ArticlePDF Available

Is animal research sufficiently evidence based to be a cornerstone of biomedical research?

Authors:
  • Safer Medicines

Abstract

Public acceptance of the use of animals in biomedical research is conditional on it producing benefits for humans. Pandora Pound and Michael Bracken argue that the benefits remain unproved and may divert funds from research that is more relevant to doctors and their patients
Is animal research sufficiently evidence based to be a
cornerstone of biomedical research?
Public acceptance of the use of animals in biomedical research is conditional on it producing benefits
for humans.Pandora Pound and Michael Bracken argue that the benefits remain unproved and
may divert funds from research that is more relevant to doctors and their patients
Pandora Pound medical sociologist 1, Michael B Bracken Susan Dwight Bliss professor of
epidemiology 2
1Bath, UK; 2Yale University Schools of Public Health and Medicine, New Haven CT, USA
Proponents of animal research claim that the benefits to humans
are self evident.1However, writing in The BMJ 10 years ago
we argued that such uncorroborated claims were inadequate in
an era of evidence based medicine.2At that time over two thirds
of UK government and charitable investment was going into
basic research,3perhaps creating an expectation that such
research was highly productive of clinical benefits. However,
when we searched for systematic evidence to support claims
about the clinical benefits of animal research we identified only
25 systematic reviews of animal experiments, and these raised
serious doubts about the design, quality, and relevance of the
included studies. As our colleagues had done earlier,4we argued
the case that systematic reviews should be extensively adopted
within animal research to synthesise and appraise findings, just
as they are in clinical research.
Poor quality and reporting of animal
studies
The overall number of systematic reviews of animal studies
remains lamentably low, with the ratio of reviews to total
number of publications being about 10-fold higher in human
studies.5In 2011 Korevaar and colleagues identified 244
systematic reviews of preclinical studies up until 2010,
estimating that the number was doubling every three years.6
As the number of systematic reviews increased, the poor quality
of much preclinical animal research became increasingly
apparent.7Evidence accumulated that many animal studies failed
to address important threats to internal and external validity,
making prediction to humans tenuous at best.8 9 For example,
the National Centre for the Replacement, Refinement and
Reduction of Animals in Research (NC3Rs) surveyed 271
animal studies conducted between 1999 and 2005 and found
that only 32 (12%) reported using random allocation to treatment
or control and that investigators were blinded to the allocation
in only 14% (5/35) of studies that used qualitative scoring.10
Systematic reviews of animal studies also revealed evidence of
selective analysis and outcome reporting bias11 as well as
publication bias12 leading to overstatement of the validity of
entire bodies of research.13
The Collaborative Approach to Meta-Analysis and Review of
Animal Data from Experimental Studies (CAMARADES) has
been at the forefront of conducting systematic reviews of animal
studies. Initially focusing on stroke, it later expanded to include
neurological disease, bone cancer, multiple sclerosis, and
Parkinson’s disease. By 2012 John Ioannidis, professor of health
research and policy at Stanford, concluded that CAMARADES
had found consistent suggestions of serious bias in animal
studies, making it: “nearly impossible to rely on most animal
data to predict whether or not an intervention will have a
favourable clinical benefit-risk ratio in human subjects.”14
Lack of benefit for humans
Concerns have been raised that compounds with little or no
therapeutic potential could proceed to clinical trials because
overoptimistic conclusions are drawn about their efficacy as a
result of flaws in experimental design and inadequate control
of bias.15-19 Several studies have shown that even the most
promising findings from animal research often fail in human
trials and are rarely adopted into clinical practice.20-22 For
example, one study found that fewer than 10% of highly
promising basic science discoveries enter routine clinical use
within 20 years.23 In stroke medicine, despite decades of
immense human, animal, and financial investment, animal
models have failed to yield a single neuroprotective treatment
for humans.24 25 Similarly, none of more than 100 drugs studied
in an established mouse model of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis,
many of which had been reported to slow down the disease, was
ultimately found to be beneficial after more rigorous
experiments. Eight of these drugs had been used in thousands
Correspondence to: P Pound pandorapound@gmail.com
For personal use only: See rights and reprints http://www.bmj.com/permissions Subscribe: http://www.bmj.com/subscribe
BMJ 2014;348:g3387 doi: 10.1136/bmj.g3387 (Published 30 May 2014) Page 1 of 3
Analysis
ANALYSIS
of patients who participated in failed clinical trials.26 A similar
lack of translation has become apparent in inflammation.27
Falling investment in basic and animal
research
Public funding bodies are becoming aware of the lack of return
on investment, and public and charitable spending on basic
research has decreased in the UK from 68.3% in 2004-5 to
59.4% in 2009-10.28 This seems wise since retrospective analysis
of the payback from research is beginning to suggest that it is
clinical rather than basic research that has most effect on patient
care.29 30 Almost half of all research involving animals in the
UK in 2012 was conducted by universities (48%), the remainder
occurring in commercial organisations (27%), public bodies
(13%), and non-profit organisations (9%).31 The drug industry
is also beginning to decrease its reliance on animal research
because each translational failure represents huge losses of
invested capital.21 32 In Europe drug companies have reportedly
decreased their use of animals by more than 25% from 2005 to
2008.33
A broken model?
The animal research community continues to cite selected
instances of how research on animals has resulted in medical
advances, or will one day do so (see www.
understandinganimalresearch.org.uk/resources/animal-research-
news-feed/). However, these convey little confidence about the
overall reliability and success of animal models, taking into
account the total evidence. Given the large amount of animal
research being undertaken, some findings will extrapolate to
humans just by chance. Understanding Animal Research, a
British organisation financed mainly by those conducting or
funding animal research, highlights four reports purporting to
support the validity of animal research,34 all of which rely solely
on expert opinion, one of the weakest forms of evidence
according to widely agreed standards.35
Would improvements in preclinical experimental procedures
and research reporting enhance the prediction from animals to
humans and provide greater benefits for humans? In an article
reviewing developments in the field of stroke, Sutherland and
colleagues note that despite researchers adhering to
recommendations intended to improve the quality of preclinical
stroke studies for over 10 years, there is no evidence of an
increased rate of successful translation.25 Others argue that
animal models will always fail to predict human outcomes
reliably because humans and animals are such complex
interactive systems with different evolutionary trajectories that
even small differences between species could be important.36
The genomic and inherent differences between rodent and
human physiology are increasingly acknowledged,37 and even
non-human primates have many differences in the epigenome
that fundamentally affect the functionality of the genome38 and
may account for their lack of success in predicting clinical
response.39-41 Even if the research was conducted faultlessly,
animal models might still have limited success in predicting
human responses to drugs and disease because of inherent
inter-species differences in molecular and metabolic pathways.42
The use of transgenic animals, in which the genome has been
changed by insertion of foreign genetic material, attempts to
increase the validity of animal models by making them more
closely resemble human phenotypes of interest. Yet transgenic
models, where genes are regarded as operating largely
independently of each other, have been criticised as limited,43
oversimplistic, and, at least to date, as contributing more to an
idea of therapeutic promise than actual clinical outcomes.21 36
Furthermore, it has been observed that transgenic animals do
not always produce the desired phenotype after cross breeding
several generations, thereby undermining the rationale for this
research strategy.26
Attempts to improve animal research and
reporting
In response to the serious deficiencies found in the conduct and
reporting of animal studies the ARRIVE (Animal Research:
Reporting In Vivo Experiments) guidelines 44 were produced in
2010. Over 300 journals and the major UK funding agencies
have endorsed these guidelines, but a recent survey of papers
published in Nature and PLoS found little improvement in
reporting standards.45 A Gold Standard Publication Checklist
has also been developed by SYRCLE (Systematic Review
Centre for Laboratory Animal Experimentation) in the
Netherlands to encourage more rigour in the conduct, not just
reporting, of animal research.46
Michael Festing, a retired Medical Research Council scientist,
recently acknowledged that few basic scientists receive any
formal teaching, most relying on what they learn from their
supervisor.47 Similarly, the leadership of the National Institutes
of Health in the US recognises that poor training may in part
be responsible for the lack of reproducibility of animal models.48
The UK Fund for the Replacement of Animals in Medical
Experiments now offers voluntary workshops in experimental
design and statistical analysis, and an online course in
experimental design (www.3rs-reduction.co.uk) has been
developed. Training is also available for preclinical investigators
to learn how to conduct systematic reviews (www.syrcle.nl).
In 2008 the Medical Research Council (MRC) funded a pilot
“research translator” at an English university hospital site to try
to facilitate the translation of findings from bench to bedside.
One of the findings from a qualitative study investigating this
initiative was that basic scientists’ motivation came from
scientific discovery rather than the application of their findings
to medicine.49 Recent attempts to improve translation within the
animal research community include the “co-clinical trial” in
which preclinical trials explicitly parallel ongoing human phase
I and II trials50 and the development of a scoring system to
identify biomarkers that better predict therapeutic success.51
Time for change
The culture within research is shifting, and animal research is
no longer as immune from challenge or criticism as it once was.
Nonetheless, although science is more self critical, in practice
it can be difficult to achieve change because stakeholders
(governments, funders, universities, allied research industries,
and researchers) may all have interests, not infrequently
financial,52 in continuing to do things as they have always been
done. Although there are also valid criticisms of clinical
research,53 urgent attention needs to be paid to the quality of
animal research for important reasons.
Much clinical research follows on from animal research. If the
foundations of the biomedical research enterprise are unsound,
then whatever is built on these foundations will be similarly
precarious.
The current situation is unethical. Poorly designed studies and
lack of methodological rigour in preclinical research may result
in expensive but ultimately fruitless clinical trials that needlessly
expose humans to potentially harmful drugs or may result in
For personal use only: See rights and reprints http://www.bmj.com/permissions Subscribe: http://www.bmj.com/subscribe
BMJ 2014;348:g3387 doi: 10.1136/bmj.g3387 (Published 30 May 2014) Page 2 of 3
ANALYSIS
other potentially beneficial therapies being withheld. Moreover,
if poorly conducted studies produce unreliable findings, any
suffering endured by animals loses its moral justification because
their use cannot possibly contribute towards clinical benefit.
Non-publication of animal studies is similarly unethical because
the animals involved cannot contribute towards the accumulation
of knowledge and because non-publication may result in further,
unnecessary animal and human experiments. 13
In addition to intensifying the systematic review effort,
providing training in experimental design and adhering to higher
standards of research conduct and reporting, prospective
registration of preclinical studies,54 and the public deposition of
(both positive and negative) findings would be steps in the right
direction.18 Greater public accountability might be provided by
including lay people in some of the processes of preclinical
research such as ethical review bodies55 and setting research
priorities.28 However, if animal researchers continue to fail to
conduct rigorous studies and synthesise and report them
accurately, and if research conducted on animals continues to
be unable to reasonably predict what can be expected in humans,
the public’s continuing endorsement and funding of preclinical
animal research seems misplaced.
We thank SABRE Research UK (www.sabre.org.uk) for the use of its
archive.
Contributors and sources: PP has conducted research in the sociology
of medicine for over two decades and has a particular interest in
evidence based medicine in animal research. MB is an epidemiologist
who teaches and has considerable experience in evidence based
medicine. He has been an active member of the Cochrane Collaboration
from its inception and has a particular interest in research methods. PP
conceived the idea for this article and wrote the first draft. MB contributed
his knowledge, expertise, and critical eye to subsequent drafts. PP is
the guarantor.
Competing interests: We have read and understood BMJ policy on
declaration of interests and have no relevant interests to declare.
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer
reviewed.
1 Matthews R. Medical progress depends on animal models—doesn’t it? J R Soc Med
2008;101:95-8.
2 Pound P, Ebrahim S, Sandercock P, Bracken M, Roberts I. Where is the evidence that
animal research benefits humans? BMJ 2004;328:514-7.
3 Chalmers I, Glasziou P. Avoidable waste in the production and reporting of research
evidence. Lancet 2009;374:869.
4Sandercock P, Roberts I. Systematic reviews of animal experiments. Lancet 2002;360:586.
5Bracken MB. Risk chance and causation: investigating the origins and treatment of disease.
Yale University Press, 2013.
6 Korevaar D, Hooft L, ter Riet G. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses of preclinical
studies: publication bias in laboratory animal experiments. Lab Anim 2011;45:225-30.
7Van Luijk J, Leenaars M, Hooijmans C, Wever K, de Vries R, Ritskes-Hoitinga M. Towards
evidence-based translational research: the pros and cons of conducting systematic reviews
of animal studies. Altex 2012;30:256-7.
8 Kimmelman J, London AJ. Predicting harms and benefits in translational trials: ethics,
evidence and uncertainty. PLoS Med 2011;8:e1001010.
9 Henderson B, Kimmelman J, Fergusson D, Grimshaw J, Hackam D. Threats to validity
in the design and conduct of preclinical efficacy studies: a systematic review of guidelines
for in vivo animal experiments. PLoS Med 2013;10:e1001489.
10 Kilkenny C, Parsons N, Kadyszewski E, Festing MFW, Cuthill IC, Fry D, et al. Survey of
the quality of experimental design, statistical analysis and reporting of research using
animals. PLoS ONE 2009;4:e7824.
11 Tsilidis K, Panagiotou O, Sena E, Aretouli E, Evangelou E, Howells D, et al. Evaluation
of excess significance bias in animal studies of neurological diseases. PLoS Biol
2013;11:e1001609.
12 Perel P, Roberts I, Sena E, Wheble P, Briscoe C, Sandercock P, et al. Comparison of
treatment effects between animal experiments and clinical trials: systematic review. BMJ
2007;334:197.
13 Sena ES, Bart van der Worp H, Bath PMW, Howells DW, Macleod MR. Publication bias
in reports of animal stroke studies leads to major overstatement of efficacy. PLoS Biol
2010;8:1-8.
14 Ioannidis JPA. Extrapolating from animals to humans. Sci Translat Med 2012;4:1-3.
15 Lindner MD. Clinical attrition due to biased preclinical assessments of potential efficacy.
Pharmacol Ther 2007;115:148-75.
16 Hackam DG. Translating animal research into clinical benefit. BMJ 2007;334:163-4.
17 Wall RJ, Shani M. Are animal models as good as we think? Theriogenology 2008;69:2-9.
18 Kimmelman J, Anderson JA. Should preclinical studies be registered? Nature Biotech
2012;30:488-9.
19 Muhlhausler BS, Bloomfield FH, Gillman MW. Whole animal experiments should be more
like human randomized controlled trials. PLoS Biol 2013;11:e1001481.
20 Hackam DG, Redelmeier DA. Translation of research evidence from animals to humans.
JAMA 2006;296:1731-2.
21 Geerts H. Of mice and men. Bridging the translational disconnect in CNS drug discovery.
CNS Drugs 2009;23:915-26.
22 Kola I, Landis J. Can the pharmaceutical industry reduce attrition rates? Nature Rev Drug
Discovery 2004;3:711-5.
23 Contopoulos-Ioannidis DG, Ntzani EE, Ioannidis JPA. Translation of highly promising
basic science research into clinical applications. Am J Med 2003;114:477-84.
24 Bart van der Worp H, Howells DW, Sena ES, Porritt MJ, Rewell S, O’Collins V, et al. Can
animal models of disease reliably inform human studies? PLoS Med 2010;7:e1000245.
25 Sutherland BA, Minnerup J, Balami JS, Arba F, Buchan AM, Kleinschnitz C.
Neuroprotection for ischaemic stroke: translation from the bench to the bedside. Int J
Stroke 2012;7:407-18.
26 Perrin P. Make mouse studies work. Nature 2014;507:423-5.
27 Seok J, Warren S, Cuenca A, Mindrinos M, Baker H, Xu W, et al. Genomic responses in
mouse models poorly mimic human inflammatory diseases. Proc Natl Acad Sci
2013;110:3507-12.
28 Chalmers I, Bracken MB, Djulbegovic B, Garattini S, Grant J, Metin Gulmezoglu A, et al.
How to increase value and reduce waste when research priorities are set. Lancet
2014;338:156-65.
29 Wooding S, Pollitt A, Castle-Clarke S, Cochrane G, Diepeveen S, Guthrie S, et al. Mental
health retrosight: identifying the attributes of successfully translated research (lessons
from schizophrenia). 2013. www.rand.org/pubs/research_briefs/RB9738.
30 Wooding S, Hanney S, Pollitt A, Buxton M, Grant J. Project retrosight: understanding the
returns from cardiovascular and stroke research. 2011. www.rand.org/pubs/research_
briefs/RB9573.
31 Home Office. Annual statistics of scientific procedures on living animals. Stationery Office,
2012.
32 US Food and Drug Administration. Innovation or stagnation. Challenge and opportunity
on the critical path to new medical products. US Department of Health and Human
Services, 2004.
33 Hartung T. Look back in anger—what clinical studies tell us about preclinical work. Altex
2014;30:275-91.
34 Understanding Animal Research. Expert and independent opinion. www.
understandinganimalresearch.org.uk/resources/expert-and-independent-opinion.
35 Centre for Evidence Based Medicine. Levels of evidence. 2009. www.cebm.net/?o=1025.
36 Shanks N, Greek R. Animal models in light of evolution. BrownWalker, 2009.
37 Leist M, Hartung T. Inflammatory findings on species extrapolations: humans are definitely
no 70-kg mice. Arch Toxicol 2013;87:563-67.
38 Boffelli D, Martin DI. Epigenetic inheritance: a contributor to species differentiation? DNA
Cell Biol 2012;31:S11-6.
39 Shanks N, Greek R. Experimental use of nonhuman primates is not a simple problem.
Nature Med 2008;14:1012-13.
40 Bailey J. Lessons from chimpanzee-based research on human disease: the implications
of genetic differences. Altern Lab Anim 2011;39:527-40.
41 Eastwood D, Findlay L, Poole S, Bird C, Wadhwa M, Moore M, et al. Monoclonal antibody
TGN1412 trial failure explained by species differences in CD28 expression on CD4+
effector memory T-cells. Br J Pharmacol 2010;161:512-26.
42 Greek R, Menache A. Systematic reviews of animal models: methodology versus
epistemology. Int J Med Sci 2013;10:206-21.
43 Lin JH. Applications and limitations of genetically modified mouse models in drug discovery
and development. Current Drug Metab 2008;9:419-38.
44 Kilkenny C, Browne WJ, Cuthill IC, Emerson M, Altman D. Improving bioscience research
reporting: the ARRIVE guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol
2010;8:e1000412.
45 Baker D, Lidster K, Sottomayor A, Amor S. Two years later: journals are not yet enforcing
the ARRIVE guidelines on reporting standards for pre-clinical animal studies. PLoS Biol
2014;12:e001756.
46 Hooijmans CR, Leenaars M, Ritskes-Hoitinga M. A gold standard publication checklist to
improve the quality of animal studies, to fully integrate the three Rs, and to make systematic
reviews more feasible. Altern Lab Animal 2010;38:167-82.
47 Festing MFW. We are not born knowing how to design and analyse scientific experiments.
Altern Lab Animal 2013;41:1-3.
48 Collins FS, Tabak LA. NIH plans to enhance reproducibility. Nature 2014;505:612-3.
49 Morgan M, Barry C, Donovan J, Sandall J, Wolfe CDA, Boaz Al. Implementing translational
biomedical research: convergence and divergence among clinical and basic scientists.
Soc Sci Med 2011;73:945-52.
50 Chen Z, Cheng K, Walton Z, Wang Y, Ebi H, Shimamura T, et al. A murine lung cancer
co-clinical trial identifies genetic modifiers of therapeutic response. Nature 2012;483:613-7.
51 Wendler A, Wehling M. Translatability scoring in drug development: eight case studies.
J Transl Med 2012;10:39.
52 Hawkes N. Initiative aims to make London Europe’s commercial centre for life sciences.
BMJ 2014;348:g2687.
53 Macleod MR, Michie S, Roberts, I, Dirnagl U, Chalmers I, Ioannidis JPA, et al. Biomedical
research: increasing value, reducing waste. Lancet 2014;383:2-6.
54 Dal-Ré R, Ioannidis JP, Bracken MB, Buffler PA, Chan AW, Franco EL, et al. Making
prospective registration of observational research a reality. Sci Transl Med 2014;6:224.
55 Brown S. Independent investigation into animal research at Imperial College. 2013. http:
//brownreport.info/wp-content/uploads/2014/02/The-Brown-Report.pdf.
Cite this as: BMJ 2014;348:g3387
© BMJ Publishing Group Ltd 2014
For personal use only: See rights and reprints http://www.bmj.com/permissions Subscribe: http://www.bmj.com/subscribe
BMJ 2014;348:g3387 doi: 10.1136/bmj.g3387 (Published 30 May 2014) Page 3 of 3
ANALYSIS
Key messages
The conduct, reporting, and synthesis of much animal research continues to be inadequate
This current situation is unethical since animals and humans participate in research that cannot produce reliable results
There is insufficient systematic evidence for the clinical benefits of animal research
Greater rigour and accountability is needed to ensure best use of public funds
For personal use only: See rights and reprints http://www.bmj.com/permissions Subscribe: http://www.bmj.com/subscribe
BMJ 2014;348:g3387 doi: 10.1136/bmj.g3387 (Published 30 May 2014) Page 4 of 3
ANALYSIS
... The poor quality of preclinical animal research became increasingly apparent as some of the touted experimental breakthroughs have not been reproducible (6,7). Furthermore, large number of molecules with little or no therapeutic value have proceeded to clinical trials because overoptimistic conclusions are drawn about their efficacy as a result of flaws in experimental design and inadequate control of bias in preclinical animal models (8). In 2012 John Ioannidis presented evidence that a serious bias existed in animal studies, making it: "nearly impossible to rely on most animal data to predict whether or not an intervention will have a favorable clinical benefit-risk ratio in human subjects" (9). ...
... It has been argued that systematic reviews should be extensively adopted within animal research to synthesize and appraise findings, just as they are used in clinical research (8,10). Despite the calls for systematic reviews to be extensively adopted also within the animal research (8,10), there are no systematic or scoping reviews on the outcome of different individual gene knockout animals in DMBA-TPA two-stage skin cancer model. ...
... It has been argued that systematic reviews should be extensively adopted within animal research to synthesize and appraise findings, just as they are used in clinical research (8,10). Despite the calls for systematic reviews to be extensively adopted also within the animal research (8,10), there are no systematic or scoping reviews on the outcome of different individual gene knockout animals in DMBA-TPA two-stage skin cancer model. The aim of this scoping review is to review the effect of each gene knockout strain on papilloma formation in DMBA-TPA two-stage mouse skin cancer model. ...
Method
Full-text available
Experimental two-stage skin cancer model induced by dimethylbenz(a)anthracene (DMBA) and 12-O-tetradecanoyl phorbol-13-acetate (TPA) is an ideal model for exploring individual genes' influence on the tumor initiation and progression in mouse. The objective for this scoping review is to evaluate the outcome of all gene knockouts on papilloma formation in DMBA-TPA two-stage skin cancer model. Four databases (PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science and Google Scholar) will be searched, and two authors will select the studies and extract the data independently. The review will narratively synthesize the currently available evidence on the topic and create a database on the phenotype of all tested gene knockout strains in the DMBA-TPA two-stage chemically induced skin cancer model.
... For example, thousands of drugs that worked in animal tests for stroke, HIV, immune system disorders, and other diseases failed when tried in humans. 6,27,28,29,30 These failures are primarily due to toxicities not predicted by animal tests or to a lack of efficacy. 6,27,28,29,30 One of the main safety problems caused by drugs is liver toxicity. ...
... 6,27,28,29,30 These failures are primarily due to toxicities not predicted by animal tests or to a lack of efficacy. 6,27,28,29,30 One of the main safety problems caused by drugs is liver toxicity. 31 A groundbreaking study found that, in a set of 27 drugs, human liver chip methods identified nearly 7 of every 8 drugs proven to be hepatoxic during clinical use after they were deemed safe by animal tests. ...
Article
Historically, most discussions about nonhuman animal experimentation consider what has become known as the 3 R's: refinement, reduction, and replacement. Refinement and reduction receive the most attention, but recent modeling advances suggest that suitable replacement of nonhuman animal testing would bolster human research and increase translatability to human health outcomes. This article discusses these modeling advances and advocates their use, especially as replacements to nonpredictive nonhuman animal protocols, and discusses growing momentum in biomedical research communities and federal agencies that favors replacement of animal testing.
... It has been argued that most interventions that work in animal models do not replicate in human clinical trials 42 and are rarely adopted into clinical practice 43 , and that data from animal studies cannot be relied upon to predict the results in humans 44 . However, these views ignore the incredibly high failure rate for compounds during preclinical testing. ...
... Much clinical research follows from animal research [8]. The practice of using animal models of human diseases for drug testing is common practice among biomedical researchers and scientists [9]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and objectives Although the goal of translational research is to bring biomedical knowledge from the laboratory to clinical trial and therapeutic products for improving health, this goal has not been well achieved as often as desired because of many barriers documented in different countries. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the challenges and opportunities of translating animal research into human trials in Ethiopia. Methods A descriptive qualitative study, using in-depth interviews, was conducted in which preclinical and clinical trial researchers who have been involved in animal research or clinical trials as principal investigator were involved. Data were analyzed using inductive thematic process. Results Six themes were emerged for challenges: lack of financial and human capacity, inadequate infrastructure, operational obstacles and poor research governance, lack of collaboration, lack of reproducibility of results and prolonged ethical and regulatory approval processes. Furthermore, three themes were synthesized for opportunities: growing infrastructure and resources, improved human capacity and better administrative processes and initiatives for collaboration. Conclusion and recommendations The study found that the identified characteristics/features are of high importance either to hurdle or enable the practice of translating animal research into human trials. The study suggests that there should be adequate infrastructure and finance, human capacity building, good research governance, improved ethical and regulatory approval process, multidisciplinary collaboration, and incentives and recognition for researchers to overcome the identified challenges and allow translating of animal research into human trials to proceed more efficiently.
... This issue was evident in our funnel plot analyses, with all meta-analyses revealing publication bias toward significant results. Although this bias did not affect the significance of our summary effect size in our study, this does ultimately raise fundamental questions about the adequacy of animal research as a cornerstone in biomedicine (76,77). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Epidemiological studies frequently reflect the concurrence of chronic pain with symptoms of anxiety and depression, particularly in women. Animal models are useful to understand the complex mechanisms underlying comorbidities, yet the wide range of methods employed and the wealth of evidence sometimes impedes effective translation and reproducibility. This systematic review and meta-analysis aims to synthesize the evidence regarding the influence of variables like sex and species on anxiety- and depressive-like behaviors in rodent models of neuropathic pain. Methods Following PROSPERO registration, we searched EMBASE, Scopus and the Web of Science from its inception to November 24th 2023, identifying 126 studies that met the inclusion criteria. The Hedge’s g value for each experiment and study was calculated, and further sub-group and meta-regression analyses were performed. Results Neuropathic pain significantly reduced the time rats and mice spent in the open arms of the elevated plus and zero mazes (g=-1.14), time in the center of the open field (g=-1.12), sucrose consumption in the sucrose preference test (g=-1.43) and grooming time in the splash test (g=-1.37), while increasing latency to feed in the novelty-suppressed feeding test (g=1.59), and immobility in the forced swimming (g=1.85) and tail suspension tests (g=1.91). Sex differences were observed, with weaker effects in females than in males for several behavioral paradigms, and funnel plots identified positive publication bias in the literature. Conclusions This meta-analysis emphasizes the effect of neuropathic pain on anxiety and depressive behaviors in rodents, highlighting the importance of investigating sex differences in future experimental studies.
... From a regulatory standpoint, conducting animal model experiments has been deemed critical and mandatory to bridge the gap between preclinical and clinical trials and bring forth the latest therapeutic breakthroughs. However, recent scrutiny from humanitarian and scientific considerations has shed light on a pressing issue: approximately 80% of promising treatments fail during clinical trials despite demonstrating effectiveness and safety in preclinical investigations [2]. Several underlying factors contribute to this disheartening reality, including inadequate characterization of relevant animal models [3], [4], suboptimal experimental quality in vivo studies, and significant interspecies differences in anatomy, pathophysiology, and immunology compared to humans. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter investigates whether animals used in research should be described as a particularly vulnerable group. First, it inquires whether research animals currently receive the protection they are due, and concludes that they do not. Indeed, it is shown that the research standards currently guiding animal research insufficiently protect animals’ basic claims. Consequently, many research animals can be considered particularly vulnerable, insofar as they run an increased risk of not receiving what they are due. Second, it argues that for animal research to be ethical, it must be made more similar to research with humans, and it is outlined what research respecting animals’ claims could look like in practice.
Preprint
Full-text available
Chronic variable stress (CVS) procedures are widely used to model depression in laboratory rodents. We systematically documented the experimental design used in mouse CVS studies, and the design of the behavioural tests used to evaluate the effect of CVS. In a subset of studies, we measured effect sizes in behavioural tests. Across 202 mouse studies, 82% used a unique CVS procedure. We took advantage of this variability to ask whether the duration and intensity of CVS procedures correlated with effects sizes obtained in five commonly-used behavioural tests: the sucrose preference test (SPT), the tail suspension test (TST), the forced swim test (FST), the open field test (OFT) and the elevated plus maze (EPM). The most evident impact of CVS procedure design on effect sizes were seen in the FST where longer-duration CVS procedures with more diverse types of stressors were associated with a smaller effect size. Next, we correlated effect sizes between behavioural tests to explore whether these tests might measure similar or different consequences of CVS. We found a positive correlation between effects sizes in the TST and FST, and in the OFT and EPM, but the two strongest positive correlations were between the EPM and TST, and between the EPM and FST. CVS studies deliberately impose suffering over long periods, and our data raise scientific and ethical questions around the stress procedures used and the behavioural tests used to evaluate them.
Article
The skin is the largest organ in the human body, and the physical and chemical factors from which human skin suffers make the human body prone to allergic and inflammatory reactions. The animal testing that has been used to evaluate the skin toxicity of drug and cosmetic ingredients in the past has been faced with increasing calls for its abolition. Therefore, there is an increasing demand for in vitro testing methods that can evaluate their efficacy, such as for anti-inflammation, etc. In this paper, a simple skin-on-a-chip coupled with a rapid multiplex immunodetection system for inflammatory cytokine detection is proposed. This platform can be used to quickly screen cosmetic ingredients and drugs.
Article
Full-text available
Assertions that the use of chimpanzees to investigate human diseases is valid scientifically are frequently based on a reported 98-99% genetic similarity between the species. Critical analyses of the relevance of chimpanzee studies to human biology, however, indicate that this genetic similarity does not result in sufficient physiological similarity for the chimpanzee to constitute a good model for research, and furthermore, that chimpanzee data do not translate well to progress in clinical practice for humans. Leading examples include the minimal citations of chimpanzee research that is relevant to human medicine, the highly different pathology of HIV/AIDS and hepatitis C virus infection in the two species, the lack of correlation in the efficacy of vaccines and treatments between chimpanzees and humans, and the fact that chimpanzees are not useful for research on human cancer. The major molecular differences underlying these inter-species phenotypic disparities have been revealed by comparative genomics and molecular biology - there are key differences in all aspects of gene expression and protein function, from chromosome and chromatin structure to post-translational modification. The collective effects of these differences are striking, extensive and widespread, and they show that the superficial similarity between human and chimpanzee genetic sequences is of little consequence for biomedical research. The extrapolation of biomedical data from the chimpanzee to the human is therefore highly unreliable, and the use of the chimpanzee must be considered of little value, particularly given the breadth and potential of alternative methods of enquiry that are currently available to science.
Article
Full-text available
The vast majority of health-related observational studies are not prospectively registered and the advantages of registration have not been fully appreciated. Nonetheless, international standards require approval of study protocols by an independent ethics committee before the study can begin. We suggest that there is an ethical and scientific imperative to publicly preregister key information from newly approved protocols, which should be required by funders. Ultimately, more complete information may be publicly available by disclosing protocols, analysis plans, data sets, and raw data.
Article
Full-text available
There is growing concern that poor experimental design and lack of transparent reporting contribute to the frequent failure of pre-clinical animal studies to translate into treatments for human disease. In 2010, the Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments (ARRIVE) guidelines were introduced to help improve reporting standards. They were published in PLOS Biology and endorsed by funding agencies and publishers and their journals, including PLOS, Nature research journals, and other top-tier journals. Yet our analysis of papers published in PLOS and Nature journals indicates that there has been very little improvement in reporting standards since then. This suggests that authors, referees, and editors generally are ignoring guidelines, and the editorial endorsement is yet to be effectively implemented.
Article
Full-text available
The vast majority of medical interventions introduced into clinical development prove unsafe or ineffective. One prominent explanation for the dismal success rate is flawed preclinical research. We conducted a systematic review of preclinical research guidelines and organized recommendations according to the type of validity threat (internal, construct, or external) or programmatic research activity they primarily address. We searched MEDLINE, Google Scholar, Google, and the EQUATOR Network website for all preclinical guideline documents published up to April 9, 2013 that addressed the design and conduct of in vivo animal experiments aimed at supporting clinical translation. To be eligible, documents had to provide guidance on the design or execution of preclinical animal experiments and represent the aggregated consensus of four or more investigators. Data from included guidelines were independently extracted by two individuals for discrete recommendations on the design and implementation of preclinical efficacy studies. These recommendations were then organized according to the type of validity threat they addressed. A total of 2,029 citations were identified through our search strategy. From these, we identified 26 guidelines that met our eligibility criteria-most of which were directed at neurological or cerebrovascular drug development. Together, these guidelines offered 55 different recommendations. Some of the most common recommendations included performance of a power calculation to determine sample size, randomized treatment allocation, and characterization of disease phenotype in the animal model prior to experimentation. By identifying the most recurrent recommendations among preclinical guidelines, we provide a starting point for developing preclinical guidelines in other disease domains. We also provide a basis for the study and evaluation of preclinical research practice. Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary.
Article
The press and other media constantly report news stories about dangerous chemicals in the environment, miracle cures, the safety of therapeutic treatments, and potential cancer-causing agents. But what exactly is actually meant by "increased risk"-should we worry if we are told that we are at twice the risk of developing an illness? And how do we interpret "reduced risk" to properly assess the benefits of noisily touted dietary supplements? Demonstrating the difficulty of separating the hype from the hypothesis, noted epidemiologist Michael Bracken clearly communicates how clinical epidemiology works. Using everyday terms, Bracken describes how professional scientists approach questions of disease causation and therapeutic efficacy to provide readers with the tools to help them understand whether warnings of environmental risk are truly warranted, or if claims of therapeutic benefit are justified.
Article
The mayor of London has launched a new initiative, designed to make the city pre-eminent in the commercialisation of the life sciences, not only in the United Kingdom but in Europe. Called MedCity, it will bring together the expertise of Imperial, University, and King’s colleges of the University of London with Oxford and Cambridge universities to create a “golden triangle” to help turn developments in bioscience into companies and jobs.The mayor, Boris Johnson, never one to shy away from a historical analogy, compared the initiative to the coming together of the “virtuosi” who launched the scientific revolution in London in the 17th century, meeting in coffee shops and setting up the Royal Society. In recent years, he argued at a launch at Imperial College’s Hammersmith campus, London had not been as successful as some other cities, failing to trumpet its scientific excellence or to generate as many new companies as were needed to exploit that excellence.He described “a valley of death” between breakthrough and commercialisation, which he blamed on the timidity of the financial sector and on “a certain fastidiousness in the scientific community about money.” While not condoning cultural greed or rapacity, he urged scientists to commercialise their ideas so that they could benefit humanity, and he said that MedCity would help make that possible.“Together with Oxford and Cambridge we form a ‘golden triangle’ of scientific innovation, and we need to channel that intellectual pre-eminence into a positive impact on our economy,” he said. “MedCity will span everything from research to clinical trials to manufacturing, across biotech, med tech, and health tech. I am in no doubt that having the whole ‘chain,’ from small spin-offs to massive companies, doing their research, clinical development and manufacturing here in London and the south east can be as important to our economy as the financial services sector is today.”Given the mayor’s ambitions, funding for the venture is modest: £2.92m (€3.5m; 4.8m)fromtheHigherEducationFundingCounciland£1.2mfromthemayorsoffice.ButElliottForster,whochairsMedCity,wasconfidentthatitcouldmakeadifference.Helikenedtheinitiativetoaconciergeservice,providingasinglefrontdoorforpeoplewithideas,capital,oradesiretocollaborate.Wewanttocreateacommunityofcollaboratorstomakeinnovationeasier,hesaid.Supportfortheideahasbeenfantastic.TraditionallytheUniversityofLondoncollegeshaveseenthemselvesascompetitors,ashaveOxfordandCambridgeuniversities,butPatrickMaxwell,whoheadstheschoolofclinicalmedicineatCambridgeandhasworkedinallfiveinstitutions,saiditwastimetosetthatrivalryaside.Theambitionthatanyonemighthavetobethebestintheworldisnotpossible,hesaid.Werebettertogether.Closeneighboursareoftengoodathavingdifferencesofopinion,butweneedtolookpastthat.Twosuccessfulspinoffcompanies,CircassiaandImanova,gaveaccountsoftheirprogress.LauraFlynn,vicepresidentofCircassia,whichdevelopsallergytreatmentsoriginallydiscoveredatImperialCollegein2006,saidthatithadaproductinphaseIIItrialsandhadrecentlycometomarket,raising£200minLondon.KevinCoxofImanova,whichworksindrugsforcancer,saidthatithadrecentlybeenboughtbyAstraZenecaforasumthatcouldreach4.8m) from the Higher Education Funding Council and £1.2m from the mayor’s office. But Elliott Forster, who chairs MedCity, was confident that it could make a difference. He likened the initiative to a concierge service, providing a single front door for people with ideas, capital, or a desire to collaborate. “We want to create a community of collaborators to make innovation easier,” he said. “Support for the idea has been fantastic.”Traditionally the University of London colleges have seen themselves as competitors, as have Oxford and Cambridge universities, but Patrick Maxwell, who heads the school of clinical medicine at Cambridge and has worked in all five institutions, said it was time to set that rivalry aside. “The ambition that any one might have to be the best in the world is not possible,” he said. “We’re better together. Close neighbours are often good at having differences of opinion, but we need to look past that.”Two successful spin-off companies, Circassia and Imanova, gave accounts of their progress. Laura Flynn, vice president of Circassia, which develops allergy treatments originally discovered at Imperial College in 2006, said that it had a product in phase III trials and had recently come to market, raising £200m in London. Kevin Cox of Imanova, which works in drugs for cancer, said that it had recently been bought by AstraZeneca for a sum that could reach 440m, depending on progress.Ara Darzi, formerly a junior health minister under Labour and now chairman of the London Health Commission, said, “MedCity is an exciting initiative for London and the whole country. My ambition for the commission is to help London become the healthiest big city in the world, and a successful and thriving life sciences sector is crucial to achieving this aim. During its call for evidence it has been clear to the commission that, with increased investment and collaboration, the life sciences in London can make a bigger contribution to London’s economy. The commission will be working closely with MedCity as it develops its recommendations that I will present to the mayor in the autumn.”
Article
Francis S. Collins and Lawrence A. Tabak discuss initiatives that the US National Institutes of Health is exploring to restore the self-correcting nature of preclinical research. A growing chorus of concern, from scientists and laypeople, contends that the complex system for ensuring the reproducibility of biomedical research is failing and is in need of restructuring 1,2 . As leaders of the US National Institutes of Health (NIH), we share this concern and here explore some of the significant interventions that we are planning. Science has long been regarded as ‘self-correcting’, given that it is founded on the replication of earlier work. Over the long term, that principle remains true. In the shorter term, however, the checks and balances that once ensured scientific fidelity have been hobbled. This has compromised the ability of today’s researchers to reproduce others’ findings.
Article
The increase in annual global investment in biomedical research--reaching US$240 billion in 2010--has resulted in important health dividends for patients and the public. However, much research does not lead to worthwhile achievements, partly because some studies are done to improve understanding of basic mechanisms that might not have relevance for human health. Additionally, good research ideas often do not yield the anticipated results. As long as the way in which these ideas are prioritised for research is transparent and warranted, these disappointments should not be deemed wasteful; they are simply an inevitable feature of the way science works. However, some sources of waste cannot be justified. In this report, we discuss how avoidable waste can be considered when research priorities are set. We have four recommendations. First, ways to improve the yield from basic research should be investigated. Second, the transparency of processes by which funders prioritise important uncertainties should be increased, making clear how they take account of the needs of potential users of research. Third, investment in additional research should always be preceded by systematic assessment of existing evidence. Fourth, sources of information about research that is in progress should be strengthened and developed and used by researchers. Research funders have primary responsibility for reductions in waste resulting from decisions about what research to do.