ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

We revisited the well-known warm–cold paradigm in a 2 (cold vs. warm) × 2 (odor vs. no odor information) between-subjects experiment. The participants were read a list of character qualities describing a target (cold vs. warm and odor vs. no odor information) before judging the target on 4 dimensions related to social desirability. The results reinforce the warm– cold dimensions as central traits. However, they also go one step further. They show that describing the target as smelling like vanilla undermined the classical cold effect. When forming impressions about the target, the individual can also take external cues (such as the odor evoked) into account.
THE SWEET SMELL...OF COLDNESS:
VANILLA AND THE WARM–COLD EFFECT
ROXANE SAINT-BAUZEL AND VALÉRIE FOINTIAT
University of Lorraine
We revisited the well-known warm–cold paradigm in a 2 (cold vs. warm) × 2 (odor vs. no
odor information) between-subjects experiment. The participants were read a list of character
qualities describing a target (cold vs. warm and odor vs. no odor information) before judging
the target on 4 dimensions related to social desirability. The results reinforce the warm–
cold dimensions as central traits. However, they also go one step further. They show that
describing the target as smelling like vanilla undermined the classical cold effect. When
forming impressions about the target, the individual can also take external cues (such as the
odor evoked) into account.
Keywords: vanilla, odor evocation, warm–cold paradigm, impression formation, social
perception, social desirability.
On one hand, many researchers have demonstrated that odors have an impact
both on human behaviors (Baron, 1997; Guéguen & Petr, 2006; Saint-Bauzel &
Fointiat, 2012) and cognitions (Lehrner, Marwinski, Lehr, Johren, & Deecke,
2005; Walla et al., 2003). Baron conducted an experiment in different odorant
zones in a mall: pleasant odor zone (pastry baking) vs. neutral odor zone
(clothes). Passersby were more inclined to accept a request for change (helping
behavior) when they were in the pleasant odor zone than were those in the neutral
odor zone. Guéguen and Petr demonstrated that the diffusion of lavender aroma
in a restaurant affects consumer behaviors, increasing both the customers’ length
of stay and the amount they purchase. More recently, Saint-Bauzel and Fointiat
have shown that the foot-in-the-door procedure (a well-known technique of
influencing people, in which a requester makes a small request before asking for
SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2013, 41(10), 1635-1640
© Society for Personality Research
http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2013.41.10.1635
1635
Roxane Saint-Bauzel and Valérie Fointiat, Department of Psychology, University of Lorraine.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Roxane Saint-Bauzel, Université de
Lorraine, PErSEUs, UFR SHA, Ile du Saulcy, BP 30309 F-57 000, Metz, France. Email: roxane.
saint-bauzel@univ-lorraine.fr
IMPRESSION FORMATION AND ODOR
1636
a larger one; Freedman & Fraser, 1966) could be affected by the odor worn by
the requester. On the other hand, compliance increased when the requester was
perfumed with vanilla. Lehrner et al. have demonstrated that orange as well as
lavender odors alter emotional states and that both odors reduce anxiety in dental
patients. Walla et al. showed that odors (e.g., rose) affected face recognition.
Thus, faces presented in association with a hedonically positive odor are less
accurately recognized, suggesting that the odor could have the status of a
distractor, perturbing the face encoding processes. In summary, odor, scent,
fragrance, and aroma taken together affect information processing.
Since Asch’s (1946) seminal studies, a considerable number of researchers in
social psychology have focused on the study of how people form impressions
about others. Asch’s participants were read one of two descriptions of the
hypothetical person as “intelligent – skillful – industrious – warm (description
A) versus cold (description B) – determined – practical – cautious” (p. 262).
Asch then asked the participants to assess this target person. It appears that the
participants developed different implicit theories about the personality of the
individual described; the description containing “cold” received a more negative
characterization than the description containing “warm”. He concluded that some
traits have a central status (e.g., warm or cold) and affect the global impression
of others, whereas other traits are more peripheral (e.g., blunt or polite) and exert
less influence on the global impression. When forming impressions about others,
we take a variety of cues into account. Some of them are easily perceptible, for
example physical appearance (Jones, 1990), whereas others are more “subtle
cues” (Hancock & Dunham, 2001, p. 325). In McArthur and Baron’s (1981)
study, the cognitive approach to impression formation was focused on the
processing of information. Theoretically, perceptions of the social environment
are derived from multisensory stimulus information. However, most researchers
have focused on the visual aspects of appearance. In other words, they have
focused on individual characteristics (e.g., his/her disposition, verbal elaboration,
behavior) but underestimated the potential role of contextual cues.
We support the idea that odors could be one of these neglected cues. Few
researchers have actually examined the impact odor has on social perception
(Baron, 1981; Fiore, 1992, 1993; Li, Moallem, Paller, & Gottfried, 2007).
Baron showed that male participants evaluated a female experimenter as more
attractive, and attributed more positive traits to her when she was pleasantly
perfumed. Fiore also observed that the components of an odor influence first
impressions concerning both the appearance and personality of others. She asked
female participants to think about hypothetical people, perfumed with various
feminine commercial fragrances, and to assess their personalities. There was
an association between flowery, fruity fragrances and female characters on one
hand, and spicy, strong fragrances and male characters on the other hand. This
IMPRESSION FORMATION AND ODOR 1637
association shows the use of a gender stereotype, bringing us to conclude that
odors will play a role at a clearly social level. More recently, Li et al. asked
participants who had unconsciously inhaled pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral
odors (ambient air) to judge neutral faces. The results showed that participants
assessed the faces more negatively when they had inhaled an unpleasant odor
than when they had inhaled a pleasant or neutral one.
In line with previous research, it appears that odors are just as essential as
environmental cues (even if they are not taken into account) as are visual aspects
of appearance. Usually, researchers explore odor cues as stimulus variables in
impression formation by exposing participants to these cues. Our aim in this
study was to test the impact of a pleasant odor evoked in the classical warm–
cold paradigm. In line with Asch’s (1946) central traits theory, we hypothesized
that a warm description would lead to a more positive evaluation than a cold
description (preliminary hypothesis). We also hypothesized that evoking a
pleasant odor would counteract the classical warm–cold effect. Thus, describing
a hypothetical person as cold will lead to a positive evaluation if the description
includes the evocation of a pleasant odor (main hypothesis).
Method
Participants were 119 undergraduate students (104 women and 15 men, Mage =
19.8 years, SD = 2.4) at Metz University, France. They participated in a 2 (central
trait: warm vs. cold) × 2 (odor-based information: vanilla vs. no information)
between-subjects experiment, in which they were asked to rate the target as a
measure of social perception. In a pilot study, we tested various odors. Based
on the procedure proposed by Chréa et al. (2009), students (n = 26) inhaled
several odors and then assessed each of them. Several dimensions were measured
(including attraction, familiarity, saliency, and intensity). Vanilla odor was highly
attractive, familiar, salient, and intense, compared with other odors (including
lemon, camphor, coffee, chocolate, and lavender).
We followed Asch’s (1946) procedure (Experiment 1), and presented the task
as following:
I shall read you a number of characteristics that belong to a particular
person, Mr X. Please, listen to them carefully and try to form an impression
of the kind of person described. I will read the list slowly and I will repeat
it once (p. 262).
For half of the participants (warm conditions), Mr X was described as
“intelligent, skillful, industrious, warm, determined, practical, and cautious”.
For the other half (cold conditions), Mr X was described as “intelligent, skillful,
industrious, cold, determined, practical and cautious” (Asch, 1946, p. 262). In
IMPRESSION FORMATION AND ODOR
1638
conditions where an odor was evoked, the target was also described as smelling
like vanilla, whereas no information about odor was given in the remaining
conditions. Then each participant rated the target on several dimensions
(charming, generous, sociable, and amusing) using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging
from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. These four adjectives, identified
as specific to high social desirability (Le Barbenchon, Cambon, & Lavigne,
2005), were aggregated into an index (Cronbach’s alpha = .72).
Results and Discussion
Consistent with Asch’s (1946) perspective (preliminary hypothesis), a main
effect of the central trait was obtained, F(1, 115) = 61.17, p < .001, 2 = .34, with
participants in the warm conditions rating the target more positively (M = 3.32,
SD = .64) than participants in the cold conditions (M = 2.34, SD = .73). A main
effect of the odor-based information was also observed, F(1, 115) = 4.25, p < .04,
2 = .03. Participants in the vanilla conditions rated the target as more desirable
(M = 2.96, SD = .71) than participants in the no odor information conditions
(M = 2.70, SD = .91). An interaction effect was observed, F(1, 115) = 3.92, p <
.05, 2 = .03. Post hoc comparisons (least significant difference) revealed that
participants in the cold, vanilla condition rated the target more positively (n = 26,
M = 2.59, SD = .65) than did participants in the cold, no odor-based information
condition (n = 22, M = 2.09, SD = .74), p < .01 (main hypothesis). No significant
difference was observed between the warm, vanilla condition (n = 38, M = 3.31,
SD = .68) and the warm, no odor-based information condition (n = 33, M = 3.32,
SD = .60).
The findings support predictions stemming from central traits theory. Asch
(1946) and the other researchers on social perception (Kelley, 1950; Widmeyer
& Loy, 1988) have demonstrated that participants’ descriptions of the personality
of a hypothetical (and sometimes a real person) can be altered by simply
interchanging adjectives representing central qualities (e.g., warm or cold).
However, they also found that the inclusion of adjectives representing peripheral
qualities (e.g., polite or blunt) did not significantly affect participants’ impressions
of the perceived personality of the person described.
As expected, odor-based information seems to moderate negative impressions
when the target is described as cold. Thus, impression formation is affected not
only by central traits (cold vs. warm), but also by extrinsic information, such as
an odor being evoked, which is not related to personality dimensions. It seems
that the odor-evoked information (vanilla) could have the status of central
information, since the evocation of vanilla associated with personality traits
could counterbalance the negative impact of the central trait cold.
IMPRESSION FORMATION AND ODOR 1639
The impact of extrinsic information on social perception has been only
partially examined. Fennis and Pruyn (2007) demonstrated that brand personality
traits could affect perceptions about an individual’s personality. Once again,
Fennis and Pruyn examined visual aspects of appearance in relation to social
perception. The effect unpleasant odors have on impression formation about
others could be considered as a limitation of our study. However, we support that
future researchers have to fit with the complexities of real life. Therefore, future
researchers would have to examine the impact of multisensory cues integrating
tactile-related information, visual-related information, and olfactory-related
information.
In this experiment, we focused on the effect of whether or not a person is
described as smelling like vanilla has on the evaluation of that person. Future
researchers could examine the effect of ambient scent on the warm–cold
paradigm. Otherwise, the effects of odor information on traits could be mediated
by the possible effects that odor information has on the participant’s mood, which
could be subsequently projected on the target person. The effect of mood as a
mediation variable has been considered in a few studies (Baron, 1997; Guéguen
& Petr, 2006), even if evidence of that mediation has not been provided in any
of them. This avenue of research could represent an interesting and innovative
challenge.
References
Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
41, 258-290. http://doi.org/bw48qw
Baron, R. A. (1981). Olfaction and human social behavior: Effects of a pleasant scent on attraction
and social perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 611-616. http://doi.org/
brt938
Baron, R. A. (1997). The sweet smell of…helping: Effects of pleasant ambient fragrance on
prosocial behavior in shopping malls. Psychological Bulletin, 23, 498-503. http://doi.org/cr9
Chréa, C., Grandjean, D., Delplanque, S., Cayeux, I., Le Calvé, B., Aymard, L., … Scherer, K. R.
(2009). Mapping the semantic space for the subjective experience of emotional responses to
odors. Chemical Senses, 34, 49-62. http://doi.org/bkhpjs
Fennis, B. M., & Pruyn, A.-T. H. (2007). You are what you wear: Brand personality influences on
consumer impression formation. Journal of Business Research, 60, 634-639. http://doi.org/
bb52mn
Fiore, A. M. (1992). Effect of composition of olfactory cues on impressions of personality. Social
Behavior and Personality, 20, 149-161. http://doi.org/fbdcdh
Fiore, A. M. (1993). Multisensory integration of visual, tactile, and olfactory aesthetic cues of
appearance. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 11, 42-52. http://doi.org/fmg78z
Freedman, J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door
technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 195-202. http://doi.org/csh
Guéguen, N., & Petr, C. (2006). Odors and consumer behavior in a restaurant. International Journal
of Hospitality Management, 25, 335-339. http://doi.org/csm
IMPRESSION FORMATION AND ODOR
1640
Hancock, J. T., & Dunham, P. J. (2001). Impression formation in computer-mediated communication
revisited: An analysis of the breadth and intensity of impressions. Communication Research, 28,
325-347. http://doi.org/cs8v95
Jones, E. E. (1990). Interpersonal perception. New York: Freeman.
Kelley, H. H. (1950). The warm-cold variable in first impressions of persons. Journal of Personality,
18, 431-439. http://doi.org/cc5r6q
Le Barbenchon, E., Cambon, L., & Lavigne, F. (2005). Désirabilité et utilité sociale de 308 adjectifs
de personnalité et 297 professions [Desirability and social utility for 308 personality descriptors
and 297 professions]. L’Année Psychologique, 105, 307-322. http://doi.org/ddzz53
Lehrner, J., Marwinski, G., Lehr, S., Johren, P., & Deecke, L. (2005). Ambient odors of orange and
lavender reduce anxiety and improve mood in a dental office. Physiology & Behavior, 86, 92-95.
http://doi.org/cb9d3w
Li, W., Moallem, I., Paller, K. A., & Gottfried, J. A. (2007). Subliminal smells can guide social
preferences. Psychological Science, 18, 1044-1049. http://doi.org/csp
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward an ecological theory of social perception.
Psychological Review, 90, 215-238. http://doi.org/dns9cw
Saint-Bauzel, R., & Fointiat, V. (2012). The sweet smell of the requester: Vanilla, camphor, and foot-
in-the-Door. Social Behavior and Personality: An international journal, 40, 369-374. http://doi.
org/m7f
Walla, P., Hufnagl, B., Lehrner, J., Mayer, D., Lindinger, G., Imhof, H., … Lang, W. (2003). Olfaction
and face encoding in humans: A magnetoencephalographic study. Cognitive Brain Research, 15,
105-115. http://doi.org/ckrnp4
Widmeyer, W. N., & Loy, J. W. (1988). When you’re hot, you’re hot! Warm-cold effects in first
impressions of persons and teaching effectiveness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80,
118-121. http://doi.org/c4wzpg
... When information arising from that individual is restricted, as in the present experimental situation, it is clear from the present data that odorants are considered when drawing conclusions about a person. This confirms previous research showing that activating the concept of an odorant verbally (such as vanilla) can alter the impression of another person [64]. The present data shows that the contribution of the olfactory information is not general, such as "this person stinks"; rather, the results of the principal analysis showed that the specific odor involved is important, with some odors being more successfully associated with a person than others. ...
Article
Full-text available
First impressions of social traits are regularly, rapidly, and readily determined from limited information about another individual. Relatively little is known about the way that olfactory information, particularly from scents that are not body odors, alters a first impression. Can the attributes of an odorant be conferred onto a person associated with that scent? To explore this, 101 participants were asked to form an impression of a hypothetical person based on the following stimuli: A gender-neutral silhouette, a list of six personal characteristics, and one of five odorants. Participants then rated the likelihood that the hypothetical person possessed each of 51 personality traits that were determined a priori as falling into six attribute categories. Participants also directly rated all odorants for the six categories and intensity. A T-test showed that ratings of the hypothetical person were less disparate from the odor that was presented during impression formation than from other odors. ANOVA revealed that the effects were heterogeneous, with odorants varying in their effectiveness in associating the hypothetical person with categories. The present data suggest that a hypothetical person can be imbued with the specific attributes of an odor and that some odors are better at contributing to impressions than others.
... On the basis of available studies [8,[27][28][29] and interviews with managers from companies dealing with aromatising, five aromas have been profiled to be suitable for use in the confectionery department. Subsequently, a survey was conducted to identify the views of respondents and associations that are associated with different types of aromas. ...
Article
Full-text available
Building a unique USP sales argument (unique selling proposition) through various forms of in-store communication comes to the fore in a challenging competitive environment. Scent as a means to influence the purchase of goods or services has a long history, however, aromachology as field of in-store communication is a matter of the present. This new trend, the importance and use of which has grown in recent years, is the subject of a wide range of research. In order to increase the efficiency of these elements, it is necessary to familiarise ourselves with the factors that affect the customer, whether that be consciously or unconsciously. Consumer neuroscience is addressed in this area. This paper deals with the comprehensive interdisciplinary investigation of the impact of selected aromatic compounds on consumer cognitive and affective processes as well as assessing the effectiveness of their implementation in food retail operations. At the end of the paper, we recommend options for the effective selection and implementation of aromatisation of different premises, by which the retailer can achieve not only a successful form of in-store communication, but also an increase the retail turnover of the store.
... Similarly, Zemke and Shoemaker (2008) found that the use of sensory cues can enhance social interaction behaviors with strangers in meeting events. In another experimental study conducted by Saint-Bauzel and Fointiat (2013), the participants were read a written list of character qualities describing a food (sweet smell vs. no smell) before judging the given food regarding social behaviors. The results of their study showed that the verbal descriptions of the food as smelling like vanilla promoted warmth-related impressions about the given food. ...
Article
Restaurateurs often use sensory, nostalgic, and brand descriptions to influence a customer’s perceptions. This research suggests that descriptive food names can influence customers’ warmth and competence impressions regarding their impending service experience. The replications of the experiments across the two menu contexts (entrée vs dessert) indicate that customers view a restaurant with sensory and nostalgic food names as offering warmer impending services compared to a restaurant with general names. Also, customers view a restaurant that utilizes brand names as providing more competent impending services than a restaurant that utilizes general names. However, there was a different pattern observed for the effects of sensory names on consumers’ warmth-related impressions in the dessert menu selection context. Further, this research proposes that warmth and competence can drive different magnitudes of anticipated satisfaction and willingness to pay more. Perceived warmth served a more important role in anticipated satisfaction than did perceived competence. Conversely, regarding willingness to pay more, perceived competence acted as a more significant factor than did perceived warmth.
Article
Full-text available
The paper explores situational and dispositional underpinnings of cooperative behavior. According to psychological research, cooperation is strongly related to affective states (Forgas, 1998) and personality dimensions (Volk, Thöni, & Ruigrok, 2011). In an experimental study we examined the conditions under which people cooperate with each other. The dispositional traits of co-workers (personality), the contribution to a collaborative effort, and a situational factor – ambient odor condition were taken into consideration. A one-way ANOVA revealed that compared to a malodorous condition, both the pleasant odor condition and the natural odor condition showed higher rates of cooperation. Further analysis indicated that only malodors influenced affective states which in turn determined social decisions. Although we found effects for the participants’ agreeableness and the coworker’s contribution to a joint work, they appeared to play a less critical role than affective states induced by the experimental odor conditions tested here.
Article
Full-text available
Several researchers have shown that odors affect human behavior. However, odors have not been studied in the context of specific compliance without pressure. Specifically, the impact of the odor worn by a requester during the foot-in-the-door procedure has not been documented. To address this issue, an experiment was carried out in an ecological setting. Using the foot-in-the-door procedure, a well-known technique for increasing the likelihood that a person will comply with one's request, the requester was perfumed with vanilla, camphor, or nothing. The results show a strong effect of the foot-in-the-door technique when the requester was perfumed with vanilla and no effect of the procedure when the requester was perfumed with camphor. These results are incompatible with the main theoretical interpretations of foot-in-the-door phenomena: self-perception and commitment theories.
Article
Full-text available
Following either a text-based, synchronous computer-mediated conversation (CMC) or a face-to-face dyadic interaction, 80 participants rated their partners' personality profile. Impressions were assessed in terms of both their breadth (the comprehensiveness of the impression) and intensity (the magnitude of the attributions). Results indicated that impressions formed in the CMC environment were less detailed but more intense than those formed face-to-face. These data provide support for theories that, in addition to acknowledging the unique constraints and characteristics of CMC, consider the cognitive strategies and heuristics involved in the impression formation process. The differential impact of a text-based medium on trait-specific impressions (e.g., extraversion, neuroticism) is also discussed in the context of a cross-modal approach to impression formation.
Article
Full-text available
Integration of olfactory, visual, and tactile aesthetic cues of appearance in the impression formation process was examined. Eighty-nine female college students, each responding to three fragrances, rated 30 fabric swatches on 7-point Likert-type scales as to the fabrics' likeliness to be worn by someone who prefers to wear a particular fragrance. Rank orders of fabrics for the two fragrances with similar olfactory structures were associated (p .05) according to Spearman correlations, suggesting multisensory integration of appearance cues. A priori visual and tactile aesthetic qualities of fabrics ranked high or low for each fragrance were examined for similarities. Similarities of visual and tactile qualities were found within each group of fabrics for each fragrance, suggesting multisensory integration of appearance cues on an aesthetic level.
Article
Full-text available
Ninety female undergraduates participated in an experiment investigating whether composition of three fragrances (perfumes) influenced impressions of personality traits of people who would wear each fragrance. Analysis of covariance with the variance due to liking of the fragrance (a covariate) removed, revealed that fragrance composition influenced impressions of personality. Impressions made of affiliated fragrances (oriental and chypre) were more closely aligned than impression of the dissimilar (floral) fragrance for the multi-item Uninhibited and Traditional Male factors and single item traits such as aggressive, confident, and assertive according to Tukey's tests. The floral fragrance produced significantly (p<.05) lower ratings in these instances. The findings suggest that compositional components of fragrances (olfactory cues) were used in formation of impressions. This has implications for research of the role of olfactory cues in social and professional interactions.
Article
Full-text available
We examined the effects of Kelley's "warm/cold" manipulation on first impressions of persons and teaching ability. A stimulus person, posing as a visiting professor, gave a "neutral" lecture to 240 university students. Before the stimulus person appeared, half of the subjects received information that he was a rather warm person, whereas the other half was told that he was a rather cold person. In turn, half of each of these groups was informed that he was a professor of physical education and the other half that he was a professor of social psychology. A 2 (warm/cold condition) × 2 (discipline of the stimulus-person ) × 2 (subjects' sex) multivariate analysis of variance revealed that (a) subjects who were led to believe that the stimulus person was warm perceived him as a more effective teacher and as less unpleasant, more sociable, less irritable, less ruthless, more humorous, less formal, and more humane than did subjects who were told that he was a cold person; and (b) neither the disciplinary status of the stimulus person nor the sex of the subjects had an effect on subjects' perception of the lecturer. Results were discussed in regard to halo, context, and status effects. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Applies the ecological approach to perception, drawing on the recent theories of J. Gibson (1979) and R. Shaw, M. Turvey and W. Mace (1982) to the social domain. The general advantages of this approach are enumerated, its applicability to social perception is documented, and its specific implications for research on emotion perception, impression formation, and causal attribution are discussed. The implications of the ecological approach for the understanding of errors in social perception are also considered. Finally, the major tenets of the ecological approach are contrasted with current cognitive approaches, and a plea is made for greater attention to the role of perception in social knowing. (100 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Résumé 139 étudiants ont évalué la désirabilité et l'utilité sociale de 308 adjectifs de personnalité et de 297 professions. Les évaluations moyennes et les écart types sont donnés pour chaque item et pour chaque type d'évaluation (désirabilité et utilité sociale). Pour chaque item un t de Student a été calculé entre les deux évaluations. Les corrélations entre ces évaluations et trois autres normes de désirabilité (en anglais et en français) varient entre .87 et .96. Mots clés : désirabilité, utilité sociale, adjectifs, professions.
Article
A total of 94 undergraduate males participated in an experiment designed to examine the impact of a pleasant scent (perfume) on interpersonal attraction and social perception. Subjects met and interacted briefly with one of two female confederates who either wore perfume or did not, and who were dressed either neatly (in a blouse, skirt, and hose) or informally (in jeans and a sweatshirt). Results indicated that the presence of a pleasant scent increased attraction toward the confederates and produced positive shifts in perceptions of several of their traits when they were dressed informally. When the confederates dressed in a neater manner, however, opposite effects were observed. These findings were interpreted as stemming from the fact that subjects reacted more favorably to the confederates when they appeared to be intermediate rather than high or low along a dimension of informality-formality.
Article
In a preliminary study, passersby in a large shopping mall were significantly more likely to help a same-sex accomplice (by retrieving a dropped pen or providing change for a dollar) when these helping opportunities took place in the presence of pleasant ambient odors (e.g., baking cookies, roasting coffee) than in the absence of such odors. Participants also reported significantly higher levels of positive affect in the presence of pleasant odors. In a second study, the order in which passersby were exposed to a helping opportunity and rated their current mood was systematically varied. Results similar to those of the first study were obtained; order of task had no effect on either mood or helping, but helping was significantly greater in the presence of pleasant fragrances than in their absence. In addition, there was some evidence that fragrance-induced increments in helping were mediated by increments in positive affect.
Article
Several studies have shown that odors have an effect on human behavior. Consumer's behavior is also affected by odors. An experiment was carried out in a restaurant where lemon and lavender aromas were diffused and compared to a no-aroma control condition. Results showed that lavender—but not lemon aroma—increased the length of stay of customers and the amount of purchasing. The hypothesis that lavender produces a relaxing effect is offered to explain the results.