ArticlePDF Available

Examining the Relationship Between Conspiracy Theories, Paranormal Beliefs, and Pseudoscience Acceptance Among a University Population

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Very little research has investigated whether believing in paranormal, conspiracy, and pseudoscientific claims are related, even though they share the property of having no epistemic warrant. The present study investigated the association between these categories of epistemically unwarranted beliefs. Results revealed moderate to strong positive correlations between the three categories of epistemically unwarranted beliefs, suggesting that believers in one type tended to also endorse other types. In addition, one individual difference measure, looking at differences in endorsing ontological confusions, was found to be predictive of both paranormal and conspiracy beliefs. Understanding the relationship between peoples' beliefs in these types of claims has theoretical implications for research into why individuals believe empirically unsubstantiated claims. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Examining the Relationship Between Conspiracy Theories, Paranormal Beliefs,
and Pseudoscience Acceptance Among a University Population
EMILIO LOBATO
1
*, JORGE MENDOZA
2
, VALERIE SIMS
1,3
and MATTHEW CHIN
1,3
1
Institute for Simulation and Training, University of Central Florida, Orlando, USA
2
Department of Political Science, Florida State University, Tallahassee, USA
3
Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, Orlando, USA
Summary: Very little research has investigated whether believing in paranormal, conspiracy, and pseudoscientic claims are re-
lated, even though they share the property of having no epistemic warrant. The present study investigated the association between
these categories of epistemically unwarranted beliefs. Results revealed moderate to strong positive correlations between the three
categories of epistemically unwarranted beliefs, suggesting that believers in one type tended to also endorse other types. In addi-
tion, one individual difference measure, looking at differences in endorsing ontological confusions, was found to be predictive of
both paranormal and conspiracy beliefs. Understanding the relationship between peoplesbeliefs in these types of claims has
theoretical implications for research into why individuals believe empirically unsubstantiated claims. Copyright © 2014 John
Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
INTRODUCTION
Believing in the unbelievable is still widely prevalent in to-
days society. These beliefs come in many forms, ranging
from psychic powers, to conspiracies by clandestine govern-
ment agencies to assassinate high-prole individuals, to out-
right denial of well-established scientic and medical facts
and theories in favor of unrealistic claims about the nature
of the universe or personal health. However, believing in
these unbelievable claims comes at cost. For example,
billions of dollars annually are spent on complementary
and alternative medical (CAM) treatments that either are
untested or have been found to have no meaningful treatment
effects (see Nahin, Barnes, Stussman, & Bloom, 2009; Ernst,
2000). This kind of economic impact, to say nothing of the
personal cost associated with foregoing empirically validated
medical treatments, speaks to the importance of examining
why people believe what they do. The study presented in this
article attempts to add a unique perspective on the issue of
belief in such empirically unsubstantiated claims.
We aim to provide a compelling rationale for investigating
more directly the relationship between paranormal, conspiracy
theory, and pseudoscience beliefs. While some research has
investigated the relationship between two of these types of
beliefs (e.g., Darwin, Neave, & Holmes, 2011; Lewandowsky,
Gignac, & Oberauer, 2013; Lewandowsky, Oberauer, &
Gignac, 2013; Swami et al., 2011), most studies have exam-
ined only one type. To date, we are aware of no study that
has investigated all three. Thus, the primary objective of this
study is to begin to ll that gap in the existing literature. This
effort is also a preliminary investigation into competing theo-
ries from these focused lines of research (e.g., on conspiracy
beliefs) to examine their potential generalizability to other
categories of beliefs (e.g., for paranormal and pseudoscience
beliefs as well).
Denitions
The exact characteristics and denitions of a paranormal be-
lief, a conspiracy theory, or a pseudoscientic theory are by
no means settled matters in science, history, or philosophy.
Conspiracies sometimes do happen (e.g., the Watergate
scandal of the Nixon administration), and sometimes, a once
accepted scientic theory becomes pseudoscientic in light
of subsequent discoveries (e.g., Lamarckian evolution).
However, because it is not in the scope of this paper to
attempt to settle the matter of what precisely a paranormal
claim, a conspiracy theory, or a pseudoscientic theory is,
we use previously proposed denitions of these terms as a
matter of pragmatism. Therefore, for the purposes of this ar-
ticle, paranormal beliefs are dened as beliefs in physical,
biological, or psychological phenomena that feature funda-
mental or core ontological properties of another ontological
category(Lindeman & Aarnio, 2006, p. 586587) where
ontological categoryrefers to a domain level classication
of phenomena as physical, biological, or psychological. A
conspiracy theory belief refers to lay beliefs that attribute
the ultimate cause of an event, or the concealment of an
event from public knowledge, to a secret, unlawful, and
malevolent plot by multiple actors working together
(Swami, Chamorro-Premuzic, & Furnham, 2010, p. 749;
adapted from Zonis & Joseph, 1994). Finally, pseudoscien-
tic beliefs can be conceptualized as cognitions about
material phenomena that claim to be scienceyet use
nonscientic evidentiary processes(Losh, Tavani, Njoroge,
Wilke, & McAuley, 2003; quoted in Losh & Nzekwe,
2011b, p. 473), and we considered science denial to fall
under this category.
Despite their differences, these belief types share funda-
mental characteristics. For one, pseudoscience proponents
make use of appeals to conspiracies to support their position,
and conspiracy theory adherents may appeal to their under-
standing of sciencewhich is often erroneous or incom-
pleteas support for their belief. That is, in some instances,
it can be difcult to tease apart when a pseudoscience
or paranormal claim ends and a conspiracy claim begins
(e.g., climate change denial; see Lewandowsky, Gignac,
*Correspondence to: Emilio Lobato, University of Central Florida, Institute
for Simulation and Training, 3100 Technology Parkway, Orlando, FL
32826, USA.
E-mail: elobato@ist.ucf.edu
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Applied Cognitive Psychology,Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
Published online 2 June 2014 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.3042
et al., 2013; Lewandowsky, Oberauer, et al., 2013). In addi-
tion, although the focus of specic unsubstantiated beliefs
may differ, the rhetorical strategies adherents use to defend
these beliefs appear to be fundamentally similar (see Boudry
& Braeckman, 2011, 2012, for review). Similarly, the con-
cept of epistemic warrant, from the philosophy of science
literature, appears relevant. The epistemic warrant refers to
the totality of evidence and knowledge that is available to
human knowledge seekers at the time in question(Hansson,
2009, p. 239) and is one feature suggested to distinguish
science from pseudoscience. This feature may also serve to
distinguish paranormal claims and conspiracy theories from
empirically supported explanations, as both paranormal and
conspiracy claims quite frequently fail to measure up to the
totality of evidencerelevant to their specic claims. As such,
we will refer to paranormal beliefs, conspiracy theory, and pseu-
doscience beliefs collectively as epistemically unwarranted.
Prior research ndings
Prior research has examined the role of many individual
differences in understanding ones propensity for believing
epistemically unwarranted claims. Taken together, the re-
sults are often conicting. For example, several studies have
shown no relationship between a persons need for cognition
(NFC), that is, the enjoyment in or tendency to engage in
cognitively demanding activities (Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein,
& Jarvis, 1996), and paranormal beliefs (e.g., Genovese,
2005; Yates & Chandler, 2000), whereas other results
suggest that participants with a higher NFC tend to endorse
less paranormal beliefs (e.g., Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli,
Koehler, & Fugelsang, 2012). By comparison, a relationship
between conspiracy theories and NFC has been previously
unsupported (Abalakina-Paap, Stephan, Craig, & Gregory,
1999). Similarly, a persons desire for control over his or
her life has been shown to be related to superstitious
behavior (e.g., Keinan, 2002), and threats to control increase
a persons perceptions of illusionary patterns (e.g., Sullivan,
Landau, & Rothschild, 2010; Whitson & Galinsky, 2008),
whereas other studies suggest that this variable does not
serve a good explanatory role in paranormal and supersti-
tious beliefs (e.g., Lindeman & Aarnio, 2006).
Gender has been examined in many studies of epistemi-
cally unwarranted belief types, and as with the aforemen-
tioned individual differences, the results are inconsistent.
Whereas some ndings have shown that women tend to
endorse medical pseudoscience more than men (e.g., Astin,
1998), other research has shown no relationship between
gender and pseudoscience acceptance (e.g., Johnson &
Pigliucci, 2004). Findings by Lindeman (2011) have shown
that gender accounts for very little of the variance when con-
trolling for another variable, ontological confusion, which
we will describe in the following text. Although most
research on conspiracy beliefs does not nd a gender differ-
ence (e.g., Darwin et al., 2011), some research has shown
slight gender differences within specic populations, such
as the US population (e.g., Bruder, Haffke, Neave,
Nouripanah, & Imhoff, 2013; Swami et al., 2011), with
women marginally more prone to conspiratorial thinking
than men.
In spite of, or perhaps because of, these inconsistent
ndings, it is important to establish the presence or absence
of a relationship between these beliefs. Inconsistent ndings
for individual difference variables may be explained by
small effect sizes, with more substantive relationships found
between the beliefs themselves. For instance, a persons
level of NFC may contribute to initially accepting an episte-
mically unwarranted claim, but once a person does so, that
becomes a less important factor in accepting or rejecting sub-
sequent unwarranted claims. Perhaps at that point, what is
important is the existing acceptance of an epistemically
unwarranted claim. Fortunately, there are several existing
theoretical frameworks that have been put forward to explain
various epistemically unwarranted belief systems.
One perspective comes from research examining the rela-
tionship between paranormal and religious beliefs that shows
people who believe in one type tend to reject the other type
of belief (Emmons & Sobal, 1981; Hergovich, Schott, &
Arendasy, 2005; Persinger & Makarec, 1990; Rice, 2003).
This pattern of results is described in terms of a substitution
effect, which states that classical paranormal beliefs and tra-
ditional religious beliefs are competing sets of beliefs and
that acceptance of one reduces the likelihood of acceptance
for the other (Rice, 2003). As such, one of our goals is to
examine if there is a substitution effect that extends beyond
examining paranormal and religious beliefs.
Some research ndings provide suggestive evidence of a
substitution effect between epistemically unwarranted belief
types. For example, Losh and Nzekwe (2011a) surveyed a
sample of the US pre-service education majors in order to
investigate the relationship between their views on the
scientic theory of evolution, pseudoscientic theories
(e.g., Creationism and Intelligent Design), and paranormal
claims. Results of their study indicated that the majority of
pre-service educators accepted the pseudoscience theories
and rejected many paranormal claims such as extraterres-
trials or magic, although sizeable minorities were undecided
about other paranormal claims, such as the existence of
ghosts and communion with the dead. These ndings sug-
gest that at least paranormal and pseudoscientic beliefs
may show a substitution effect.
From the literature on conspiracy theories, research has
shown a different pattern of belief. Conspiracy theory be-
lievers will tend to readily accept any conspiracy theory
presented to them. As described by Goertzel (1994), these
conspiracy theory believers are monological thinkers;
believers take each conspiracy belief as evidence for other
conspiracies. The more conspiracies a monological thinker
believes in, the more likely he or she is to believe in any
new conspiracy theory which may be proposed(p. 740). In-
vestigations by Swami and colleagues (Swami et al., 2010)
provide conrmatory evidence of that suggestion, even for
ctitious conspiracies (Swami et al., 2011). Another investi-
gation has found that this is the case even for conspiracy
theories that come to mutually exclusive conclusions (Wood,
Douglas, & Sutton, 2012).
This second theoretical position of monological thinking
can be applied to the research that has been carried out exam-
ining multiple types of epistemically unwarranted beliefs.
Recent research efforts have found a positive relationship
618 E. Lobato et al.
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
between conspiracy theory and rejection of well-established
science (Lewandowsky, Gignac, et al., 2013; Lewandowsky,
Oberauer, et al., 2013) and between conspiracy theory and
paranormal acceptance (Darwin et al., 2011; Swami et al.,
2011). Finally, paranormal beliefs have been found to be
related to beliefs in pseudoscientic claims (Eder, Turic,
Milasowszky, Van Adzin, & Herogivch, 2011; Lindeman,
2011). Although monological thinking was not necessarily
considered as an explanatory factor between the epistemi-
cally unwarranted types of beliefs in these studies, the nd-
ings can nonetheless be interpreted as providing suggestive
evidence that monological thinking may be a contributing
factor. This study serves as an initial examination into the
possibility that monological thinking applies across these
three types of epistemically unwarranted beliefs by investi-
gating whether they are positively correlated with each other.
Related to, though distinct from, monological thinking is
the recent suggestion from the literature on paranormal
beliefs that ontological knowledge provides a coherent frame-
work for understanding why some people believe fantastic
claims. Lindeman and her colleagues (Lindeman & Aarnio,
2007; Lindeman & Saher, 2007; Lindeman, Svedholm,
Takada, Lönnqvist, & Verkasalo, 2011; Svedholm, Lindeman,
& Lipsanen, 2010) have conducted a series of studies into the
relationship between paranormal beliefs, CAM beliefs, and the
concept of core ontological knowledge. Here, core ontological
knowledge refers to a persons understanding of the world
along three broad yet distinct categories: physical, biological,
and psychological. These core ontologies are learned without
necessary formal instruction and provide a foundation for
understanding evolutionarily important entities and events
(Lindeman et al., 2011). Findings from these studies show that
believers in paranormal or CAM claims fail to accurately
distinguish between physical, biological, and psychological
domains as separate ontological categories. Instead, believers
may rely on naïve conceptualizations of these domains, which
result in ontological confusions such as considering lifeless en-
tities as having attributes unique to living entities. The re-
searchers interpreted their ndings within the context of dual
process theories of cognitive psychology (see Epstein, Pacini,
Denes Raj, & Heier, 1996; Evans, 2003). Dual process
theories propose that humans engage in two types of cognitive
processing of information. One type of processing is character-
ized by intuitive and autonomous processing of information,
whereas the other type of processing is analytical and character-
ized by the ability to cognitively decouple sensory representa-
tions (Stanovich & Toplak, 2012). Naïve or folk physics,
biology, and psychology may reect intuitive information
processing, inuencing a persons propensity to believe in a par-
ticular claim regardless of its epistemic value. Several of these
studies have also found that women tend to be more inclined to-
ward this intuitive cognitive process than men (e.g., Aarnio
& Lindeman, 2005; Lindeman, 2011), which may help
explain why some studies nd that women endorse more para-
normal (e.g., Aarnio & Lindeman, 2005) and, occasionally, con-
spiracy beliefs (e.g., Swami et al., 2011). We thus investigated
the possibility that the tendency to make ontological confusions
is related to epistemically unwarranted beliefs generally, as
opposed to just paranormal beliefs or CAM beliefs as previously
reported (e.g., Lindeman, 2011; Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007).
Research question and design
The research questions that guided this study were as
follows: rst, is there a signicant correlation in accepting
paranormal beliefs, conspiracy theories, and pseudoscience?
and second, do any of the theories described in the preceding
section apply generally to accepting epistemically
unwarranted beliefs? To address these questions, we asked
the participants to ll out a novel questionnaire asking about
acceptance of epistemically unwarranted claims as well as
multiple individual difference measures (see Section 2.2).
METHODS
Participants
Participants were 480 undergraduate students at a large uni-
versity in the southeastern USA recruited through an online
recruitment process offered by the campus Psychology
Department. They were given course credit for participation.
Twenty-ve response sets (5.2%) were not analyzed because
of incomplete data or the same IP address being recorded for
multiple response sets. The nal sample consists of 455 re-
sponses (296 female participants; M
age
= 21.0, SD
age
= 5.6).
On the basis of the demographic information provided,
religious afliation was recoded into two categories: non-
religious participants and Christian participants. Other reli-
gious afliations accounted for a too small percentage of
the sample to be considered separately. Non-religious partic-
ipants were considered any participant who reported being
agnostic, or atheist, or specically mentioned no religion in
their response. This accounted for 125 response sets.
Christian participants were considered any participant who
reported being Catholic, Protestant, Christian (either generi-
cally or non-denominational), or some denomination of
Christianity (e.g., Baptist and Methodist). This accounted
for 278 response sets.
Measures
The following measures were used to assess individual
differences that might be predictive of belief endorsement.
1
Need for cognition
An 18-item scale that measured participantsengagement
and enjoyment of cognitive activities was used (α= .91;
Cacioppo et al., 1996). Participants responded on a ve-
point scale on how much each item is a characteristic of them
(1 = extremely uncharacteristic of you and 5 = extremely
characteristic of you). An example of the items included
on this scale is I would prefer complex to simple problems.
1
One additional scale was administered in this study, the Need for Emotion
Scale, a 12-item scale that measures whether participants seek out and enjoy
emotionally evocative stimuli (Raman, Chattopadhyay, & Hoyer, 1995).
This was included to examine whether a propensity toward accepting episte-
mically unwarranted beliefs is related to seeking out emotionally charged
stimuli. However, this scale was not found to be signicantly related to
the paranormal, conspiracy, or pseudoscience subscales of the Belief survey
with any analysis technique used in this study. As such, we have elected to
not include it in the main text, but we report its presence in the study here for
the sake of completeness.
Relationship between unwarranted beliefs 619
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
Desirability of control (DOC)
A scale was used to measure the participantsdesire for con-
trol over their life, which consisted of 20 statements (α= .79;
Burger & Cooper, 1979). Participants indicated on a seven-
point scale the degree to which the statement applies to them
(1 = the statement doesnt apply to me at all and 7 = the
statement always applies to me). An example of the items
included on this scale is I prefer a job where I have a lot
of control over what I do and when I do it.
Mini-IPIP
Personality was measured using the Mini-International
Personality Item Pool (Mini-IPIP; Donnellan, Oswald, Baird,
& Lucas, 2006), an abbreviated version of the Five-Factor In-
ventory (McCrae & Costa, 1987). The Mini-IPIP consisted of
20 statements, four for each factor of personality: extraver-
sion, conscientiousness, agreeableness, openness, and neu-
roticism (α= .85, .72, .70, .69, and .70 for each factor,
respectively). Participants indicated on a ve-point scale to
what degree they agree or disagree with each statement
(1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree).
Core knowledge confusion (CORE)
Ontological confusion was examined using an English
version of the CORE scale developed by Lindeman and col-
leagues (α= .93; Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007; Lindeman et al.,
2011). This was a 30-item scale examining different varieties
of core ontological confusions (e.g., Natural, lifeless objects
are living; articial objects are animate). Participants were
presented with a statement representing one of the types of
core ontological confusions and were asked to determine
whether the statement was literally true or not literally true.
An example of statements in this scale is Stars live in the
sky, which describes a physical entity (i.e., stars) as
possessing a biological property (i.e., life). Eight ller items
were included in the scale.
A novel 37-item questionnaire investigating acceptance of
epistemically unwarranted beliefs was created for this study
(see Table 1; hereafter, Belief survey). Many items used in
this questionnaire were adopted from other scales, such as
the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (Tobacyk, 2004), and
studies on conspiracy theory acceptance such as Goertzel
(1994) and Abalakina-Paap et al. (1999). A few additional
items not previously investigated in the existing literature
were included. All items were a priori dened as paranor-
mal, conspiracy, or pseudoscientic. Participants were
instructed to read each statement and select on a ve-point
scale how much they agree or disagree with the claim
(1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree). For analysis,
scores were recoded such that a higher score reected a
greater rejection of the epistemically unwarranted belief.
Procedure
The entire procedure was conducted online, using the survey
system Qualtrics (www.qualtrics.com). Participants were
instructed that they were participating in a study examining
the relationship between personality and beliefs. Participants
rst lled out the Belief survey. Items on the Belief survey
were presented in random order. Then, participants lled
out the NFC, DOC, and Mini-IPIP scales. These scales were
presented in random order. Instructions for each scale were
provided at the top of each web page. Finally, participants
lled out a demographic questionnaire.
RESULTS
Cronbachs alpha for the Belief survey and the three sub-
scales was calculated to assess reliability. The reliability of
the whole scale was .87. Reliabilities for the subscales were
.86 for paranormal beliefs, .67 for conspiracy beliefs, and .76
for pseudoscience beliefs. Mean scores and standard devia-
tions for individual items are shown in Table 1. The overall
mean score was 3.64 (SD = 0.45). For each subscale, the
mean scores were 3.59 (SD = 0.66) for paranormal items,
3.35 (SD = 0.45) for conspiracy items, and 3.43 (SD = 0.50)
for pseudoscience items.
Correlations between beliefs
Pearson product moment correlations showed that beliefs in
paranormal, conspiracy, and pseudoscientic claims all were
moderately to strongly positively correlated, as shown in
Table 2. Overall, those who endorsed one type of epistemi-
cally unwarranted belief tended to endorse other types.
Demographic differences
A 2 (gender: male and female) × 2 (religious afliation:
Christian or non-religious) between-subjects Analysis of Vari-
ance (ANOVA) on participantsscores on the Belief survey re-
vealed a signicant main effect for gender [F(1, 398) = 22.52,
p<.001, η
p
2
= .05] and religious afliation [F(1, 398) = 55.20,
p<.001, η
p
2
= .12]. These effects were not qualied by an
interaction between gender and religious afliation
[F(1, 398) = 1.96, p=.162, η
p
2
= .01]. The mean response for
male participants (M=3.86, SD = 0.04) on the Belief scale
was higher than the mean response for female participants
(M= 3.64, SD = 0.03). The mean response for participants
who reported a Christian afliation (M=3.58, SD = 0.03)
was lower than the mean response for participants who
reported no religious afliation (M=3.92, SD = 0.04).
We subsequently ran a 2 (gender: male and female) × 2
(religious afliation: Christian or non-religious) between-
subjects Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) to
examine demographic differences in scores on the paranormal,
conspiracy, and pseudoscience subscales of the Belief scale.
Results again showed a signicant main effect for gender
[F(3, 446) = 10.05, p<.001, η
p
2
= .06] and a signicant main
effect for religious afliation [F(3, 446) = 38.95, p<.000,
η
p
2
= .21] but no signicant interaction effect [F(3,
446) = 1.81, p= .145, η
p
2
= .01]. For the male participants,
mean responses were higher than those for the female
participants (see Figure 1), and the mean responses for
non-religious participants were higher than the mean re-
sponses for the Christian participants (see Figure 2). This
indicates that men relative to women and non-religious
participants relative to Christian participants were more
likely to reject epistemically unwarranted beliefs.
620 E. Lobato et al.
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
Predictors of epistemically unwarranted beliefs
Hierarchical multiple linear regression on the Belief scale
was performed with 10 individual difference variables:
gender, religious afliation, NFC, DOC, CORE, and the ve
mini-IPIP subscales (see Table 3). For the mean score on the
Belief scale, the overall model was signicant, accounting
for 36.2% of the variance [F(10, 401) = 22.17, p<.001].
Gender, religious afliation, NFC, DOC, CORE, extraver-
sion, and neuroticism were signicant predictors. Overall,
being male (r=.25), having no religious afliation
(r=.36), having a higher NFC (r= .25) and greater DOC
Table 1. Belief survey items with mean (standard deviation) scores and endorsement rate
Item
Mean score
(SD)
Endorsement rate
(%)
b
1. The variety of species of life that exist today is best explained
by the scientic theory of evolution. (S)
3.67 (1.23) 63.3
2. The earth is experiencing a period of global warming that is caused
by many factors including human activities. (S)
3.89 (1.06) 74.0
3. Childhood vaccines are one causal factor in the development of autism.
a
(S) 3.35 (0.99) 41.5
4. Human immunodeciency virus (HIV) later develops into acquired
immunodeciency syndrome (AIDS). (S)
4.24 (0.91) 82.0
5. A person chooses to be homosexual, bisexual, or heterosexual.
a
(S) 3.44 (1.33) 53.4
6. US astronauts have been to the moon. (C) 4.45 (0.79) 90.4
7. Members of the US government were involved in the planning and execution
of the events that happened on 11 September 2001.
a
(C)
3.90 (1.05) 67.1
8. President John F. Kennedy was assassinated by Lee Harvey Oswald,
who acted alone. (C)
3.40 (1.04) 50.1
9. Members of the US government were involved in a conspiracy to
assassinate Martin Luther King, Jr.
a
(C)
3.59 (1.04) 52.9
10. Creatures known popularly as Big Foot, the Loch Ness Monster, and/or
the Chupacabra exist.
a
(P)
3.84 (1.08) 67.0
11. Extraterrestrial life forms have visited Earth and abducted human beings.
a
(P) 3.70 (1.07) 58.0
12. The age of the earth is approximately 4.3 billion years. (S) 3.59 (1.13) 57.4
13. The shape of the earth is roughly a sphere. (S) 4.22 (0.97) 84.8
14. After people die, they still interact with the living as ghosts.
a
(P) 3.39 (1.17) 46.6
15. Certain objects, such as rabbitsfeet and four-leafed clovers, genuinely bring good luck.
a
(P) 3.89 (1.04) 69.0
16. Homeopathic treatments are just as valid as traditional medical treatments for
serious illnesses.
a
(S)
3.22 (1.01) 37.8
17. Some humans have special mental abilities, such as the ability to move objects using only
mental force or to predict the future with perfect accuracy.
a
(P)
3.95 (1.05) 70.5
18. Prayer is effective at treating people with terminal diseases.
a
(S) 2.95 (1.31) 34.1
19. Reiki healing, otherwise known as Palm healing, is effective in healing the body.
a
(S) 3.74 (0.97) 57.6
20. The Holocaust of the 1930s and 1940s was responsible for the deaths of over 11 million people,
of whom six million were Jewish. (C)
4.54 (.78) 92.1
21. President Barack H. Obama is a native born US citizen. (C) 3.87 (1.14) 67.5
22. Most human beings only use approximately 10% of their brain.
a
(S) 2.88 (1.19) 38.4
23. The beginning of the universe is best explained by the Big Bang Theory. (S) 3.13 (1.29) 42.8
24. Human beings have souls that continue to exist after the body dies.
a
(P) 2.44 (1.21) 18.5
25. A full moon causes people to behave oddly.
a
(P) 3.49 (1.22) 55.4
26. Astrology is a valid explanation for the behaviors and personality of people.
a
(P) 3.85 (1.06) 67.2
27. The Bermuda Triangle is a special location on the planet that, for some reason, causes ships
and aircraft to crash or disappear more often than anywhere else on the planet.
a
(P)
2.89 (1.16) 30.5
28. Deceased individuals communicate with living people through séances or Oijua boards.
a
(P) 3.82 (1.11) 61.7
29. Black cats are unlucky.
a
(P) 4.07 (1.04) 77.1
30. The musician Elvis Presley is dead. (C) 4.59 (0.70) 84.7
31. The singer John Lennon was the target of an assassination conspiracy.
a
(C) 3.14 (0.95) 29.2
32. The rappers Tupac Shakur and Biggie Smalls were victims of assassination conspiracies,
which were covered up.
a
(C)
3.09 (1.05) 30.5
33. The singer Kurt Cobain was murdered by his wife, singer Courtney Love.
a
(C) 3.40 (0.90) 36.7
34. Lost city of Atlantis is a myth or a legend. (P) 3.36 (1.06) 46.3
35. Some numbers and dates are more lucky or unlucky than others, such as Friday the 13th
or the number 7.
a
(P)
3.75 (1.14) 63.3
36. Condoms used properly during sexual intercourse are very effective at preventing the spread
of sexual transmitted diseases and pregnancy. (S)
4.18 (0.97) 83.5
37. Geological objects, such as certain crystals, precious metals, or magnets, have intrinsic
mystical properties.
a
(P)
3.77 (1.09) 63.3
Note:
a
Denotes an item that was reverse coded, such that higher scores indicate a less epistemically unwarranted belief structure. Endorsement ratefor these
items should be considered rejection rate.
b
Calculated by summing the participants who responded with strongly agreeor agree.
(P), Paranormal; (C), Conspiracy theory; (S), Pseudoscience; SD, standard deviation.
Statements were presented to participants in random order.
Relationship between unwarranted beliefs 621
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
(r= .17), making fewer ontological confusions (r=.44), and
being less extraverted (r=.13) and less neurotic (r=.19)
were associated with a less epistemically unwarranted belief
structure, as measured by the Belief scale.
Hierarchical multiple linear regression analyses were
conducted for each of the three subscales. For these analyses,
in line with our hypotheses about the importance of ontolog-
ical knowledge and believing in other types of epistemically
unwarranted beliefs, the other two Belief survey subscales
and CORE were entered as independent variables in step 1,
and the other individual difference variables were entered
as independent variables in step 2. Results are presented in
Table 4. CORE and the conspiracy and pseudoscience sub-
scales were the most important predictors of the paranormal
beliefs subscale. In step 1, these variables contributed signif-
icantly to paranormal beliefs and predicted 34.9% of the
variance [F(3, 448) = 80.03, p<.001]. The other eight inde-
pendent variables in step 2 increased prediction of paranor-
mal beliefs by 5.2% [F(8, 439) = 24.29, p<.001]. All inde-
pendent variables predicted 40.1% of the variation in belief
in paranormal phenomena. CORE and the paranormal and
pseudoscience subscales were the most important predictors
of the conspiracy belief subscale. In step 1, 37.9% of the var-
iance in conspiracy theory beliefs was accounted for by these
three variables [F(3, 448) = 91.15, p<.001]. The other eight
independent variables in step 2 increased the predicted vari-
ance by an additional 2.6% [F(8, 439) = 24.50, p<.001].
Together, all independent variables predicted 40.5% of the
variance in belief in conspiracy theories. For the pseudosci-
ence subscale, CORE and the paranormal and conspiracy
subscales entered in step 1 accounted for 25.3% of the vari-
ance [F(3, 448) = 50.53, p<.001]. The other eight variables
in step 2 increased the predicted variance by an additional
14.8% [F(8, 439) = 24.47, p<.001]. Thus, all independent
variables explained 40.1% of the variance in pseudoscientic
beliefs and science denial.
DISCUSSION
This study represents a preliminary investigation into the
relationship between beliefs in different epistemically
unwarranted claims, and as such, the results are only sugges-
tive. Results suggest that substitution theory is insufcient to
account for the pattern of beliefs found in this study. Instead,
our results showed moderate to strong positive correlations
between these three categories of epistemically unwarranted
beliefs. Participants who endorsed one type of epistemically
unwarranted belief also tended to endorse others. Although
previous research has found evidence of a positive rela-
tionship between conspiracy ideation and science denial
(Lewandowsky, Gignac, et al., 2013; Lewandowsky, Oberauer,
et al., 2013), between paranormal belief and pseudoscientic
claims (Eder et al., 2011; Lindeman, 2011), and between con-
spiracy and paranormal beliefs (Swami et al., 2010, 2011),
the study presented here is the rst one to present results indi-
cating a positive correlation between believing in conspiracy
theories, the paranormal, and the pseudoscience.
More important, our results also extend ndings regarding
core ontological knowledge and epistemically unwarranted
Table 2. Correlations among the different types of beliefs
Paranormal Conspiracy Pseudoscience
Paranormal .52 .36
Conspiracy .49
Pseudoscience
Note:
N= 455.
All correlations signicant at p<.001 level.
Figure 1. Mean scores for male and female participants
Figure 2. Mean scores for Christian and non-religious participants
Table 3. Predictors of epistemically unwarranted beliefs
Belief survey
Predictors βt
Gender .13 2.92**
RA .24 5.47***
NFC .12 2.46*
DOC .12 2.42*
CORE .35 8.33***
Extraversion .15 3.23**
Agreeableness .08 1.74
Conscientiousness .05 1.19
Openness .07 1.58
Neuroticism .11 2.50*
Note:N= 455.
*p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.
CORE, core ontological confusions; RA, religious afliation;
NFC, need for cognition; DOC, desirability of control.
Gender coded such that 1 = male and 2 = female.
Religious afliation coded such that 1 = non-religious and 2 = Christian.
622 E. Lobato et al.
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
beliefs. Prior research has found that believing in more core
ontological confusions is related to a greater belief in para-
normal claims (Lindeman et al., 2008) and specic instances
of pseudoscience found in CAM (Lindeman, 2011). Our
results suggest a similar relationship between ontological
confusions and conspiracy theory beliefs. This could suggest
that the underlying structure of these types of belief is simi-
lar, in spite of supercial differences in the content of each
belief. That is, conspiracy theories may reect an ontologi-
cally confused conception of reality, as paranormal beliefs
do. Such theories typically attribute malevolent intentional
agency as direct causal factors for events, despite no evidence
to support such an attribution. Alternatively, it could suggest
a common cognitive style across believers. That is, a greater
propensity toward intuitive thinking may be common to both
paranormal believers and conspiracy theorists. Ontological
confusions have been shown to be strongly related to a
greater predisposition toward an intuitive cognitive style
(Lindeman, 2011; Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007; Svedholm
et al., 2010). A greater predisposition toward intuitive cogni-
tion may open up individuals for believing in a diverse set of
unsubstantiated claims.
However, for the pseudoscience subscale, the measure of
ontological confusion was not found to be a statistically
signicant predictor variable. Pseudoscience and science de-
nial beliefs may be less likely to be a direct manifestation of
ontological confusion, despite pseudoscience belief being
positively correlated with beliefs (see also Eder et al., 2011;
Lewandowsky, Gignac, et al., 2013; Lewandowsky, Oberauer,
et al., 2013). Pseudoscience and science denial may show the
level of overlap with paranormal and conspiracy beliefs found
in this study because the scientic community has a history of
consistently rebuking paranormal claims and conspiracy theo-
ries that invoke poorly understood science. Beyond individual
differences in ontological knowledge, the other individual
difference variables appear to play a statistically signicant
but small explanatory role in understanding why people may
accept an epistemically unwarranted claim. The small amount
of variance explained by these other individual difference vari-
ables may provide some explanation for the conicting results
found in prior studies. The effects of variables such as gender,
the NFC, desire for control, or differences in personality may
simply be too small to consistently observe, whereas the
effects of the measure of core ontological confusions were
robust. The only exception to this was for religious afliation,
which contributed sizable variance for predicting pseudosci-
ence acceptance. This is most likely due to the nature of several
specic items in the pseudoscience subscale of the Belief sur-
vey, such as those asking about belief in evolutionary theory,
the age of the earth, or etiology of a persons sexual orienta-
tion. In the modern USA, at least, there is a strong connection
between ones personal beliefs about these issues and onesre-
ligious commitments. Although much has been published in
the last decade of the relationship between religious afliation
and rejection of specicscientic concepts, such as evolution
(e.g.,Baker, 2013; Lombrozo, Thanukos, & Weisberg, 2008;
Miller, Scott, & Okamoto, 2006), this study nds that the rela-
tionship holds true for other scientic conclusions. However,
the only two broad religious perspectives that could be
compared in this study were Christians and non-religious par-
ticipants. Follow-up research with a more religiously diverse
sample is necessary to conrm this as a general conclusion,
however.
The present study is also limited in that it recruited a
university student sample. As such, the results may not gen-
eralize to the general population. Given the relatively high
rate of endorsement for unwarranted claims in the general
population (see National Science Foundation, 2006 for rates
Table 4. Predictors of epistemically unwarranted beliefs at the subscale level
Paranormal Conspiracy Pseudoscience
Predictors βtβtβt
Step 1
CORE .28 6.77*** .11 2.58** .06 1.37
Paranormal .35 8.20*** .12 2.48*
Conspiracy .37 8.20*** .40 8.24***
Pseudoscience .11 2.48* .33 8.24***
Step 2
Gender .10 2.34* .03 .68 .07 1.68
RA .05 1.03 .07 1.58 .34 8.51***
NFC .12 2.72* .03 .69 .06 1.37
DOC .06 1.26 .10 2.13* .10 2.13
Extraversion .03 .79 .10 2.46* .01 .30
Agreeableness .00 .03 .02 .56 .05 1.29
Conscientiousness .07 1.81 .05 1.19 .07 1.63
Openness .08 1.92 .05 1.23 .03 .76
Neuroticism .12 4.01*** .00 .08 .01 .32
Note: N = 455.
*p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.
CORE, core ontological confusions; RA, religious afliation;
NFC, need for cognition; DOC, desirability of control.
Gender coded such that 1 = male and 2 = female.
Religious afliation coded such that 1 = non-religious and 2 = Christian.
Relationship between unwarranted beliefs 623
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
of pseudoscience acceptance), the mean score on the Belief
scale obtained in this study may not accurately reect how
the general population would respond to these items. How-
ever, because one of the roles of education is to impart
knowledge onto students and correct their misinterpretations
in an effort to prepare them for successfully navigating the
world, it is still important to understand college students
who subscribe to epistemically unwarranted beliefs.
This study has several additional limitations that should be
noted. The Belief survey is an ad hoc measure of specic
paranormal, conspiracy, and pseudoscience beliefs and not
a measure of a general predisposition toward paranormal,
conspiratorial, or pseudoscientic thinking. This may have
contributed to several specicndings in this study, such
as the amount of variance for pseudoscience beliefs
explained by religious afliation, or the strength of the corre-
lations between the different types of epistemically
unwarranted beliefs. As noted in the Introduction, the precise
boundary between a pseudoscientic belief, a conspiracy
theory, and a paranormal claim is fuzzy. Although we
attempted to word our Belief survey questions in such a
way that highlighted the paranormal, pseudoscientic, or
conspiracy overtones of a given belief (e.g., extraterrestrial
life forms have visited Earth and abducted human beings
emphasizes the paranormal component of the existence of
extraterrestrial life and not any associated conspiracy ele-
ments, such as assumptions of government cover-ups of
alien landings), we cannot say for certain how tightly these
various elements for these specic beliefs are linked in the
minds of believers.
Although ad hoc measures such as our Belief survey are
the predominant technique for researching peoplesaccep-
tance of these categories of belief, future efforts should focus
on the development of measures for a general predisposition
toward paranormal, conspiracy, and pseudoscience thinking.
Recent investigations into conspiracy beliefs have begun to
do so with the development of a Conspiracy Mentality
Questionnaire (Bruder et al., 2013) and the Generic Conspir-
acist Beliefs scale (Brotherton, French, & Pickering, 2013).
Designing similar measures for generic paranormal and pseu-
doscientic thinking would be a useful research endeavor.
CONCLUSION
This study provides evidence for the idea that there is signif-
icant overlap between believers in paranormal, conspiracy,
and pseudoscience claims. A measure of core ontological
confusions was found to be predictive of an individuals ten-
dency to subscribe to paranormal and conspiracy-oriented
epistemically unwarranted beliefs. This provides an opportu-
nity for educators and public policy makers to come together
and develop new methods for correcting such mistaken be-
liefs in a new light. For instance, developing school curricula
that directly address ontological knowledge at an earlier age
may help students grow to better appreciate the distinctions
between the physical, biological, and psychological domains
and complement critical thinking skills aimed at protecting
people from believing the unbelievable. In addition, the nd-
ings from this study provide support for the position that
research into beliefs that are paranormal, conspiratorial, or
pseudoscientic should consider approaching these topics
as manifestations of similar intuitive cognitions, namely,
naïve ontological conceptions. Although there are certain
differences between these beliefs, mainly at the supercial
level (i.e., the content of the specic belief), this study
provides evidence that they share an underlying cognitive
foundation, warranting further investigation.
As a nal point, several of the beliefs in the current study
are often considered silly or inconsequential. However, the
present results add to a growing literature that indicates that
this view of certain epistemically unwarranted beliefs as in-
consequential is not tenable. As just one example, there are
real costs associated with foregoing traditional and empiri-
cally validated medical treatments in favor of CAM treat-
ments, from both health and economic perspectives and both
personal and societal levels (Ernst, 2000). A greater under-
standing of how these paranormal, conspiracy, and pseudo-
science beliefs are related is certainly warranted. If people
believing in lucky rabbitsfeet are more likely to also
endorse CAM practices, a better understanding of the rela-
tionship between these beliefs is needed in order to develop
public policy and educational practices aimed at correcting
such mistaken beliefs. Being able to correct peoples
misconceptions about how reality operates with regard to such
silly beliefs as lucky charms may also serve to protect them
against buying into sham treatments such as homeopathy.
REFERENCES
Aarnio, K., & Lindeman, M. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, education, and
thinking styles. Personality and Individual Differences,39, 12271236.
Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999).
Belief in conspiracies. Political Psychology,20, 637647.
Astin, J. A. (1998). Why patients use alternative medicine: Results of a na-
tional study. JAMA,279(19), 15481553.
Baker, J. O. (2013). Acceptance of evolution and support for teaching crea-
tionism in public schools: The conditional impact of educational attain-
ment. Journal for the Scientic Study of Religion,52(1), 216228.
Boudry, M., & Braeckman, J. (2011). Immunizing strategies and epistemic
defense mechanisms. Philosophia,39, 145161.
Boudry, M., & Braeckman, J. (2012). How convenient! The epistemic
rationale of self-validating belief systems. Philosophical Psychology,
25, 341364.
Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief
in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in
Psychology,4, 279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279
Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013).
Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories
across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in
Psychology,4, 225. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225
Burger, J. M., & Cooper, H. M. (1979). The desirability of control. Motiva-
tion and Emotion,3, 381393.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996).
Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of
individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin,
119(2), 197253.
Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories:
The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation, and schizotypy. Person-
ality and Individual Differences,50, 12891293.
Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006). The
mini-IPIP scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the big ve factors of
personality. Psychological Assessment,18, 192203.
624 E. Lobato et al.
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
Eder, E., Turic, K., Milasowszky, N., Van Adzin, K., & Herogivch, A.
(2011). The relationship between paranormal belief, creationism, intelli-
gent design, and evolution at secondary schools in Vienna (Austria).
Science & Education,20, 517534.
Emmons, C. F., & Sobal, J. (1981). Paranormal beliefs: Functional alterna-
tives to mainstream religion? Review of Religious Research,22, 301312.
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual
differences in intuitive-experiential and analytic-rational thinking styles.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,71, 390405.
Ernst, E. (2000). The role of complementary and alternative medicine.
BMJ [British Medical Journal],321(7269), 11331135.
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning.
Trends in Cognitive Science,7, 454459.
Genovese, J. E. (2005). Paranormal beliefs, schizotypy, and thinking styles
among teachers and future teachers. Personality and Individual
Differences,39(1), 93102.
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology,15,
731742.
Hansson, S. O. (2009). Cutting the Gordian knot of demarcation. Interna-
tional Studies in the Philosophy of Science,23, 237243.
Hergovich, A., Schott, R., & Arendasy, M. (2005). Paranormal belief and re-
ligiosity. The Journal of Parapsychology,69, 293304.
Johnson, M., & Pigliucci, M. (2004). Is knowledge of science associated
with higher skepticism of pseudoscientic claims? The American Biology
Teacher,66(8), 536548.
Keinan, G. (2002). The effects of stress and desire for control on
superstitious behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28(1), 102108.
Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Oberauer, K. (2013). The role of con-
spiracist ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PLoS
One,8(10), e75637.
Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., & Gignac, G. (2013). NASA faked the moon
landing therefore (climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the motivated
rejection of science. Psychological Science,24(5), 622633.
Lindeman, M. (2011). Biases in intuitive reasoning and belief in comple-
mentary and alternative medicine. Psychology and Health,26, 371382.
Lindeman, M., & Aarnio, K. (2006). Paranormal beliefs: Their dimensional-
ity and correlates. European Journal of Personality,20, 585602.
Lindeman, M., & Aarnio, K. (2007). Superstitious, magical, and paranormal
beliefs: An integrative model. Journal of Research in Personality,41(4),
731744.
Lindeman, M., & Saher, M. (2007). Vitalism, purpose, and superstition.
British Journal of Psychology,98,3344.
Lindeman, M., Cederström, S., Simola, P., Simula, A., Ollikainen, S., &
Riekki, T. (2008). Sentences with core knowledge violations increase
the size of N400 among paranormal beliefs. Cortex,44, 13071315.
Lindeman, M., Svedholm, A. M., Takada, M., Lönnqvist, J., & Verkasalo,
M. (2011). Core knowledge confusion among university students.
Science & Education,20, 439451.
Lombrozo, T., Thanukos, A., & Weisberg, M. (2008). The importance of
understanding the nature of science for accepting evolution. Evolution:
Education and Outreach,1(3), 290298.
Losh, S. C., & Nzekwe, B. (2011a). Creatures in the classroom: Preservice
teacher beliefs about fantastic beasts, magic, extraterrestrials, evolution,
and creation. Science & Education,20(5), 473489.
Losh, S. C., & Nzekwe, B. (2011b). The inuence of education major: How
diverse preservice teachers view pseudoscience topics. Journal of Science
Education and Technology,20, 579591.
Losh, S. C., Tavani, C. M., Njoroge, R., Wilke, R., & McAuley, M. (2003).
What does education really do? Educational dimensions and pseudosci-
ence support in the American general public, 1979-2001. The Skeptical
Inquirer,27,3035.
Nahin, R. L., Barnes, P. M., Stussman, B. J., & Bloom, B. (2009). Costs of
complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) and frequency of visits
to CAM practitioners: United States, 2007. National Health Statistics
Report,18,114.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the ve-factor model of
personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,52(1), 8190.
Miller, J. D., Scott, E. C., & Okamoto, S. (2006). Public acceptance of
evolution. Science,313(5788), 765766.
National Science Foundation. (2006). Science and engineering: Indicators
2006. Science and technology: Public attitudes and public understanding.
Retrieved from: http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/seind06/c7/c7s2.htm
(accessed May 29, 2008)
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A.
(2012). Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief.
Cognition,123, 335346.
Persinger, M. A., & Makarec, K. (1990). Exotic beliefs may be substitutes
for religious beliefs. Perceptual and Motor Skills,71,1618.
Raman, N. V., Chattopadhyay, P., & Hoyer, W. D. (1995). Do consumers
seek emotional situations: The need for emotion scale. Advances in
Consumer Research,22, 537542.
Rice, T. W. (2003). Believe it or not: Religious and other paranormal beliefs
in the United States. Journal for the Scientic Study of Religion,42,
95106.
Stanovich, K. E., & Toplak, M. E. (2012). Dening features versus inciden-
tal correlates of type 1 and type 2 processing. Mind & Society,11,313.
Sullivan, D., Landau, M. J., & Rothschild, Z. K. (2010). An existential func-
tion of enemyship: Evidence that people attribute inuence to personal
and political enemies to compensate for threats to control. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,98(3), 434.
Svedholm, A. M., Lindeman, M., & Lipsanen, J. (2010). Believing in the
purpose of events: Why does it occur and is it supernatural? Applied
Cognitive Psychology,24, 252265.
Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered
questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual
difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracy beliefs. Applied Cognitive
Psychology,24, 749761.
Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., &
Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria:
Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between
individual psychological differences and real-world and ctitious
conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology,120, 443463.
Tobacyk, J. J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. The International
Journal of Transpersonal Studies,23,9498.
Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory
pattern perception. Science,322(5898), 115117.
Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012). Dead and alive:
Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and
Personality Science,3(6), 767773.
Yates, G. C. R., & Chandler, M. (2000). Where have all the skeptics gone?:
Patterns of new age beliefs and anti-scientic attitudes in preservice
primary teachers. Research in Science Education,30(4), 377387.
Zonis, M., & Joseph, C. M. (1994). Conspiracy thinking in the Middle East.
Political Psychology,15, 443459.
Relationship between unwarranted beliefs 625
Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28: 617625 (2014)
... The aim of this thesis is to study whether this phenomenon could be responsible for the emergence of erroneous beliefs in our daily lives. The term epistemically unwarranted beliefs refers to those beliefs that are not founded on reliable reasoning or credible data (Lobato et al., 2014). Following Lobato et al. (2014), these beliefs might be classified as paranormal, conspiracy, pseudoscientific beliefs and science denialism. ...
... The term epistemically unwarranted beliefs refers to those beliefs that are not founded on reliable reasoning or credible data (Lobato et al., 2014). Following Lobato et al. (2014), these beliefs might be classified as paranormal, conspiracy, pseudoscientific beliefs and science denialism. The term paranormal beliefs (which includes superstitious beliefs; Tobacyk, 2004;Griffiths et al., 2018) refers to those that in the case of being true, would contradict fundamental scientific principles (Broad, 1949). ...
... Pseudoscientific beliefs are those concerning phenomena that, despite professing to be "scientific," are based on non-scientific evidentiary methods, such as authoritative assertions, anecdotes or vague explanations (Losh & Nzekwe, 2011). Finally, science denialism, which is classified by some authors as a form of pseudoscience (Lobato et al., 2014;Fasce & Picó, 2019), refers to the intentional refusal to accept well-established scientific theories, creating an artificial controversy among scientists (Fasce & Picó, 2019). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The prevalence of pseudoscientific beliefs in society has notable repercussions on critical domains such as health and education. Causal illusions have been proposed as a potential cognitive bias underlying the formation and perpetuation of such beliefs. This doctoral thesis aims to delve into the relationship between causal illusion and the endorsement of pseudoscientific beliefs, through different studies. The first one focuses on the development of a tool to measure pseudoscience belief endorsement (Pseudoscience Endorsement Scale, PES) and testing its possible association with causal judgment scores obtained in a passive (i.e., null) contingency detection task, in which participants observed a series of medical records indicating whether or not the patient took a certain infusion and whether or not he or she was subsequently cured of a headache. The results showed that participants who scored higher on the PES exhibited stronger causal illusions on the contingency detection task. In a second study, volunteers engaged in active contingency learning tasks in which they could manipulate the presence or absence of a potential cause to explore its impact on the outcome, thus reflecting not only information interpretation, but also search strategies. The findings consistently demonstrated that individuals with stronger pseudoscientific beliefs exhibited heightened causal illusions, irrespective of differential search strategies. Finally, a third study was conducted in order to explore other cognitive and sociodemographic correlates of pseudoscientific beliefs. The results indicated that the level of endorsement of pseudoscience showed positive correlations with other unwarranted beliefs, such as paranormal and conspiracist beliefs and science denialism, and negative correlations with scientific knowledge, cognitive reflection scores, and bullshit sensitivity, a measure that represents the ability to differentiate between legitimate motivational statements and profound-sounding but devoid of meaning sentences. Taken together, these results suggest a robust link between pseudoscientific beliefs and the development of causal illusions, which arises regardless of whether those with higher pseudoscientific beliefs employ different information search strategies. Therefore, our research emphasizes the complex interaction between cognitive biases and belief systems, offering insights into possible approaches aimed at reducing the impact of pseudoscientific beliefs within society.
... The examination of irrational beliefs, which are also identified in the literature as unsubstantiated claims (Bensley et al., 2020), epistemically suspect beliefs (Pennycook et al., 2015), unwarranted beliefs (Lobato et al., 2014), and instances of contaminated mindware (Stanovich et al., 2016), has evolved within three distinct lines of research. One line explored beliefs that break ontological assumptions about nature (Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007), such as belief in paranormal and extrasensory perception (S. ...
... Irrational beliefs are often shaped by specific socio-political contexts, making them more than merely individual. Previous research found a somewhat higher propensity for paranormal beliefs among women, mixed results for pseudoscientific beliefs, and mostly no gender differences in conspiracist beliefs (Green & Douglas, 2018;Lobato et al., 2014;Stanković et al., 2022;Šrol, 2022, Ward & King, 2020. Age was typically not a significant predictor of irrational beliefs (Šrol, 2022), although some research reported weak negative correlations (Majima et al., 2022;Torres et al., 2023), especially with conspiracist beliefs (Green & Douglas, 2018). ...
... Findings also frequently show that irrational beliefs are related to more conservative (right-wing) political views (Fasce et al., 2020;Galliford & Furnham, 2017;Jastrzębski & Chuderski, 2022;Lobato & Zimmerman, 2019;Šrol, 2022;Torres et al., 2023). While religiousness may not be inherently irrational 1 , previous findings indicate that it is closely related to the endorsement of irrational beliefs (Čavojová & Ersoy, 2020;Lindeman et al., 2023;Lobato et al., 2014;Pennycook et al., 2012;Šrol, 2022). ...
... Several studies have noted an affinity between correlations of conspiracy theory beliefs and other contested knowledge claims such as belief in paranormal phenomena (e.g. Darwin et al. 2011;Swami et al. 2011;Lobato et al. 2014;Oliver and Wood 2014). A suggested explanation of this positive relationship is that knowledge claims of conspiracies and paranormal and supernatural phenomena are effectively unfalsifiable (e.g. ...
... Although not paranormal per se (though some may be), conspiracy theories have been identified as positively associated with alternative spiritual and paranormal ideas and practices (Swami et al. 2011;Lobato et al. 2014;Lantian et al. 2016;Wood and Douglas 2018). This confluence of alternative spirituality and conspiracy theories has recently been approached by the term conspirituality (Ward and Voas 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
This article explores conspiracy mentality occurrence in Sweden as part of a 2020 survey of paranormal beliefs, practices, and experiences. Using the idea of a shared sociocultural milieu of alternative views and practices of knowledge as a departure point, the relationship between conspiracy mentality and paranormalism, viewed as instances of cultic, rejected, and stigmatized knowledge, is tested through multivariate regression. As part of the regression model, the effects of other social predictors (e.g. gender, income, and cultural values) are also assessed. A nonparametric analysis further explores the relationship between conspiracy mentality and voting intention. The results of multivariate regression show that the dependent variables only predict conspiracy mentality to a limited extent. Predictors that significantly contribute to predicting conspiracy mentality are gender (being male), higher levels of paranormalism, lower income, conservation as opposed to openness-to-change values, and self-transcendence as opposed to self-enhancement values. Meanwhile, the nonparametric test pointed to distinctly higher conspiracy mentality levels among sympathizers with the right-wing populist party the Sweden Democrats and those who favoured political parties not represented in parliament.
... Studies on pseudoscientific beliefs have been mainly carried out with undergraduate students [25,45,46], while only a few studies have focused on upper secondary school students [47]. For instance, Afonso and Gilbert [45] interviewed about 50 Portuguese science and nonscience undergraduate students exploring their beliefs on water dowsing, namely the ability to find groundwater using a forked wooden stick. ...
... There are also significant differences between the PSEUDO-QM score and the score on the conspiracy theories scale. These results are in line with previous studies, in which the authors found that scores on the scale measuring conspiracy theories were on average lower than scores on the scale measuring paranormal and generic pseudoscientific beliefs [46]. Finally, we found that teachers scored significantly higher on the generic pseudoscientific beliefs scale than on the PSEUDO QM scale. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has increased the interest in the possible causes of the spread of science-based fake news. Indeed, such phenomenon can lead to a generalized distrust of science with serious consequences for our society, ranging from vaccine avoidance to climate change denial. Research in educational psychology has only recently begun to explore the psychological drivers and relationships between misinformation, conspiracy theories and pseudoscientific beliefs. However, existing studies have not focused on specific scientific domains and have involved samples with individuals of different age and sociocultural background. In this empirical study, we focus specifically on the pseudoscientific beliefs of high school students and teachers in a science area where fake news are spreading rapidly on websites and social media, namely, quantum mechanics (QM). To this end, we used a 21-item instrument specifically developed by our group in a previous study to measure pseudoscientific beliefs in QM. The cross-sectional sample included N = 1119 high school students ( female students = 52 % ) and N = 125 high school teachers. The data collected were analyzed using the following methods: confirmatory factor analysis to establish the validity of the instrument; multiple correspondence analysis, to transform categorical variables measuring sociocultural background into continuous variables; repeated measures analysis of variance and linear regression to describe which factors most influence students’ and teachers’ pseudoscientific beliefs. The results show that, for students, endorsement of pseudoscientific beliefs in QM depends on the following factors: trust in public and science institutions, scientific content consumption, type of high school attended, and QM literacy. For teachers, endorsement of pseudoscientific beliefs in QM depends on the type of degree obtained, QM literacy, perceived usefulness of teaching QM, and confidence in teaching QM. Our study suggests that outreach and popularization interventions for high school students focused on QM, as well as teacher training courses on QM, should also incorporate elements of the nature of science, i.e., on how we develop scientific knowledge, to provide more effective tools to help teachers and students distinguish between correct claims and plausible but false claims, using both prebunking and debunking approaches. Published by the American Physical Society 2024
... A growing body of work in recent years has investigated conspiracy theories. In doing so, the scholarship has attempted to investigate what conspiracy theories are (Douglas & Sutton, 2023), whether they matter , whether conspiracy theories have increased over time , whether conspiracy beliefs form belief systems , whether the use of social media affects conspiracy beliefs , whether political extremists are more likely to have conspiracy beliefs , and whether conspiracy believers are more prone to believe in pseudo-science and in the paranormal (Lobato et al., 2014). More importantly, the studies conducted in this stream of inquiry have shown that conspiracy theories can be used as political tools, shape political context, and are often related to political ideologies (Douglas et al., 2019). ...
... Lobato et al., 2014;Sutton & Douglas, 2020). ...
Article
This article suggests that since people who believe in magic and those who believe in conspiracy theories produce meaning and make sense of the world by engaging in the same process of signification, they should be more likely to have one such belief if they hold the other. To test this proposition, we perform both macro‐ and micro‐level analyses using, respectively, cross‐national datasets and an original set of Tanzanian data. Our findings from both sets of analyses reveal a strong association between witchcraft beliefs and the belief in conspiracy theories. Related Articles Gainous, Jason, and Bill Radunovich. 2008. “Religion and Core Values: A Reformulation of the Funnel of Causality.” Politics & Policy 33(1): 154–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747‐1346.2005.tb00213.x . Richey, Sean. 2017. “A Birther and a Truther: The Influence of the Authoritarian Personality on Conspiracy Beliefs.” Politics & Policy 45(3): 465–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/polp.12206 . Tubadji, Annie. 2023. “You'll Never Walk Alone: Loneliness, Religion, and Politico‐economic Transformation.” Politics & Policy 51(4): 661–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/polp.12538 .
... This, in part, explains why believers are more likely to endorse epistemically suspect beliefs. These are notions, not based upon reasoned or reliable evidence, which conflict with prevailing conceptions of the world [5]. Specific named Traditional Paranormal Beliefs (TPB, 5-items measuring customary supernatural concepts: traditional religious belief and witchcraft) and New Age Philosophy (NAP, 11-items assessing paranormal abilities: psi, spiritualism, and precognition). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent research indicates that paranormal belief, in the absence of allied cognitive-perceptual and psychopathology-related factors, is not associated with negative wellbeing outcomes. However, investigators have historically reported relationships between specific facets of belief (e.g., superstition) and stress vulnerability. These typically derive from the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (RPBS), which has questionable psychometric integrity. The main issue being that several RPBS items perform poorly. Noting this, the present paper re-examined the relationship between paranormal belief and stress using the Rasch purified version of the RPBS. This comprises two dimensions, called Traditional Paranormal Belief (TPB) and New Age Philosophy (NAP). These are operationalised in terms of function. Specifically, whether belief provides a sense of control at the social (TPB) or individual level (NAP). Accordingly, this study examined whether TPB and NAP were differentially predictive of levels of perceived stress. In this context, stress served as an indicator of well-being. A sample of 3084 participants (Mage = 50.31, SD = 15.20, range 18–91) completed the RPBS alongside the 10-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10). Confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modelling revealed that TPB was significantly predictive of higher Distress, and lower Coping. NAP was neither predictive of Distress nor Coping. These findings support the notion that TPB is attendant with external control, particularly the notion that unknown supernatural forces/powers influence existence.
Article
This study investigates how people who believe in conspiracy theory (CT) hold those beliefs. It considers whether believing a conspiracist explanation of an issue or event permits or precludes also believing a non‐conspiracist explanation—that is, the degree to which endorsing CTs can be dialogical. Dialogicality involves the coexistence of conspiracist and non‐conspiracist beliefs in advancing claims and contrasts with the view that all conspiracist belief endorsement is monological or ‘closed’ from other explanations. Thematic analysis of 39 semi‐structured interviews revealed five modalities of belief coexistence: cognitive dissonance between beliefs , analogical beliefs , target‐dependent beliefs , synthetic beliefs , and integrative beliefs . This spanned beliefs about the self, ingroup, outgroup, reality, action and the future, on topics concerning science, religion, and politics and society. These findings challenge the view that all CTs are monological belief systems: CT believers recruit both conspiracist and non‐conspiracist explanations in building their worldview. Several hypotheses for further research emerge from this challenge to the basic assumption of much research on CTs.
Article
Vjerovanje u paranormalne pojave vrsta je znanstveno neutemeljenih vjerovanja koja se odnosi na pripisivanje nadnaravnih uzroka svakodnevnim i prirodnim pojavama. Cilj ovog istraživanja bio je ispitati ulogu povjerenja u znanost i znanstvene pismenosti u objašnjavanju vjerovanja u paranormalne pojave. Znanstvena pismenost je definirana kao sposobnost znanstvenog rezoniranja i razumijevanja znanstvene metodologije, do sada rijetko istraživanog prediktora znanstveno neutemeljenih vjerovanja. U istraživanju su sudjelovali studenti koji su ispunili skalu aktivnog otvorenog mišljenja, skalu povjerenja u znanost i skalu znanstvenog rezoniranja, procijenili su važnost vjere u životu i političku orijentaciju. Rezultati hijerarhijske regresijske analize su pokazali da i povjerenje u znanost i znanstveno rezoniranje imaju značajan samostalan doprinos u objašnjavanju varijance vjerovanja u paranormalno povrh one objašnjene spolom, religioznosti, političkom orijentacijom i aktivnim otvorenim mišljenjem.
Article
Full-text available
Throughout human history, political leaders, oppositional forces, and businesspeople have frequently coordinated in secret for their own benefit and the public’s disadvantage. In these cases, conspiracy theories are capable of accurately describing our environment. However, the vast majority of research today operationalizes conspiracy theories as irrational beliefs that contradict our everyday knowledge. It is not surprising, then, that belief in implausible conspiracy theories has been associated with suboptimal information processing. To get a richer understanding of the phenomenon, we argue that researchers should have this limitation in mind when designing future studies.
Article
Critical thinking (CT) is considered one of the most important soft skills for student training and development, especially in an academic context in which students are immersed in a process of technologizing their teaching-learning process, are exposed to a large amount of information and are also great users of social media. The main objective of this study is to rigorously assess how new media literacy (NML) can influence the ability and willingness of students to think critically, and how CT and NML influence students’ perceptions of performance. Contrary to what is found in most of the literature, NML appears as a relevant antecedent to develop students’ skills and dispositions to think critically. These findings raise the need to deepen the study of the relationship between NML and CT.
Article
Full-text available
Surveyed 348 residents of southwestern New Jersey and found that most believed that several of a list of 10 conspiracy theories were at least probably true. Ss who believed in 1 conspiracy were more likely also to believe in others. Belief in conspiracies was correlated with anomia, lack of interpersonal trust, and insecurity about employment. Blacks and Hispanics were more likely to believe in conspiracy theories than were Whites. Younger Ss were slightly more likely to believe in conspiracy theories, but there were few significant correlations with gender, educational level, or occupational category. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Conspiracy thinking is defined as a pattern of explanatory reasoning about events and situations of personal, social, and historical significance in which a "conspiracy" is the dominant or operative actor. While conspiracy thinking exists to some extent probably in every society, the authors note the special prevalence of this type of thinking in the Arab-Iranian-Muslim Middle East, and offer a psychoanalytically based approach to conspiracy thinking based on theories of the paranoid process. The authors also attempt to identify aspects of Arab-Iranian-Muslim culture that may predispose individuals from that culture to conspiracy thinking, especially child-rearing practices, attitudes toward sexuality, and the role of secrecy.
Article
Using a 1978 national Gallup poll, we test the hypothesis that belief in nonreligious paranormal phenomena is a functional alternative to religion. Results indicate that those with no religious preference, or for whom religious beliefs are unimportant, are more likely to believe in nonreligious and less likely to believe in religious paranormal phenomena. Baptists (in contrast with more liberal Protestant denominations) are more likely to believe in the religious paranormal and less likely to believe in the nonreligious. Controlling for demographic variables reduces but does not eliminate the functional alternative pattern.
Article
Public acceptance of evolution remains low in the United States relative to other Western countries. Although advocates for the scientific community often highlight the need for improved education to change public opinion, analyses of data from a national sample of American adults indicate that the effects of educational attainment on attitudes toward evolution and creationism are uneven and contingent upon religious identity. Consequently, higher education will only shift public attitudes toward evolution and away from support for teaching creationism in public schools for those who take non-"literalist" interpretive stances on the Bible, or to the extent that it leads to fewer people with literalist religious identities.
Article
The findings of past research on the relationship between paranormal belief and religiosity are inconclusive. The aim of this study was to examine the relationship based on a sample from Austria and also with respect to different aspects of paranormal belief as well as religious belief. A sample of 596 students completed a measure of paranormal belief and a questionnaire on various indices of religiosity. The results revealed rather low but significant correlations between paranormal belief and religiosity. Intrinsic religiosity and self-reported religiosity were much more strongly related to paranormal belief than was extrinsic religiosity. For subjects without religious affiliation, the relationship between religiosity and paranormal belief was higher than for Catholics and Protestants. These results suggest a modified version of the substitution hypothesis indicating that paranormal belief can be, but must not necessarily be, a substitute for traditional religion.