenotype of C57BL/6J mice prenatally and neonatally
exposed to cigarette smoke. Neurotoxicol Teratol 35: 34–45.
Anderson AE, Hure AJ, Forder PM, Powers J, Kay-Lambkin FJ, Loxton DJ (2014).
Risky drinking patterns are being continued into pregnancy: a prospective cohort
study. PLoS One 9: e86171. This sobering study found that over a third of
women continue risky drinking into pregnancy, especially binge drinking.
The authors suggest a need to address alcohol consumption prior to
pregnancy.
Annas GJ (2001). Testing poor pregnant patients for cocaine–physicians as police
investigators. N Engl J Med 344: 1729–1732.
Arjune D, Bodnar RJ (1989). Post-natal morphine differentially affects opiate and
stress analgesia in adult rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 98: 512–517.
Aroyewun O, Barr GA (1982). The effects of opiate antagonists on milk intake of
preweanling rats. Neuropharmacology 21: 757–762.
Aroyewun OO, Barr GA (1983). Effects of chronic antenatal and postnatal
administration of narcotics on naloxone-induced anorexia in preweanling rats.
Neuropharmacology 22: 329–336.
Arria AM, Derauf C, LaGasse LL, Grant P, Shah R, Smith L et al (2006).
Methamphetamine and other substance use during pregnancy: preliminary
estimates from the infant development, environment, and lifestyle (IDEAL) study.
Matern Child Health J 10: 293–302.
Astley SJ, Little RE (1990). Maternal marijuana use during lactatio n and infant
development at one year. Neurotoxicol Teratol 12: 161–168.
Bakstad B, Sarfi M, Welle-Strand GK, Ravndal E (2009). Opioid maintenance
treatment during pregnancy: occurrence and severity of neonatal abstinence
syndrome. A national prospective study. Eur Addict Res 15: 128–134.
Balaraman S, Tingling JD, Tsai PC, Miranda RC (2013). Dysregulation of microRNA
expression and function contributes to the etiology of fetal alcohol spectrum
disorders. Alcohol Res 35: 18–24.
Ball SW, Gilman SE, Mick E, Fitzmaurice G, Ganz ML, Seidman LJ et al (2010).
Revisiting the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and
ADHD. J Psychiatr Res 44: 1058–1062.
Bandstra ES, Morrow CE, Accornero VH, Mansoor E, Xue L, Anthony JC (2011).
Estimated effects of in utero cocaine exposure on language development
through early adolescence. Neurotoxicol Teratol 33: 25–35.
Bardo MT, Bhatnagar RK, Gebhart GF (1982). Differential effects of chronic
morphine and naloxone on opiate receptors, monoamines, and morphine-
induced behaviors in preweanling rats. Brain Res 256: 139–147.
Bardo MT, Bhatnagar RK, Gebhart GF (1983). Age-related differences in the effect
of chronic administration of naloxone on opiate binding in rat brain.
Neuropharmacology 22: 453–461.
Barg J, Simantov R (1989). Developmental profile of kappa, mu and delta opioid
receptors in the rat and guinea pig cerebellum. Dev Neurosci 11: 428–434.
Barr AM, Panenka WJ, MacEwan GW, Thornton AE, Lang DJ, Honer WG et al
(2006). The need for speed: an update on methamphetamine addiction.
J Psychiatry Neurosci 31: 301–313.
Bashore RA, Ketchum JS, Staisch KJ, Barrett CT, Zimmermann EG (1981). Heroin
addiction and pregnancy. West J Med 134: 506–514.
Bassi JA, Rosso P, Moessinger AC, Blanc WA, James LS (1984). Fetal growth
retardation due to maternal tobacco smoke exposure in the rat. Pediatr Res 18:
127–130.
Bauman PS, Levine SA (1986). The development of children of drug addicts. Int J
Addict 21: 849–863.
Behnke M, Smith VC (2013). Prenatal substance abuse: short- and long-term
effects on the exposed fetus. Pediatrics 131: e1009–e1024.
Bekkhus M, Skjothaug T, Nordhagen R, Borge AI (2010). Intrauterine exposure to
caffeine and inattention/overactivity in children. Acta Paediatr 99: 925–928.
Belcheva MM, Bohn LM, Ho MT, Johnson FE, Yanai J, Barron S et al (1998). Brain
opioid receptor adaptation and expression after prenatal exposure to buprenor-
phine. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 111: 35–42.
Belcheva MM, Dawn S, Barg J, McHale RJ, Ho MT, Ignatova E et al (1994).
Transient down-regulation of neonatal rat brain mu-opioid receptors upon in
utero exposure to buprenorphine. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 80: 158–162.
Bennett DS, Mohamed FB, Carmody DP, Bendersky M, Patel S, Khorrami M et al
(2009). Response inhibition among early adolescents prenatally exposed to
tobacco: an fMRI study. Neurotoxicol Teratol 31: 283–290.
Bennett DS, Mohamed FB, Carmody DP, Malik M, Faro SH, Lewis M (2013).
Prenatal tobacco exposure predicts differential brain function during working
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
78
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
memory in early adolescence: a preliminary investigation. Brain Imaging Behav 7:
49–59.
Betancourt LM, Yang W, Brodsky NL, Gallagher PR, Malmud EK, Giannetta JM et al
(2011). Adolescents with and without gestational cocaine exposure: longitudinal
analysis of inhibitory control, memory and receptive language. Neurotoxicol
Teratol 33: 36–46.
Bhide PG (2009). Dopamine, cocaine and the development of cerebral cortical
cytoarchitecture: a review of current concepts. Semin Cell Dev Biol 20: 395–402.
Bigsby R, LaGasse LL, Lester B, Shankaran S, Bada H, Bauer C et al (2011).
Prenatal cocaine exposure and motor performance at 4 months. Am J Occup
Ther 65: e60–e68.
Binder T, Vavrinkova B (2008). Prospective randomised comparative study of the
effect of buprenorphine, methadone and heroin on the course of pregnancy,
birthweight of newborns, early postpartum adaptation and course of the
neonatal abstinence syndrome (NAS) in women followed up in the outpatient
department. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 29: 80–86.
Bjorklund O, Kahlstrom J, Salmi P, Fredholm BB (2008). Perinatal caffeine, acting on
maternal adenosine A(1) receptors, causes long-lasting behavioral changes in
mouse offspring. PLoS One 3: e3977.
Bloch M, Althabe F, Onyamboko M, Kaseba-Sata C, Castilla EE, Freire S et al
(2008). Tobacco use and secondhand smoke exposure during pregnancy: an
investigative survey of women in 9 developing nations. Am J Public Health 98:
1833–1840.
Bodineau L, Cayetanot F, Sadani-Makki F, Bach V, Gros F, Lebleu A et al (2003).
Consequences of in utero caffeine exposure on respiratory output in normoxic
and hypoxic conditions and related changes of Fos expression: a study on
brainstem-spinal cord preparations isolated from newborn rats. Pediatr Res 53:
266–273.
Bonnin A, Goeden N, Chen K, Wilson ML, King J, Shih JC et al (2011). A transient
placental source of serotonin for the fetal forebrain. Nature 472: 347–350.
Bonnin A, Levitt P (2011). Fetal, maternal, and placental sources of serotonin and
new implications for developmental programming of the brain. Neuroscience
197: 1–7. A landmark study demonstrating a new, direct role for 5-HT in
maternal-placental-fetal interactions that could underlie the developmental
impacts of 5-HT on long-lasting mental health outcomes.
Borgen LA, Davis WM, Pace HB (1971). Effects of synthetic 9 -tetrahydrocanna-
binol on pregnancy and offspring in the rat. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 20: 480–486.
Bracken MB, Triche EW, Belanger K, Hellenbrand K, Leaderer BP (2003).
Association of maternal caffeine consumption with decrements in fetal growth.
Am J Epidemiol 157: 456–466.
Brent RL, Christian MS, Diener RM (2011). Evaluation of the reproductive
and developmental risks of caffeine. Birth Defects Res B Dev Reprod Toxicol
92: 152–187.
Broening HW, Morford LL, Inman-Wood SL, Fukumura M, Vorhees CV (2001). 3,4-
methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy)-induced learning and memory im-
pairments depend on the age of exposure during early development. J Neurosci
21: 3228–3235.
Brown HL, Britton KA, Mahaffey D, Brizendine E, Hiett AK, Turnquest MA (1998).
Methadone maintenance in pregnancy: a reappraisal. Am J Obstet Gynecol 179:
459–463.
Bruin JE, Gerstein HC, Holloway AC (2010). Long-term consequences of fetal and
neonatal nicotine exposure: a critical review. Toxicol Sci 116: 364–374.
Discusses the issues involved with nicotine replacement therapy in
pregnant women.
Brunell SC, Spear LP (2006). Effects of acute ethanol or amphetamine
administration on the acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition in
adolescent and adult rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 186: 579–586.
Bubenikova-Valesova V, Kacer P, Syslova K, Rambousek L, Janovsky M,
Schutova B et al (2009). Prenatal methamphetamine exposure affects the
mesolimbic dopaminergic system and behavior in adult offspring. Int J Dev
Neurosci 27: 525–530.
Bublitz MH, Stroud LR (2012). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring
brain structure and function: review and agenda for future research. Nicotine Tob
Res 14: 388–397.
Buckley NE, Hansson S, Harta G, Mezey E (1998). Expression of the CB1 and CB2
receptor messenger RNAs during embryonic development in the rat. Neu-
roscience 82: 1131–1149.
Budetti PP (1993). Achieving a uniform federal primary care policy. Opportunities
presented by national health reform. JAMA 269: 498–501.
Bunikowski R, Grimmer I, Heiser A, Metze B, Schafer A, Obladen M (1998).
Neurodevelopmental outcome after prenatal exposure to opiates. Eur J Pediatr
157: 724–730.
Buscariollo DL, Fang X, Greenwood V, Xue H, Rivkees SA, Wendler CC (2014).
Embryonic caffeine exposure acts via a1 adenosine receptors to alter adult
cardiac function and DNA methylation in mice. PLoS One 9: e87547.
Caldwell J, Dring LG, Williams RT (1972). Metabolism of ( 14 C)methamphetamine
in man, the guinea pig and the rat. Biochem J 129: 11–22. Demonstrated how
methamphetamine is metabolized in both animal and human models.
Campbell NG, Koprich JB, Kanaan NM, Lipton JW (2006). MDMA administration to
pregnant Sprague-Dawley rats results in its passage to the fetal compartment.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 28: 459–465. Demonstrated a strong correlation
between fetal amniotic fluid and fetal brain—suggesting amniotic fluid
could estimate fetal MDMA levels.
Campolongo P, Trezza V, Cassano T, Gaetani S, Morgese MG, Ubaldi M et al (2007).
Perinatal exposure to delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol causes enduring cognitive
deficits associated with alteration of cortical gene expression and neurotrans-
mission in rats. Addict Biol 12: 485–495.
Campolongo P, Trezza V, Palmery M, Trabace L, Cuomo V (2009). Developmental
exposure to cannabinoids causes subtle and enduring neurofunctional altera-
tions. Int Rev Neurobiol 85: 117–133. Excellent overview of the clinical and
preclinical data on developmental marijuana exposure.
Canales JJ, Ferrer-Donato A (2014). Prenatal exposure to alcohol and 3,4-
methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) alters adult hippocampal neurogen-
esis and causes enduring memory deficits. Dev Neurosci 36: 10–17.
CARE Study Group (2008). Maternal caffeine intake during pregnancy and risk of
fetal growth restriction: a large prospective observational study. BMJ 337: a2332.
This large prospective longitudinal observational study found that caffeine
consumption throughout pregnancy was associated with an increased risk
of fetal growth restriction.
Carmody DP, Bennett DS, Lewis M (2011). The effects of prenatal cocaine exposure
and gender on inhibitory control and attention. Neurotoxicol Teratol 33: 61–68.
Study highlights the importance of considering gender specificity in
prenatal cocaine effects.
Carter S, Paterson J, Gao W, Iusitini L (2008). Maternal smoking during pregnancy
and behaviour problems in a birth cohort of 2-year-old Pacific children in
New Zealand. Early Hum Dev 84: 59–66.
Chang L, Cloak C, Jiang CS, Farnham S, Tokeshi B, Buchthal S et al (2009).
Altered neurometabolites and motor integration in children exposed to
methamphetamine in utero.Neuroimage 48: 391–397.
Chang L, Smith LM, LoPresti C, Yonekura ML, Kuo J, Walot I et al (2004). Smaller
subcortical volumes and cognitive deficits in children with prenatal methamphe-
tamine exposure. Psychiatry Res 132: 95–106.
Chen R, Clifford A, Lang L, Anstey KJ (2013). Is exposure to secondhand smoke
associated with cognitive parameters of children and adolescents?–a systematic
literature review. Ann Epidemiol 23: 652–661.
Chiang YC, Hung TW, Ho IK (2013). Development of sensitization to methamphe-
tamine in offspring prenatally exposed to morphine, methadone and buprenor-
phine. Addict Biol (e-pub ahead of print 3 April 2013). Outlined a potential
mechanism for the effects of prenatal buprenorphine exposure on the
effects of drugs of abuse.
Chiang YC, Hung TW, Lee CW, Yan JY, Ho IK (2010). Enhancement of tolerance
development to morphine in rats prenatally exposed to morphine, methadone,
and buprenorphine. J Biomed Sci 17: 46.
Chiriboga CA, Kuhn L, Wasserman GA (2007). Prenatal cocaine exposures and
dose-related cocaine effects on infant tone and behavior. Neurotoxicol Teratol
29: 323–330.
Chistyakov V, Patkina N, Tammimaki A, Talka R, Salminen O, Belozertseva I et al
(2010). Nicotine exposure throughout early development promotes nicotine self-
administration in adolescent mice and induces long-lasting behavioural changes.
Eur J Pharmacol 640: 87–93.
Cleary BJ, Eogan M, O’Connell MP, Fahey T, Gallagher PJ, Clarke T et al (2012).
Methadone and perinatal outcomes: a prospective cohort study. Addiction 107:
1482–1492.
Cloak CC, Ernst T, Fujii L, Hedemark B, Chang L (2009). Lowerdiffusion in white matter
of children with prenatal methamphetamine exposure. Neurology 72: 2 068–2075.
Coleman T (2008). Reducing harm from tobacco smoke exposure during
pregnancy. Birth Defects Res C Embryo Today 84: 73–79.
Coles CD, Platzman KA, Lynch ME, Freides D (2002). Auditory and visual sustained
attention in adolescents prenatally exposed to alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 26:
263–271.
Conti AC, Young C, Olney JW, Muglia LJ (2009). Adenylyl cyclases types 1 and 8
promote pro-survival pathways after ethanol exposure in the neonatal brain.
Neurobiol Dis 33: 111–118.
Cordeaux Y, Missfelder-Lobos H, Charnock-Jones DS, Smith GCS (2008).
Stimulation of contractions in human myometrium by serotonin is unmasked
by smooth muscle relaxants. Reprod Sci 15: 727–734.
Cornelius MD, Day NL (2009). Developmental consequences of prenatal tobacco
exposure. Curr Opin Neurol 22: 121–125.
Cornelius MD, De Genna NM, Leech SL, Willford JA, Goldschmidt L, Day NL
(2011). Effects of prenatal cigarette smoke exposure on neurobehavioral
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
79
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
outcomes in 10-year-old children of adolescent mothers. Neurotoxicol Teratol
33: 137–144.
Cornelius MD, Goldschmidt L, DeGenna N, Day NL (2007). Smoking during
teenage pregnancies: effects on behavioral problems in offspring. Nicotine Tob
Res 9: 739–750.
Cornelius MD, Taylor PM, Geva D, Day NL (1995). Prenatal tobacco and marijuana
use among adolescents: effects on offspring gestational age, growth, and
morphology. Pediatrics 95: 738–743.
Covington CY, Nordstrom-Klee B, Ager J, Sokol R, Delaney-Black V (2002). Birth to
age 7 growth of children prenatally exposed to drugs: a prospective cohort study.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 24: 489–496.
Coyle MG, Salisbury AL, Lester BM, Jones HE, Lin H, Graf-Rohrmeister K et al
(2012). Neonatal neurobehavior effects following buprenorphine versus metha-
done exposure. Addiction 107: 63–73.
Crandall JE, Hackett HE, Tobet SA, Kosofsky BE, Bhide PG (2004). Cocaine
exposure decreases GABA neuron migration from the ganglionic eminence to
the cerebral cortex in embryonic mice. Cereb Cortex 14: 665–675.
Czogala J, Goniewicz ML, Fidelus B, Zielinska-Danch W, Travers MJ, Sobczak A
(2013). Secondhand exposure to vapors from electronic cigarettes. Nicotine Tob
Res 16: 655–662.
da Silva RS, Hoffman A, de Souza DO, Lara DR, Bonan CD (2005). Maternal
caffeine intake impairs MK-801-induced hyperlocomotion in young rats. Eur J
Pharmacol 509: 155–159.
Dahl RE, Scher MS, Williamson DE, Robles N, Day N (1995). A longitudinal study of
prenatal marijuana use. Effects on sleep and arousal at age 3 years. Arch Pediatr
Adolesc Med 149: 145–150.
Day N, Sambamoorthi U, Taylor P, Richardson G, Robles N, Jhon Y et al (1991).
Prenatal marijuana use and neonatal outcome. Neurotoxicol Teratol 13:
329–334.
Day NL, Goldschmidt L, Thomas CA (2006). Prenatal marijuana exposure contributes
to the prediction of marijuana use at age 14. Addiction 101: 1313–1322.
Day NL, Richardson GA (1991). Prenatal alcohol exposure: a continuum of effects.
Semin Perinatol 15: 271–279.
Day NL, Richardson GA (2004). An analysis of the effects of prenatal alcohol
exposure on growth: a teratologic model. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet
127C: 28–34. A seminal review on the effects of prenatal alcohol on growth
demonstrating an interaction between the environment and prenatal
exposure.
Day NL, Richardson GA, Goldschmidt L, Cornelius MD (2000). Effects of prenatal
tobacco exposure on preschoolers’ behavior. J Dev Behav Pediatr 21: 180–188.
Day NL, Richardson GA, Goldschmidt L, Robles N, Taylor PM, Stoffer DS et al
(1994). Effect of prenatal marijuana exposure on the cognitive development of
offspring at age three. Neurotoxicol Teratol 16: 169–175.
de Cubas MM, Field T (1993). Children of methadone-dependent women:
developmental outcomes. Am J Orthopsychiatry 63: 266–276.
De Giorgio A, Comparini SE, Intra FS, Granato A (2012). Long-term alterations of
striatal parvalbumin interneurons in a rat model of early exposure to alcohol.
J Neurodev Disord 4: 18.
De Guio F, Mangin JF, Riviere D, Perrot M, Molteno CD, Jacobson SW et al (2014).
A study of cortical morphology in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders.
Hum Brain Mapp 35: 2285–2296.
de la Monte SM, Wands JR (2010). Role of central nervous system insulin
resistance in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J Popul Ther Clin Pharmacol 17:
e390–e404.
de la Torre R, Farre M, Roset PN, Pizarro N, Abanades S, Segura M et al (2004).
Human pharmacology of MDMA: pharmacokinetics, metabolism, and disposi-
tion. Ther Drug Monit 26: 137–144.
De Montis GM, Devoto P, Angioi RM, Curreli V, Tagliamonte A (1983). In utero
exposure to methadone produces a stable decrease of the cortex 5-HT transport
system in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 90: 57–63.
De Vries TJ, Van Vliet BJ, Hogenboom F, Wardeh G, Van der Laan JW, Mulder AH
et al (1991). Effect of chronic prenatal morphine treatment of mu-opioid receptor-
regulated adenylate cyclase activity and neurotransmitter release in rat brain
slices. Eur J Pharmacol 208: 97–104.
Dearlove JC, Betteridge TJ (1992). Stillbirth due to intravenous amphetamine.
Br Med J 304: 548–548.
Della Grotta S, LaGasse LL, Arria AM, Derauf C, Grant P, Smith LM et al (2010).
Patterns of methamphetamine use during pregnancy: results from the Infant
Development, Environment, and Lifestyle (IDEAL) Study. Matern Child Health J
14: 519–527.
Derauf C, Lagasse LL, Smith LM, Newman E, Shah R, Neal CR et al (2012).
Prenatal methamphetamine exposure and inhibitory control among young
school-age children. J Pediatr 161: 452–459.
Dias RB, Rombo DM, Ribeiro JA, Henley JM, Sebastiao AM (2013). Adenosine:
setting the stage for plasticity. Trends Neurosci 36: 248–257.
DiNieri JA, Wang X, Szutorisz H, Spano SM, Kaur J, Casaccia P et al (2011).
Maternal cannabis use alters ventral striatal dopamine D2 gene regulation in the
offspring. Biol Psychiatry 70: 763–769. Excellent example of clinical data with
simultaneous support from an animal model.
Dixon SD, Bejar R (1989). Echoencephalographic findings in neonates associated
with maternal cocaine and methamphetamine use—incidence and clinical
correlates. J Pediatr 115: 770–778. Findings have revealed congenital
anomalies in methamphetamine exposed neonates include cardiac
anomalies, cranial abnormalities, and abnormal brain development closely
resembling those in ill, asphyxiated infants.
Doremus TL, Brunell SC, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP (2003). Anxiogenic effects during
withdrawal from acute ethanol in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav 75: 411–418.
Dow-Edwards D (2011). Translational issues for prenatal cocaine studies and the
role of environment. Neurotoxicol Teratol 33: 9–16.
Dow-Edwards D, Iijima M, Stephenson S, Jackson A, Weedon J (2014). The effects
of prenatal cocaine, post-weaning housing and sex on conditioned place
preference in adolescent rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 231: 1543–1555.
Dwyer JB, Broide RS, Leslie FM (2008). Nicotine and brain development. Birth
Defects Res C Embryo Today 84: 30–44.
Dwyer JB, McQuown SC, Leslie FM (2009). The dynamic effects of nicotine on the
developing brain. Pharmacol Ther 122: 125–139.
Eckstrand KL, Ding Z, Dodge NC, Cowan RL, Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW et al
(2012). Persistent dose-dependent changes in brain structure in young adults
with low-to-moderate alcohol exposure in utero. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 36:
1892–1902. A detailed and comprehensive study following low-to-moder-
ate alcohol exposure demonstrating significant and long-lasting changes in
brain structure.
El Marroun H, Tiemeier H, Steegers EA, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, Verhulst FC et al
(2009). Intrauterine cannabis exposure affects fetal growth trajectories: the
Generation R Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48: 1173–1181.
England LJ, Levine RJ, Mills JL, Klebanoff MA, Yu KF, Cnattingius S (2003). Adverse
pregnancy outcomes in snuff users. Am J Obstet Gynecol 189: 939–943.
Enters EK, Guo HZ, Pandey U, Ko DJ, Robinson SE (1991). The effect of prenatal
methadone exposure on development and nociception during the early postnatal
period of the rat. Neurotoxicol Teratol 13: 161–166.
Eriksson M, Jonsson B, Steneroth G, Zetterstrom R (1994). Cross-sectional growth
of children whose mothers abused amphetamines during pregnancy. Acta
Paediatr 83: 612–617.
Eriksson M, Jonsson B, Zetterstrom R (2000). Children of mothers abusing
amphetamine: head circumference during infancy and psychosocial develop-
ment until 14 years of age. Acta Paediatr 89: 1474–1478. Extensive follow-up
data on affective, behavioral, and cognitive outcomes following prenatal
amphetamines-exposed children from birth to age 14.
Eriksson PS, Ronnback L (1989). Effects of prenatal morphine treatment of rats on
mortality, bodyweight and analgesic response in the offspring. Drug Alcohol
Depend 24: 187–194.
Esposito ER, Horn KH, Greene RM, Pisano MM (2008). An animal model of
cigarette smoke-induced in utero growth retardation. Toxicology 246: 193–202.
Espy KA, Fang H, Johnson C, Stopp C, Wiebe SA (2011). Prenatal tobacco
exposure: developmental outcomes in the neonatal period. Dev Psychol 47:
153–156.
Fajemirokun-Odudeyi O, Sinha C, Tutty S, Pairaudeau P, Armstrong D, Phillips T
et al (2006). Pregnancy outcome in women who use opiates. Eur J Obstet
Gynecol Reprod Biol 126: 170–175.
Farber NB, Creeley CE, Olney JW (2010). Alcohol-induced neuroapoptosis in the
fetal macaque brain. Neurobiol Dis 40: 200–206.
Farid WO, Dunlop SA, Tait RJ, Hulse GK (2008). The effects of maternally
administered methadone, buprenorphine and naltrexone on offspring: review of
human and animal data. Curr Neuropharmacol 6: 125–150.
Farid WO, Lawrence AJ, Krstew EV, Tait RJ, Hulse GK, Dunlop SA (2012).
Maternally administered sustained-release naltrexone in rats affects offspring
neurochemistry and behaviour in adulthood. PLoS One 7: e52812.
Fayyad J, De Graaf R, Kessler R, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, Demyttenaere K et al
(2007). Cross-national prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry 190: 402–409.
Ferguson v. City of Charleston (2001): Ferguson v. City of Charleston, No. 99-936,
SUPREME COURT OF THE UNITED STATES.
Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Northstone K (2002). Maternal use of cannabis and
pregnancy outcome. BJOG 109: 21–27.
Finger B, Schuetze P, Eiden RD (2014). Behavior problems among cocaine
exposed children: role of physiological regulation and parenting. Neurotoxicol
Teratol 42: 51–59.
Finnegan LP, Connaughton JF Jr, Kron RE, Emich JP (1975). Neonatal abstinence
syndrome: assessment and management. Addict Dis 2: 141–158.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
80
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
Fischer G, Johnson RE, Eder H, Jagsch R, Peternell A, Weninger M et al (2000).
Treatment of opioid-dependent pregnant women with buprenorphine. Addiction
95: 239–244.
Flores G, de Jesus Gomez-Villalobos M, Rodriguez-Sosa L (2011). Prenatal
amphetamine exposure effects on dopaminergic receptors and transporter in
postnatal rats. Neurochem Res 36: 1740–1749.
Foltran F, Gregori D, Franchin L, Verduci E, Giovannini M (2011). Effect of alcohol
consumption in prenatal life, childhood, and adolescence on child development.
Nutr Rev 69: 642–659.
Ford DH, Rhines RK (1979). Prenatal exposure to methadone HCL in relationship to
body and brain growth in the rat. Acta Neurol Scand 59: 248–262.
Frederick AL, Stanwood GD (2009). Drugs, biogenic amine targets and the
developing brain. Dev Neurosci 31: 7–22.
Fried PA (1982). Marihuana use by pregnant women and effects on offspring: an
update. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 4: 451–454.
Fried PA, Makin JE (1987). Neonatal behavioural correlates of prenatal exposure to
marihuana, cigarettes and alcohol in a low risk population. Neurotoxicol Teratol 9:
1–7.
Fried PA, O’Connell CM, Watkinson B (1992a). 60- and 72-month follow-up of
children prenatally exposed to marijuana, cigarettes, and alcohol: cognitive and
language assessment. J Dev Behav Pediatr 13: 383–391.
Fried PA, Smith AM (2001). A literature review of the consequences of prenatal
marihuana exposure. An emerging theme of a deficiency in aspects of executive
function. Neurotoxicol Teratol 23: 1–11. Comprehensive review of how
prenatal marijuana affects cognition and executive function.
Fried PA, Watkinson B (1988). 12- and 24-month neurobehavioural follow-up of
children prenatally exposed to marihuana, cigarettes and alcohol. Neurotoxicol
Teratol 10: 305–313.
Fried PA, Watkinson B (1990). 36- and 48-month neurobehavioral follow-up of
children prenatally exposed to marijuana, cigarettes, and alcohol. J Dev Behav
Pediatr 11: 49–58.
Fried PA, Watkinson B (2000). Visuoperceptual functioning differs in 9- to 12-year olds
prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicol Teratol 22:1120.
Fried PA, Watkinson B, Dillon RF, Dulberg CS (1987). Neonatal neurological status
in a low-risk population after prenatal exposure to cigarettes, marijuana, and
alcohol. J Dev Behav Pediatr 8: 318–326.
Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R (1992b). A follow-up study of attentional behavior in
6-year-old children exposed prenatally to marihuana, cigarettes, and alcohol.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 14: 299–311.
Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R (1998). Differential effects on cognitive functioning in
9- to 12-year olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicol
Teratol 20: 293–306.
Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R (1999). Growth from birth to early adolescence in
offspring prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marijuana. Neurotoxicol Teratol 21:
513–525.
Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R (2003). Differential effects on cognitive functioning in
13- to 16-year-olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotox-
icol Teratol 25: 427–436.
Fried PA, Watkinson B, Willan A (1984). Marijuana use during pregnancy and
decreased length of gestation. Am J Obstet Gynecol 150: 23–27.
Gabriel M, Taylor C, Burhans L (2003). In utero cocaine, discriminative avoidance
learning with low-salient stimuli and learning-related neuronal activity in rabbits
(Oryctolagus cuniculus). Behav Neurosci 117: 912–926.
Galineau L, Belzung C, Kodas E, Bodard S, Guilloteau D, Chalon S (2005). Prenatal
3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) exposure induces long-term
alterations in the dopaminergic and serotonergic functions in the rat. Brain Res
Dev Brain Res 154: 165–176.
Garavan H, Morgan RE, Mactutus CF, Levitsky DA, Booze RM, Strupp BJ (2000).
Prenatal cocaine exposure impairs selective attention: evidence from serial
reversal and extradimensional shift tasks. Behav Neurosci 114: 725–738.
Gatzke-Kopp LM, Beauchaine TP (2007). Direct and passive prenatal nicotine
exposure and the development of externalizing psychopathology. Child
Psychiatry Hum Dev 38: 255–269.
Gill AC, Oei J, Lewis NL, Younan N, Kennedy I, Lui K (2003). Strabismus in infants of
opiate-dependent mothers. Acta Paediatr 92: 379–385.
Goldschmidt L, Day NL, Richardson GA (2000). Effects of prenatal marijuana
exposure on child behavior problems at age 10. Neurotoxicol Teratol 22: 325–336.
Goldschmidt L, Richardson GA, Willford J, Day NL (2008). Prenatal marijuana
exposure and intelligence test performance at age 6. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 47: 254–263.
Goldschmidt L, Richardson GA, Willford JA, Severtson SG, Day NL (2012). School
achievement in 14-year-old youths prenatally exposed to marijuana. Neurotoxicol
Teratol 34: 161–167.
Goodlett CR, Horn KH, Zhou FC (2005). Alcohol teratogenesis: mechanisms of
damage and strategies for intervention. Exp Biol Med (Maywood) 230: 394–406.
Gouin K, Murphy K, Shah PS (2011). Effects of cocaine use during pregnancy on
low birthweight and preterm birth: systematic review and metaanalyses. Am J
Obstet Gynecol 204: 340, e341-312.
Graf WD, Kekatpure MV, Kosofsky BE (2013). Prenatal-onset neurodevelopmental
disorders secondary to toxins, nutritional deficiencies, and maternal illness.
Handb Clin Neurol 111: 143–159.
Granato A (2006). Altered organization of cortical interneurons in rats exposed to
ethanol during neonatal life. Brain Res 1069: 23–30.
Granato A, Di Rocco F, Zumbo A, Toesca A, Giannetti S (2003). Organization of
cortico-cortical associative projections in rats exposed to ethanol during early
postnatal life. Brain Res Bull 60: 339–344.
Granato A, Palmer LM, De Giorgio A, Tavian D, Larkum ME (2012). Early exposure
to alcohol leads to permanent impairment of dendritic excitability in neocortical
pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 32: 1377–1382. A detailed, mechanistic
analysis of the effects of early postnatal ethanol in the rat (similar to third
trimester in human) on cortical neuron excitability. Dual whole-cell
recordings from the soma and distal apical dendrites revealed a lower
number and a shorter duration of dendritic spikes ethanol-treated animals,
resulting in reduced action potentials recorded at the soma after dendritic
current injection.
Granato A, Van Pelt J (2003). Effects of early ethanol exposure on dendrite growth
of cortical pyramidal neurons: inferences from a computational model. Brain Res
Dev Brain Res 142: 223–227.
Gray KA, Day NL, Leech S, Richardson GA (2005). Prenatal marijuana exposure:
effect on child depressive symptoms at ten years of age. Neurotoxicol Teratol 27:
439–448.
Gressens P, Mesples B, Sahir N, Marret S, Sola A (2001). Environmental factors and
disturbances of brain development. Semin Neonatol 6: 185–194.
Griffith DR, Azuma SD, Chasnoff IJ (1994). Three-year outcome of children exposed
prenatally to drugs. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 33: 20–27.
Guo HZ, Enters EK, McDowell KP, Robinson SE (1990). The effect of prenatal
exposure to methadone on neurotransmitters in neonatal rats. Brain Res Dev
Brain Res 57: 296–298.
Guo X, Spencer JW, Suess PE, Hickey JE, Better WE, Herning RI (1994). Cognitive
brain potential alterations in boys exposed to opiates: in utero and lifestyle
comparisons. Addict Behav 19: 429–441.
Gupta M, Mulvihill AO, Lascaratos G, Fleck BW, George ND (2012). Nystagmus and
reduced visual acuity secondary to drug exposure in utero: long-term follow-up.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 49: 58–63.
Hamilton LR, Czoty PW, Gage HD, Nader MA (2010). Characterization of the
dopamine receptor system in adult rhesus monkeys exposed to cocaine
throughout gestation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 210: 481–488.
Hammer RP Jr, Seatriz JV, Ricalde AR (1991). Regional dependence of morphine-
induced mu-opiate receptor down-regulation in perinatal rat brain. Eur J
Pharmacol 209: 253–256.
Hans SL (1989). Developmental consequences of prenatal exposure to methadone.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 562: 195–207. Suggested a major role often overlooked in
previously published studies of socioeconomic status of control and
exposed groups greatly confounding outcomes.
Harlan RE, Song DD (1994). Prenatal morphine treatment and the development of
the striatum. Regul Pept 54: 117–118.
Harrod SB, Lacy RT, Morgan AJ (2012). Offspring of prenatal IV nicotine exposure
exhibit increased sensitivity to the reinforcing effects of methamphetamine. Front
Pharmacol 3: 116.
Harvey JA (2004). Cocaine effects on the developing brain: current status. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev 27: 751–764.
Harvey JA, Romano AG, Gabriel M, Simansky KJ, Du W, Aloyo VJ et al (2001).
Effects of prenatal exposure to cocaine on the developing brain: anatomical,
chemical, physiological and behavioral consequences. Neurotox Res 3:
117–143.
Hashimoto-Torii K, Kawasawa YI, Kuhn A, Rakic P (2011). Combined transcriptome
analysis of fetal human and mouse cerebral cortex exposed to alcohol. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA 108: 4212–4217.
Hauser KF, McLaughlin PJ, Zagon IS (1987). Endogenous opioids regulate dendritic
growth and spine formation in developing rat brain. Brain Res 416: 157–161.
Hauser KF, McLaughlin PJ, Zagon IS (1989). Endogenous opioid systems and the
regulation of dendritic growth and spine formation. J Comp Neurol 281: 13–22.
Heath CJ, King SL, Gotti C, Marks MJ, Picciotto MR (2010). Cortico-thalamic
connectivity is vulnerable to nicotine exposure during early postnatal develop-
ment through alpha4/beta2/alpha5 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Neuropsy-
chopharmacology 35: 2324–2338. Identifies not only a particular behavior
associated with prenatal nicotine exposure, but also the mechanism,
circuitry, and critical exposure period.
Hellemans KG, Verma P, Yoon E, Yu WK, Young AH, Weinberg J (2010). Prenatal
alcohol exposure and chronic mild stress differentially alter depressive- and
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
81
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
anxiety-like behaviors in male and female offspring. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 34:
633–645.
Hickey JE, Suess PE, Newlin DB, Spurgeon L, Porges SW (1995). Vagal tone
regulation during sustained attention in boys exposed to opiates in utero. Addict
Behav 20: 43–59.
Hicks SD, Middleton FA, Miller MW (2010). Ethanol-induced methylation of cell
cycle genes in neural stem cells. J Neurochem 114: 1767–1780.
Himes SK, Stroud LR, Scheidweiler KB, Niaura RS, Huestis MA (2013). Prenatal
tobacco exposure, biomarkers for tobacco in meconium, and neonatal growth
outcomes. J Pediatr 162: 970–975.
Ho E, Karimi-Tabesh L, Koren G (2001). Characteristics of pregnant women who
use ecstasy (3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine). Neurotoxicol Teratol 23:
561–567.
Hohmann CF (2003). A morphogenetic role for acetylcholine in mouse cerebral
neocortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27: 351–363.
Hou Y, Tan Y, Belcheva MM, Clark AL, Zahm DS, Coscia CJ (2004). Differential
effects of gestational buprenorphine, naloxone, and methadone on mesolimbic
mu opioid and ORL1 receptor G protein coupling. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 151:
149–157.
Hovious JR, Peters MA (1984). Analgesic effect of opiates in offspring of opiate-
treated female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 21: 555–559.
Howell KK, Lynch ME, Platzman KA, Smith GH, Coles CD (2006). Prenatal alcohol
exposure and ability, academic achievement, and school functioning in
adolescence: a longitudinal follow-up. J Pediatr Psychol 31: 116–126.
Hoyme HE, May PA, Kalberg WO, Kodituwakku P, Gossage JP, Trujillo PM et al
(2005). A practical clinical approach to diagnosis of fetal alcohol spectrum
disorders: clarification of the 1996 institute of medicine criteria. Pediatrics 115:
39–47.
Huang LZ, Liu X, Griffith WH, Winzer-Serhan UH (2007). Chronic neonatal nicotine
increases anxiety but does not impair cognition in adult rats. Behav Neurosci
121: 1342–1352.
Hulse G, O’Neil G (2002). Using naltrexone implants in the management of the
pregnant heroin user. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 42: 569–573.
Hulse GK, Arnold-Reed DE, O’Neil G, Hansson RC (2003). Naltrexone implant
and blood naltrexone levels over pregnancy. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 43:
386–388.
Hulse GK, Milne E, English DR, Holman CD (1997). The relationship between
maternal use of heroin and methadone and infant birth weight. Addiction 92:
1571–1579.
Hulse GK, Milne E, English DR, Holman CD (1998). Assessing the relationship
between maternal opiate use and neonatal mortality. Addiction 93: 1033–1042.
Hulse GK, O’Neil G, Arnold-Reed DE (2004). Methadone maintenance vs.
implantable naltrexone treatment in the pregnant heroin user. Int J Gynaecol
Obstet 85: 170–171.
Hulse GK, O’Neill G, Pereira C, Brewer C (2001). Obstetric and neonatal outcomes
associated with maternal naltrexone exposure. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 41:
424–428. One of the first clinical reports of prenatal naltrexone exposure.
Hunt RW, Tzioumi D, Collins E, Jeffery HE (2008). Adverse neurodevelopmental
outcome of infants exposed to opiate in-utero. Early Hum Dev 84: 29–35.
Hurd YL, Wang X, Anderson V, Beck O, Minkoff H, Dow-Edwards D (2005).
Marijuana impairs growth in mid-gestation fetuses. Neurotoxicol Teratol 27:
221–229.
Hurt RD, Renner CC, Patten CA, Ebbert JO, Offord KP, Schroeder DR et al (2005).
Iqmik–a form of smokeless tobacco used by pregnant Alaska natives: nicotine
exposure in their neonates. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 17: 281–289.
Hutchings DE, Hamowy AS, Williams EM, Zmitrovich AC (1996). Prenatal
administration of buprenorphine in the rat: effects on the rest-activity cycle at
22 and 30 days of age. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 55: 607–613.
Hutchings DE, Martin BR, Gamagaris Z, Miller N, Fico T (1989). Plasma
concentrations of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in dams and fetuses following
acute or multiple prenatal dosing in rats. Life Sci 44: 697–701.
Hutchings DE, Zmitrovich A, Brake SC, Malowany D, Church S, Nero TJ (1992).
Prenatal administration of methadone using the osmotic minipump: effects on
maternal and offspring toxicity, growth, and behavior in the rat. Neurotoxicol
Teratol 14: 65–71.
Hutchings DE, Zmitrovich AC, Brake SC, Church SH, Malowany D (1993). Prenatal
administration of methadone in the rat increases offspring acoustic startle
amplitude at age 3 weeks. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15: 157–164.
Indredavik MS, Brubakk AM, Romundstad P, Vik T (2007). Prenatal smoking
exposure and psychiatric symptoms in adolescence. Acta Paediatr 96: 377–382.
Institute of Medicine (1996). Fetal Alcohol Syndrome:Diagnosis, Epidemiology,
Prevention, and Treatment. The National Academies Press: Washington DC.
Jacobsen LK, Slotkin TA, Mencl WE, Frost SJ, Pugh KR (2007). Gender-specific
effects of prenatal and adolescent exposure to tobacco smoke on auditory and
visual attention. Neuropsychopharmacology 32: 2453–2464.
Jacobsen LK, Slotkin TA, Westerveld M, Mencl WE, Pugh KR (2006). Visuospatial
memory deficits emerging during nicotine withdrawal in adolescents with
prenatal exposure to active maternal smoking. Neuropsychopharmacology 31:
1550–1561.
Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW (1999). Drinking moderately and pregnancy. Effects on
child development. Alcohol Res Health 23: 25–30.
Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Sokol RJ, Martier SS, Ager JW, Kaplan-Estrin MG
(1993). Teratogenic effects of alcohol on infant development. Alcohol Clin Exp
Res 17: 174–183.
Jansson LM, Velez M, Harrow C (2009). The opioid-exposed newborn: assessment
and pharmacologic management. J Opioid Manag 5: 47–55.
Jeng W, Wong AW, Ting AKR, Wells PG (2005). Methamphetamine-enhanced
embryonic oxidative DNA damage and neurodevelopmental deficits. Free Radic
Biol Med 39: 317–326.
Jones HE, Chisolm MS, Jansson LM, Terplan M (2013). Naltrexone in the treatment
of opioid-dependent pregnant women: the case for a considered and measured
approach to research. Addiction 108: 233–247.
Jones J, Rios R, Jones M, Lewis D, Plate C (2009). Determination of amphetamine
and methamphetamine in umbilical cord using liquid chromatography-tandem mass
spectrometry. J Chromatogr B A nalyt Technol Biomed Life Sci 877: 3701–3706.
Jones KL (1986). Fetal alcohol syndrome. Pediatr Rev 8: 122–126.
Jones LB, Stanwood GD, Reinoso BS, Washington RA, Wang HY, Friedman E et al
(2000). In utero cocaine-induced dysfunction of dopamine D1 receptor signaling
and abnormal differentiation of cerebral cortical neurons. J Neurosci 20:
4606–4614.
Joya X, Pujadas M, Falcon M, Civit E, Garcia-Algar O, Vall O et al (2010). Gas
chromatography-mass spectrometry assay for the simultaneous quantification of
drugs of abuse in human placenta at 12th week of gestation. Forensic Sci Int
196: 38–42. Used a chromatography-mass spectrometry assay for the
simultaneous quantification of drugs of abuse—including amphetamine,
methamphetamine, MDMA, methadone, cocaine, morphine, nicotine, and
cotinine—in human placenta at 12th week of gestation.
Jutras-Aswad D, DiNieri JA, Harkany T, Hurd YL (2009). Neurobiological
consequences of maternal cannabis on human fetal development and its
neuropsychiatric outcome. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 259: 395–412.
Kabir ZD, McCarthy DM, Bhide PG, Kosofsky BE (2013). Cup of Joe: a brain
development ‘no’? Sci Transl Med 5: 197fs130.
Kable JA, Coles CD, Lynch ME, Carroll J (2009). The impact of maternal smoking
on fast auditory brainstem responses. Neurotoxicol Teratol 31: 216–224.
Kaltenbach K, Berghella V, Finnegan L (1998). Opioid dependence during
pregnancy. Effects and management. Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am 25:
139–151.
Kandall SR, Albin S, Lowinson J, Berle B, Eidelman AI, Gartner LM (1976).
Differential effects of maternal heroin and methadone use on birthweight.
Pediatrics 58: 681–685.
Katayama H, Itami S, Koizumi H, Tsutsui M (1987). Epidermal cell culture using
Sephadex beads coated with denatured collagen (cytodex 3). J Invest Dermatol
88: 33–36.
Kater SB, Lipton SA (1995). Neurotransmitter regulation of neuronal outgrowth,
plasticity and survival in the year 2001. Trends Neurosci 18: 71–72.
Keating E, Goncalves P, Campos I, Costa F, Martel F (2009). Folic acid uptake by
the human syncytiotrophoblast: interference by pharmacotherapy, drugs of
abuse and pathological conditions. Reprod Toxicol 28: 511–520.
Keimpema E, Mackie K, Harkany T (2011). Molecular model of cannabis sensitivity
in developing neuronal circuits. Trends Pharmacol Sci 32: 551–561.
Key AP, Ferguson M, Molfese DL, Peach K, Lehman C, Molfese VJ (2007). Smoking
during pregnancy affects speech-processing ability in newborn infants. Environ
Health Perspect 115: 623–629.
Killinger CE, Robinson S, Stanwood GD (2012). Subtle biobehavioral effects
produced by paternal cocaine exposure. Synapse 66: 902–908.
Kirby ML (1983). Changes in [3H]naloxone binding in spinal cord of rats treated
prenatally with morphine. Neuropharmacology 22: 303–307.
Kirby ML, Holtzmann SG (1982). Effects of chronic opiate administration of
spontaneous activity of fetal rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 16: 263–269.
Kollins SH, McClernon FJ, Fuemmeler BF (2005). Association between smoking
and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a population-based
sample of young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62: 1142–1147.
Koprich JB, Campbell NG, Lipton JW (2003a). Neonatal 3,4-methylenedioxy-
methamphetamine (ecstasy) alters dopamine and serotonin neurochemistry and
increases brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the forebrain and brainstem of the
rat. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 147: 177–182.
Koprich JB, Chen EY, Kanaan NM, Campbell NG, Kordower JH, Lipton JW
(2003b). Prenatal 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) alters explora-
tory behavior, reduces monoamine metabolism, and increases forebrain tyrosine
hydroxylase fiber density of juvenile rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol 25: 509–517.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
82
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
Kotimaa AJ, Moilanen I, Taanila A, Ebeling H, Smalley SL, McGough JJ et al (2003).
Maternal smoking and hyperactivity in 8-year-old children. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 42: 826–833.
Kuwahara MD, Sparber SB (1981). Prenatal withdrawal from opiates interferes with
hatching of otherwise viable chick fetuses. Science 212: 945–947.
Ladhani NN, Shah PS, Murphy KE (2011). Prenatal amphetamine exposure and
birth outcomes: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Am J Obstet Gynecol
205: 219 e211-217. An extensive meta-analysis of 10 studies of low-to-
moderate risk of bias submits that AMPH exposure in pregnancy is
associated with higher odds of preterm birth, low birth weight, and small
size for gestational age.
LaGasse LL, Derauf C, Smith LM, Newman E, Shah R, Neal C et al (2012). Prenatal
methamphetamine exposure and childhood behavior problems at 3 and 5 years
of age. Pediatrics 129: 681–688.
Lambers DS, Clark KE (1996). The maternal and fetal physiologic effects of nicotine.
Semin Perinatol 20: 115–126.
Langley K, Heron J, Smith GD, Thapar A (2012). Maternal and paternal smoking
during pregnancy and risk of ADHD symptoms in offspring: testing for
intrauterine effects. Am J Epidemiol 176: 261–268.
Langley K, Holmans PA, van den Bree MB, Thapar A (2007). Effects of low birth
weight, maternal smoking in pregnancy and social class on the phenotypic
manifestation of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and associated antisoc ial
behaviour: investigation in a clinical sample. BMC Psychiatry 7: 26.
Lauder JM (1993). Neurotransmitters as growth regulatory signals: role of receptors
and second messengers. Trends Neurosci 16: 233–240. A classic review by a
pioneer in the field, describing how neurotransmitters can regulate
morphogenetic activities such as proliferation, differentiation, cell motility
and metamorphosis.
Lawrence J, Xiao D, Xue Q, Rejali M, Yang S, Zhang L (2008). Prenatal nicotine
exposure increases heart susceptibility to ischemia/reperfusion injury in adult
offspring. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 324: 331–341.
Lebel C, Warner T, Colby J, Soderberg L, Roussotte F, Behnke M et al (2013). White
matter microstructure abnormalities and executive function in adolescents with
prenatal cocaine exposure. Psychiatry Res 213: 161–168.
Leech SL, Richardson GA, Goldschmidt L, Day NL (1999). Prenatal substance
exposure: effects on attention and impulsivity of 6-year-olds. Neurotoxicol Teratol
21: 109–118.
Lejeune C, Simmat-Durand L, Gourarier L, Aubisson S (2006). Prospective
multicenter observational study of 260 infants born to 259 opiate-dependent
mothers on methadone or high-dose buprenophine substitution. Drug Alcohol
Depend 82: 250–257.
Lenoir D, Barg J, Simantov R (1984). Characterization and down-regulation of
opiate receptors in aggregating fetal rat brain cells. Brain Res 304: 285–290.
Leon D, Albasanz JL, Ruiz MA, Martin M (2005). Chronic caffeine or theophylline
intake during pregnancy inhibits A1 receptor function in the rat brain.
Neuroscience 131: 481–489.
Lester BM, Dreher M (1989). Effects of marijuana use during pregnancy on newborn
cry. Child Dev 60: 765–771.
Lester BM, Padbury JF (2009). Third pathophysiology of prenatal cocaine exposure.
Dev Neurosci 31: 23–35. Effects of cocaine on fetal development may also
act by altering genetic programming causing acute and long-term adverse
effects.
Lester BM, Tronick EZ, LaGasse L, Seifer R, Bauer CR, Shankaran S et al (2002). The
maternal lifestyle study: effects of substance exposure during pregnancy on
neurodevelopmental outcome in 1-month-old infants. Pediatrics 110: 1182–1192.
Levin ED, Briggs SJ, Christopher NC, Rose JE (1993). Prenatal nicotine exposure
and cognitive performance in rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15: 251–260.
Levin ED, Lawrence S, Petro A, Horton K, Seidler FJ, Slotkin TA (2006). Increased
nicotine self-administration following prenatal exposure in female rats. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 85: 669–674.
Levine TP, Liu J, Das A, Lester B, Lagasse L, Shankaran S et al (2008). Effects of
prenatal cocaine exposure on special education in school-aged children.
Pediatrics 122: e83–e91.
Lewis BA, Kirchner HL, Short EJ, Minnes S, Weishampel P, Satayathum S et al
(2007). Prenatal cocaine and tobacco effects on children’s language trajectories.
Pediatrics 120: e78–e85.
Lewis BA, Minnes S, Short EJ, Weishampel P, Satayathum S, Min MO et al (2011).
The effects of prenatal cocaine on language development at 10 years of age.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 33: 17–24.
Li Z, Santhanam P, Coles CD, Ellen Lynch M, Hamann S, Peltier S et al (2013).
Prenatal cocaine exposure alters functional activation in the ventral prefrontal
cortex and its structural connectivity with the amygdala. Psychiatry Res 213:
47–55.
Lichtblau L, Sparber SB (1981). Opiate withdrawal in utero increases neonatal
morbidity in the rat. Science 212: 943–945.
Lidow MS, Koh PO, Arnsten AF (2003). D1 dopamine receptors in the mouse
prefrontal cortex: immunocytochemical and cognitive neuropharmacological
analyses. Synapse 47: 101–108.
Lindblad F, Hjern A (2010). ADHD after fetal exposure to maternal smoking. Nicotine
Tob Res 12: 408–415.
Linn S, Schoenbaum SC, Monson RR, Rosner R, Stubblefield PC, R yan KJ (1983).
The association of marijuana use with outcome of pregnancy. Am J Public Health
73: 1161–1164.
Little BB, Snell LM, Gilstrap LC 3rd (1988). Methamphetamine abuse during
pregnancy: outcome and fetal effects. Obstet Gynecol 72: 541–544.
Litto WJ, Griffin JP, Rabii J (1983). Influence of morphine during pregnancy
on neuroendocrine regulation of pituitary hormone secretion. J Endocrinol 98:
289–295.
Liu J, Lester BM, Neyzi N, Sheinkopf SJ, Gracia L, Kekatpure M et al (2013).
Regional brain morphometry and impulsivity in adolescents following prenatal
exposure to cocaine and tobacco. JAMA Pediatr 167: 348–354.
Lloyd SA, Faherty CJ, Smeyne RJ (2006). Adult and in utero exposure to cocaine
alters sensitivity to the Parkinsonian toxin 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahy-
dropyridine. Neuroscience 137: 905–913.
Long DT, King MA, Sheehan J (1992). Comparative evaluation of image
segmentation methods for volume quantitation in SPECT. Med Phys 19: 483–
489.
Loomans EM, Hofland L, van der Stelt O, van der Wal MF, Koot HM, Van den Bergh
BR et al (2012). Caffeine intake during pregnancy and risk of problem behavior in
5- to 6-year-old children. Pediatrics 130: e305–e313.
Lorenzo AM, Leon D, Castillo CA, Ruiz MA, Albasanz JL, Martin M (2010). Maternal
caffeine intake during gestation and lactation down-regulates adenosine A1
receptor in rat brain from mothers and neonates. J Neurosci Res 88: 1252–1261.
Lotfipour S, Ferguson E, Leonard G, Perron M, Pike B, Richer L et al (2009).
Orbitofrontal cortex and drug use during adolescence: role of prenatal exposure
to maternal smoking and BDNF genotype. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 1244–1252.
Report indicating that prenatal cigarette exposure produces long-term
behavioral and neurological aberrations.
Lotfipour S, Leonard G, Perron M, Pike B, Richer L, Seguin JR et al (2010). Prenatal
exposure to maternal cigarette smoking interacts with a polymorphism in the
alpha6 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor gene to influence drug use and striatum
volume in adolescence. Mol Psychiatry 15: 6–8.
Lu R, Liu X, Long H, Ma L (2012). Effects of prenatal cocaine and heroin exposure
on neuronal dendrite morphogenesis and spatial recognition memory in mice.
Neurosci Lett 522: 128–133.
Luo J, Miller MW (1998). Growth factor-mediated neural proliferation: target of
ethanol toxicity. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 27: 157–167.
Lupton C, Burd L, Harwood R (2004). Cost of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Am
J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet 127C: 42–50.
Maas U, Kattner E, Weingart-Jesse B, Schafer A, Obladen M (1990). Infrequent
neonatal opiate withdrawal following maternal methadone detoxification during
pregnancy. J Perinat Med 18: 111–118.
Mactutus CF, Herman AS, Booze RM (1994). Chronic intravenous model for studies
of drug (Ab)use in the pregnant and/or group-housed rat: an initial study with
cocaine. Neurotoxicol Teratol 16: 183–191. The first rodent intravenous
cocaine during pregnancy animal model.
Malanga CJ 3rd, Kosofsky BE (1999). Mechanisms of action of drugs of abuse on
the developing fetal brain. Clin Perinatol 26: 17–37. v-vi.
Mansi G, Raimondi F, Pichini S, Capasso L, Sarno M, Zuccaro P et al (2007).
Neonatal urinary cotinine correlates with behavioral alterations in newbor ns
prenatally exposed to tobacco smoke. Pediatr Res 61: 257–261.
Mantha K, Laufer BI, Singh SM (2014). Molecular changes during neurodevelop-
ment following second-trimester binge ethanol exposure in a mouse model of
fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: from immediate effects to long-term adaptation.
Dev Neurosci 36: 29–43.
Martinez A, Kastner B, Taeusch HW (1999). Hyperphagia in neonates withdrawing
from methadone. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 80: F178–F182.
Mattson SN, Riley EP, Gramling L, Delis DC, Jones KL (1998). Neuropsychological
comparison of alcohol-exposed children with or without physical features of fetal
alcohol syndrome. Neuropsychology 12: 146–153.
Mayes LC (1994). Neurobiology of prenatal cocaine exposure effect on developing
monoamine systems. Infant Mental Hlth J 15: 121–133.
Mayes LC (2002). A behavioral teratogenic model of the impact of prenatal cocaine
exposure on arousal regulatory systems. Neurotoxicol Teratol 24: 385–395.
Mayes LC, Cicchetti D, Acharyya S, Zhang H (2003). Developmental trajectories of
cocaine-and-other-drug-exposed and non-cocaine-exposed children. J Dev
Behav Pediatr 24: 323–335.
McCarthy DM, Bhide PG (2012). Prenatal cocaine exposure decreases parvalbu-
min-immunoreactive neurons and GABA-to-projection neuron ratio in the medial
prefrontal cortex. Dev Neurosci 34: 174–183.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
83
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
McCarthy DM, Brown AN, Bhide PG (2012). Regulation of BDNF expression by
cocaine. Yale J Biol Med 85: 437–446.
McElhatton PR, Bateman DN, Evans C, Pughe KR, Thomas SH (1999). Congenital
anomalies after prenatal ecstasy exposure. Lancet 354: 1441–1442.
McGinty JF, Ford DH (1980). Effects of prenatal methadone on rat brain
catecholamines. Dev Neurosci 3: 224–234.
McKnight v. State of South Carolina (2008): McKnight v. State of South Carolina,.
26484, THE STATE OF SOUTH CAROLINA In The Supreme Court.
McLaughlin AA, Minnes S, Singer LT, Min M, Short EJ, Scott TL et al (2011).
Caregiver and self-report of mental health symptoms in 9-year old children with
prenatal cocaine exposure. Neurotoxicol Teratol 33: 582–591.
McLaughlin PJ, Tobias SW, Lang CM, Zagon IS (1997a). Chronic exposure to the
opioid antagonist naltrexone during pregnancy: maternal and offspring effects.
Physiol Behav 62: 501–508.
McLaughlin PJ, Tobias SW, Lang CM, Zagon IS (1997b). Opioid receptor blockade
during prenatal life modifies postnatal behavioral development. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 58: 1075–1082.
McLaughlin PJ, Zagon IS, White WJ (1978). Perinatal methadone exposure in rats.
Effects on body and organ development. Biol Neonate 34: 48–54.
Medina AE, Krahe TE, Ramoa AS (2006). Restoration of neuronal plasticity by a
phosphodiesterase type 1 inhibitor in a model of fetal alcohol exposure.
J Neurosci 26: 1057–1060.
Medina Jimenez M, Lujan Estrada M, Rodriguez R (1997). Influence of chronic
prenatal and postnatal administration of naltrexone in locomotor activity induced
by morphine in mice. Arch Med Res 28: 61–65.
Mehmedic Z, Chandra S, Slade D, Denham H, Foster S, Patel AS et al (2010).
Potency trends of Delta9-THC and other cannabinoids in confiscated cannabis
preparations from 1993 to 2008. J Forensic Sci 55: 1209–1217.
Mereu G, Fa M, Ferraro L, Cagiano R, Antonelli T, Tattoli M et al (2003). Prenatal
exposure to a cannabinoid agonist produces memory deficits linked to
dysfunction in hippocampal long-term potentiation and glutamate release. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 100: 4915–4920.
Meyer JS, Grande M, Johnson K, Ali SF (2004). Neurotoxic effects of MDMA
(‘ecstasy’) administration to neonatal rats. Int J Dev Neurosci 22: 261–271.
Meyer KD, Zhang H, Zhang L (2009). Prenatal cocaine exposure abolished
ischemic preconditioning-induced protection in adult male rat hearts: role of
PKCepsilon. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 296: H1566–H1576.
Meyer KD, Zhang L (2009). Short- and long-term adverse effects of cocaine abuse
during pregnancy on the heart development. Ther Adv Cardiovasc Dis 3: 7–16.
Miller MW (2007). Exposure to ethanol during gastrulation alters somatosensory-
motor cortices and the underlying white matter in the macaque. Cereb Cortex
17: 2961–2971.
Miller MW, Hu H (2009). Lability of neuronal lineage decisions is revealed by acute
exposures to ethanol. Dev Neurosci 31: 50–57.
Minnes S, Robin NH, Alt AA, Kirchner HL, Satayathum S, Salbert BA et al (2006).
Dysmorphic and anthropometric outcomes in 6-year-old prenatally cocaine-
exposed children. Neurotoxicol Teratol 28: 28–38.
Minnes S, Singer LT, Min MO, Lang AM, Ben-Harush A, Short E et al (2014). Compar-
ison of 12-year-old children with prenatal exposure to cocaine and non-exposed
controls on caregiver ratings of executive function. JYouthAdolesc43: 53–69.
Minozzi S, Amato L, Bellisario C, Ferri M, Davoli M (2013). Maintenance agonist
treatments for opiate-dependent pregnant women. Cochrane Database Syst Rev
12: CD006318. Demonstrated that the evidence for suggesting methadone
treatment over buprenorphine treatment during pregnancy is actually not
thoroughly demonstrated.
Money KM, Stanwood GD (2013). Developmental origins of brain disorders: roles
for dopamine. Front Cell Neurosci 7: 260.
Morrow BA, Elsworth JD, Roth RH (2002a). Male rats exposed to cocaine in utero
demonstrate elevated expression of Fos in the prefrontal cortex in response to
environment. Neuropsychopharmacology 26: 275–285. A terrific rat model of
intravenous cocaine furing pregnancy.
Morrow BA, Elsworth JD, Roth RH (2002b). Prenatal cocaine exposure disrupts
non-spatial, short-term memory in adolescent and adult male rats. Behav Brain
Res 129: 217–223.
Muhammad A, Mychasiuk R, Nakahashi A, Hossain SR, Gibb R, Kolb B (2012).
Prenatal nicotine exposure alters neuroanatomical organization of the developing
brain. Synapse 66: 950–954.
Mychasiuk R, Muhammad A, Gibb R, Kolb B (2013). Long-term alterations to
dendritic morphology and spine density associated with prenatal exposure to
nicotine. Brain Res 1499: 53–60.
Nash K, Sheard E, Rovet J, Koren G (2008). Understanding fetal alcohol spectrum
disorders (FASDs): toward identification of a behavioral phenotype. Scientific-
WorldJournal 8: 873–882.
Nash K, Stevens S, Rovet J, Fantus E, Nulman I, Sorbara D et al (2013). Towards
identifying a characteristic neuropsychological profile for fetal alcohol spectrum
disorders. 1. Analysis of the Motherisk FASD clinic. J Popul Ther Clin Pharmacol
20: e44–e52.
Nassogne MC, Evrard P, Courtoy PJ (1998). Selective direct toxicity of cocaine on
fetal mouse neurons. Teratogenic implications of neurite and apoptotic neuronal
loss. Ann N Y Acad Sci 846: 51–68.
Navarro M, Rubio P, de Fonseca FR (1995). Behavioural consequences of maternal
exposure to natural cannabinoids in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 122: 1–14.
Excellent guide on the role of developmental marijuana and resulting
behavioral aberrations, primarily in animal models.
Nguyen D, Smith LM, Lagasse LL, Derauf C, Grant P, Shah R et al (2010).
Intrauterine growth of infants exposed to prenatal methamphetamine: results
from the infant development, environment, and lifestyle study. J Pediatr 157:
337–339.
Oei JL, Kingsbury A, Dhawan A, Burns L, Feller JM, Clews S et al (2012).
Amphetamines, the pregnant woman and her children: a review. J Perinatol 32:
737–747. An extensive and detailed review on perinatal AMPH use and its
outcomes.
Oliveira MT, Rego AC, Macedo TR, Oliveira CR (2003). Drugs of abuse induce
apoptotic features in PC12 cells. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1010: 667–670.
Oliveira MT, Rego AC, Morgadinho MT, Macedo TR, Oliveira CR (2002). Toxic
effects of opioid and stimulant drugs on undifferentiated PC12 cells. Ann N Y
Acad Sci 965: 487–496.
Ornoy A, Michailevskaya V, Lukashov I, Bar-Hamburger R, Harel S (1996). The
developmental outcome of children born to heroin-dependent mothers, raised at
home or adopted. Child Abuse Negl 20: 385–396.
Ornoy A, Segal J, Bar-Hamburger R, Greenbaum C (2001). Developmental
outcome of school-age children born to mothers with heroin dependency:
importance of environmental factors. Dev Med Child Neurol 43: 668–675.
Clearly demonstrated that improved environment cannot compensate for
all effects seen with prenatal heroin exposure.
Oro AS, Dixon SD (1987). Perinatal cocaine and methamphetamine exposure:
maternal and neonatal correlates. J Pediatr 111: 571–578.
Pappalardo-Carter DL, Balaraman S, Sathyan P, Carter ES, Chen WJ, Miranda RC
(2013). Suppression and epigenetic regulation of MiR-9 contributes to ethanol
teratology: evidence from zebrafish and murine fetal neural stem cell models.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 37: 1657–1667.
Parikh R, Hussain T, Holder G, Bhoyar A, Ewer AK (2011). Maternal methadone
therapy increases QTc interval in newborn infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal
Ed 96: F141–F143.
Paul AP, Pohl-Guimaraes F, Krahe TE, Filgueiras CC, Lantz CL, Colello RJ et al
(2010). Overexpression of serum response factor restores ocular dominance
plasticity in a model of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J Neurosci 30: 2513–
2520.
Paul JA, Logan BA, Krishnan R, Heller NA, Morrison DG, Pritham UA et al (2014).
Development of auditory event-related potentials in infants prenatally exposed to
methadone. Dev Psychobiol 56: 1119–1128.
Pauly JR, Slotkin TA (2008). Maternal tobacco smoking, nicotine replacement and
neurobehavioural development. Acta Paediatr 97: 1331–1337. Overview of
developmental tobacco research that stresses the need for safer
alternatives.
Pertschuk LP, Ford DH, Rainford EA (1977). Localization of methadone in fetal rat
eye by the immunofluorescence technic. Exp Eye Res 24: 547–552.
Piper BJ, Farelli JD, Meyer JS (2009). Dissociation between serotonin neurotoxicity
and brain-derived neurotrophic factor induction following neonatal MDMA
exposure in rats. Dev Neurosci 31: 90–94.
Piper BJ, Meyer JS (2006). Increased responsiveness to MDMA in adult rats treated
neonatally with MDMA. Neurotoxicol Teratol 28: 95–102. A well-designed and
carried out study demonstrating that developmental MDMA administration
can induce long-lasting functional abnormalities, including enhanced
sensitivity toMDMA itself.
Poland ML, Dombrowski MP, Ager JW, Sokol RJ (1993). Punishing pregnant drug
users: enhancing the flight from care. Drug Alcohol Depend 31: 199–203.
Popova S, Stade B, Bekmuradov D, Lange S, Rehm J (2011). What do we know
about the economic impact of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder? A systematic
literature review. Alcohol Alcohol 46: 490–497.
Porciuncula LO, Sallaberry C, Mioranzza S, Botton PH, Rosemberg DB (2013). The
Janus face of caffeine. Neurochem Int 63: 594–609. This is an excellent
comprehensive review of the known effects of developmental caffeine
exposure.
Powrozek TA, Miller MW (2009). Ethanol affects transforming growth factor beta1-
initiated signals: cross-talking pathways in the developing rat cerebral wall. J
Neurosci 29: 9521–9533.
Ramamoorthy JD, Ramamoorthy S, Leibach FH, Ganapathy V (1995). Human
placental monoamine transporters as targets for amphetamines. Am J Obstet
Gynecol 173: 1782–1787.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
84
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
Rando K, Chaplin TM, Potenza MN, Mayes L, Sinha R (2013). Prenatal cocaine
exposure and gray matter volume in adolescent boys and girls: relationship to
substance use initiation. Biol Psychiatry 74: 482–489.
Reijneveld SA, Brugman E, Hirasing RA (2002). Excessive infant crying: definitions
determine risk groups. Arch Dis Child 87: 43–44.
Ren JQ, Malanga CJ, Tabit E, Kosofsky BE (2004). Neuropathological con-
sequences of prenatal cocaine exposure in the mouse. Int J Dev Neurosci 22:
309–320.
Ricalde AA, Hammer RP Jr (1990). Perinatal opiate treatment delays growth of
cortical dendrites. Neurosci Lett 115: 137–143.
Richardson GA, Day NL, Goldschmidt L (1995). Prenatal alcohol, marijuana, and
tobacco use: infant mental and motor development. Neurotoxicol Teratol 17:
479–487. Evidence of cognitive deficits in children as young as 9 months of
age with prenatal alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco exposure.
Richardson GA, Goldschmidt L, Larkby C, Day NL (2013). Effects of prenatal
cocaine exposure on child behavior and growth at 10 years of age. Neurotoxicol
Teratol 40: 1–8.
Richardson GA, Goldschmidt L, Willford J (2008). The effects of prenatal cocaine
use on infant development. Neurotoxicol Teratol 30: 96–106.
Richardson GA, Ryan C, Willford J, Day NL, Goldschmidt L (2002). Prenatal alcohol
and marijuana exposure: effects on neuropsychological outcomes at 10 years.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 24: 309–320.
Riley EP, McGee CL (2005). Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: an overview
with emphasis on changes in brain and behavior. Exp Biol Med (Maywood) 230:
357–365.
Robinson SE (2002). Effects of perinatal buprenorphine and methadone exposures
on striatal cholinergic ontogeny. Neurotoxicol Teratol 24: 137–142.
Robinson SE, Maher JR, Wallace MJ, Kunko PM (1997). Perinatal methadone
exposure affects dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin in the weanling rat.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 19: 295–303.
Robinson SE, Mo Q, Maher JR, Wallace MJ, Kunko PM (1996). Perinatal exposure
to methadone affects central cholinergic activity in the weanling rat. Drug Alcohol
Depend 41: 119–126.
Robinson SE, Wallace MJ (2001). Effect of perinatal buprenorphine exposure on
development in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 298: 797–804.
Rodgers AB, Morgan CP, Bronson SL, Revello S, Bale TL (2013). Paternal stress
exposure alters sperm microRNA content and reprograms offspring HPA stress
axis regulation. J Neurosci 33: 9003–9012.
Rosen TS, Johnson HL (1985). Long-term effects of prenatal methadone
maintenance. NIDA Res Monogr 59: 73–83.
Rosenthal DG, Weitzman M, Benowitz NL (2011). Nicotine addiction: mechanisms
and consequences. Int J Mental Health 40: 22–38. Thorough discussion of the
consequences of developmental tobacco exposure.
Rubio P, Rodriguez de Fonseca F, Munoz RM, Ariznavarreta C, Martin-Calderon JL,
Navarro M (1995). Long-term behavioral effects of perinatal exposure to delta 9-
tetrahydrocannabinol in rats: possible role of pituitary-adrenal axis. Life Sci 56:
2169–2176.
Ruckinger S, Rzehak P, Chen CM, Sausenthaler S, Koletzko S, Bauer CP et al
(2010). Prenatal and postnatal tobacco exposure and behavioral problems in
10-year-old children: results from the GINI-plus prospective birth cohort study.
Environ Health Perspect 118: 150–154.
Rudnick G, Wall SC (1992). The molecular mechanism of ‘ecstasy’
[3,4-methylenedioxy-methamphetamine (MDMA)]: serotonin transporters are
targets for MDMA-induced serotonin release. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:
1817–1821.
Saadani-Makki F, Frugiere A, Gros F, Gaytan S, Bodineau L (2004). Involvement
of adenosinergic A1 systems in the occurrence of respiratory perturbations
encountered in newborns following an in utero caffeine exposure. a study on
brainstem-spinal cord preparations isolated from newborn rats. Neuroscience
127: 505–518.
Sahir N, Bahi N, Evrard P, Gressens P (2000). Caffeine induces in vivo premature
appearance of telencephalic vesicles. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 121: 213–217.
Sahir N, Evrard P, Gressens P (2004). Caffeine induces sonic hedgehog gene
expression in cultured astrocytes and neurons. J Mol Neurosci 24: 201–205.
Sampson PD, Streissguth AP, Bookstein FL, Little RE, Clarren SK, Dehaene P et al
(1997). Incidence of fetal alcohol syndrome and prevalence of alcohol-related
neurodevelopmental disorder. Teratology 56: 317–326.
Sanchez ES, Bigbee JW, Fobbs W, Robinson SE, Sato-Bigbee C (2008). Opioid
addiction and pregnancy: perinatal exposure to buprenorphine affects myelina-
tion in the developing brain. Glia 56: 1017–1027. Provided evidence that
suggests a mechanism for altered myelination patterns seen in prenatal
opiate exposed neonates.
Santiago SE, Huffman KJ (2013). Prenatal nicotine exposure increases anxiety and
modifies sensorimotor integration behaviors in adult female mice. Neurosci Res
79: 41–51.
Sarfi M, Martinsen H, Bakstad B, Roislien J, Waal H (2009). Patterns in sleep-
wakefulness in three-month old infants exposed to methadone or buprenor-
phine. Early Hum Dev 85: 773–778.
Sari Y, Zhou FC (2004). Prenatal alcohol exposure causes long-term serotonin
neuron deficit in mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28: 941–948.
Scher MS, Richardson GA, Coble PA, Day NL, Stoffer DS (1988). The effects of
prenatal alcohol and marijuana exposure: disturbances in neonatal sleep cycling
and arousal. Pediatr Res 24: 101–105.
Schindler SD, Eder H, Ortner R, Rohrmeister K, Langer M, Fischer G (2003).
Neonatal outcome following buprenorphine maintenance during conception and
throughout pregnancy. Addiction 98: 103–110.
Schneider M (2009). Cannabis use in pregnancy and early life and its
consequences: animal models. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 259: 383–393.
Schneider T, Ilott N, Brolese G, Bizarro L, Asherson PJ, Stolerman IP (2011).
Prenatal exposure to nicotine impairs performance of the 5-choice serial reaction
time task in adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 1114–1125. One of first
studies to show prenatal nicotine alters attention in rodents.
Scott JC, Woods SP, Matt GE, Meyer RA, Heaton RK, Atkinson JH et al (2007).
Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis.
Neuropsychol Rev 17: 275–297.
Shankaran S, Das A, Bauer CR, Bada HS, Lester BM, Wright LL et al (2011).
Prenatal cocaine exposure and small-for-gestational-age status: effects on
growth at 6 years of age. Neurotoxicol Teratol 33: 575–581.
Silva CG, Metin C, Fazeli W, Machado NJ, Darmopil S, Launay PS et al (2013).
Adenosine receptor antagonists including caffeine alter fetal brain development
in mice. Sci Transl Med 5: 197ra104. This exciting paper found that caffeine
or another A2A receptor antagonist delayed the migration of specific
populations of neurons during brain maturation, increased seizure-
susceptibility, and impaired memory.
Silva L, Zhao N, Popp S, Dow-Edwards D (2012). Prenatal tetrahydrocannabinol
(THC) alters cognitive function and amphetamine response from weaning to
adulthood in the rat. Neurotoxicol Teratol 34: 63–71.
Singer LT, Arendt R, Minnes S, Farkas K, Salvator A (2000). Neurobehavioral
outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants. Neurotoxicol Teratol 22: 653–666 . Well-
designed and implemented study corroborated with previous findings of early
effects in cocaine-exposed infants taking into account multiple variables.
Singer LT, Moore DG, Fulton S, Goodwin J, Turner JJ, Min MO et al (2012).
Neurobehavioral outcomes of infants exposed to MDMA (Ecstasy) and other
recreational drugs during pregnancy. Neurotoxicol Teratol 34: 303–310. Most
current study of prenatal MDMA exposure predicting poorer infant mental
and motor development at 4 and 12 months of age in a dose-dependent
manner.
Skelton MR, Williams MT, Vorhees CV (2008). Developmental effects of 3,4-
methylenedioxymethamphetamine: a review. Behav Pharmacol 19: 91–111.
Slamberova R (2012). Drugs in pregnancy: the effects on mother and her progeny.
Physiol Res 61: S123–S135.
Slamberova R, Pometlova M, Syllabova L, Mancuskova M (2005). Learning in the
place navigation task, not the new-learning task, is altered by prenatal
methamphetamine exposure. Dev Brain Res 157: 217–219.
Slotkin TA (2004). Cholinergic systems in brain development and disruption by
neurotoxicants: nicotine, environmental tobacco smoke, organophosphates.
Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 198: 132–151. Another excellent review by one of the
leaders in the nicotine research field.
Slotkin TA (2008). If nicotine is a developmental neurotoxicant in animal studies,
dare we recommend nicotine replacement therapy in pregnant women and
adolescents? Neurotoxicol Teratol 30: 1–19.
Slotkin TA, Pinkerton KE, Seidler FJ (2006). Perinatal environmental tobacco smoke
exposure in rhesus monkeys: critical periods and regional selectivity for effects
on brain cell development and lipid peroxidation. Environ Health Perspect 114:
34–39.
Slotkin TA, Weigel SJ, Seidler FJ, Whitmore WL (1982). Toxic effects of maternal
methadone administration on cardiac development in the neonatal rat: potential
participation of altered polyamine levels in growth impairment. Toxicol Lett 14:
131–135. Provided a potential mechanism for transient cardiac abnormal-
ities seen in patients.
Smith AM, Fried PA, Hogan MJ, Cameron I (2004). Effects of prenatal marijuana on
response inhibition: an fMRI study of young adults. Neurotoxicol Teratol 26: 533–
542. One of first studies to demonstrate that the effects of prenatal
marijuana last well into adulthood.
Smith AM, Fried PA, Hogan MJ, Cameron I (2006a). Effects of prenatal marijuana on
visuospatial working memory: an fMRI study in young adults. Neurotoxicol Teratol
28: 286–295.
Smith L, Yonekura ML, Wallace T, Berman N, Kuo J, Berkowitz C (2003). Effects of
prenatal methamphetamine exposure on fetal growth and drug withdrawal
symptoms in infants born at term. J Dev Behav Pediatr 24: 17–23.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
85
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
Smith LM, LaGasse LL, Derauf C, Grant P, Shah R, Arria A et al (2006b). The infant
development, environment, and lifestyle study: effects of prenatal methamphe-
tamine exposure, polydrug exposure, and poverty on intrauterine growth.
Pediatrics 118: 1149–1156.
Smith LM, LaGasse LL, Derauf C, Newman E, Shah R, Haning W et al (2011).
Motor and cognitive outcomes through three years of age in children exposed to
prenatal methamphetamine. Neurotoxicol Teratol 33: 176–184.
Soellner DE, Grandys T, Nunez JL (2009). Chronic prenatal caffeine exposure
impairs novel object recognition and radial arm maze behaviors in adult rats.
Behav Brain Res 205: 191–199.
Soepatmi S (1994). Developmental outcomes of children of mothers dependent on
heroin or heroin/methadone during pregnancy. Acta Paediatr Suppl 404: 36–39.
Sorenson CA, Raskin LA, Suh Y (1991). The effects of prenatal nicotine on radial-
arm maze performance in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 40: 991–993.
Spano MS, Ellgren M, Wang X, Hurd YL (2007). Prenatal cannabis exposure
increases heroin seeking with allostatic changes in limbic enkephalin systems in
adulthood. Biol Psychiatry 61: 554–563.
Spear LP (2014). Adolescents and alcohol: acute sensitivities, enhanced intake,
and later consequences. Neurotoxicol Teratol 41C: 51–59. An up-to-date
comprehensive review of critical brain changes during adolescence and
how alcohol induces behavioral, cognitive, and neural alterations that are
highly specific, replicable, persistent and dependent on the timing of the
exposure during maturation.
Spear LP, Varlinskaya EI (2005). Adolescence. Alcohol sensitivity, tolerance, and
intake. Recent Dev Alcohol 17: 143–159.
Stanwood GD, Levitt P (2003). Repeated i.v. cocaine exposure produces long-
lasting behavioral sensitization in pregnant adults, but behavioral tolerance in
their offspring. Neuroscience 122: 579–583.
Stanwood GD, Levitt P (2004). Drug exposure early in life: functional repercussions
of changing neuropharmacology during sensitive periods of brain development.
Curr Opin Pharmacol 4: 65–71.
Stanwood GD, Parlaman JP, Levitt P (2005). Anatomical abnormalities in
dopaminoceptive regions of the cerebral cortex of dopamine D1 receptor
mutant mice. J Comp Neurol 487: 270–282.
Stanwood GD, Washington RA, Levitt P (2001a). Identification of a sensitive period
of prenatal cocaine exposure that alters the development of the anterior cingulate
cortex. Cereb Cortex 11: 430–440.
Stanwood GD, Washington RA, Shumsky JS, Levitt P (2001b). Prenatal cocaine
exposure produces consistent developmental alterations in dopamine-rich
regions of the cerebral cortex. Neuroscience 106: 5–14.
Steingart RA, Abu-Roumi M, Newman ME, Silverman WF, Slotkin TA, Yanai J
(2000a). Neurobehavioral damage to cholinergic systems caused by prenatal
exposure to heroin or phenobarbital: cellular mechanisms and the reversal of
deficits by neural grafts. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 122: 125–133.
Steingart RA, Silverman WF, Barron S, Slotkin TA, Awad Y, Yanai J (2000b). Neural
grafting reverses prenatal drug-induced alterations in hippocampal PKC and
related behavioral deficits. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 125: 9–19. Provided
evidence for the functional significance of hippocampal cholinergic deficits
in models of prenatal opiate exposure.
Stene-Larsen K, Borge AI, Vollrath ME (2009). Maternal smoking in pregnancy and
externalizing behavior in 18-month-old children: results from a population-based
prospective study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48: 283–289.
Stroud LR, Paster RL, Goodwin MS, Shenassa E, Buka S, Niaura R et al (2009a).
Maternal smoking during pregnancy and neonatal behavior: a large-scale
community study. Pediatrics 123: e842–e848.
Stroud LR, Paster RL, Papandonatos GD, Niaura R, Salisbury AL, Battle C et al
(2009b). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and newborn neurobehavior:
effects at 10 to 27 days. J Pediatr 154: 10–16.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2008). The NSDUH
Report—Alcohol Use among Pregnant Women and Recent Mothers: 2002 to
2007. Office of Applied Studies: Rockville, MD.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2009). The NSDUH
Report: Substance Use among Women During Pregnancy and Following
Childbirth. Office of Applied Studies: Rockville, MD.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2013). Results from
the 2012 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National
Findings. Administration, S. A. a. M. H. S: Rockville, MD.
Sundelin Wahlsten V, Sarman I (2013). Neurobehavioural development of pre-
school-age children born to addicted mothers given opiate maintenance
treatment with buprenorphine during pregnancy. Acta Paediatr 102: 544–549.
Svensson AL, Bucht N, Hallberg M, Nyberg F (2008). Reversal of opiate-induced
apoptosis by human recombinant growth hormone in murine foetus primary
hippocampal neuronal cell cultures. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105: 7304–7308.
Tempel A (1991). Visualization of mu opiate receptor downregulation following
morphine treatment in neonatal rat brain. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 64: 19–26.
Tennes K, Avitable N, Blackard C, Boyles C, Hassoun B, Holmes L et al (1985).
Marijuana: prenatal and postnatal exposure in the human. NIDA Res Monogr 59:
48–60.
Thompson BL, Levitt P, Stanwood GD (2005). Prenatal cocaine exposure specifically
alters spontaneous alternation behavior. Behav Brain Res 164: 107–116.
Thompson BL, Levitt P, Stanwood GD (2009a). Prenatal exposure to drugs: effects
on brain development and implications for policy and education. Nat Rev
Neurosci 10: 303–312.
Thompson VB, Heiman J, Chambers JB, Benoit SC, Buesing WR, Norman MK et al
(2009b). Long-term behavioral consequences of prenatal MDMA exposure.
Physiol Behav 96: 593–601.
Thompson VB, Koprich JB, Chen EY, Kordower JH, Terpstra BT, Lipton JW (2012).
Prenatal exposure to MDMA alters noradrenergic neurodevelopment in the rat.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 34: 206–213.
Tong VT, Dietz PM, Morrow B, D’Angelo DV, Farr SL, Rockhill KM et al (2013).
Trends in smoking before, during, and after pregnancy–Pregnancy Risk
Assessment Monitoring System, United States, 40 sites, 2000-2010. MMWR
Surveill Summ 62: 1–19.
Treit S, Lebel C, Baugh L, Rasmussen C, Andrew G, Beaulieu C (2013). Longitudinal
MRI reveals altered trajectory of brain development during childhood and
adolescence in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. JNeurosci33: 10098–10109.
Trezza V, Campolongo P, Cassano T, Macheda T, Dipasquale P, Carratu MR et al
(2008a). Effects of perinatal exposure to delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol on the
emotional reactivity of the offspring: a longitudinal behavioral study in W istar rats.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 198: 529–537.
Trezza V, Cuomo V, Vanderschuren LJ (2008b). Cannabis and the developing brain:
insights from behavior. Eur J Pharmacol 585: 441–452.
Tronick EZ, Messinger DS, Weinberg MK, Lester BM, Lagasse L, Seifer R et al
(2005). Cocaine exposure is associated with subtle compromises of infants’ and
mothers’ social-emotional behavior and dyadic features of their interaction in the
face-to-face still-face paradigm. Dev Psychol 41: 711–722.
Tsang D, Ho KP, Wen HL (1986). Effect of maternal methadone administration on
the development of multiple forms of monoamine oxidase in rat brain and liver.
Brain Res 391: 187–192.
Twomey J, LaGasse L, Derauf C, Newman E, Shah R, Smith L et al (2013).
Prenatal methamphetamine exposure, home environment, and primary caregiver
risk factors predict child behavioral problems at 5 years. Am J Orthopsychiatry
83: 64–72. Study invested the possible association between prenatal
methamphetamine exposure and child behavioral problems at 5 years
and closely examining home environment and primary caregiver risk
factors.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2010). How Tobacco Smoke
Causes Disease: The Biology and Behavioral Basis for Smoking-Attributable
Disease: A Report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA.
Ungerer M, Knezovich J, Ramsay M (2013). In utero alcohol exposure, epigenetic
changes, and their consequences. Alcohol Res 35: 37–46.
UNODC (2013). World Drug Report 2013. United Nations Publication.
Vaglenova J, Parameshwaran K, Suppiramaniam V, Breese CR, Pandiella N, Birru S
(2008). Long-lasting teratogenic effects of nicotine on cognition: gender specificity
and role of AMPA receptor function. Neurobiol Learn Mem 90: 527–536.
Valenzuela CF, Morton RA, Diaz MR, Topper L (2012). Does moderate drinking harm
the fetal brain? Insights from animal models. Trends Neurosci 35: 284–292.
van Baar A (1990). Development of infants of drug dependent mothers. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 31: 911–920.
van Baar A, de Graaff BM (1994). Cognitive development at preschool-age of
infants of drug-dependent mothers. Dev Med Child Neurol 36: 1063–1075.
van Baar AL, Soepatmi S, Gunning WB, Akkerhuis GW (1994). Development after
prenatal exposure to cocaine, heroin and methadone. Acta Paediatr Suppl 404:
40–46.
van Tonningen-van Driel MM, Garbis-Berkvens JM, Reuvers-Lodewijks WE (1999).
[Pregnancy outcome after ecstasy use; 43 cases followed by the Teratology
Information Service of the National Institute for Public Health and Environment
(RIVM)]. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd 143: 27–31.
Vargas GC, Pildes RS, Vidyasagar D, Keith LG (1975). Effect of maternal heroin
addiction on 67 liveborn neonates. Withdrawal symptoms, small body size, and
small head circumference were frequent findings. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 14:
751–753.
Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP (2002). Acute effects of ethanol on social behavior of
adolescent and adult rats: role of familiarity of the test situation. Alcohol Clin Exp
Res 26: 1502–1511.
Vassoler FM, White SL, Schmidt HD, Sadri-Vakili G, Pierce RC (2013).
Epigenetic inheritance of a cocaine-resistance phenotype. Nat Neurosci 16:
42–47.
Vathy I, Katay L (1992). Effects of prenatal morphine on adult sexual behavior and
brain catecholamines in rats. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 68: 125–131.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
86
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
Villarreal DM, Derrick B, Vathy I (2008). Prenatal morphine exposure attenuates the
maintenance of late LTP in lateral perforant path projections to the dentate gyrus
and the CA3 region in vivo.J Neurophysiol 99: 1235–1242.
Vorhees CV, Inman-Wood SL, Morford LL, Broening HW, Fukumura M, Moran MS
(2000). Adult learning deficits after neonatal exposure to D-methamphetamine:
selective effects on spatial navigation and memory. J Neurosci 20: 4732–4739.
Vorhees CV, Reed TM, Skelton MR, Williams MT (2004). Exposure to 3,4-
methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) on postnatal days 11-20 induces
reference but not working memory deficits in the Morris water maze in rats:
implications of prior learning. Int J Dev Neurosci 22: 247–259.
Wakschlag LS, Leventhal BL, Pine DS, Pickett KE, Carter AS (2006a). Elucidating
early mechanisms of developmental psychopathology: the case of prenatal
smoking and disruptive behavior. Child Dev 77: 893–906.
Wakschlag LS, Pickett KE, Kasza KE, Loeber R (2006b). Is prenatal smoking
associated with a developmental pattern of conduct problems in young boys?
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 45: 461–467.
Walhovd KB, Watts R, Amlien I, Woodward LJ (2012). Neural tract development of
infants born to methadone-maintained mothers. Pediatr Neurol 47: 1–6.
Demonstrated novel finding of altered development of myelination patterns
in prenatal opiate exposed neonates.
Wang X, Dow-Edwards D, Anderson V, Minkoff H, Hurd YL (2004). In utero
marijuana exposure associated with abnormal amygdala dopamine D2 gene
expression in the human fetus. Biol Psychiatry 56: 909–915.
Wang X, Dow-Edwards D, Keller E, Hurd YL (2003). Preferential limbic expression of
the cannabinoid receptor mRNA in the human fetal brain. Neuroscience 118:
681–694.
Wang Y, Han TZ (2009). Prenatal exposure to heroin in mice elicits memory deficits
that can be attributed to neuronal apoptosis. Neuroscience 160: 330–338.
Warren KR, Li TK (2005). Genetic polymorphisms: impact on the risk of fetal alcohol
spectrum disorders. Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol Teratol 73: 195–203.
Weitzman M, Gortmaker S, Sobol A (1992). Maternal smoking and behavior
problems of children. Pediatrics 90: 342–349.
Welle-Strand GK, Skurtveit S, Jones HE, Waal H, Bakstad B, Bjarko L et al (2013).
Neonatal outcomes following in utero exposure to methadone or buprenorphine:
a National Cohort Study of opioid-agonist treatment of Pregnant Women in
Norway from 1996 to 2009. Drug Alcohol Depend 127: 200–206.
Willford JA, Richardson GA, Leech SL, Day NL (2004). Verbal and visuospatial
learning and memory function in children with moderate prenatal alcohol
exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28: 497–507.
Williams MT, Blankenmeyer TL, Schaefer TL, Brown CA, Gudelsky GA, Vorhees CV
(2003a). Long-term effects of neonatal methamphetamine exposure in rats on
spatial learning in the Barnes maze and on cliff avoidance , corticosterone
release, and neurotoxicity in adulthood. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 147: 163–175.
Williams MT, Moran MS, Vorhees CV (2003b). Refining the critical period for
methamphetamine-induced spatial deficits in the Morris water maze. Psycho-
pharmacology (Berl) 168: 329–338.
Williams MT, Morford LL, Wood SL, Rock SL, McCrea AE, Fukumura M et al
(2003c). Developmental 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) impairs
sequential and spatial but not cued learning independent of growth, litter effects
or injection stress. Brain Res 968: 89–101.
Williams MT, Morford LL, Wood SL, Wallace TL, Fukumura M, Broening HW et al
(2003d). Developmental D-methamphetamine treatment selectively induces
spatial navigation impairments in reference memory in the Morris water maze
while sparing working memory. Synapse 48: 138–148.
Williams MT, Vorhees CV, Boon F, Saber AJ, Cain DP (2002). Methamphetamine
exposure from postnatal day 11 to 20 causes impairments in both behavioral
strategies and spatial learning in adult rats. Brain Res 958: 312–321. Data
suggest that sensorimotor deficits do not account for the long-term spatial
learning and memory deficits in animals exposed neonatally to metham-
phetamine.
Wilson GS, Desmond MM, Wait RB (1981). Follow-up of methadone-treated and
untreated narcotic-dependent women and their infants: health, developmental,
and social implications. J Pediatr 98: 716–722.
Winslow JT, Insel TR (1990). Serotonergic modulation of rat pup ultrasonic vocal
development: studies with 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine. J Pharmacol
Exp Ther 254: 212–220.
Witter FR, Niebyl JR (1990). Marijuana use in pregnancy and pregnancy outcome.
Am J Perinatol 7: 36–38.
Wittert G, Hope P, Pyle D (1996). Tissue distribution of opioid receptor gene
expression in the rat. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 218: 877–881.
Wong CS, Lee YJ, Chiang YC, Fan LW, Ho IK, Tien LT (2014). Effect of prenatal
methadone on reinstated behavioral sensitization induced by methamphetamine
in adolescent rats. Behav Brain Res 258: 160–165.
Xu D, Wu Y, Liu F, Liu YS, Shen L, Lei YY et al (2012a). A hypothalamic-pituitary-
adrenal axis-associated neuroendocrine metabolic programmed alteration in
offspring rats of IUGR induced by prenatal caffeine ingestion. Toxicol Appl
Pharmacol 264: 395–403.
Xu D, Zhang B, Liang G, Ping J, Kou H, Li X et al (2012b). Caffeine-induced
activated glucocorticoid metabolism in the hippocampus causes hypothalamic-
pituitary-adrenal axis inhibition in fetal rats. PLoS One 7: e44497.
Yanai J, Pick CG, Rogel-Fuchs Y, Zahalka EA (1992). Alterations in hippocampal
cholinergic receptors and hippocampal behaviors after early exposure to
nicotine. Brain Res Bull 29: 363–368.
Yang SN, Liu CA, Chung MY, Huang HC, Yeh GC, Wong CS et al (2006). Alterations
of postsynaptic density proteins in the hippocampus of rat offspring from the
morphine-addicted mother: beneficial effect of dextromethorphan. Hippocam-
pus 16: 521–530.
Yaniv SP, Naor Z, Yanai J (2004). Prenatal heroin exposure alters cholinergic
receptor stimulated activation of the PKCbetaII and PKCgamma isoforms. Brain
Res Bull 63: 339–349.
Zabaneh R, Smith LM, LaGasse LL, Derauf C, Newman E, Shah R et al (2012). The
effects of prenatal methamphetamine exposure on childhood growth patterns
from birth to 3 years of age. Am J Perinatol 29: 203–210.
Zagon IS, Gibo DM, McLaughlin PJ (1992). Ontogeny of zeta (zeta), the opioid
growth factor receptor, in the rat brain. Brain Res 596: 149–156.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1977a). Effect of chronic maternal methadone exposure
on perinatal development. Biol Neonate 31: 271–282.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1977b). Effects of chronic morphine administration on
pregnant rats and their offspring. Pharmacology 15: 302–310.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1977c). Morphine and brain growth retardation in the rat.
Pharmacology 15: 276–282.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1978). Perinatal methadone exposure and its influence
on the behavioral ontogeny of rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 9: 665–672.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1979). Motor activity and learning ability in rats perinatally
exposed to methadone. NIDA Res Monogr 27: 121–127.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1982). Neuronal cell deficits following exposure to
methadone in rats. Experientia 38: 1214–1216.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1983). Somatic and neurobiological alterations in the
progeny of female rats treated with methadone prior to mating. NIDA Res
Monogr 43: 184–189.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1984). Naltrexone modulates body and brain develop-
ment in rats: a role for endogenous opioid systems in growth. Life Sci 35: 2057–
2064.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1987). Endogenous opioid systems regulate cell
proliferation in the developing rat brain. Brain Res 412: 68–72. Provided some
of the initial evidence that endogenous opiates function to inhibit neuronal
proliferation.
Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ, Thompson CI (1979). Learning ability in adult female rats
perinatally exposed to methadone. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 10: 889–894.
Zagon IS, Tobias SW, Hytrek SD, McLaughlin PJ (1998). Opioid receptor blockade
throughout prenatal life confers long-term insensitivity to morphine and alters mu
opioid receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 59: 201–207.
Zagon IS, Wu Y, McLaughlin PJ (1999). Opioid growth factor and organ
development in rat and human embryos. Brain Res 839: 313–322.
Zhen X, Torres C, Wang HY, Friedman E (2001). Prenatal exposure to cocaine
disrupts D1A dopamine receptor function via selective inhibition of protein
phosphatase 1 pathway in rabbit frontal cortex. J Neurosci 21: 9160–9167.
Zhou FC, Sari Y, Powrozek T, Goodlett CR, Li TK (2003). Moderate alcohol
exposure compromises neural tube midline development in prenatal brain. Brain
Res Dev Brain Res 144: 43–55.
Zhou FC, Sari Y, Powrozek TA (2005). Fetal alcohol exposure reduces serotonin
innervation and compromises development of the forebrain along the seroto-
nergic pathway. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 29: 141–149.
Zhu J, Lee KP, Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Bhide PG (2014). Transgenerational
transmission of hyperactivity in a mouse model of ADHD. J Neurosci 34: 2768–
2773. The authors stunningly demonstrated transgenerational transmission
of prenatal nicotine-induced hyperactivity in mice via the maternal but not
the paternal line of descent, suggesting that transgenerational transmis-
sion may propagate environmentally induced ADHD phenotypes.
Zhu J, Zhang X, Xu Y, Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Bhide PG (2012). Prenatal nicotine
exposure mouse model showing hyperactivity, reduced cingulate cortex volume,
reduced dopamine turnover, and responsiveness to oral methylphenidate
treatment. J Neurosci 32: 9410–9418.
Zmitrovich AC, Brake SC, Liu PY, Hamowy AS, Hutchings DE (1994). Prenatal
administration of methadone in the rat: acoustic startle amplitude and the rest-
activity cycle at 30 days of age. Neurotoxicol Teratol 16: 251–255.
Prenatal effects of drugs of abuse
EJ Ross et al
...............................................................................................................................................................
87
REVIEW
..............................................................................................................................................
Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS
... Prenatal cocaine exposure disrupts the monoaminergic neurotransmitter system in the prefrontal cortex, affecting emotional and behavioral regulation and stress response (McCarthy et al., 2014;Thompson et al., 2009), although its outcomes are highly variable depending on the specific dosage, chronicity, gestational timing of the exposure, and developmental domain (Vorhees, 1989). Children prenatally exposed to cocaine also tend to be exposed in utero to other substances such as alcohol, which could further undermine fetal brain development (for the reviews, see Ross et al., 2015;Eiden et al., 2023). While prenatal cocaine exposure alone does not appear to lower global intelligence (Ross et al., 2015), longitudinal studies of children with prenatal cocaine exposure, including this cohort, have identified executive function (Karpova et al., 2021;Minnes et al., 2016), language (Lewis et al., 2013;Powers et al., 2023), and behavioral problems (Bada et al., 2011;Min, Minnes, Park, et al., 2018). ...
... Children prenatally exposed to cocaine also tend to be exposed in utero to other substances such as alcohol, which could further undermine fetal brain development (for the reviews, see Ross et al., 2015;Eiden et al., 2023). While prenatal cocaine exposure alone does not appear to lower global intelligence (Ross et al., 2015), longitudinal studies of children with prenatal cocaine exposure, including this cohort, have identified executive function (Karpova et al., 2021;Minnes et al., 2016), language (Lewis et al., 2013;Powers et al., 2023), and behavioral problems (Bada et al., 2011;Min, Minnes, Park, et al., 2018). Greater prenatal alcohol exposure (maternal average absolute alcohol use per day) was found to have a small yet lasting impact on learning and memory, executive function, reading, and math achievement in a meta-analysis of six longitudinal cohorts with propensity score matching for co-occurring risks (Jacobson et al., 2021); studies using fatty acid ethyl esters (FAEE), the nonoxidative metabolites of ethanol analyzed in meconium, indicated that higher concentrations of FAEE were related to poorer verbal comprehension, working memory, and Full-Scale IQ and to more caregiver-reported aggressive and delinquent behaviors (Singer et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Utilizing a birth cohort of primarily Black, low‐income urban adolescents ( N = 371), the present study investigated how internal assets might change over time with sex/gender as a moderator. Internal assets (commitment to learning, positive values, social competencies, positive identity) were assessed at ages 12, 15, and 17 via the Developmental Assets Profile. All internal asset domains decreased from 12 to 15 years in both boys and girls, except positive identity in boys. For girls, these decreases, except in social competencies, remained unchanged from 15 to 17 years, whereas boys improved in all domains during the same period except commitment to learning. Across all three assessments, girls reported greater commitment to learning than boys. Boys reported higher positive identity than girls at age 17. Greater parental attachment was associated with higher scores in all domains of internal assets.
... These systems have advanced our understanding of diseases such as schizophrenia, Rett syndrome, Fragile X syndrome, and fetal alcohol syndrome [49,51]. Despite calls for multifaceted approaches in POE research [32,52,53], human stem cell models are largely absent in this field [54,55]. This may be due, in part, to earlier, popular human neural progenitor cell lines losing key protein expression, like opioid receptors mu (OPRM1), kappa (OPRK1), and delta (OPRD1) and display of aberrant growth likely due to immortalization [56,57]. ...
Article
Full-text available
A growing body of clinical literature has described neurodevelopmental delays in infants with chronic prenatal opioid exposure and withdrawal. Despite this, the mechanism of how opioids impact the developing brain remains unknown. Here, we developed an in vitro model of prenatal morphine exposure and withdrawal using healthy human induced pluripotent stem cell (iPSC)-derived midbrain neural progenitors in monolayer. To optimize our model, we identified that a longer neural induction and regional patterning period increases expression of canonical opioid receptors mu and kappa in midbrain neural progenitors compared to a shorter protocol (OPRM1, two-tailed t-test, p = 0.004; OPRK1, p = 0.0003). Next, we showed that the midbrain neural progenitors derived from a longer iPSC neural induction also have scant toll-like receptor 4 (TLR4) expression, a key player in neonatal opioid withdrawal syndrome pathophysiology. During morphine withdrawal, differentiating neural progenitors experience cyclic adenosine monophosphate overshoot compared to cell exposed to vehicle (p = 0.0496) and morphine exposure conditions (p, = 0.0136, 1-way ANOVA). Finally, we showed that morphine exposure and withdrawal alters proportions of differentiated progenitor cell fates (2-way ANOVA, F = 16.05, p < 0.0001). Chronic morphine exposure increased proportions of nestin positive progenitors (p = 0.0094), and decreased proportions of neuronal nuclear antigen positive neurons (NEUN) (p = 0.0047) compared to those exposed to vehicle. Morphine withdrawal decreased proportions of glial fibrillary acidic protein positive cells of astrocytic lineage (p = 0.044), and increased proportions of NEUN-positive neurons (p < 0.0001) compared to those exposed to morphine only. Applications of this paradigm include mechanistic studies underscoring neural progenitor cell fate commitments in early neurodevelopment during morphine exposure and withdrawal.
... First, we might have underestimated the associations between in-utero exposure to antibiotics and childhood hearing loss, particularly for non-aminoglycoside antibiotics, due to the limited number of studies reporting detailed timing and dosage of exposure and the inconsistent findings among these studies. Second, the absence of information on the severity of maternal infections complicates efforts to disentangle the potential interplay between infection-related factors and ototoxicity [45,55]. Third, significant heterogeneity across included studies limited the generalizability of our findings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Antibiotic exposure during pregnancy is common, accounting for over 80% of all medications prescribed. Antibiotics in pregnancy are linked to increased childhood disease risk, through direct toxicity or potentially microbiome dysbiosis. This systematic review investigated the relationship between in-utero exposure to antibiotics and childhood hearing loss. Methods: We searched Ovid Medline, Embase, and PubMed for studies examining antibiotic exposure during pregnancy and its associations with hearing loss in offspring. Studies with children whose mothers had data on antibiotic exposure during pregnancy were selected. The meta-analysis calculated (1) pooled prevalence of childhood hearing loss and (2) pooled odds ratios (ORs) for associations between in-utero exposure to antibiotics and childhood hearing loss. Results: Of 1244 studies identified, 18 met the inclusion criteria. Among 161,053 children exposed in-utero to antibiotics, 4368 developed hearing loss. The pooled prevalence of childhood hearing loss was 0.9% (95% CI 0.0–2.8%, I² = 99.6%). In-utero exposure to antibiotics was associated with an increased risk of childhood hearing loss (pooled OR 1.2, 95% CI 1.1 to 1.3, I² = 15.4%). Aminoglycoside exposure during pregnancy was associated with a higher risk of hearing loss (pooled OR 1.2, 95% CI 1.1 to 1.3, I² = 38.4%), while exposure to other antibiotic classes showed no association. Conclusions: The prevalence of childhood hearing loss among those exposed to antibiotics during pregnancy is high. Although the overall risk appears modest, aminoglycosides are linked to a significantly higher risk, suggesting maternal aminoglycoside exposure may indicate a risk for child hearing loss. Further research is needed to clarify causal pathways and long-term effects of in-utero exposure to antibiotics.
... As per the United States National Poison Data Center, ~40% of substance exposure cases correspond to the pediatric population [8]. Additionally, maternal substance use causes debilitating effects on newborns, including cognitive impairment, neonatal abstinence syndrome, respiratory insufficiency and behavioral problems [9]. In 2017, the American College of Obstetrics and Gynecology (ACOG) and the American Society of Addiction Medicine jointly recommended all pregnant women be universally screened for substance use at the first prenatal visit [10]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Clinical testing for drugs of abuse typically involves initial screening followed by confirmatory testing. Due to limited evidence-based guidelines, the healthcare provider makes the decision to confirm abnormal screens based on the clinical context. This two-step approach proved to be inadequate in scenarios like maternal substance abuse and subsequent fetal/ newborn exposure. The goal of this study is to assess and improve the confirmatory testing rate of abnormal screens among pregnant patients at our women’s center. Methods A retrospective chart review was conducted to assess the confirmation rates among positively screened pregnant patients, and a lab stewardship initiative was implemented to remind ordering physicians about the importance of confirmatory drug tests. Abnormal screens were classified as expected positives based on the medication-related interference, social history and self-reported substance use from the provider notes. Results Only 28% of pregnant patients with unexpected positive drug screens underwent confirmatory testing during the pre- intervention period, which rose significantly to 67% during the post-intervention period. Furthermore, outcome analysis revealed that 50% of patients with concordant confirmatory test results were referred to social work and psychiatry in the post-intervention period. Conclusions This study highlights the value of laboratory stewardship in optimizing drug testing practices for pregnant patients.
Article
Full-text available
The maternal microbiome is increasingly being recognized as a key determinant in various neonatal health outcomes, including offspring immunity, metabolism, brain function, and behavior. While the oral, vaginal, skin, and gut microbiota are significant contributors to the offspring’s postnatal gut microbial seeding, the composition and diversity of the maternal gut microbiome during pregnancy seems to be critical in shaping neonatal health outcomes, even prior to birth. Growing evidence suggests that the balance among the microbial groups in the gut and their interactions with the host are crucial for health. Dysbiotic communities in pregnancy and early in life may lead to disease processes in offspring, though the specific processes by which maternal gut microbes affect offspring gut microbial development are unknown. Here, we summarize research examining gut microbial shifts during pregnancy, and their effects on the diversity and composition of the infant microbiome and on early health outcomes. We also discuss current theories for how the maternal gastrointestinal (GI) tract influences neonatal seeding, and how probiotics during the perinatal period may affect offspring health outcomes.
Chapter
Opioid use disorder (OUD) continues to be a global problem, with particularly high opioid usage rates in the United States. One major contributor to this crisis has been the high rate of opioid prescriptions, which has increased access to opioids and contributed to many vulnerable individuals becoming dependent or addicted. Many of these affected people are women of reproductive age, which in turn results in many women using or abusing opioids during pregnancy and thus many infants being exposed to illicit opioids. OUD is typically treated with either methadone or buprenorphine (BUP), two effective opioid-based medications for OUD (MOUD). BUP has recently gained more attention and replaced methadone as the “gold standard” of treatment since its unique pharmacodynamic properties seem to result in better compliance, less withdrawal symptoms, and improved infant outcomes compared to methadone. However, the effects of BUP exposure on the long-term outcome of the offspring and mother-infant dyad are not fully understood. This chapter will review the current state of the literature regarding effects of gestational opioid exposure on offspring outcomes, focusing on morphine as a commonly used illicit substance and BUP as a widely used MOUD. Collectively, the literature reviewed here highlights the need for future research into the impact of gestational opioid use on mothers, their care behavior, and their subsequent mother-infant bonds.
Article
Full-text available
This case report describes a 41-year-old woman with no significant medical history and a normal body mass index (BMI), who presented with ureterohydronephrosis due to a 5.5mm x 9mm calculus composed primarily of calcite (CaCO3) at the ureterovesical junction. The kidney stone, associated with cystitis and perirenal fat infiltration, was spontaneously expelled and subsequently analyzed. Optical microscopy revealed a grey homogeneous stone with a rough surface and white crystals upon examination. Fouriertransforminfrared spectroscopy (FTIR-ATR) confirmed the stone’s composition as pure calcite, displaying characteristic absorption bands indicative of its crystalline structure. The patient reported long-term use of multiple vitamins and plantbased supplements, possibly contributing to stone formation. The discussion includes insights on calcite urolithiasis, highlighting factors such as alkaline urine pH and calcium metabolism that can influence stone formation, underscoring the complexity of managing kidney stone risk in supplement users.
Article
Full-text available
This study assessed the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure on cognitive functioning, using an intravenous (IV) rodent model that closely mimics the pharmacokinetics seen in humans after smoking or IV injection and that avoids maternal stress and undernutrition. Cocaine-exposed males were significantly impaired on a 3-choice, but not 2-choice, olfactory serial reversal learning task. Both male and female cocaine-exposed rats were significantly impaired on extradimensional shift tasks that required shifting from olfactory to spatial cues; however, they showed no impairment when required to shift from spatial to olfactory cues. In-depth analyses of discrete learning phases implicated deficient selective attention as the basis of impairment in both tasks. These data provide clear evidence that prenatal cocaine exposure produces long-lasting cognitive dysfunction, but they also underscore the specificity of the impairment.
Article
Full-text available
Use of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA; ecstasy) has increased dramatically in recent years, yet little is known about its effects on the developing brain. Neonatal rats were administered MDMA on days 1–10 or 11–20 (analogous to early and late human third trimester brain development). MDMA exposure had no effect on survival but did affect body weight gain during treatment. After treatment, body weight largely recovered to 90–95% of controls. MDMA exposure on days 11–20 resulted in dose-related impairments of sequential learning and spatial learning and memory, whereas neonatal rats exposed on days 1–10 showed almost no effects. At neither stage of exposure did MDMA-treated offspring show effects on swimming ability or cued learning. Brain region-specific dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine changes were small and were not correlated to learning changes. These findings suggest that MDMA may pose a previously unrecognized risk to the developing brain by inducing long-term deleterious effects on learning and memory.
Article
Full-text available
The effects of neonatal d-methamphetamine (MA) treatment on cued and spatial learning and memory were investigated. MA was administered to neonatal rats on postnatal days 11-20. All groups received four subcutaneous injections per day. Group MA40-4 received 40 mg. kg(-1). d(-1) of MA in four divided doses (10 mg/kg per injection). Group MA40-2 received 40 mg. kg(-1). d(-1) of MA in two divided (20 mg/kg/injection) and saline for the other two injections per day. Controls received saline for four injections per day. As adults, both MA groups showed no differences in swimming ability in a straight swimming channel. The MA40-4 group showed no differences in cued learning, but was impaired in hidden platform learning in the Morris water maze on acquisition. They also showed reduced memory performance on probe trials. Similar trends were seen on reversal learning and reversal probe trials. Reduced platform-size learning trials caused spatial learning impairments to re-emerge in the MA40-4 group. The MA40-2 group showed no differences in straight channel swimming, but was slower at finding the visible platform during cued learning. They were also impaired during acquisition and memory trials in the Morris hidden platform maze. They showed a similar trend on reversal learning and memory trials, but were not different during reduced platform-size learning trials. When the MA40-2 group's performance on hidden platform learning and memory trials was adjusted for cued trial performance, the spatial learning deficits remained. Deficits of spatial learning and memory are a selective effect of neonatal methamphetamine treatment irrespective of other learning and performance variables.
Article
Full-text available
We critique published incidences for fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) and present new estimates of the incidence of FAS and the prevalence of alcohol-related neurodevelopmental disorder (ARND). We first review criteria necessary for valid estimation of FAS incidence. Estimates for three population-based studies that best meet these criteria are reported with adjustment for underascertainment of highly exposed cases. As a result, in 1975 in Seattle, the incidence of FAS can be estimated as at least 2.8/1000 live births, and for 1979–81 in Cleveland, ∼4.6/1,000. In Roubaix, France (for data covering periods from 1977–1990), the rate is between 1.3 and 4.8/1,000, depending on the severity of effects used as diagnostic criteria. Utilizing the longitudinal neurobehavioral database of the Seattle study, we propose an operationalization of the Institute of Medicine's recent definition of ARND and estimate its prevalence in Seattle for the period 1975–1981. The combined rate of FAS and ARND is thus estimated to be at least 9.1/1,000. This conservative rate—nearly one in every 100 live births—confirms the perception of many health professionals that fetal alcohol exposure is a serious problem.Teratology 56:317–326, 1997. © 1997 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Article
Background: The prevalence of opiate use among pregnant women can range from 1% to 2% to as high as 21%. Just in the United States alone, among pregnant women with hospital delivery, a fourfold increase in opioid use is reported from 1999 to 2014 (Haight 2018). Heroin crosses the placenta, and pregnant, opiate-dependent women experience a six-fold increase in maternal obstetric complications such as low birth weight, toxaemia, third trimester bleeding, malpresentation, puerperal morbidity, fetal distress and meconium aspiration. Neonatal complications include narcotic withdrawal, postnatal growth deficiency, microcephaly, neuro-behavioural problems, increased neonatal mortality and a 74-fold increase in sudden infant death syndrome. This is an updated version of the original Cochrane Review first published in 2008 and last updated in 2013. Objectives: To assess the effectiveness of any maintenance treatment alone or in combination with a psychosocial intervention compared to no intervention, other pharmacological intervention or psychosocial interventions alone for child health status, neonatal mortality, retaining pregnant women in treatment, and reducing the use of substances. Search methods: We updated our searches of the following databases to February 2020: the Cochrane Drugs and Alcohol Group Specialised Register, CENTRAL, MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO, CINAHL, and Web of Science. We also searched two trials registers and checked the reference lists of included studies for further references to relevant randomised controlled trials (RCTs). Selection criteria: Randomised controlled trials which assessed the efficacy of any pharmacological maintenance treatment for opiate-dependent pregnant women. Data collection and analysis: We used the standard methodological procedures expected by Cochrane. Main results: We found four trials with 271 pregnant women. Three compared methadone with buprenorphine and one methadone with oral slow-release morphine. Three out of four studies had adequate allocation concealment and were double-blind. The major flaw in the included studies was attrition bias: three out of four had a high dropout rate (30% to 40%), and this was unbalanced between groups. Methadone versus buprenorphine: There was probably no evidence of a difference in the dropout rate from treatment (risk ratio (RR) 0.66, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.37 to 1.20, three studies, 223 participants, moderate-quality evidence). There may be no evidence of a difference in the use of primary substances between methadone and buprenorphine (RR 1.81, 95% CI 0.70 to 4.68, two studies, 151 participants, low-quality evidence). Birth weight may be higher in the buprenorphine group in the two trials that reported data MD;-530.00 g, 95%CI -662.78 to -397.22 (one study, 19 particpants) and MD: -215.00 g, 95%CI -238.93 to -191.07 (one study, 131 participants) although the results could not be pooled due to very high heterogeneity (very low-quality of evidence). The third study reported that there was no evidence of a difference. We found there may be no evidence of a difference in the APGAR score (MD: 0.00, 95% CI -0.03 to 0.03, two studies,163 participants, low-quality evidence). Many measures were used in the studies to assess neonatal abstinence syndrome. The number of newborns treated for neonatal abstinence syndrome, which is the most critical outcome, may not differ between groups (RR 1.19, 95% CI 0.87 to1.63, three studies, 166 participants, low-quality evidence). Only one study which compared methadone with buprenorphine reported side effects. We found there may be no evidence of a difference in the number of mothers with serious adverse events (AEs) (RR 1.69, 95% CI 0.75 to 3.83, 175 participants, low-quality evidence) and we found there may be no difference in the numbers of newborns with serious AEs (RR 4.77, 95% CI 0.59, 38.49,131 participants, low-quality evidence). Methadone versus slow-release morphine: There were no dropouts in either treatment group. Oral slow-release morphine may be superior to methadone for abstinence from heroin use during pregnancy (RR 2.40, 95% CI 1.00 to 5.77, one study, 48 participants, low-quality evidence). In the comparison between methadone and slow-release morphine, no side effects were reported for the mother. In contrast, one child in the methadone group had central apnoea, and one child in the morphine group had obstructive apnoea (low-quality evidence). Authors' conclusions: Methadone and buprenorphine may be similar in efficacy and safety for the treatment of opioid-dependent pregnant women and their babies. There is not enough evidence to make conclusions for the comparison between methadone and slow-release morphine. Overall, the body of evidence is too small to make firm conclusions about the equivalence of the treatments compared. There is still a need for randomised controlled trials of adequate sample size comparing different maintenance treatments.
Article
Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders constitute a major public health problem. This article presents an overview of important issues that surround these disorders and emphasizes the structural and neurobehavioral consequences associated with prenatal exposure to alcohol. Diagnostic criteria are discussed, and possible moderating factors for the range of outcomes are mentioned. In addition, the prevalence of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders is described, and estimates of the financial impact of these disorders are given. Heavy prenatal alcohol exposure can severely affect the physical and neurobehavioral development of a child. Autopsy and brain imaging studies indicate reductions and abnormalities in overall brain size and shape, specifically in structures such as the cerebellum, basal ganglia, and corpus callosum. A wide range of neuropsychological deficits have been found in children prenatally exposed to alcohol, including deficits in visuospatial functioning, verbal and nonverbal learning, attention, and executive functioning. These children also exhibit a variety of behavioral problems that can further affect their daily functioning. Children exposed to alcohol prenatally, with and without the physical features of fetal alcohol syndrome, display qualitatively similar deficits. Determining the behavioral phenotypes that result from heavy prenatal alcohol exposure is critical, because the identification of these children is crucial for early interventions. In addition, knowing which brain areas are involved might enable the development of better intervention strategies. However, intervention needs to go beyond the affected individual to prevent future cases. As evidenced by the staggering financial impact these disorders have on society, prevention efforts need to be aimed at high-risk groups, and this issue needs to be made a high priority in terms of public health.
Chapter
Methadone maintenance treatment (MMT) has successfully lessened the use of other opiates for opioid-dependent pregnant women. MMT produces short-term developmental outcomes, such as neonatal abstinence syndrome and early cognitive and motor delays, but potential long-term effects of prenatal methadone exposure on infant development are less understood, especially in the crosstalk with other addictive agents. Topographic overlaps between opioid and dopamine neurons are found in the central nervous system, suggesting that interactions exist between these two systems. Evidence has shown that prenatal methadone treatment leads to a long-lasting change in the brain’s rewarding system, which can enhance adolescent susceptibility to the development of addictive-like behavior in adolescent rats. The prenatal methadone-induced changes in offspring brain development, which may contribute to addictive-like behavior, include changes in dopamine metabolites, neuronal terminal maturation, early stages of myelination, maturation of connective tracts, and glia activation.