ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Research conducted primarily during the 1970s and 1980s supported the assertion that carefully constructed text illustrations generally enhance learners' performance on a variety of text-dependent cognitive outcomes. Research conducted throughout the 1990s still strongly supports that assertion. The more recent research has extended pictures-in-text conclusions to alternative media and technological formats and has begun to explore more systematically the “whys,” “whens,” and “for whoms” of picture facilitation, in addition to the “whethers” and “how muchs.” Consideration is given here to both more and less conventional types of textbook illustration, with several “tenets for teachers” provided in relation to each type.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Illustrations
Pictorial Illustrations Still Improve Students' Learning from Text
Russell N. Carney
Southwest Missouri State University
Joel R. Levin
University of Arizona
Running Head: Illustrations Improve Text Learning
Illustrations 2
Abstract
Research conducted primarily during the 1970s and 1980s supported the assertion that
carefully constructed text illustrations generally enhance learners' performance on a variety of
text-dependent cognitive outcomes. Research conducted throughout the 1990s still strongly
supports that assertion. The more recent research has extended pictures-in-text conclusions to
alternative media and technological formats and has begun to explore more systematically the
"whys", "whens", and "for whoms" of picture facilitation, in addition to the "whethers" and "how
muchs." Consideration is given here to both more and less conventional types of textbook
illustration, with several "tenets for teachers" provided in relation to each type.
Illustrations 3
Pictorial Illustrations Still Improve Students' Learning from Text
In 1994, elaborate cave paintings were discovered in Ardennes, France, paintings that are
thought to be 30,000 years old. Far from being primitive, these animal paintings were skillfully
executed (Cutting & Massironi, 1998). As this find illustrates, from very early on people have
created pictures. Perhaps these early paintings served as "adjunct aids" to storytellers, playing
a role in humankind's development. Similarly, illustrations have been a part of our more recent
development via the picture storybooks of our childhoods.
Although the empirical educational research evidence strongly indicates that storybook
pictures may interfere with "learning to read" (i.e., the initial stages of extracting words and
meaning from text – see, for example, Levie, 1987; Levin, 1983; and Samuels, 1970), Fang
(1996, p. 136) suggests that "the contributions of pictures to the overall development of
children's literate behavior seems to be overwhelmingly greater than its potential dangers." In
that regard, Fang (1996) lists six roles that pictures play in storybooks. Pictures may serve to
help: (a) establish the setting; (b) define/develop the characters; (c) extend/develop the plot;
(d) provide a different viewpoint; (e) contribute to the text's coherence; and (f) reinforce the text.
Fang goes on to list several benefits that pictures provide, including such things as motivating
the reader, promoting creativity, serving as mental scaffolds, fostering aesthetic appreciation,
and promoting children's language and literacy. Also supportive of pictures in children's
storybooks, Goodman, Maras, and Birdseye (1994) decry the way in which illustrations have
been altered in order to force them into a basal format. Describing storybooks as both "an art
form and a genre of literature" (p. 20), they argue against such alteration and advocate the use
of storybooks in their original format as part of the reading curriculum.
Picture storybooks are sometimes called "twice-told tales" because both mediums, verbal
and pictorial, may tell the story. Such pictures are representational in nature, illustrating what is
Illustrations 4
described in the text. Further, the pictures in storybooks may go beyond this role by adding
additional details (e.g., Stewig, 1992). As Patricia Gauch, an author of children's books, has
observed: "Art, when it's really good, doesn't imitate or mirror the text. Rather, it adds a new
dimension that goes way beyond the words" (Raymond, 1995, p. 64). Using Maurice Sendak's
Where the Wild Things Are as an example, Sipe (1998) suggests that the child constructs
meaning through the interplay of text and image, which vary somewhat in content.
Although pictures in storybooks may go beyond text content, our focus in this review is on
the contributions that pictures make in complementing the text – serving as adjunct aids for
"reading to learn" (i.e., the processing of – which includes perceiving, understanding, and
remembering – text information). More than ever in our society, written prose is accompanied
by illustrations (e.g., David, 1998). In our schools, and in contrast to the densely worded
textbooks of the past, modern undergraduate texts are richly appointed with pictorial
illustrations, diagrams, photos, and the like. Even 15 years ago, this trend was apparent. Smith
and Elifson (1986) compared history textbooks of the 1960s with those of the 1980s and found a
tremendous increase in the number of pictures. On university campuses, many instructors
make use of software programs such as Powerpoint to include relevant pictures (and video) in
their classroom presentations. Computer software and internet sites routinely provide pictures
and illustrations as adjuncts to text content.
Given the ever-increasing use of pictures in connection with text in these and other
contexts, it is timely to review recent pictorial research. In that regard, we will focus here on
empirical research studies that have appeared in the professional literature primarily from the
1990s through the present. As a preview of what is to come, it is clear from that literature that
pictures (at least, well-selected or well-constructed pictures) reliably improve the reading-to-
learn process – just as had been concluded on the basis of the pictures-in-text research
Illustrations 5
literature through the 1980s (e.g., Levie, 1987; Levie & Lentz, 1982; Levin, Anglin, & Carney,
1987; Levin & Mayer, 1993; Mandl & Levin, 1989; Schallert, 1980).
Functions of Pictures in Prose
As was just noted, reviews of the effects of pictures on students' text processing
conducted in the 1980s examined reading-to-learn studies and found advantages for pictures as
text adjuncts. In particular, we (Levin et al., 1987) structured our meta-analytic review in terms
of Levin's (1981) five functions that pictures serve in text processing – four conventional
functions (decorational, representational, organizational, interpretational) and one more
unconventional one (transformational). Briefly, decorational pictures simply adorn the page,
bearing little or no relationship to the text content. In contrast, representational pictures mirror
part or all of the text content and are by far the most commonly used type of illustration.
Organizational pictures provide a useful structural framework for the text content.
Interpretational pictures help to clarify difficult text (e.g., representing blood pressure in terms of
a pump system). Finally, transformational pictures include systematic mnemonic (memory-
enhancing) components that are designed to improve a reader's recall of text information. Our
meta-analysis of the available empirical studies yielded the effect sizes that are presented in
Figure 1. Whereas purely decorational pictures exhibited virtually no beneficial text-learning
effects, the remaining effect sizes ranged from moderate benefits (for representational pictures)
to quite substantial benefits (for transformational pictures).1
We concluded our review with a light-hearted list that was dubbed the "ten
commandments of picture facilitation" (Levin et al., 1987, pp. 73-77). The commandments were
as follows:
1. Pictures shalt be judiciously applied to text, to remember it wholly. This is a basic caveat
that the remaining commandments pertain to picture principles for improving students'
learning of and memory for text content, and not necessarily to other text-related
Illustrations 6
outcomes (e.g., print acquisition, engagement and enjoyment, processing speed,
application and transfer).
2. Pictures shalt honor the text. That is, the picture needs to correspond to the text. As we
have seen, purely decorational pictures do not improve students' learning of text content.
3. Pictures shalt not bear false fitness to the text. Conflicting pictures are most likely not
helpful, and may even hinder learning.
4. Pictures shalt not be used in the presence of "heavenly" bodies of prose. If the text is
highly memorable to begin with, there is no need to add pictures.
5. Pictures shalt not be used with text cravin' for images. That is, if the text content directly
elicits useful mental images in students (as is often the case with engaging concrete
narrative texts), providing pictures or instructing students to generate their own text-
related imagery is often superfluous.
6. Pictures shalt not be prepared in vain. A reader's possession of basic reading skills is
required for picture benefits to emerge. Pictures are intended as text supplements rather
than as text substitutes.
7. Pictures shalt be faithfully created from generation to generation. As an analog to
Commandment 6, this means that for a learner to generate beneficial internal pictorial
representations of the text (i.e., visual images), the learner must similarly possess
adequate basic reading skills.
8. Pictures shalt not be adulterated. This emphasizes the need to design good-quality
pictures.
9. Pictures shalt be appreciated for the art they art. Veridical "pictorial" representations
should be distinguished from "figural" representations, such as graphs or flow charts.
Illustrations 7
10. Pictures shalt be made to perform their appropriate functions. The amount and type of
prose-learning facilitation are related of the type of picture selected.
As an addendum to pictures-in-text functional analyses and commandments, Levin
and Mayer (1993) proposed seven "C" principles for explaining the "whys" of picture
facilitation. In particular, they suggest that pictures improve students' learning from text
because they make the text more: concentrated (focused, with respect to directing a
reader's attention), compact/concise ("a picture is worth a thousand words"), concrete (the
representation function), coherent (the organization function), comprehensible (the
interpretation function), correspondent (relating unfamiliar text to a reader's prior
knowledge), and codable (the mnemonic transformation function). Further, Levin and
Mayer adapt Bransford's (1979) version of Jenkins' (1979) tetrahedral model to argue that
four variables must be taken into account when considering the "whys", "whens," and "for
whoms" of picture facilitation: desired performance outcomes (e.g., comprehension,
memory, transfer), the nature of the illustrations (e.g., that they must be related to the text
content), the nature of the text (e.g., the more difficult the text is to understand, the more
likely that pictures will help), and learner characteristics (e.g., learners lacking domain-
relevant background knowledge are likely to benefit more from illustrations) – see also
Mayer (1992) and Gyselinck & Tardieu (1999).
In a related review, Peeck (1993) lists a number of reasons why pictures should facilitate
learning, including: increasing motivation, focusing attention, depth of processing, clarification of
text content, dual-coding theory, distinctive encoding, decreasing interference/decay, processing
support for the type of information typically extracted from a specific type of text (e.g., Waddill &
McDaniel, 1992; Waddill, McDaniel, & Einstein, 1988), and serving as mental models (e.g.,
Glenberg & Langston, 1992; Gyselinck & Tardieu, 1994). Nevertheless, Peeck goes on to doubt
Illustrations 8
that pictures contribute much to text processing in real-life situations. As Weidenmann (1989)
argued, for a variety of reasons "good pictures fail." For example, pictures are often viewed as
"easy" material and may be examined only superficially by learners.
Peeck also describes picture effects in relation to the tetrahedron model. In contrast to the
Levin and Mayer (1993) review, which focused largely on illustration-text characteristics and
correspondence, Peeck's article emphasizes the latter part of Bransford's (1979) model: learner
characteristics and learning activities. Learner characteristics of importance are the age of the
learner, the learner's reading ability, and the learner's "visual literacy" (i.e., one's ability to "read"
pictures). Peeck describes several attempts to teach such literacy, including approaches by
Constable, Cambell, and Brown (1988) and Higgins (1979). He recommends teaching visual
literacy in the context of teaching reading comprehension (e.g., Palincsar & Brown, 1984).
Shifting to learning activities, Peeck asserts that simply asking students to pay more attention to
pictures is not likely to increase the students' processing of them. He cites three studies
(Bernard, 1990; Reinking, Hayes & McEneaney, 1988; and Weidenmann, 1989) suggesting that
the effect of text illustrations is enhanced when explicit instructions or cues are provided (Peeck,
1993, p. 234).
Peeck concludes with a helpful summary table. Here, his recommendation for the optimal
processing of adjunct pictures is to "tell the student to do something with the illustration" and to
require a "controllable" product (p. 235). For example, one might ask students to label features
of illustrations accompanying text – analogous to Dean and Kulhavy's (1981) findings with
respect to labeling maps.
Recent Research
We now briefly review several pictures-in-text studies published within the last decade.
This review is grouped according to the posited Levin et al. (1987) functions of the particular
Illustrations 9
pictures examined: representational, organizational, interpretational, and transformational.
Because of space limitations, our summary is selective (although, we hope, representative),
rather than comprehensive. In addition, we include here only research findings based on
"pictures" as text-provided visual illustrations (on the page, or on the computer screen) and not
findings based on "pictures" as learner-generated visual imagery (in the head) – see Levin et al.
(1987) for both the distinction and a meta-analytic summary of both literatures through the
1980s.2
Representational Pictures
Recall that representational pictures literally depict or overlap (part or all of) the text
content. They are undoubtedly the most common and pervasive type of pictorial text adjunct.
Adler (1993) completed a dissertation in which she looked at how different directions for
processing representational pictures affected students' recall of text information dealing with
water safety and rescue in emergency situations. College undergraduates were randomly
assigned to one of four picture-processing conditions (Adler, 1993, p. 49): no explicit processing
directions, explicit processing (e.g., "How many objects are in the picture? Write your answer
below."), semantic elaboration (e.g., "Specifically, how does the picture relate to the text? Write
your answer below."), and interrogative elaboration (e.g., answering a "why" or "what" question,
such as "What other things are thrown the way this is thrown?"). Adler found a statistical
advantage for the interrogative elaboration treatment.
Using Paivio's (e.g., 1971, 1986) dual-coding theory as a theoretical backdrop, David
(1998) conducted several experiments concerning the usefulness of incorporating a
representational picture into a news item. As David (1998, p. 182) notes: "Because the basic
purpose of the representation function is to make the story concrete, it provides an ideal
framework to test the interaction between news story concreteness and the facilitative role of
Illustrations 10
pictures." In Experiment 1, for example, undergraduates read a randomized set of 30 news
stories dealing with celebrities. The 30 stories were presented via computer, and of these, half
included a representational photograph. Following a 30-min filler task, students were asked to
recall the names of the celebrities. David found a recall advantage for the text/picture condition
relative to the text-only condition (corroborating Levin and Berry's, 1980, conclusions derived
from children listening to news stories with and without pictorial accompaniments). Additional
experiments replicated this basic finding and also supported the notion that concrete news in
general was better remembered than abstract news. Moreover, in one experiment (Exp. 3)
David found that adding a picture to a concrete news story was more beneficial than adding a
picture to an abstract news story.
Many of the representational pictures studies reviewed through the 1980s consisted of
children processing narrative passages. A recently reported study by Rubman and Waters
(2000) extended Lesgold, Levin, Shimron, and Guttmann's (1975) earlier work on young
children's (first graders') cumulative construction of a picture (through the use of illustrated
cutout figures and backgrounds) while listening to a story. In the Rubman and Waters study,
third and sixth graders engaged in a similar picture-construction task while reading a passage
on their own. In addition to replicating the Lesgold et al. (1975) finding of increased story recall
by picture-constructing children (relative to no-picture control children), Rubman and Waters
reported that the children who constructed pictures of the story were better able to detect
inconsistencies that were embedded in the stories (i.e., contradictions with either preceding text
information or common knowledge). The latter result was assumed by the authors to reflect
superior comprehension monitoring (e.g., Markman, 1979) on the part of children who were
provided with the pictorial accompaniments.
Organizational Pictures
Illustrations 11
As was stated earlier, organizational pictures provide a structural framework for the text
content. Betrancourt and Bisseret (1998) wanted to know how best to present the text
information in conjunction with an organizational picture.3 Toward that end, they presented
pictorial information on a computer through pop-up windows. Three displays on the computer
screen were compared: a "split" display, in which the text and picture appeared in separate
parts of the screen; an "integrated" display, in which the text and picture appeared in close
proximity; and a "pop-up" display, in which the pictorial elements appeared when the user
clicked a link on the screen. The authors summarize their findings as follows: "The integration
of text information via pop-up fields increased the learning efficiency compared with a split
format, but this advantage is significant only with regard to the integration of text-picture
information, as opposed to either pictorial or textual information alone. Second, the information
was more quickly retrieved from memory when the material was integrated (spatially or in
pop-up fields) as compared to a split display...These results support the hypothesis that an
integrated display (either spatially or in pop-up windows) improves subjects' performance in
memorizing a labeled schema" (Betrancourt & Bisseret, 1998, p. 268). The authors go on to list
three "pragmatic" reasons why the pop-up approach might be preferred: (a) it saves screen
space and hence improves legibility; (b) it allows either text or picture to appear in the
foreground – there is a tendency for learners to pay more attention to the text than the picture
(e.g., Levie & Lentz, 1982; Peeck, 1993); and (c) it is more interactive for the learner, and hence
improves motivation and performance.
Betrancourt and Bisseret (1998, p. 272) additionally speculate that "the integrated format
helps the learners mentally integrate the two sources of information and allows the learners to
avoid splitting their attention between the two media. Therefore, fewer cognitive resources
would be required to process the document, thus increasing the remaining working-memory
Illustrations 12
capacity allocated to learning." Such interpretations mesh well with those offered by other
researchers with respect to the function of integrated pictures in learning science and
mathematics concepts and skills (e.g., Marcus, Cooper, & Sweller, 1996; Mayer & Anderson,
1992; Mayer & Moreno, 1998; Mousavi, Low, & Sweller, 1995) – as will become apparent in the
immediately following section.
Interpretational Pictures
By far, the greatest number of research studies during the decade of the 1990s have
examined interpretational pictures as clarifiers of difficult-to-understand material (often scientific
or other technical concepts). In that regard, Richard Mayer and his colleagues have extensively
investigated the use of adjunct illustrations in facilitating students' understanding of scientific
explanations (see also Mayer's article in this issue). Mayer (1989) suggested that four
conditions must be met for pictorial illustrations to be helpful: (1) the text must describe a
cause-and-effect system; (2) the illustrations must reasonably depict the system or process
under consideration; (3) appropriate outcome measures must be selected; and (4) the learners
must be inexperienced with respect to the targeted content domain. As a representative
example, Mayer and Gallini (1990) conducted three experiments dealing with scientific devices
(see Figure 2). In these studies, a no-illustration (control) condition was compared to two
illustration conditions. One presented static pictures of a machine (e.g., a braking system), with
labels for each step ("steps" pictures); and the second displayed the "on" and "off" states of the
machine, with labels for each part and step ("parts-and-steps" pictures). It was found that
parts-and-steps pictures improved students' conceptual information recall and problem solving,
particularly for low prior-knowledge students. Mayer and Gallini refer to parts-and-steps
pictures as "explanative" illustrations and suggest that such illustrations serve as "runnable
mental models" for the learner (see also Glenberg & Langston, 1992; and Gyselinck & Tardieu,
Illustrations 13
1994, 1999). More recently, Mayer and Moreno (1998) compared on-screen text with concurrent
narration as adjuncts to a computer-generated animation that presented the process of lightning
formation. They found performance advantages for the concurrent narration condition and
offered a "split-attention" account in the context of Paivio's (e.g., 1986) dual-processing model of
working memory.
Reid and Beveridge (1990) conducted a computer-based experiment dealing with
illustrated science texts.4 One hundred eighty 14-year-olds were directed to study three
illustrated science topics presented by computer: "transport of sugar in plants," "exchange of
gases in the leaf," and "conduction of the nerve" (p. 77). The length of the text ran from 232 to
245 words. The computer recorded the time that students spent on the sentences and pictures.
Additionally, it recorded at which point in the sentences the student first looked at the picture.
General findings were that: (a) more difficult topics were associated with more time looking at
pictures (consistent with Levin and Mayer's, 1993, and Mayer's, 1989, assumptions); and
(b) "less successful" students spent more time looking at the pictures than did their "more
successful" counterparts. Note, however, that although the first finding is relatively interpretable
(because "text difficulty" levels were defined on an a priori basis), the second finding is not
(because "success" levels were defined post hoc on the basis of students' learning of the
experimental texts and, therefore, there is no clear separation of the effects of student
characteristics, picture looking, and text learning).
Riding and Douglas (1993) examined students' "cognitive style" in relation to their learning
about car brake systems under two computerized presentation formats. Secondary students
were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions: (1) text plus accompanying
verbal descriptions, and (2) text plus accompanying pictorial illustrations. Completion of the
computer activity and the test was followed by a cognitive-style assessment, with the focal style
Illustrations 14
being the verbal-imagery dimension. In brief, "imagers" performed better (learned more) than
"verbalizers" with the pictures format, whereas verbalizers performed better than imagers with
the descriptions format.
Prompted, in part, by the findings of an assessment team that concluded that "few texts
and their accompanying illustrations [are]...even marginally clear and easy to comprehend"
(Benson, 1995, p. 2; see also Blystone & Dettling, 1990), Benson (1995) conducted an
interesting dissertation investigation titled "Problems in picturing text." In that study, 15 experts
from three disciplines (textbook illustration design, editing, and biology) were asked to diagnose
and solve problems related to three high-school level biology topics that were accompanied by
illustrations. One illustration (which we would characterize as "representational") depicted the
flow of ice in New England during a particular ice age. A second illustration (characterized as
"organizational"), illustrated the life cycle of a fern. The third illustration (characterized as
"interpretational") displayed the life cycle of black bread mold.5 The three text/illustration
combinations were also read and interpreted by 14 first-year undergraduate biology students.
Perhaps suprisingly, the experts were unable to predict the undergraduates'
misinterpretations of the illustrations, and some did not even notice problems in the
text/illustration combinations themselves. Benson suggests that such problems may happen for
two reasons. First, they may occur because the various experts producing text/illustration
combinations are not working face-to-face. Second, and relatedly, problems may occur
because experts working independently may "advocate their own problem solving frameworks
rather than synthesize their perspectives with those of other experts" (Benson, 1995, p. vi).
Balluerka (1995) compared adjunct instructional aids in the context of a relatively long
scientific passage dealing with the photocopying process (1336 words). The four conditions
provided either: (a) no adjunct aids (control); (b) written instructions that provided an overview;
Illustrations 15
(c) the same written instructions plus directions to form a study outline; and (d) an illustration
(which we would characterize as an "interpretational" picture) that depicted the systems
described in the text. Students were allowed 22 minutes to study the text and the
corresponding adjunct aids. Dependent measures included a 5-item application test, and a
24-item true/false recognition test, either immediately or after 24 hours. Balluerka's results
(based on both the immediate and delayed tests) indicated that all three adjunct aids facilitated
performance in comparison to the no-aids control group.
Iding (1997) conducted three experiments in which questions were designed to facilitate
the processing of scientific diagrams in textbooks. In these studies, questions either: (a)
replaced traditional figure captions on the illustrations (Exp. 1); or (b) were placed directly in the
text (Exps. 2 & 3). Based on her research, Iding concluded that the use of questions did not
facilitate learning. She explained her findings in terms of previous "cognitive load" theorizing
(e.g., Mousavi et al., 1995). That is, "questions about illustrations might cause cognitive
resources to be unnecessarily and deleteriously expended in the text-diagram integration
process" (Iding, 1997, p. 22).
Ollerenshaw, Aidman, and Kidd (1997) compared the performance of undergraduates
under four study conditions, in a passage dealing with the operation of pumps: text only, text
plus diagram with labels, text plus diagram illustrating major operating stages, or "full
multimedia" (text and an animated simulation were computer projected in a darkened room). In
the initial session, students completed a test of prior knowledge, similar to that of Mayer and
Gallini (1990). In a second session, students completed a measure of learning style (the "Study
Process Questionnaire"), and, following study of the pumps passage in their respective
conditions for 10 minutes, they took a text comprehension test. In general, the authors found
that students provided with computer-simulated multimedia diagrams outperformed students in
Illustrations 16
the other three conditions. Moreover, from a student-differences perspective (and in accord with
both Mayer and Gallini's, 1990, findings and Levin and Mayer's, 1993, resultant assumptions),
the authors found that "...students with low prior knowledge profited most from comprehensive,
informative visual illustrations" (Ollerenshaw et al., 1997, p. 235).
Weidenmann, Paechter, and Hartmannsgruber (1999) conducted two computer
experiments involving the structuring and sequencing of interpretational illustrations. The
learning material dealt with three topics: the awarding of "quality badges," solar energy, and the
effect of stress on hormones. Three different picture formats were compared: (a) a top-down,
"zoom" presentation; (b) a step-by-step presentation; and (c) a static picture. Studying from
these presentations was followed by an unrelated activity and then a test over the content.
Participants also assessed their strategy's effectiveness and stated the order of their
preferences for the three picture formats. In the latter instance, students tended to prefer the
"zoom" presentation. Interestingly, however, students' preferences for particular strategies were
not related to how well they learned the material. The authors concluded that the particular
strategy students use is less important than the amount of time spent processing the pictures.
Transformational (Mnemonic) Pictures
Levin and his colleagues have examined adjunct transformational (mnemonic) pictures as
aids to students' learning from text (for earlier reviews, see Levin, 1982; and McCormick &
Levin, 1987). During the past decade, Dretzke (1993) examined the effects of mnemonic
illustrations on the prose recall of younger (17-29 years), middle-aged (40-50 years), and older
(60-84 years) adults. Within each age group, participants were randomly assigned to one of
three conditions, two of which are relevant to this review: mnemonic illustration and text-only
control. Based on the earlier work of Levin, Shriberg, and Berry (1983), the text material
Illustrations 17
consisted of six passages describing fictitious cities and five associated concrete attributes, as,
for example:
Belleview is attractively situated at the base on an inactive volcano, which last
erupted in the eighteenth century. Hot air balloon rides provide a thrilling way for
visitors to take in the lovely surroundings. A large automobile museum in the city
boasts to have the best collection of turn-of-the-century classics that can be found in
this part of the country. This is also the home of skilled craftsmen known for their
handmade musical instruments. Every summer, thousands of folks from all over the
world come here to compete against the best in an Olympic-style marathon.
(Dretzke, 1993, pp. 493-494)
In the mnemonic condition, the cities' names were represented as illustrated "keywords" (e.g., a
bell for Belleview), along with the pictured city attributes (see Figure 3). Dretzke found that
mnemonic illustrations were useful in facilitating both younger and middle-aged adults' recall of
concrete text material (relative to the text-only control condition).6 Keyword illustrations served
to organize participants' subsequent recall of the text information (i.e., attribute clustering) at all
three age levels. Further, Dretzke found a relationship between verbal ability and recall
performance for older adults in both the mnemonic and control conditions, suggesting that
"...with increasing age it becomes more difficult for adults of relatively lower verbal ability to
process effectively the information presented in complex pictorial interactions" (p. 499).
Levin and Levin (1990) reported a series of experimental studies (including those of Levin,
Rosenheck, & Levin, 1988, and Rosenheck, Levin, & Levin, 1989) in which a pictorial mnemonic
taxonomy, or "mnemonomy," was constructed to organize the content from a botany passage
(see Figure 4). Of particular interest to these investigators was whether mnemonic illustrations
could enable students go "beyond the information given" (e.g., Bruner, 1966) and assist them in
Illustrations 18
performing "higher-order" cognitive application tasks (Levin, 1986), such as those involving
inference, problem solving, and analogical and syllogistic reasoning based on the botany
content. Combined with separate mnemonic illustrations for solidifying unfamiliar terminology
and definitions, the pictorial mnemonomy was found to be a potent facilitator of students'
information reconstruction and application performance (relative to performance in a free-study
condition), both on immediate tests and on delayed tests up to two months later.
More recently, Levin's research group has extended their positive higher-order mnemonic
findings to other learning-from-text tasks. For example, Atkinson et al. (1999) conducted three
experiments in which the text-processing benefits of rows-and-columns matrix structures,
mnemonic illustrations, and their combination (in a mnemonic matrix, or "mnematrix"), were
examined. A central conclusion of these studies was that the memorial benefits of mnemonic
illustrations and mnematrices may be more critical than the "...computational-efficiency
properties of [conventional] matrices with respect to performing well on tasks...requiring memory
for previously studied factual information, including the organization and manipulation of that
information" (Atkinson et al., 1999, p. 356). The authors argue that the ready access to
information that mnemonic strategies afford can facilitate students' acquisition of higher-order
concepts and skills. Additional recent research (e.g., Atkinson, Levin, Beitzel, & Glover, 1999)
has replicated and extended the initial Atkinson et al. findings.
As a final transformational-illustration example, Rummel, Levin, & Beitzel (2001) recently
constructed a lengthy two-topic text passage in which was presented a variety of theoretical
(Topic 1) and psychometric (Topic 2) conceptions of intelligence. The passage was given to 40
college students under one of two conditions, mnemonic and free study. In the mnemonic
condition, illustrations relating people's names (e.g., a thirsty person for Thurstone, a gardener
for Gardner) to both their associated topic (theory or measurement) and their specific
Illustrations 19
contributions. For both factual memory (a relatively "lower level" name-fact matching task) and
essay writing (a relatively "higher level" critical-information production task), students in the
mnemonic condition outperformed their free-study counterparts.
Guidelines for Educators Considering Text-Accompanying Illustrations
We conclude this pictures-in-text review with a list of 10 practical suggestions for
educators, or what might be called "10 tenets for teachers." The first four tenets are derived
from our earlier "ten commandments" of pictorial facilitation of text information (Levin et al.,
1987).
1. Select pictures that overlap with text content. Learning benefits occur when pictures
and text provide congruent, or supporting, information. Decorational illustrations may help to
make the text more attractive or more marketable, but they are unlikely to enhance desired
outcomes related to understanding, remembering, or applying the text content. As a corollary
related to the pervasive use of pictures in teaching children beginning reading skills (i.e.,
phonemic awareness, word decoding, and word recognition): With precious few exceptions
(e.g., Levin, 1983, pp. 219-223), the bulk of the research literature suggests that providing
representational picture adjuncts is not a good idea.
2. Easy-to-follow texts that are highly concrete and engaging (e.g., interesting narrative
passages) readily elicit visual imagery in students and therefore are unlikely to yield additional
cognitive benefits from the inclusion of pictures.
3. Prerequisite basic reading skills are required on the part of the student for positive
effects of pictures to emerge. At the same time, young children or other students lacking such
skills can improve their listening comprehension and recall with well-selected pictorial
accompaniments.
Illustrations 20
4. Choose pictures with an eye toward the desired functions they are to play, namely –
and applying earlier-presented terminology – representational (to make the text more concrete),
organizational (to make the text more coherent), interpretational (to make the text more
comprehensible), or transformational (to make the text more codable – and more memorable),
in light of the desired learning outcomes.
The remaining six tenets are derived from our review of some of the more recent pictures-
in-text literature.
5. In general, the more complex the text, the more likely that pictures will be helpful (Levin
& Mayer, 1993). In particular, explanative (or interpretational) pictures will function as useful
mental models if: (a) the text describes a cause-and-effect system or complex process (e.g.,
Mayer & Gallini, 1990); and (b) the learners are relatively inexperienced in the content domain
(e.g., Mayer & Gallini, 1990; Ollerenshaw et al., 1997).
6. To yield the maximum benefits from pictures as text adjuncts, direct students to do
something with the picture that yields a controllable product, such as labeling the features of the
illustration (Peeck, 1993) or structuring the process so that students are certain to be
constructing a veridical pictorial representation of the passage (e.g., Rubman & Waters, 2000) .
Adler (1993) found that asking "why" or "what" questions about pictures was useful, although it
should be noted that Iding (1997) did not find question benefits in another context.
7. Computer software that uses integrated or pop-up displays may be more effective than
those using split displays in which the picture and text appear in separated locations on the
screen (Betrancourt & Bisseret,1998). This picture-text adjacency principle is consistent with
Mayer's conclusions from his "multimedia" investigations (see, Mayer, this volume). Simply put,
adjunct aids should be proximally adjunct!
Illustrations 21
8. You may also want/need to consider students' individual learning styles. For example,
Riding and Douglas (1993) found that students displaying an "imager" cognitive style profited
more from animated pictorial adjuncts than did students displaying a "verbalizer" style. In a
complementary vein two decades earlier, Levin, Divine-Hawkins, Kerst, and Guttmann (1974,
Exp. 2) reported that students who were adept at remembering pictured objects benefitted more
from instructions to generate text-related visual images while they read (relative to students who
were not as adept at remembering pictured objects).
9. Realize that even professionally designed pictures and illustrations in textbooks are not
necessarily perfect, nor easy for students to comprehend or remember (e.g., Benson, 1995; see
also Guri, 1985). Thus, even though a particular textbook illustration may be designed to be
cognitively useful, it may turn out to be functionally useless unless the learner perceives the
illustrated content or process in the intended manner.
10. Finally – and of special significance to the present authors – consider the use of
transformational (mnemonic) pictures as pictorial adjuncts to text. Although mnemonic
illustrations are rarely encountered in current textbooks (see, for example, Mayer, 1992),
teachers can learn how to develop creative and powerful illustrations of this kind. Nontechnical
accounts of mnemonic principles and procedures targeted at educators, students, and just
regular folks have been provided by Carney and Levin (1998), Carney, Levin, and Levin (1993),
Higbee (1988), and Levin (1980, 1998), among others. Mnemonic illustrations have been
associated with impressive memory-for-text gains in a large number of experimental studies
dealing with a variety of text topics and genres (e.g., Levin, 1995; McCormick & Levin, 1987),
with such illustrations producing higher-order application gains as well (e.g., Atkinson et al.,
1999; Levin & Levin, 1990). At the same time, with the numerous mnemonic text-learning
successes have come selected mnemonic text-learning failures (e.g., Ho, 1999; Renandya et
Illustrations 22
al., 1993). An important research priority is to identify the situational characteristics that
distinguish between the successes and failures so that more specifically "useful" mnemonic
illustration guidelines can be developed.
Conclusion
Although our 10 commandments of pictures-in-text facilitation (Levin et al., 1987) were not
chiseled in stone, they nonetheless have stood the test of a more than a decade's worth of time.
Indeed, the first four of our current 10 tenets for teachers were derived from those
commandments. Our additional six teacher-directed suggestions come from research
conducted throughout the decade of the 1990s – research that, as we move into the new
millennium, increasingly involves "pictures" as encapsulated in computer displays involving
image maps, animations, video clips, hypermedia, and beyond. Indeed, the ease with which
instructors can add pictorial elements to Web-based course presentations will likely lead to their
proliferation in the future. In that regard, it has been noted that novice PowerPoint users often
present too much verbal information on the screen. Complementarily, we caution instructors of
the future not to flood learners with adjunct computer graphics, but rather to deploy them
judiciously. An interesting, yet-to-be-addressed, empirical question is whether the
"cyberstudents" (Wang & Newlin, 2000) of the new millennium will differ from the "liberstudents"
of the century past in their ability to process picture and text information comprehensively, and
with comprehension.
In many ways, pictures – and guidelines for their effective use – transcend the medium.
Whether ancient cave painting or computer screen icon, pictures are part of the human
experience. Based on our review of the empirical literature, carefully constructed illustrations
continue to receive high marks as text adjuncts. Accordingly, and with an appreciative wink at
Illustrations 23
Paul and Art, the following closing tribute is in order: Pictures, we still praise thee after all these
years!
References
Adler, C. (1993, Sept.). Directed picture processing: The effects for learners on recall of
related text. Dissertation Abstracts International, 54(3-A): 863.
Atkinson, R. K., Levin, J. R., Beitzel, B. D., & Glover, T. A. (1999, Apr.). In search of the
unique cognitive benefits of mnemonic matrices. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association, Montreal.
Atkinson, R. K., Levin, J. R., Kiewra, K. A., Meyers, T., Kim, S. , Atkinson, L. A.,
Renandya, W. A., & Hwang, Y. (1999). Matrix and mnemonic text-processing adjuncts:
Comparing and combining their components. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 342-357.
Balluerka, N. (1995). The influence of instructions, outlines, and illustrations on the
comprehension and recall of scientific texts. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20,
369-375.
Benson, P. J. (1995, May). Problems in picturing text. Dissertation Abstracts International
Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, Vol. 55 (11-A): 3357.
Bernard, R. M. (1990). Using extended captions to improve learning from instructional
illustrations. British Journal of Educational Technology, 21, 215-225.
Betrancourt, M., & Bisseret, A. (1998). Integrating textual and pictorial information via
pop-up windows: An experimental study. Behaviour and Information Technology, 17, 263-273.
Blystone, R., & Dettling, B. (1990). Visual literacy in science textbooks: In M. B. Rowe
(Ed.), What research says to the science teacher: The process of knowing (Vol. 6, pp. 19-40).
Washington D. C.: National Science Teachers Association.
Bransford, J. D. (1979). Human cognition: Learning, understanding, and remembering.
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. New York: Norton.
Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (1998). Coming to terms with the keyword method in
introductory psychology: A “neuronmnemonic” example. Teaching of Psychology, 25, 132-134.
Carney, R. N., Levin, M. E., & Levin, J. R. (1993). Mnemonic strategies: Instructional
techniques worth remembering. Teaching Exceptional Children, 25(4), 24-30.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (rev. ed.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Constable, H., Cambell, B., & Brown, R. (1988). Sectional drawings from science
textbooks: An experimental investigation into pupils' understanding. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 58, 89-102.
Cutting, J. E., & Massironi, M. (1998). Pictures and their special status in perceptual and
cognitive inquiry. In J. Hochberg et al. (Eds.). Perception and cognition at the century's end:
Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 137-168). San Diego: Academic Press.
David, P. (1998). News concreteness and visual-verbal association: Do news pictures
narrow the recall gap between concrete and abstract news? Human Communication Research,
25, 180-201.
Dean, R. S., & Kulhavy, R. W. (1981). The influence of spatial organization in prose
learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 57-64.
Dretzke, B. J. (1993). Effects of pictorial mnemonic strategy usage on prose recall of
young, middle-aged, and older adults. Educational Gerontology, 19, 489-502.
Fang, Z. (1996). Illustrations, text, and the child reader: What are pictures in children's
storybooks for? Reading Horizons, 37, 130-142.
Glenberg, A. M., & Langston, W. E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures
help to build mental models. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129-151.
Goodman, K., Maras, L., & Birdseye, D. (1994). Look! Look! Who stole the pictures from
the picture books? The basalization of picture books. New Advocate, 7(1), 1-24.
Guri, S. (1985, March-April). The function of diagrams in learning from social science self-
study texts. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research
Association, Chicago.
Gyselinck, V., & Tardieu, H. (1994). Illustrations, mental models, and comprehension of
instructional text. In W. Schnotz & R. W. Kulhavy (Eds.), Comprehension and graphics.
Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Gyselinck, V., & Tardieu, H. (1999). The role of illustrations in text comprehension: What,
when, for whom, and why? In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of
mental operations during reading. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Higbee, K. L. (1988). Your memory: How it works and how to improve it (2nd ed.). New
York: Prentice Hall.
Higgins, L. C. (1979). Effects of strategy-oriented training on children's inference from
pictures. Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 27, 265-280.
Ho, E. (1999). The heart of the matter: The use of mnemonics and analogies in learning
science text. Unpublished masters thesis, University of Wisconsin – Madison.
Iding, M. K. (1997). Can questions facilitate learning from illustrated science texts?
Reading Psychology, 18, 1-29.
Jenkins, J. J. (1979). Four points to remember: A tetrahedral model of memory
explanations. In Cermak, L. S., and Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), Levels of processing in human
memory (pp. 429-446). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lesgold, A. M., Levin, J. R., Shimron, J., & Guttmann, J. (1975). Pictures and young
children's learning from oral prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 636-642.
Levie, W. H. (1987). Research on pictures: A guide to the literature. In D. M. Willows & H.
A. Houghton (Eds.). The psychology of illustration: I. Basic research (pp. 1-50). New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Levie, W. H., & Lentz, R. (1982). Effects of text illustrations: A review of research.
Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 30, 195-232.
Levin, J. R. (1980). Try a new method of vocabulary instruction. Weekly Reader
(Teacher's Edition 4), 61(25), 1-3.
Levin, J. R. (1981). On functions of pictures in prose. In F. J. Pirozzolo & M. C. Wittrock
(Eds.), Neuropsychological and cognitive processes in reading (pp. 203-228). New York:
Academic Press.
Levin, J. R. (1982). Pictures as prose-learning devices. In A. Flammer & W. Kintsch
(Eds.), Discourse processing (pp. 412-444). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Levin, J. R. (1983). Pictorial strategies for school learning: Practical illustrations. In M.
Pressley & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Cognitive strategy research: Educational applications (pp. 213-
237). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Levin, J. R. (1986). Four cognitive principles of learning-strategy instruction. Educational
Psychologist, 21, 3-17.
Levin, J. R. (1995). Stalking the wild mnemos: Research that's easy to remember. In G.
G. Brannigan (Ed.), The enlightened educator: Research adventures in the schools (pp.
85-108). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Levin, J. R. (1998). How to remember (almost) everything. In M. Suid (Ed.), The kids’
how to do (almost) everything guide (pp. 126-128). Palo Alto, CA: Monday Morning Books.
Levin, J. R., Anglin, G. J., & Carney, R. N. (1987). On empirically validating functions of
pictures in prose. In D. M. Willows & H. A. Houghton (Eds.), The psychology of illustration: I.
Basic research ( pp. 51-85). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Levin, J. R., & Berry, J. K. (1980). Children's learning of all the news that's fit to picture.
Educational Communica tion and Technology Journal, 28, 177-185.
Levin, J. R., & Divine-Hawkins, P., Kerst, S. M., & Guttmann, J. (1974). Individual
differences in learning from pictures and words: The development and application of an
instrument. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 296-303.
Levin, J. R., & Mayer, R. E. (1993). Understanding illustrations in text. In B. K. Britton, A.
Woodward, & M. Brinkley (Eds.), Learning from textbooks (pp. 95-113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Levin, J. R., Shriberg, L. K., & Berry, J. K. (1983). A concrete strategy for remembering
abstract prose. American Educa tional Research Journal, 20, 277-290.
Levin, M. E., & Levin, J. R. (1990). Scientific mnemonomies: Methods for maximizing
more than memory. American Education al Research Journal, 27, 301-321.
Levin, M. E., Rosenheck, M. B., & Levin, J. R. (1988). Mnemonic text-processing
strategies: A teaching science for science teaching. Reading Psychology, 9, 343-363.
Mandl, H., & Levin, J. R. (Eds.), Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures.
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Marcus, N., Cooper, M., & Sweller, J. (1996). Understanding instructions. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 88, 49-63.
Markman, E. M. (1979). Realizing that you don't understand: Elementary school
children's awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50, 643-655.
Mayer, R. E. (1989). Systematic thinking fostered by illustrations in scientific text. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 81, 240-246.
Mayer, R. E. (1992). Illustrations that instruct. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional
psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 253-284). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mayer, R. E., & Anderson, R. (1992). The instructive animation: Helping students build
connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 84, 444-452.
Mayer, R. E., & Gallini, J. K. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words?
Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 715-726.
Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (1998). A split-attention effect in multimedia learning:
Evidence for dual processing systems in working memory. Journal of Educational Psychology,
90, 312-320.
Mayer, R. E., Steinhoff, K., Bower, G., & Mars, R. (1995). A generative theory of textbook
design: Using annotated illustrations to foster meaningful learning of science text. Educational
Technology Research and Development, 43(1), 31-43.
McCormick, C. B., & Levin, J. R. (1987). Mnemonic prose-learning strategies. In M. A.
McDaniel & M. Pressley (Eds.), Imagery and related mnemonic processes: Theories, individual
dif ferences, and applications (pp. 392-406). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory
and visual presentation modes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 319-334.
Ollerenshaw, A., Aidman, E., & Kidd, G. (1997). Is an illustration always worth ten
thousand words? Effects of prior knowledge, learning style, and multimedia illustrations on text
comprehension. International Journal of Instructional Media, 24, 227-238.
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Co.
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual - coding approach. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension fostering
and monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117-175.
Peeck, J. (1993). Increasing picture effects in learning from illustrated text. Learning and
Instruction, 3, 227-238.
Raymond, A. (1995). Author, editor-in-chief, teacher: Patricia Lee Gauch. Teaching Pre
K - 8, 26(1), 62-64.
Reid, D. J., & Beveridge, M. (1990). Reading illustrated science texts: A micro-computer
investigation of children's strategies. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 60, 76-87.
Reinking, D. R., Hayes, D. A., & McEneaney, J. E. (1988). Good and poor readers use
of explicitly cued graphic aids. Journal of Reading Behavior, 20, 229-243.
Renandya, W. A., Hwang, Y., Rich, J. D., Ruffalo, S. L., Levin, M. E., & Levin, J. R. (1993,
Apr.) Explorations in mnemonic mythology. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association, Atlanta.
Riding, R. J., & Douglas, G. (1993). The effect of cognitive style and mode of
presentation on learning performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 297-307.
Rosenheck, M. B., Levin, M. E., & Levin, J. R. (1989). Learning botany concepts
mnemonically: Seeing the forest and the trees. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 196-
203.
Rubman, C. N., & Waters, H. S. (2000). A, B seeing: The role of constructive processes in
children's comprehension monitoring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 503-514.
Rummel, N., Levin, J. R., & Beitzel, B. D. (2001). Can mnemonic strategies enhance
students’ processing and recall of integrated text? Unpublished manuscript.
Samuels, S. J. (1970). Effects of pictures on learning to read, comprehension and
attitudes. Review of Educational Research, 40, 397-407.
Schallert, D. L. (1980). The role of illustrations in reading comprehension. In R. J. Spiro,
B. C. Bruce, & W. F. Brewer (Eds.) Theoretical issues in reading comprehension: Perspectives
from cognitive psychology, linguistics, artificial intelligence, and education (pp. 503-524).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sipe, L. R. (1998). How picture books work: A semiotically framed theory of text-picture
relationships. Children's Literature in Education, 29, 97-108
Smith, B. D., & Elifson, J. M. (1986). Do pictures make a difference in college textbooks?
Reading Horizons, 26, 270-277. .
Stewig, J. W. (1992). Reading pictures, reading text: Some similarities. The New
Advocate, 5(1), 11-22.
Waddill, P. J., & McDaniel, M. A. (1992). Pictorial enhancement of text memory:
Limitations imposed by picture type and comprehension skill. Memory and Cognition, 20,
472-482.
Waddill, P. J., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1988). Illustrations as adjuncts to prose:
A test-appropriate processing approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 457-464.
Wang, A. Y., & Newlin, M. H. (2000). Characteristics of students who enroll and succeed in
psychology web-based classes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 137-143.
Weidenmann, B. (1989). When good pictures fail: An information-processing approach to
the effect of illustrations. In H. Mandl & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Knowledge acquisition from text and
pictures (pp. 157-171). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Weidenmann, B., Paechter, M., & Hartmannsgruber, K. (1999). Structuring and
sequencing of comples text-picture combinations. European Journal of Psychology of
Education, 14, 185-202.
Footnotes
Russell N. Carney is in the Department of Psychology at Southwest Missouri State
University and Joel R. Levin is in the Department of Educational Psychology at the University of
Arizona. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Russell N. Carney,
Department of Psychology, Southwest Missouri State University, Springfield, MO 65804.
Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to RussellCarney@smsu.edu.
1. The effect sizes and interpretations are based on Cohen's (1988) d, or the standardized-
difference between an experimental (here, picture) and control (here, no picture)
condition.
2. As an aside that is especially apropos for present purposes, we note that: (1) professional
journal articles typically consist of densely worded technical text; (2) such text often can
benefit from clarifying pictorial accompaniments; but (3) pictures, diagrams, and figures
take up precious journal space, adding to the cost of an already costly enterprise.
Nevertheless, it is ironic that one often reads research articles focusing on the effects of
text-accompanying illustrations without encountering even a single illustration of the
illustration used in the research.
3. Our characterizing Betrancourt and Bisseret's (1998) pictures as "organizational" is based
on the example illustration provided in the published article.
4. Although this article included six tables and three figures, no examples of the pictures
under study were provided. Inasmuch as the texts addressed science (biology) concepts,
we have included the study under the "interpretational" heading.
5. We have placed this research under the "interpretational" heading because one of the
illustrations is regarded as "interpretational" and all illustrations focused on science
concepts.
6. Recall performance in the older adult sample was relatively low across the board.
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Average effect size by picture function, across all units (from Levin, Anglin, &
Carney, 1987, p. 68).
Figure 2. Illustration of a brake system (from Mayer & Gallini, 1990, p. 716).
Figure 3. Transformational illustration designed to represent details about the ficticious city of
Belleview (from Dretzke, 1993, p. 494)
Figure 4. Pictorial "mnemonomy" designed to promote students' higher-order learning (from
Levin & Levin, 1990, p. 305).
... first. That is, animated videos can capture and maintain the audience's attention through their dynamic visual elements, such as movement, color, and visual metaphors (Berk, 2011;Carney & Levin, 2002). Studies have shown that audiences are more engaged and less likely to experience cognitive overload when exposed to animated visuals compared to static slides (Hoffler & Leutner, 2007;Mayer & Moreno, 2002). ...
... Animated videos can be particularly useful for topics that involve complex or abstract ideas, as they help make these concepts more concrete and understandable to the audience (Berk, 2011;Hoffler & Leutner, 2007). Further they accentuate on "maintaining attention" and claim that "changing imagery and motion in animated videos can help sustain the audience's focus and engagement throughout the presentation, especially for longer speaking engagements" (Berk, 2011;Carney & Levin, 2002). Atkinson, Garner & Alley underline the importance "to establish credibility", arguing that "high-quality, professionally-produced animated videos can contribute to the speaker's credibility and authority (Atkinson, 2005;Garner & Alley, 2013). ...
... Furthermore, animated videos possess unique characteristics that differentiate them from traditional static visuals, such as slides or images (Rosenzweig, 2023). By leveraging the power of motion, color, and dynamic imagery, animated videos can captivate audiences, clarify complex ideas, and reinforce key messages in a memorable way (Berk, 2011;Carney & Levin, 2002). However, the specific mechanisms by which animated videos contribute to the effectiveness of public speaking presentations remain underexplored in the existing literature and frameworks. ...
Article
The paper examines how the current ability of creativity can authentically convey the technology-driven information to diverse audiences as well as explores its transformative impact on the presentation performances. The article gives a special emphasis on animated videos to testify their role in enhancing target audience's comprehension, clarity and overall experience. The authors focus on the two applications (www.renderforest.com and www.powtoon.com) that claim to be helpful examples of digital platforms to create funny and inspiring animated video scenarios as additional tools in making public speaking more engaging. On conducting a literature review and providing accounts of findings and analysis as well as peer-reviewed empirical studies on the problem in question, this paper explores how animated videos can engage audiences, reinforce key messages, explain abstract concepts, maintain attention, and establish a speaker’s credibility. Keywords: animated videos, visualization of information, visual presentation, visual aids, public speaking, audience engagement, information conveying, message reinforcement.
... To the best of our knowledge, the closest a study has come is the use of the loci method [36,38,46], which mainly involves thinking of a place and associating it with given information. Research has shown that contrary to fears of images hindering the reading of text, images actually compliment the text and may even provide additional details as the dual coding theory holds [5,55,56]. This is why picture books are sometimes called "twice-told tales" [56]. ...
... Research has shown that contrary to fears of images hindering the reading of text, images actually compliment the text and may even provide additional details as the dual coding theory holds [5,55,56]. This is why picture books are sometimes called "twice-told tales" [56]. With regards to mnemonics, associating both images and text with given information will likely establish a greater connection rather than overwhelm the user. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mnemonics hold potential for promoting long-term memory. Hence, they are being leveraged in serious games aimed to support long-term retention and retrieval of information. However, there is limited work focused on synthesizing the published research and findings on mnemonics serious games with a view to uncovering the extent of their application and effectiveness. This scoping review aims to bridge this gap. Articles were retrieved from four databases (ACM Library, IEEE Xplore, Scopus, and Web of Science). The criteria for inclusion were that the papers must be user studies that focused on mnemonics and serious games at the same time, were written in English, and were published in peer-reviewed journals or conferences. Two researchers, with the guidance of a senior researcher, independently and collaboratively assessed the eligibility of the retrieved papers using the PRISMA flowchart, elicited the relevant data, and tabulated the results in tables and charts using the GPS (game play, purpose, and scope) model. There were 12 papers that were accepted in this scoping review. Overall, most of the mnemonics serious games had a positive effect on memory, suggesting that they hold potential for promoting long-term memory, especially in memorization-intensive instructions, where a good number of students still struggle to retain taught material due to pedagogical, personal, and social challenges. However, more research still needs to be conducted, especially in the area of player-created mnemonics and teaching users how mnemonics can be effectively created using visualization and elaboration techniques.
... Research has shown that illustrated documents are more effective than text-only materials in improving learning outcomes (multimedia effect; e.g., Butcher, 2014), although this effect depends on the type of illustrations used (Carney & Levin, 2002). According to Mayer's (2014) cognitive theory of multimedia learning (CTML), multimedia learning involves the selection and organization of information from both text and pictures in separate channels of working memory, resulting in the creation of two mental models (a text-based and an image-based mental model). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Previous research has demonstrated that quizzing can improve self‐regulation processes and learning performances. However, it remains unclear whether quizzes in multimedia material bring similar benefits, and whether interindividual differences such as working memory capacity (WMC) modulate quizzing effects. Aims This study aimed to examine the effects of embedded quizzes (multiple quizzes within the learning sequence) with feedback during a multimedia learning sequence on self‐regulated processing (monitoring and control) and learning performances on repeated (material already tested) and new (untested) questions, and possible WMC moderation on performances. Sample and Methods Participants were 59 students who learned a multimedia neuroscience course module in either a no‐quiz or embedded quizzes condition. With regard to self‐regulated processing, control was assessed with eye‐tracking measures (fixation durations and transitions between texts and illustrations), and monitoring by comparing self‐reported judgements of learning with actual performances (calibration). At the end, students' WMC was measured, and learning performances were assessed with repeated and new questions. Results Quizzing improved learning performances on repeated and new questions, and enhanced monitoring (better calibration). Students with low WMC benefited more from quizzing for new questions than those with high WMC. However, contrary to our hypothesis, the embedded quizzes group allocated less time to the learning than the no‐quiz group. Conclusions Embedded quizzes are relevant tools to help students recalibrate their monitoring process and foster more effective self‐regulated learning. Results confirm that the use of quizzes can be recommended to improve the learning of multimedia material, especially among students with low WMC.
... Second, fonts alone may not have enough visual appeal or memorability compared to pictures or colors to evoke strong memories. Visual cues such as colors or pictures may be more effective because they are more salient and easier to remember (Carney & Levin, 2002). Third, a habituation effect may be possible. ...
... Halliday (1978: 39) holds that language as a social semiotic is essentially a system of 'meaning potential,' and grammar is not a set of rules for reference but a source for 'meaning-making.' Based on the theory of Systemic Functional Linguistics, some scholars have carried out research on graphic relations in multimodal discourse at the micro level: Schriver proposed five patterns of graphic relations from an informational perspective (Schriver, 1997: 365); Researchers (Carney & Levin, 2002;Marsh & White, 2003) categorised the picture functions of multimodal discourse from a functional perspective; Martinec and Salway proposed graphic interaction relations on the basis of Barthes' theory (Martinec & Salway, 2005). By introducing the multimodal analysis method, scholars have enriched the research content and application fields of visual grammar, expanding the scope of research into diverse contexts. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social developments are bringing about changes in how we communicate and represent ideas. With the tendency of globalisation, marine ecosystems are facing threats such as overfishing, pollution, and climate change, and marine ecological protection has become a focus of attention for all countries. This paper adopts Kress and van Leeuwen’s framework of visual grammar analysis to compare and analyse the use of symbols in Chinese and Western marine ecological protection posters from the perspectives of representational meaning, interactive meaning, and compositional meaning. Qualitative and quantitative analyses of 100 Chinese posters and 100 Western posters reveal similarities and differences in symbol selection, cultural context, and compositional approach. Results show that Chinese posters tend to integrate traditional cultural elements and natural symbols to convey the concept of environmental protection in a harmonious and warm way, such as the use of ink painting style and paper-cutting art to represent marine organisms, while Western posters focus on directly displaying the hazards of marine pollution by means of strong visual contrasts and modern design elements to stimulate the audience’s sense of urgency and responsibility, such as the display of marine animals trapped by plastic rubbish. This research not only deepens the understanding of the theory of visual grammar but also provides valuable references for the design of future environmental posters, emphasising the importance of using visual symbols and communication strategies in different cultural contexts in order to convey environmental messages more effectively and to promote the development of global marine conservation.
... (accessed on 15 August 2024). Graphics in the classroom, especially when accompanied by text, are thought to improve comprehension by drawing attention to important information, depicting abstract concepts in a concrete way, and facilitating the encoding of the information [48,49,52,53]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Children who enter the US education system as emerging bilinguals (or English Learners) show significant gaps in test scores and graduation rates compared to their English monolingual peers. Dual-language education programs may provide an instructional context that capitalizes on emerging bilingual children’s strengths and supports their academic performance while they acquire English. However, prior research has shown that integrating semantic knowledge across language systems poses a challenge. Supports may mitigate these challenges. Thus, across two studies, we examined instructional practices that may facilitate the integration of academic content across English and Spanish in elementary-age children enrolled in dual-language education programs. In Study 1, we examined whether facts presented through reading-while-listening or children’s silent self-paced reading more effectively supported learning the facts and subsequent integration for dual-language students in grades 3 and 4 (n = 56; Mage = 9.54 years). In Study 2, we examined whether combining graphics with text was more effective in supporting fact learning and cross-language integration compared to facts presented as text alone in dual-language students in grades 4 and 5 (n = 67; Mage = 10.48 years). Overall, the studies replicate the benefits of reading-while-listening and graphics for learning directly taught facts, but underscore the difficulty in integrating semantic knowledge across lessons and languages.
Preprint
Full-text available
Pictures in physics education go beyond instructional functions and serve affective roles, such as attracting attention, creating fascination, and fostering engagement with the depicted content. Recognizing the importance of these affective functions highlights the need to understand and utilize aesthetic pictures in a research-based educational environment. Prior research suggests that aesthetic and affective attractiveness in pictures enhances enjoyment and engagement with the physics content. This paper offers three main contributions: Firstly, it conceptualizes and presents research-based criteria for selecting pictures perceived as aesthetically pleasing, drawing on insights from psychology and physics education research. Following these criteria, aesthetic pictures related to a given curricular content can be selected. Secondly, the paper applies these criteria to selecting pictures showing geometrical optics. It then delves into an evaluation of students' aesthetic and affective perception of the selected pictures. A validated instrument measured these responses, showing strong reliability (aesthetic perception: αC\alpha_C = 0.87 [0.85, 0.89]; affective perception: αC\alpha_C = 0.82 [0.80, 0.85]). Thirdly, it combines decorative and instructional functions in tasks and compares students' perceptions of aesthetic pictures (AP) and classroom experiment pictures (CEP) in junior high school (N = 118), using a crossover design. Results indicated significantly better aesthetic and affective evaluations for APs, with large effect sizes (AP vs. CEP, aesthetic and affective perception: d = 1.05 - 1.56 and 0.85 - 1.48, respectively). We conclude that the here developed and investigated criteria are useful for selecting aesthetic and affective pictures. This provides a basis for further leveraging their educational potential to create fascination and engagement in science education.
Chapter
The impressive number and quality of chapters represented by this volume testifies both to the usefulness of mnemonic techniques and to the importance of conducting controlled research that continues to probe their “workings.” From a theoretical standpoint, one can strive to account for the mechanisms associated with the operation of mnemonic strategies, which in turn will permit precise specifications of the conditions related to their effectiveness (see, for example, Bellezza, Chapter 2, this volume; Desrochers & Begg, Chapter 3, this volume). From an applied educational standpoint, one can strive to design and prescribe different varieties of mnemonic strategies for different varieties of classroom curricula and students (e.g., Levin, 1985; Mastropieri, Scruggs, & Levin, Chapter 16, this volume).
Article
Considering that an illustration can be regarded as one possible external expression of a part of a mental model, we investigated the role of illustrations in the strengthening of a mental model built from an instructional text. In a first phase, two groups of subjects read a text dealing with the cellular division process. In a second phase, a group saw drawings alone which illustrated the process, whereas the other group read the text again. In each phase, two comprehension tests were used to evaluate the representation built by subjects. The course of reading was interrupted by questions assumed to reflect either a linguistic representation (paraphrases) or a mental model (inferences), and subjects had to verify inference statements at the end of the presentation. Results showed that gain in performance between the two phases was the same whether the subjects had seen the illustrations or the text again. This absence of difference was observed during as well as at the end of the presentation, and for paraphrases as well as for inferences. It therefore appears that the mental model has to reach a sufficient level of elaboration, in order that illustrations alone can reinforce it. Résumé: Considérant qu'une illustration peut ětre vue comme une expression possible ďune partie ďun modèle mental, nous avons étudié le rôle ďillustrations dans ľenrichissement ďun modèle mental construit à partir ďun texte à visée didactique. Dans une première phase, deux groupes de sujets lisaient un texte traitant du processus de la division cellulaire. Dans une deuxième phase, un groupe voyait des dessins seuls qui illustraient le processus, alors que ľautre groupe relisait le texte. Dans chaque phase, deux tests de compréhension ont été utilisés pour évaluer les représentations construites. Le cours de la lecture a été interrompu par des questions supposées refléter soit une représentation linguistique (paraphrases) soit un modèle mental (inférences), et les sujets devaient vérifier des énoncés inférés en fin de présentation. Les résultats ont montré que le gain de performances entre les deux phases était le měme que les sujets aient vu les illustrations ou relu le texte. Cette absence de différence a été observée aussi bien en cours qu'en fin de présentation et pour les paraphrases comme pour les inférences. Il semble donc que le modèle mental doive atteindre un niveau ďélaboration suffisant pour que des illustrations présentées seules puissent le renforcer.
Chapter
In this chapter I will provide examples, taken from a variety of school learning tasks, in which the potential utility of pictorial strategies has been explored. Included in the discussion are not only examples of situations in which pictures have proven helpful, but also examples of situations in which either negligible or negative effects of pictures have emerged. Along the way, appropriate task analyses will permit reasonable accounts of the various outcomes obtained to date, as well as speculations about those that might be anticipated in the future. Special attention will be given to the language arts area and, in particular, to word recognition and prose comprehension. Throughout the chapter, use will be made of the distinctions among picture functions that I have recently proposed in the context of prose comprehension (Levin, 1981b). These “functional” distinctions will prove helpful in understanding exactly what the addition of pictures to a task may and may not be expected to accomplish.
Article
In three experiments, students read expository passages concerning how scientific devices work, which contained either no illustrations (control), static illustrations of the device with labels for each part (parts), static illustrations of the device with labels for each major action (steps), or dynamic illustrations showing the "off" and "on" states of the device along with labels for each part and each major action (parts-and-steps). Results indicated that the parts-and-steps (but not the other) illustrations consistently improved performance on recall of conceptual (but not nonconceptiual) information and creative problem solving (but not verbatim retention), and these results were obtained mainly for the low prior-knowledge (rather than the high prior-knowledge) students. The cognitive conditions for effective illustrations in scientific text include appropriate text, tests, illustrations, and learners.
Article
In 2 experiments, students studied an animation depicting the operation of a bicycle tire pump or an automobile braking system, along with concurrent oral narration of the steps in the process (concurrent group), successive presentation of animation and narration (by 4 different methods), animation alone, narration alone, or no instruction (control group). On retention tests, the control group performed more poorly than each of the other groups, which did not differ from one another. On problem-solving tests, the concurrent group performed better than each of the other groups, which did not differ from one another. These results are consistent with a dual-coding model in which retention requires the construction of representational connections and problem solving requires the construction of representational and referential connections. An instructional implication is that pictures and words are most effective when they occur contiguously in time or space.
Article
In 2 experiments, students who lacked prior knowledge about car mechanics read a passage about vehicle braking systems that either contained labeled illustrations of the systems, illustrations without labels, labels without illustrations, or no labeled illustrations. Students who received passages that contained labeled illustrations of braking systems recalled more explanative than nonexplanative information as compared to control groups, and performed better on problem solving transfer but not on verbatim recognition as compared to control groups. Results support a model of meaningful learning in which illustrations can help readers to focus their attention on explanative information in text and to reorganize the information into useful mental models.