ArticlePDF Available

Aging and foraging efficiency in an orb-web spider

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Aging is often associated with reduced behavioral performance such as decreased locomotion or food consumption, related to a deterioration in physiological functions. In orb-web spiders, webs are used to capture prey and aging can affect web-building behavior and web structure. Here, we investigated the effect of aging on prey capture in the orb-web spider Zygiella x-notata. The ability of adult females to capture flies was examined at different ages. The rate of prey capture did not change with age, but older spiders took more time to subdue and capture the prey. Alterations which appeared in web structure with age (increase in the number of anomalies affecting radii and capture spiral) affected prey capture behavior. Furthermore, the analysis of individual performance (carried out on 17 spiders at two different ages) showed that older females spent more time handling the prey and finding it in the web. Our results suggest that, in the laboratory, age does not affect prey capture rates but it influences prey capture behavior by affecting web structure or/and spider motor functions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ARTICLE
Aging and foraging efficiency in an orb-web spider
Myle
`ne Anotaux Camille Toscani
Raymond Leborgne Nicolas Cha
ˆline
Alain Pasquet
Received: 24 March 2013 / Accepted: 25 April 2014
ÓJapan Ethological Society and Springer Japan 2014
Abstract Aging is often associated with reduced behav-
ioral performance such as decreased locomotion or food
consumption, related to a deterioration in physiological
functions. In orb-web spiders, webs are used to capture prey
and aging can affect web-building behavior and web struc-
ture. Here, we investigated the effect of aging on prey capture
in the orb-web spider Zygiella x-notata. The ability of adult
females to capture flies was examined at different ages. The
rate of prey capture did not change with age, but older spiders
took more time to subdue and capture the prey. Alterations
which appeared in web structure with age (increase in the
number of anomalies affecting radii and capture spiral)
affected prey capture behavior. Furthermore, the analysis of
individual performance (carried out on 17 spiders at two
different ages) showed that older females spent more time
handling the prey and finding it in the web. Our results
suggest that, in the laboratory, age does not affect prey
capture rates but it influences prey capture behavior by
affecting web structure or/and spider motor functions.
Keywords Aging Prey capture efficiency Orb-web
spider Web structure Zygiella x-notata
Introduction
Aging is a progressive natural process in which deteriora-
tion of physiological functions is often associated with
deficits in behavioral performance, which ultimately lead
to death (Arking 1998). Many studies on vertebrate models,
both in the laboratory and natural populations, have led to a
better understanding of aging mechanisms and its conse-
quences for the organism (Austad and Fischer 1991;
Holmes and Austad 1995; Perret and Aujard 2006; Ricklefs
2010). In invertebrates, dipterans such as Drosophila
melanogaster have been used extensively to study age-
related behavioral changes (Grotewiel et al. 2005; Simon
et al. 2006; Lliadi and Boulianne 2010; Jones and
Grotewiel 2011). Most of the aging studies in flies focused
on the decline in behavioral functions linked with age,
including memory, olfaction, and biological rhythms (for
review, see Grotewiel et al. 2005). Furthermore, several
studies have demonstrated a decrease in locomotion with
increasing age in insects (Le Bourg and Minois 1999;
Ridgel et al. 2003). This loss in mobility could influence an
animal’s ability to acquire resources. Food intake is one of
the vital functions that decrease with age. In mammals,
aging is also associated with declines in food consumption
(quantity and quality ingested) (Blanton et al. 1998; McCue
1995). Increasing age has also been shown to be associated
with a decrease in foraging efficiency in the honeybee
Apis mellifera (Tofilsky 2000) and other invertebrates
(Grotewiel et al. 2005; Moya-Larano 2002). However,
none of these studies examined the link between locomotor
abilities and foraging efficiency in aging animals. Orb-web
M. Anotaux and C. Toscani equally contributed to this study.
M. Anotaux (&)R. Leborgne A. Pasquet
Laboratoire Expression et Evolution des Comportements,
Faculte
´des Sciences et Techniques, Universite
´de Lorraine, BP
239 Bld des Aiguillettes, 54506 Vandoeuvre-Les-Nancy, France
e-mail: m.anotaux@voila.fr
C. Toscani (&)
Ecole nationale ve
´te
´rinaire d’Alfort, UMR 7179 CNRS MNHN,
Universite
´Paris-Est, 94704 Maisons-Alfort, France
e-mail: toscani.camille@hotmail.fr
N. Cha
ˆline
Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expe
´rimentale et Compare
´e EA 4443,
Universite
´Paris 13, 99 avenue J.B. Cle
´ment,
93430 Villetaneuse, France
123
J Ethol
DOI 10.1007/s10164-014-0404-6
spiders can be used for this purpose because there is a
direct link between locomotor activity and prey capture
efficiency (Foelix 2011). In fact, spiders first use their legs
(and their whole body) to build a web that will intercept
prey and second to capture prey according to a specific
behavioral sequence of capture (go to the prey, catch it,
immobilize it, and, for some spiders, transport it) (Foelix
2011). Web construction and prey capture are thus two
complementary aspects of orb-spider foraging behavior.
Orb-web spiders are sit-and-wait predators that, unlike
Lycosidae or Salticidae spiders, do not actively search for
prey but instead invest time and energy in the construction
of an orb-web, a geometrical structure which serves as a
trap for prey capture (Heiling and Herberstein 2000; Scharf
et al. 2011). Once the web is built, the spiders wait in the
center or hide in a retreat until a prey strikes the structure.
Prey capture efficiency depends on the orb-web capacity to
(1) intercept prey, (2) inform the spider about the location
of the prey, (3) retain prey long enough for the spider to
subdue it before it escapes or tumbles away, and (4) on the
spider’s capture behavior after prey interception (Coslov-
sky and Zschokke 2009; Sensenig et al. 2011). These
capture abilities depend on silk properties and on the
amount of silk invested for web construction (capture
thread length, capture area) as well as on the arrangement
of the radii and capture threads (Vollrath 1992; Foelix
2011; Harmer et al. 2011). The arrangement of radii and
capture threads is particularly important for successful
capture because, when the spider hides in a retreat at the
edge of the web, silky threads serve to intercept and retain
the prey by absorbing the energy of the struggling prey
without breaking. Vibrations are also transmitted by radii
and a signal thread connecting the center of the web to the
retreat (Zschokke 2000). Recently, we demonstrated that
aging affects orb-web structure. Webs built by old spiders
are smaller, geometrically less regular, and have more
anomalies than webs built by younger spiders (Anotaux
et al. 2012); we suggested that these web modifications
could be due to a decline in spider mobility during web
construction and/or in neurological function.
In this study, we used the orb-web spider Zygiella x-
notata (Araneae, Araneidae, Clerk) to test the effects of age
on foraging efficiency. In this species, spiders build an orb-
web and then wait for a prey in a retreat. When a prey
strikes the web, the spider captures it by following a
characteristic capture behavioral sequence: after
approaching the prey, it immobilizes it by biting and
wrapping it with silk, then it transports the prey to the
retreat to consume it. By comparing prey capture sequen-
ces, their duration and the frequency of their different
components with age, we expect that (1) the rate of prey
capture decreases with aging, (2) the duration of the
behavioral acts increases, and (3) the access to prey and
prey manipulation to subdue it is more difficult.
Materials and methods
Zygiella x-notata is a widespread medium-sized spider
(carapace width for adult females: 1.5 mm) in northern
Europe, which builds its web preferentially in the vicinity
of human buildings. It constructs an orb-web, which is
generally characterized by the presence of a free sector in
the upper part (Fig. 1), and feeds primarily on flying prey
(generally Diptera). In eastern France, its development
cycle is annual: the juveniles leave the eggs sacs at the
beginning of spring, reproduction starts in summer with
mating, females lay eggs in September–October, and
juveniles hatch 3 weeks later and stay in cocoons until
spring. While males die after reproduction, the majority of
Fig. 1 An orb-web built by
Zygiella x-notata. The standard
method used by the spider to
reach the prey (here represented
by the fly Lucilia caesar in the
middle of the lower section of
the web) is shown in red.An
example of an anomaly is also
shown (i.e. discontinuous
thread)
J Ethol
123
females disappear when winter arrives, but some can sur-
vive to the next spring (Jones 1983; The
´venard et al. 2004;
Bel-Venner and Venner 2006). Once adult, the lifespan of
females is approximately from 5 to 7 months (Juberthie
1954; The
´venard et al. 2004).
The spiders used in this study were captured as subad-
ults (in August and September) and reared in the laboratory
in plastic boxes (10 9792.5 cm). They were sprayed
with water and fed once a week with a fly (Lucilia caesar).
They had their last meal 4 days before the tests. All the
spiders used for the tests molted and became adults in the
laboratory, so their exact adult age was known. They were
all virgin females.
Since the body mass of spiders may influence web
construction characteristics and varies with age (Venner
et al. 2003), spiders were weighed before being placed in
experimental conditions (balance: Sartorius BASIC
BA110S, precision 0.1 mg).
Procedure
To collect data on prey capture efficiency, spiders were
placed individually into wooden frames (50 950 9
10 cm) closed by two windowpanes in which they could
build their web. Frames were incubated under controlled
conditions (temperature 20°–22°, hygrometry 55 %, and
luminous cycle of 12 h, light from 0800 to 2000 hours) for
72 h. The spiders were then put back into their respective
boxes once they had made a web or after a maximum of
72 h even if they had not constructed a web. The presence
of a web was checked every day. As soon as a spider
completed a web, the frame was opened and web param-
eters were directly measured using electronic callipers.
Photos (Lumix FZ18 camera) were taken by placing webs
in front of a black panel using artificial light.
After web measurements, we carried out a prey capture
trial. For this, a living prey was placed in the middle of the
lower section of the web (Fig. 1) and then we observed and
quantified spider capture behaviors. The prey used was a
fly (Lucilia caesar) of smaller size than the spider but that
could be considered as a large prey for this spider species
(prey size approximately 2/3 of the spider size and with a
weight of 25 ±5 mg). The observations of spider behavior
began once the prey became entangled in the web.
Parameters
Web parameters
From direct measurements, we estimated the spider’s
investment in the web by calculating the total length of the
capture spiral (capture thread length, CTL) (following
Venner et al. 2000,2001 method).
Anomalies
Anomalies in web construction were identified on photos
and counted (as defined by Pasquet et al. 2013; Fig. 2).
Anomalies can affect the radii or capture spiral, which is the
silky area defined by the presence of sticky threads. For
radii, we counted: the number of supernumerary, deviated,
and ‘Y’ radii (Pasquet et al. 2013; Fig. 2a). For the capture
spiral, we counted the number of stops and returns, holes,
silk threads of the capture spiral stuck together between two
radii, and the number of nonparallel and discontinuous silk
threads in the capture spiral (Pasquet et al. 2013; Fig. 2b).
Prey capture parameters
Before placing the prey in the web, all spiders were in the
same position: they were all in their retreat with a foreleg
(L1) in contact with a signal thread (Fig. 1). We took into
account the following parameters:
Contact latency Its first movement characterizes the
first reaction of the spider after the prey is introduced
into the web; contact latency is the time between this
first movement and the first contact with the prey. In
general, the spider reaches the prey following the
sequence: retreat, signal thread, center of the web and
prey (see Fig. 1).
Handling time Handling is a spider behavior (bites and
wrappings) used to subdue the prey. Handling time
begins after the first contact between the spider and the
prey and stops when the spider leaves the capture site
with or without the prey.
Number of bites A bite is characterized by the
introduction of the spider’s fangs into the prey. A
spider can bite the prey several times during a capture
before it finds a suitable site (i.e. articulation of the prey
or specific places with a thinner cuticle). A bite stops
when the spider’s fangs leave the prey. The number of
bites was noted. There was no way of knowing whether
a spider bite was associated with an injection of venom.
Number of wrappings Wrapping is a special behavior
during which the spider uses silk to immobilize the
prey. During this behavior, the silk is extruded from the
spinnerets and projected onto the prey; it is visible and
easy for the observer to count the number of wrapping
sequences. This behavior may be reproduced several
times during a capture. For each prey capture, we took
into account the number of wrappings.
Prey transport time After subduing the prey, the spider
then transports it to its retreat before ingestion. To
transport the prey back to the retreat, the spider hangs it
on its spinnerets. Several events can interrupt the
transport on the way back: the prey can become
J Ethol
123
entangled in the sticky spiral and the spider can lose it,
or the spider can go the wrong way and not find the best
way back to its retreat. The spider might also return to
its retreat without the prey and stay motionless before
coming back and going on with its previous activity.
Each interruption during its return to the retreat was
counted, and the total duration of all interruptions was
included in the time of transport, which ended when the
spider reached its retreat with the prey.
Activity time This is the total time during which the
spider was actively capturing the prey. This was
calculated by adding the contact latency, the handling
time, and the transport time.
Statistical analyses
Variation in predatory performance with age could be
observed at two different levels: the inter-individual level
between spiders of different ages or the intra-individual
level by comparing the capture performance of spiders at
two different ages.
For the inter-individual analysis, a total of 78 spiders
(aged from 17 to 261 days) weighing between 12 and
73 mg were used. Multiple linear regressions, using the
‘lm’ function of the ‘nlme’ package (Pinheiro et al. 2005)
for R (v.2.15.0), were performed where (1) age and body
mass were the independent variables and web parameter
Fig. 2 a The different type of
radius anomalies defined for
Zygiella x-notata orb-webs
(from Pasquet et al. 2013). A
‘super-numerary radius’ takes
its origin from a spiral thread
unit and not from the center of
the web; a ‘deviated radius’
presents a deviation [5°
compared to a rectilinear
trajectory; a ‘Y’ radius is a
radius separated abnormally
into two radii. bThe different
types of capture spiral
anomalies defined for Zygiella
x-notata orb-webs (Pasquet
et al. 2013). A ‘return’ is when
two spiral units end in one point
(triangular ending) or two points
(rectangular ending) of a radius,
interrupting the spiral; a ‘hole’
is defined by the absence of at
least one spiral unit between
two adjacent spiral units; ‘two
or more spiral units stuck
together’ and ‘discontinuity of
the spiral’ define anomalies of
interruptions in the capture
spiral; ‘non-parallel spiral units’
defines a spiral thread stuck to
another one on a radius, forming
a triangle
J Ethol
123
was the dependant variable, and (2) age, body mass, CTL,
number of anomalies affecting radii, and number of
anomalies of the capture spiral were the independent
variables and each prey capture parameter was the depen-
dant variable. In the tables showing the results of multiple
linear regression analysis, the multiple regression coeffi-
cient ‘b’ referred to a non-standardized coefficient, and
‘‘ b’ to a standardized coefficient.
In the second analysis, we compared the performance of
17 spiders tested at two different ages. The spiders used in
this analysis were at least 50 days older between test 1 and
test 2 (age of the spiders for test 1: mean =109 ±19 days;
and age for test 2: mean =197 ±33 days). The age of the
laboratory-reared spiders in test 1 was similar to that reached
by adult females in natural conditions (Bel-Venner and
Venner 2006), whereas the age of the spiders in test 2 was
much higher than the average age in natural conditions. To
take into account repeated measurements for each spider,
linear mixed-effects regression models, using the ‘lmer’
function of the ‘lme4’ package (Bates et al. 2013) for R, were
carried out where (1) age and body mass were the indepen-
dent variables, and web parameters were the dependant
variables, and (2) age, body mass, CTL, number of anomalies
affecting radii, and number of anomalies of the capture spiral
were the independent variables, and each prey capture
parameter was the dependant variable. Random individual
effects accounted for repeated measures. The final restricted
maximum-likelihood model, including only significant
effects, was achieved by deletion of the non-significant
interactions and additive effects from the primary model
using the backward stepwise method. Ftests of the signifi-
cance of effects were computed with models derived from
the final model. Pvalues for each effect were obtained by
likelihood ratio tests of the full model with the effect in
question against the model without the effect in question.
A Shapiro–Wilk test was applied to determine whether
the sample data were likely to derive from a normally
distributed population. Most variables were not normally
distributed, thus most prey capture parameters were loga-
rithmically transformed. The normality of model residuals
was verified by calculating a Q–Q plot and a Q–Q line.
Significance was considered at p\0.05.
Results
Variation in the predatory performance with age
at Inter-individual level
Relationship between age and web properties
All 78 webs constructed by the spiders retained the prey
deposited in the lower region of the web. Thus, the rate of
prey capture by web retention was 100 %. The two-pre-
dictor model was able to account for 20 % of the variance
in CTL (F
2,75
=9.51, p\0.001, R
2
=0.20). Body mass
had a significant positive coefficient, indicating that spiders
with higher body mass are expected to build webs with a
longer CTL (Table 1). Spider age had a significant negative
coefficient, indicating that older spiders built smaller webs
(Fig. 3). Multiple linear regression analysis, with capture
spiral anomalies (F
3,69
=1.19, p=0.32, R
2
=0.05) or
radius anomalies (F
3,69
=0.35, p=0.79, R
2
=0.02),
found that none of the independent variables contributed to
the multiple regression models.
Relationship between age and prey capture parameters
Since all spiders successfully caught the prey, the rate of
capture due to capture behavior was 100 %. Multiple linear
regression analysis was conducted to examine the rela-
tionship between prey capture behavior—contact latency,
handling time, number of bites, number of wrappings,
transport time, and activity time—and five potential pre-
dictors—age and spider body mass, CTL, number of cap-
ture spiral anomalies, and number of radius anomalies. The
best model for predicting the contact latency only included
the independent variables: body mass, age, number of radii
Table 1 Results of linear regression analyses of variance of CTL
Variable Multiple regression coefficients t
bb
Intercept 469.164 6.35***
Body mass 5.59 0.31 2.80**
Age -1.68 -0.45 -4.10***
** p\0.01, *** p\0.001
Fig. 3 Partial regression plot for the dependent variable CTL and the
independent variable age
J Ethol
123
anomalies, and the interaction between body mass and age
(F
4,68
=2.07, p=0.09, R
2
=0.11). Only the number of
radius anomalies had a significant positive coefficient,
indicating that the spider needed more time to detect the
prey when there was a higher number of radii anomalies
(Table 2; Fig. 4).
To predict the variance in handling time, only the
number of radius anomalies was removed, and the four
predictor model was able to account for 30 % of the var-
iance in handling time (F
4,65
=7.06, p\0.001,
R
2
=0.30). Body mass and CTL had significant negative
coefficient, indicating that spiders with higher body mass
or who built webs with a higher CTL were expected to
handle prey for shorter periods (Table 3; Fig. 5).
The two predictors used in the best model accounted for
10 % of the variance of the transport time (F
2,74
=3.90,
p=0.02, R
2
=0.10). Body mass had a significant nega-
tive regression coefficient, indicating that larger spiders
spent less time transporting their prey (Table 4).
In contrast, age had a significant positive coefficient,
indicating that older spiders transport their prey more
slowly (Fig. 6). When multiple linear regression analysis
was carried out with bite number and the number of
wrappings, none of the independent variables contributed
to the multiple regression models.
Only three of the independent variables were used in the
best model and these were able to explain 31 % of the
variance of the activity time (F
3,74
=11.06, p\0.001,
R
2
=0.31). Once again, body mass and CTL had signifi-
cant negative regression coefficient, indicating that spiders
with higher body mass or who made webs with a higher
CTL were expected to be active for shorter time periods
(Table 5). Age had a significant positive coefficient,
Table 2 Results of linear regression analyses of variance of contact
latency
Variable Multiple regression
coefficients
t
bb
Intercept -1.74e-01 -1.10
Body mass 7.77e-03 0.53 1.70
Age 1.63e-03 0.56 1.62
Number of radius anomalies 1.46e-02 0.29 2.41*
Body mass:age -4.48e-05 -0.85 -1.64
*p\0.05
Fig. 4 Partial regression plot for the dependant variable contact
latency and the independent variable number of radius anomalies
Table 3 Results of linear regression analyses of variance of handling
time
Variable Multiple regression
coefficients
t
bb
Intercept 2.64 23.14****
Body mass -0.62e-02 -0.31 -2.63**
Age 0.26e-03 0.06 0.53
Number of capture spiral
anomalies
-0.28e-02 -0.18 -1.69*
CTL -0.37e-03 -0.32 -2.74***
*p\0.05, ** p\0.01, *** p\0.001, **** p\0.0001
Fig. 5 Partial regression plot for the dependant variable handling
time and the independent variable CTL
Table 4 Results of linear regression analyses of variance of transport
time
Variable Multiple regression coefficients t
bb
Intercept 1.171 5.50***
Body mass -0.014 -0.67 -2.48*
Age 0.002 0.46 2.07*
*p\0.05, *** p\0.001
J Ethol
123
suggesting that older spiders were active for longer periods
of time (Fig. 7).
Variation in the predatory performance with age
at the intra-individual level
Relationship between age and web properties
Linear mixed-effect analysis was used to study the effects
of age and body mass on web parameters (CTL, number of
capture spiral anomalies and number of radius anomalies)
for spiders at two different ages. None of the predictors,
however, was able to explain the variance in the dependent
variables.
Relationship between age and prey capture properties
Linear mixed-effects regression models showed that there
was a significant association between age and handling
time (v
2
=7.24, p=0.007), with an increase in handling
time of 1.43 ±1.95 s for each day increase in age.
Transport time varied significantly with the number of
radius anomalies (v
2
=16.40, p=0.006), increasing with
the increase in radius anomalies (log, 0.05 ±0.04 s for
each radius anomalies). Age tended to affect transport time
(v
2
=3.52, p=0.06), increasing it by log: 0.07 ±0.01 s
each day. Activity time varied significantly with age
(v
2
=11.80, p\0.001) and CTL (v
2
=4.03, p=0.04),
increasing for each day increase in age (log,
0.020 ±0.005 s) and for each increase in CTL of one cm
(log, 0.007 ±0.001 s). However, none of the predictors
were able to explain the variance in contact latency.
Discussion
In the present study, we examined the effect of age on the
efficiency and behavioral changes in spider foraging.
Contrary to our predictions, the rate of prey capture by
spiders did not differ with age. The prey (flies) was directly
deposited in the web and thus it was perhaps easier for the
spiders to capture it than in nature. Our results showed that
with age there are some changes in time management
during spider capture behavior, and that age-induced
changes in web properties also appeared to have an indirect
effect on these behaviors.
The consequences of aging on behavioral performance
are variable depending on the activity (foraging, repro-
duction, locomotion; Grotewiel et al. 2005). These are
Fig. 6 Partial regression plot for the dependant variable transport
time and the independent variable age
Table 5 Results of linear regression analyses of variance of activity
time
Variable Multiple regression coefficients t
bb
Intercept 2.51 23.77***
Body mass -0.83e-02 -0.37 -3.42**
Age 0.12e-02 0.26 2.33*
CTL -0.04e-02 -0.29 -2.68**
*p\0.05, ** p\0.01, *** p\0.001
Fig. 7 Partial regression plot for the dependant variable activity time and the independent variables age and CTL
J Ethol
123
often linked to a decrease in locomotor activity in ver-
tebrates (i.e. in rats; Altun et al. 2007) and invertebrates
(i.e. in nematodes; Murakami and Murakami 2005), or in
Drosophila (Le Bourg and Minois 1999). In particular,
in Drosophila melanogaster, locomotor behavior, such as
negative geotaxis, exploratory activity, fast phototaxis,
and flight ability, decline with age (Grotewiel et al.
2005). In spiders, it has also been shown that age can
influence locomotor behavior, i.e., Lycosa tarantula,
(Araneae, Lycosidae), an iteroparous species that repro-
duces in two successive years, is more productive in the
first year than in the second (Moya-Larano 2002). Fur-
thermore, in this latter study, the author showed that, in
the second year, females were less active with a lower
locomotor activity than the first-year females. For the
orb-web spider Zygiella x-notata, it was recently shown
that aging affects web geometry, and thus web-building
behavior (Anotaux et al. 2012; Toscani et al. 2012). In
these papers, the authors also suggested that these age-
induced changes in the webs could have a locomotor
origin with an associated degradation of the mechanical
properties of the spiders’ legs.
In our study, activity time during prey capture increased
with age; comparison of prey capture parameters for the
same spider at least 50 days later showed that this increase
in the activity time was due to an increase in handling time.
It is possible that the locomotor behavior used to immo-
bilize the prey becomes more difficult with age because old
spiders tire more rapidly during the struggle with prey than
younger spiders.
It is known that body mass can lead to variations in web-
building behavior (Venner et al. 2003; Kunter et al. 2010)
and foraging effort (Venner et al. 2003). When spider body
mass increased, the amount of silk used per web decreased,
while their foraging effort increased (Venner et al. 2003).
As the feeding conditions were uniform for all spiders used
in our laboratory experiment, body mass should not have
had an effect and cannot explain differences in capture
performance. However, in nature, spiders rarely feed to
satiation and body mass may play an important role in
determining differences in performance in young and old
adults.
We also showed that changes in web construction affect
foraging effort: the activity time increased and spiders took
longer to handle the prey when less silk was used to make a
web. Anotaux et al. (2012) suggested that spiders invest
less silk in their web with aging because aging affects the
glands that produce spiral silk and leads to a reduction in
silk reserves. In our study, it appeared that, to compensate
for the decrease in silk investment, which occurs with age
and can lower the probability of capturing prey by reducing
the capture area, spiders took longer to reach the prey to
ensure that it did not escape.
Our results showed that the contact latency between the
spider and the prey increased with the number of anomalies
affecting radii; spiders took more time to detect the prey
because radii with anomalies do not correctly transmit prey
vibrations, or because spiders are less skilled at moving on
radii affected by anomalies. This could also explain why, in
the intra-individual approach, transport time was found to
increase with the number of anomalies.
We found a significant relationship between mass/age
and some web properties at the inter-individual level but
not in the second approach based on intra-individual rela-
tionships. This difference is probably due to a methodo-
logical problem. For the inter-individual comparison, 78
spiders aged from 17 to 261 days were used whereas, in the
intra-individual comparison, we used only 17 spiders and
the range of ages was reduced (range, 109–197 days) (see
‘‘ Materials and methods’). Thus in the second case, we
reduced the variability in spider age and mass and this
could explain the absence of a relationship between these
parameters and web properties found in the inter-individual
comparison.
Initially, we expected that prey capture efficiency would
decrease with age. However, the observed changes in
handling behavior and age-induced changes in web
geometry did not alter prey capture rates. Nevertheless, the
time to complete capture behavior activities did increase
with age and access to prey appeared to be more difficult.
These findings therefore suggest that, although in the lab-
oratory aging did not have a dramatic impact on spider
capture performance, in the wild, these age-induced chan-
ges in behavior could affect spider survival due to longer
exposure to predators.
Acknowledgments The University of Nancy supported this work
with a grant to M. Anotaux. The study is supported by a grant of the
CNRS program ‘‘Longe
´vite
´et Vieillissement’’. We thank the
reviewers for their invaluable comments. We also thank J. Marchal
and L. Bahans who helped collect the spiders in 2008 and 2009 and
assisted with spider breeding in the laboratory in 2009. Finally, we
thank Dr. Leigh Gebbie, LGK Australia, for corrections.
References
Altun M, Bergman E, Edstrom E, Johnson H, Ulfhake B (2007)
Behavioral impairments of the aging rat. Physiol Behav
92:911–923
Anotaux M, Marchal J, Cha
ˆline N, Desquilbet L, Leborgne R, Gilbert
C, Pasquet A (2012) Ageing alters spider orb-web construction.
Anim Behav 84:1113–1121
Arking R (1998) The biology of aging: observations and principles.
Sinauer, Sunderland
Austad SN, Fischer KE (1991) Mammalian aging, metabolism, and
ecology: evidence from the bats and marsupials. J Gerontol
46:B47–B53
Bates DM, Maechler M, Bolker B (2013) lme4: linear mixed-effects
models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999999-2
J Ethol
123
Bel-Venner MC, Venner S (2006) Mate-guarding strategies and male
competitive ability: results from a field study in an orb-weaving
spider. Anim Behav 71:1315–1322
Blanton CA, Horwitz BA, Murtagh-Mark C, Gietzen DW, Griffey
SM, McDonald RB (1998) Meal patterns associated with the
age-related decline in food intake in the Fischer 344 rat. Am
Physiol Soc 44(5):R1494–R1502
Coslovsky M, Zschokke S (2009) Asymmetry in orb-webs: an
adaptation to web building costs? J Insect Behav 22:29–38
Foelix RF (2011) Biology of spiders, 3rd edn. Oxford University
Press, Oxford
Grotewiel MS, Martin I, Bhandari P, Cook-Wiens E (2005)
Functional senescence in Drosophila melanogaster. Ageing
Res Rev 4:372–397
Harmer AMT, Blackledge TA, Madin JS, Herberstein ME (2011)
High-performance spider webs: integrating biomechanics, ecol-
ogy and behaviour. J R Soc Interface 8:457–471
Heiling AM, Herberstein ME (2000) Interpretations of orb-web
variability: a review of past and current ideas. Ekol Bratisl
19:97–106
Holmes DJ, Austad SN (1995) The evolution of avian senescence
patterns: implications for understanding primary aging pro-
cesses. Am Zool 35:307–317
Jones D (1983) Guide des Araigne
´es et des Opilions d’Europe.
Delachaux et Niestle
´, Paris
Jones M, Grotewiel MS (2011) Drosophila as a model for age-related
impairment in locomotor and others behaviors. Exp Gerontol
46(5):320–325
Juberthie C (1954) Sur les cycles biologiques des Araigne
´es. Bull Soc
Hist Nat Toulouse 89:299–318
Kunter M, Gregoric M, Li D (2010) Mass predicts web asymmetry in
Nephila spiders. Naturwissenschaften 97:1097–1105
Le Bourg E, Minois N (1999) A mild stress, hypergravity exposure,
postpones behavioral aging in Drosophila melanogaster. Ger-
ontology 34(2):157–172
Lliadi KG, Boulianne GL (2010) Age-related behavioral changes in
Drosophila. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1197:9–18
McCue JD (1995) The naturalness of dying. JAMA 273:1039–1043
Moya-Larano J (2002) Senescence and food limitation in a slowly
ageing spider. Funct Ecol 16:734–741
Murakami S, Murakami H (2005) The effects of aging and oxidative
stress on learning behavior in C. elegans. Neurobiol Aging
26:899–905
Pasquet A, Marchal J, Anotaux M, Leborgne R (2013) Imperfections
in a perfect architecture: the orb web of spiders. Eur J Entomol
110(3):413–500
Perret M, Aujard F (2006) Vieillissement et rythmes biologiques chez
les primates. Me
´d Sci 22:279–283
Pinheiro JC, Bates DM, Debroy S, Sarkar D (2005) Nlme: linear and
non linear mixed-effects models. R package version 3.1-83, R
foundation for statistical computing, Vienna
Ricklefs RE (2010) Insights from comparative analyses of aging in
birds and mammals. Aging Cell 9(2):273–284
Ridgel AL, Ritzmann RE, Scheafer PL (2003) Effects of aging on
behaviour and leg kinematics during locomotion in two species
of cockroach. J Exp Biol 206:4453–4465
Scharf I, Lubin Y, Ovadia O (2011) Foraging decisions and
behavioural flexibility in trap-building predators: a review. Biol
Rev Camb Philos Soc 86(3):626–639
Sensenig AT, Agnarsson I, Blackledge TA (2011) Adult spiders use
tougher silk: ontogenetic changes in web architecture and silk
biomechanics in the orb-weaver spider. J Zool 285:28–38
Simon AF, Liang DT, Krantz DE (2006) Differential decline in
behavioral performance of Drosophila melanogaster with age.
Mech Ageing Dev 127:647–651
The
´venard L, Leborgne R, Pasquet A (2004) Web-building manage-
ment in an orb-weaving spider, Zygiella x-notata: influence of
prey and conspecifics. C R Biol 327:84–92
Tofilsky A (2000) Senescence and learning in honeybee (Apis
mellifera) workers. Acta Neurobiol Exp 60:35–39
Toscani C, Leborgne R, Pasquet A (2012) Analysis of web building
anomalies in the orb weaving spider Zygiella x-notata (Araneae,
Araneidae). Arachnol Mitt 43:79–83
Venner S, Pasquet A, Leborgne R (2000) Web-building behaviour in
the orb weaving spider Zygiella x-notata: influence of experi-
ence. Anim Behav 59:603–611
Venner S, Thevenard L, Pasquet A, Leborgne R (2001) Estimation of the
web’s capturethread length in orb-weavingspiders: determiningthe
most efficient formula. Ann Entomol Soc Am 94(3):490–496
Venner S, Bel-Venner MC, Pasquet A, Leborgne R (2003) Body-
mass-dependant cost of web-building behavior in an orb weaving
spider, Zygiella x-notata. Naturwissenschaften 90:269–272
Vollrath F (1992) Analysis and interpretation of orb spider explora-
tion and web-building behavior. Adv Stud Behav 21:147–199
Zschokke S (2000) Form and function of the orb-web. Eur Arachnol
2002:99–106
J Ethol
123
... Studies on various different spider species have shown that variation in web structure can influence which and how many prey are caught (Uetz et al., 1978;Chacon & Eberhard, 1980;Sensenig et al., 2010), whether prey are retained (Blackledge & Zevenbergen, 2006;Blamires et al., 2017c), and, in cob-web building species, how much protection the web provides from predators (Zevenbergen et al., 2008). Web structure may also reflect a spider's body condition (Blackledge & Zevenbergen, 2007;Dirienzo & Montiglio, 2016b), recent experience (Nakata & Ushimaru, 1999;Venner et al., 2000), and/or age (Anotaux et al., 2012(Anotaux et al., , 2014. Spider web structure is therefore an important phenotype for various components of a spider's fitness. ...
... Alternatively, spiders may change their position in the syndrome as they age, spinning wider and less dense webs (and possibly reacting less to predation) as they grow. For example, in the orb-web spider Zygiella x-notata, webs have a shorter total thread length, become less regular, and have more "anomalies" as individuals age (Anotaux et al., 2012(Anotaux et al., , 2014, and these changes increase prey handling time (Anotaux et al., 2014). We did not detect a decrease in web evenness over time in our study, as it took place at a time scale (1 month, although each individual was tracked for considerably less time than this) where we might not expect to see much measurable aging. ...
... Alternatively, spiders may change their position in the syndrome as they age, spinning wider and less dense webs (and possibly reacting less to predation) as they grow. For example, in the orb-web spider Zygiella x-notata, webs have a shorter total thread length, become less regular, and have more "anomalies" as individuals age (Anotaux et al., 2012(Anotaux et al., , 2014, and these changes increase prey handling time (Anotaux et al., 2014). We did not detect a decrease in web evenness over time in our study, as it took place at a time scale (1 month, although each individual was tracked for considerably less time than this) where we might not expect to see much measurable aging. ...
Article
Extended phenotypes are traits that exist outside the physical body of organisms. Despite their role in the lives of the organisms that express them and other organisms influenced by extended phenotypes, the consistency and covariance with morphological and behavioural traits of extended phenotypes has rarely been evaluated. We repeatedly measured an extended phenotype involved in prey acquisition (web structure) of wild orb-weaving spiders (Micrathena vigorsii), which re-build their webs daily. We related web structure to behaviours and spider body length. Web diameter and web density were repeatable among individuals, reaction to a predation threat was very marginally so, and response to a prey stimulus and web evenness were not repeatable. Larger spiders spun wider webs, had webs with increased thread spacing, and the spider possibly tended to react more slowly to a predation threat. When a spider built a relatively larger web it was also a relatively less dense and less even web. The repeatability of web construction and relationship with spider body size we found may be common features of intra-population variation in web structure in spiders. By estimating the consistency and covariances of extended phenotypes we can begin to evaluate what maintains their variation and how they might evolve.
... The foraging efficiency of an actively hunting predator may decrease with age due to agedependent decline in moving, whereas a sit-and-wait predator that does not require moving to forage is expected to be unaffected by an age-dependent decline of mobility foraging efficiency. However, few studies have examined the effects of an age-dependent decline in mobility on the foraging efficiency and food intake in animals (but see Anotaux et al., 2014). ...
... On the other hand, the food intake of a sit-and-wait predator may not depend on age, even when walking traits showed a negative correlation with age. A previous study that used the orb-web spider Zygiella x-notata, which is a sit-and-wait predator, showed that the foraging rate did not decrease with age, although the foraging speed did decrease with age (Anotaux et al., 2014). A possible explanation for the positive correlation between the amount of predation and age is that the A. venator may have experienced a long period of predation on T. castaneum in the laboratory, which may have increased their predatory abilities over time. ...
Article
Animal behaviours often dependent on age. In many insect species, walking shows an age-dependent decline, and food intake may also be dependent on age. However, few studies have investigated the relationship between age and walking or food intake. In the present study, we compared walking traits and food intake among individuals of different ages in the assassin bug Amphibolus venator (Hemiptera, Reduviidae). The present results showed an age-dependent decline in walking, similar to findings in many animal species. On the other hand, food intake showed a positive correlation with age. Therefore, the decline in walking did not lead to a decline in feeding. The positive relationship between food intake and age may be related to the type of predation, sit-and-wait, used by A. venator via alterations in investment in reproductive traits with age.
... The direct relationship between the signal amplitude and radii tension might also explain the observations made for spiders such as C. octotuberculata, which tenses the radii to increase its chances of detecting prey [22]. Another takeout from this study can be the crucial effect of the spider's inertia assisting in finding the direction of the signal source, which might explain the behaviour of spiders such as Z. x-notata first arriving at the hub after sensing a vibratory signal through their signal thread, instead of choosing the shortest path [30,31]. This study highlights the effect of spider's weight on the tension distribution of the web, which in turn affects the spider's web-building behaviour. ...
Article
Full-text available
Spider orb web is a sophisticated structure that needs to fulfil multiple roles, such as trapping prey and transmitting web-borne signals. When building their web, heavier spiders tend to increase the pretension on the web, which seems counterintuitive since a tighter web would decrease the chances of stopping and retaining prey. In this article, we claim that heavier orb-weaving spiders increase tension on the web in order to reduce the attenuation of the vibratory signal coming from the bottom part of the web. We support our claim by first building a detailed spider web model, which is tuned by a tension-adjusting algorithm to fit the experimentally observed profiles. Then, the effects of the spider weight and the web tension on the signal transmittance properties are investigated using state-of-the-art finite element analysis tools.
... This decrease in prey-catching ability is attributed to the variability in the spider's web-building activity. There are positive relationships between the number of anomalies and certain web characteristics, such as mesh size, capture area, capture thread length, and width of the lowest half of the web [28][29][30]. The existence of anomalies indicates a flawed construction of webs. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
... This decrease in prey-catching ability is attributed to the variability in the spider's web-building activity. There are positive relationships between the number of anomalies and certain web characteristics, such as mesh size, capture area, capture thread length, and width of the lowest half of the web [28][29][30]. The existence of anomalies indicates a flawed construction of webs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary Spiders are considered natural polyphagous bio-control agents of agro-ecosystems and the best indicators of environmental health. They have the ability to adapt to different environments and are, therefore, present in almost all types of terrestrial ecosystems. The behavior of spiders is influenced by different biotic and abiotic factors, such as roadside disturbance, which may affect normal web-building behavior. Spiders, Neoscona vigilans, were captured, body measurements were taken, and their web characteristics along the road premises were calculated. Web characteristics had a significant association with spider body characteristics and also with the distance from the road, which changed gradually as the webs were constructed closer to the road. Road disturbance had a negative impact on normal web construction as more defects were observed in the webs constructed closer to the road, while dramatically more perfect webs were observed as the distance from the road increased. We concluded that disturbances such as roadside traffic had diverse effects on spider behavior. The outcomes of this study provide insight into the role of traffic on the livelihood of spiders encountering human-influenced disturbance in the ecosystem. Abstract Orb web spiders are common and highly diversified animals found in almost all habitats. They have remarkable plasticity against biotic and abiotic factors, making them excellent indicators of environmental health. The web creation behavior of spiders is influenced by disturbances in the environment. The aim of this research was to observe the alteration in the web-building behavior of Neoscona vigilans caused by human activities, specifically traffic disturbances. Spider webs were located and photographed at nighttime along the roadside, and their web characteristics were calculated. Spiders were captured from webs for their body measurements. Spider fourth leg length, carapace width, and body length had a significant association with web size and diameter, CTL, capture area, and mesh size. The quantity of trapped prey, the height of the plant, and the foliage radius increased with the distance from the road. Conversely, anchor points and web elevation from the ground dropped. The highest and lowest proportions of anomalies (modifications/defects) were recorded as holes (52.7%) in 105 webs (100%) and supernumerary (0.7%) in 55 webs (52.4%), respectively. Road disturbance had a negative influence on the spider’s behavior as the webs formed in close proximity to the road had a higher frequency of anomalies, with a gradual decrease distantly. We can gain further insight into how different environmental changes, disruptions, and pollutants lead to this imperfection in the otherwise flawless perfect structure of spider webs.
... This decrease in prey-catching ability is attributed to the variability in the spider's web-building activity. There are positive relationships between the number of anomalies and certain web characteristics, such as mesh size, capture area, capture thread length, and width of the lowest half of the web [28][29][30]. The existence of anomalies indicates a flawed construction of webs. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Orb web spiders are common and highly diversified animals found in almost all habitats. They have remarkable plasticity against biotic and abiotic factors, making them excellent indicators of environmental health. The web-creation behavior of spiders is influenced by disturbances in the environment. The aim of the research was to observe the alteration in the web-building behavior of Neoscona vigilans caused by human activities, specifically traffic disturbance. Spider webs were located and photographed at nighttime along the roadside, and their web characteristics were calculated. Spiders were captured from webs for their body measurements. Spider 4th leg length, carapace width, and body length had a significant association with web size and diameter, CTL, capture area, and mesh size. The quantity of trapped prey, the height of the plant, and the foliage radius increased with the distance from the road. Conversely, anchor points and web elevation from the ground dropped. The highest and lowest proportion of anomalies (modifications/defects) were recorded as holes (52.7%) in 105 webs (100%) and supernumerary (0.7%) in 55 webs (52.4%), respectively. Road disturbance had a negative influence on the spider’s behavior as the webs formed in close proximity to the road had a higher frequency of anomalies with a gradual decrease distantly. We can gain further insight into how different environmental changes, disruptions, and pollutants lead to this imperfection in the otherwise flawless perfect structure of spider webs.
... When resources are limited, animals must allocate their resources and energy between defense and foraging to maximize their fitness (McArthur et al. 2014;Bonnot et al. 2017;Camp et al. 2017;Wang et al. 2022). Therefore, foraging decisions could reflect trade-offs between food intake and safety (Godvik et al. 2009;Embar 2014;Javier et al. 2018), and could be influenced by an animal's age, life stage, or internal state (Scharf et al. 2011;Anotaux et al. 2014;Keziah & Dhruba 2015;Pasquet et al. 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Detritus‐based, bell‐shaped cobwebs are an ideal model to research the plasticity of web architecture due to clearly separate defense and foraging components. We performed a thorough field investigation on the web architectures of Campanicola campanulata to research its cobweb architecture variation during the growth process and analyzed the energy trade‐offs between foraging and defense at different developmental stages. The results indicated that as female C. campanulata growth progressed, they dedicated more energy to defense and less energy to foraging, while males dedicated less energy to both defense and foraging through the growth period. We hypothesize that cobweb spiders dedicate an increasing amount of energy to safety based on evidence obtained from their life‐history. Meanwhile, we present a new model to investigate web architecture variation and provide a new framework to quantify the energy allocation between foraging and predator defense for web‐building spiders. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved
... Sex of the specimens was determined based on the presence of the epigyne in females and swollen pedipalps in males. Each species was identified based on characteristic abdominal markings and body sizes [23,[73][74][75]. All specimens were kept in 50 mL falcon tubes and transported to the lab where they were housed as per [23]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Venom compositions include complex mixtures of toxic proteins that evolved to immobilize/dissuade organisms by disrupting biological functions. Venom production is metabolically expensive, and parsimonious use is expected, as suggested by the venom optimisation hypothesis. The decision-making capacity to regulate venom usage has never been demonstrated for the globally invasive Noble false widow Steatoda nobilis (Thorell, 1875) (Theridiidae). Here, we investigated variations of venom quantities available in a wild population of S. nobilis and prey choice depending on venom availability. To partially determine their competitiveness, we compared their attack rate success, median effective dose (ED50) and lethal dose (LD50), with four sympatric synanthropic species: the lace webbed spider Amaurobius similis, the giant house spider Eratigena atrica, the missing sector orb-weaver Zygiella x-notata, and the cellar spider Pholcus phalangioides. We show that S. nobilis regulates its venom usage based on availability, and its venom is up to 230-fold (0.56 mg/kg) more potent than native spiders. The high potency of S. nobilis venom and its ability to optimize its usage make this species highly competitive against native European spiders sharing the same habitats.
Thesis
Full-text available
Spider webs are a highly plastic extended phenotype which can be adjusted to a variety of conditions, the most accessible way to investigate web variation is by calculating its geometry. A growing body of work details multiple, interacting, urban factors that impact arthropod populations and behaviour. Urbanisation can act as a driver of niche creation; orb webs on car side mirrors are a common sight, this is a novel, unstudied, urban niche which has been rapidly colonised in the last 50 years. Has Z. x-notata made behavioural adaptations to their web geometry to occupy the niche of the car mirror? To answer this question, webs were photographed in the field in 3 settings, on car mirrors, urban and natural structures. Web dimensions were later measured using ImageJ. Geometry which could be influenced by conditions associated with the car mirror were identified: capture spiral thread length, and %, total thread length, mesh height, signal thread length, free-sector area and number of radii. Kruskal-Wallis tests revealed geometrical variation between the 3 conditions; capture thread length % was significantly greater in car mirror webs (60%) than urban (46%) or natural webs (43%), while car webs were significantly shorter (701 cm) than urban webs (1071 cm), and signal threads belonging to urban webs were found to be significantly longer on average (9 cm) than those found on natural structures (6.8 cm) or car mirrors (6.3 cm). Other results were not significant. These results suggest that Z. x-notata has made behavioural adaptations to its web spinning, and energy expenditure strategy to occupy this new niche. This has positive implications for the adaptability of orb-weaving spiders to growing urbanisation, in a time of great anthropogenic disturbance and arthropod species decline.
Article
Full-text available
Se registra por primera vez la presencia de la araña Zygiella x-notata (Clerck) para la Región de Tarapacá (Chile), con base en ejemplares capturados en roqueríos costeros (rompeolas) de la ciudad de Iquique. Se discute acerca de la colonización de esta especie en este tipo de estructuras costeras artificiales y su posible expansión al resto de la ciudad.
Article
Full-text available
Among animal constructions, spider's orb webs represent regular geometrical architecture models. Their construction is the result of successive, simple and reproducible behavioural patterns, often considered as stereotyped. It has recently been shown that spider's building behaviours vary, which can alter web regularity. The final capture spiral results from the laying of successive threads between two radii, here termed 'spiral units'. We defined a theoretical normal web, as a web in which each turn of the final spiral should be parallel to the preceding one. Weaving of the spiral units sometimes leads to anomalies in the orb web. Anomalies were identified and analysed in the orb-weaving spider Zygiella x-notata (Clerck, 1757). From video recordings of web construction, we noted the displacements of the legs and of the abdomen of the spider. We compared the frequency of displacements, and their duration, between the construction of spiral units that produce a normal turn and ones that produce an anomalous turn. The position of the legs on the web's threads was also analysed. Results showed that anomalies were not the consequences of a modification in activity but more likely the result of the position on the radii of the fourth leg. These results suggest that spiders use local information to build the final capture spiral.
Article
Full-text available
Aging is often associated with locomotor deficits. Behavior in aged Blaberus discoidalis cockroaches was analyzed during horizontal walking, climbing, righting and inclined walking. Adult animals showed a decrease in spontaneous locomotion with increasing age. Tarsal abnormalities, termed `tarsus catch', were often present in aged individuals. In `tarsus catch', the prothoracic leg catches on the mesothoracic leg during the swing phase. This deficit causes alterations of the gait, but animals are able to regain a tripod gait after the perturbation. The tibio-tarsal joint angle in individuals with `tarsus catch' was significantly less than in intact animals. Structural defects were consistently associated with `tarsus catch'. The tracheal tubes in the tarsus and around the tibio-tarsal joint were often discolored and the tarsal pads were hardened in aged cockroaches. All aged individuals were able to climb. However, prior to climbing, some animals with `tarsus catch' failed to show postural changes that are normally seen in young animals. Aged individuals can right as rapidly as 1-week-old adults. However, animals with `tarsus catch' take longer to right than aged intact individuals. Old cockroaches have difficulty climbing an incline of 45°, and leg slipping is extensive. Slipping may be caused by tarsal degeneration, but animals that are unsuccessful in inclined walking often show uncoordinated gaits during the attempt. Escape behavior was examined in aged American cockroaches (Periplaneta americana). They do not show normal escape. However, after decapitation, escape movements return, suggesting that degeneration in head ganglia may actually interfere with escape. These findings provide evidence for age-related changes both in the periphery and in the central nervous system of cockroaches and stress the importance of multi-level approaches to the study of locomotion.
Article
Full-text available
Among animal constructions, the orb webs of spiders are perfect geometrical architectural models. Webs are constructed by means of a succession of behaviours, which are often considered as instinctive and stereotyped. Anomalies in web structure are considered to be a consequence of variability in behaviour. In order to assess the variability in web structure, the spider Zygiella x-notata was allowed to build webs in the laboratory under standardized conditions. Webs (n = 127) were examined for anomalies, which were defined as modifications in the geometrical structure. Ten anomalies in the structure of webs produced by females, aged from 16 to 21 days after their adult moult, are described. The frequency of anomalies per web varies from 5 to 98. A very low percentage of the spiral elements of a web (less than 5%) were affected. Anomalies in the radii were less numerous (mean = 3 ± 3) than those in the sticky spiral (mean = 21 ± 11). Anomalies in the sticky spiral were different and more frequent in the peripheral than central parts of the webs. They were also relatively more numerous in the upper than in the lower part of the webs. Total number of anomalies was positively correlated with particular web characteristics (capture area, length of capture thread, width of the lower part of the web and mean distance between two spiral turns in the lower part) but not to the physical characteristics of the spiders (mass and size). Our results indicate that the orb webs of spiders, which are considered to be perfect geometrical structures, include anomalies mainly in the structure of the sticky spiral. These anomalies were the result of variability in behaviour during web building. In conclusion, their identification and description enable one to use them in spider webs as an index of behavioural variability.
Article
Full-text available
Ageing is known to induce profound effects on physiological functions but only a few studies have focused on its behavioural alterations. Orb-webs of spiders, however, provide an easily analysable structure, the result of complex sequences of stereotypical behaviours that are particularly relevant to the study of ageing processes. We chose the orb spider Zygiella x-notata as an invertebrate organism to investigate alterations in web geometry caused by ageing. Parameters taken into account to compare webs built by spiders at different ages were: the length of the capture spiral (CTL), the number of anomalies per cm, and four parameters of web regularity (the angle between radii, the number of spiral thread units connecting two successive radii, the parallelism and the coefficient of variation of the distances between silk threads of two adjacent spiral turns). All web parameters were related to ageing. Two groups of spiders emerged: short-and long-lived spiders (with a higher body mass), with an average life span of 150 and 236 days, respectively. In both short-and long-lived spiders' webs, the CTL and the silk thread parallelism decreased, while the variation of the distances between silk threads increased. However, the number of anomalies per cm and the angle between radii increased in short-lived spiders only. These modifications can be explained by ageing alterations in silk investment and cognitive and/or locomotor functions. Orb-web spiders would therefore provide a unique invertebrate model to study ageing and its processes in the alterations of behavioural and cognitive functions. (C) 2012 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Article
The understanding of web-building behaviour in orb-web spiders has undergone several paradigm shifts. In the past, orb-web construction was assumed to be limited to genetically-controlled design patterns, suggesting that meaningful variation only existed at the species level. Subsequently, it was recognised that variation in web design also exists within species and that this variation was linked to the prey capture ability of webs. Another approach to interpreting individual variation is within a decision-making construct. The web-building decisions of spiders may thus be ruled by algorithms or mechanical constraints. Similarly, individual decisions may reflect foraging strategies aimed to maximise food intake. Our own work suggests that experience in web-building and prey capture may also contribute to individual variation of orb-web design. Using several key publications as well as recently collected data we discuss past and current ideas to interpret orb-web variability.
Article
This is only a book review of Biology of Spiders by Ranier Foelix. There is no text from the book. Please do not request the text of the book from me. The review, should you wish to access it, is readily available online from the Florida Entomologist.
Article
Flies were submitted to two weeks of hypergravity in a centrifuge (3 or 5 g), starting at the second day of imaginal life, and their behavior (spontaneous locomotor activity, patterns of movement, and climbing activity) was observed from removal of the centrifuge to an older age; the usual effects of age on these behaviors were generally observed. Hypergravity-kept flies had worse behavioral scores on removal of centrifuge than those always kept at 1 g. When they aged, they got either similar or better scores than 1 g flies, which indicates that their behavioral aging may be slower. These results show that a mild stress such as hypergravity, which has been previously shown to increase the longevity of males and resistance to heat shock in both sexes, is an environmental manipulation postponing aging in flies.