Article

Persistence of literacy problems: Spelling in adolescence and at mid-life

MRC Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry Centre, King's College London Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry (Impact Factor: 6.46). 03/2009; 50(8):893-901. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02079.x
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT

Developmental reading problems show strong persistence across the school years; less is known about poor readers' later progress in literacy skills.
Poor (n = 42) and normally developing readers (n = 86) tested in adolescence (ages 14/15 years) in the Isle of Wight epidemiological studies were re-contacted at mid-life (ages 44/45 years). Participants completed a spelling test, and reported on educational qualifications, perceived adult spelling competence, and problems in day-to-day literacy tasks.
Individual differences in spelling were highly persistent across this 30-year follow-up, with correlations between spelling at ages 14 and 44 years of r = .91 (p < .001) for poor readers and r = .89 (p < .001) for normally developing readers. Poor readers' spelling remained markedly impaired at mid-life, with some evidence that they had fallen further behind over the follow-up period. Taking account of adolescent spelling levels, continued exposure to reading and literacy demands in adolescence and early adulthood was independently predictive of adult spelling in both samples; family social background added further to prediction among normally developing readers only.
By adolescence, individual differences in spelling and its related sub-skills are highly stable. Encouraging young people with reading disabilities to maintain their exposure to reading and writing may be advantageous in the longer term.

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: Andrew Pickles
  • Source
    • "This disorder occurs in approximately 7% of school-aged children across cultures[Shaywitz et al., 1990;Zhang et al., 1998]. Dyslexia usually persists into adulthood and severely affects academic performance, career development and quality of life[Maughan et al., 2009]. Accurate identification of dyslexic individuals and ascertaining the neural substrates of dyslexia remain challenging[Gabrieli, 2009]. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Developmental dyslexia has been hypothesized to result from multiple causes and exhibit multiple manifestations, implying a distributed multidimensional effect on human brain. The disruption of specific white-matter (WM) tracts/regions has been observed in dyslexic children. However, it remains unknown if developmental dyslexia affects the human brain WM in a multidimensional manner. Being a natural tool for evaluating this hypothesis, the multivariate machine learning approach was applied in this study to compare 28 school-aged dyslexic children with 33 age-matched controls. Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and diffusion tensor imaging were acquired to extract five multitype WM features at a regional level: white matter volume, fractional anisotropy, mean diffusivity, axial diffusivity, and radial diffusivity. A linear support vector machine (LSVM) classifier achieved an accuracy of 83.61% using these MRI features to distinguish dyslexic children from controls. Notably, the most discriminative features that contributed to the classification were primarily associated with WM regions within the putative reading network/system (e.g., the superior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, thalamocortical projections, and corpus callosum), the limbic system (e.g., the cingulum and fornix), and the motor system (e.g., the cerebellar peduncle, corona radiata, and corticospinal tract). These results were well replicated using a logistic regression classifier. These findings provided direct evidence supporting a multidimensional effect of developmental dyslexia on WM connectivity of human brain, and highlighted the involvement of WM tracts/regions beyond the well-recognized reading system in dyslexia. Finally, the discriminating results demonstrated a potential of WM neuroimaging features as imaging markers for identifying dyslexic individuals. Hum Brain Mapp, 2016. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Full-text · Article · Jan 2016 · Human Brain Mapping
  • Source
    • "Beidas,Khateb &amp; Breznitz, 2013;Re, Tressoldi, Cornoldi &amp; Lucangeli, 2014). The low self-estimation of the ability to read is typical of students with dyslexia (Maughan et al., 2009;Schulte-Körne, Deimel &amp; Remschmidt, 1997). The results from the students' self-report are purely descriptive, and the focus here is on the comparison between the estimates of the different abilities. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Broadened recruitment to higher education is on the agenda in many countries, and it is also widely recognized that the number of dyslexic students entering higher education is increasing. In Sweden, as in many other European countries, higher education institutions are required to accommodate students with dyslexia. The present study focuses on the study outcome for 50 students with diagnosed dyslexia, mainly in teacher education and nurses' training, at three universities in Northern Sweden. The students trusted their own ability to find information on the Internet but mistrusted their own abilities in reading course books and articles in English and in taking notes. The mean rate of study was 23.5 European Credit Transfer and Accumulation System credits per semester, which is slightly below the national baseline of 26.7. The results show that more than half of the students are examined at a normal rate of study but that about one fifth have a very low rate of study. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Key Messages Most students with dyslexia can compensate for their reading problems.Taking notes during lessons and reading in foreign language may be especially difficult for students with dyslexia.Diagnoses should distinguish between reading comprehension and word decoding.More than half of the students with dyslexia can achieve at a normal rate of study.One-fifth of the students with dyslexia may need a longer period of study than other students.
    Full-text · Article · Oct 2015 · Dyslexia
  • Source
    • "persist into adulthood (e.g., Høien & Lundberg, 2000; cf. Maughan et al., 2009). It is therefore important to specifically understand the nature and correlates of spelling and writing skills or disabilities. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Writing difficulties are common among children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but the nature of these difficulties has not been well studied. Here we relate behavioral, psycholinguistic, cognitive (memory/executive), and graphomotor measures to spelling skills in school-age girls with ADHD (n = 30) and an age-matched group of typically developed spellers (TYPSP, n = 35). When subdividing the ADHD group into those with poor (ADHDPSP, n = 19) and typical spelling (ADHDTYPSP, n = 11), the two subgroups did not differ with regard to inattentive or hyperactive-impulsive symptom severity according to parent or teacher ratings. Both ADHD subgroups also had equally severe difficulties in graphomotor control-handwriting and (parent ratings of) written expression as compared to the TYPSP group. In contrast, ADHDPSP had problems relative to ADHDTYPSP and TYPSP on phonological and orthographic recoding (choice tasks) and verbal memory (digit span) and were more likely to make commissions on a continuous performance task (CPT). Further analyses using the collapsed ADHD group showed that both digit span and the presence of CPT commissions predicted spelling performance independently of each other. Finally, results showed that phonological recoding skills mediated the association between digit span and spelling performance in ADHD. Theoretical and educational implications are discussed.
    Full-text · Article · Dec 2012 · Journal of Learning Disabilities
Show more