Content uploaded by Benjamin Pageaux
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Benjamin Pageaux on May 14, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
LETTER TO THE EDITOR
The Psychobiological Model of Endurance Performance:
An Effort-Based Decision-Making Theory to Explain
Self-Paced Endurance Performance
Benjamin Pageaux
ÓSpringer International Publishing Switzerland 2014
To the Editor,
In a recent review published in this journal [1], Renfree and
colleagues demonstrated the importance of considering
decision-making theories to understand self-paced endur-
ance performance. The authors aimed to examine current
models/theories of decision-making in an attempt to
explain the manner in which regulation of muscular work
(pacing) is achieved during self-paced endurance perfor-
mance. As explained by the authors, it is crucial that
models explaining self-paced endurance performance take
into account both internal (e.g. perception of effort, phys-
iological responses) and external (e.g. tactical decisions,
presence of competitors) factors. Interestingly, among all
models presented in their review, the authors omitted to
present an effort-based decision-making model recently
proposed to explain self-paced endurance performance: the
psychobiological model (of endurance performance) [2].
The psychobiological model has been shown to provide a
valid explanation of the effects of both psychological [3,4]
and physiological [5] manipulations on endurance perfor-
mance during constant-load exercise (time to exhaustion).
Recently, its explanatory validity was extended to self-
paced exercise where endurance performance was altered
by psychological (mental fatigue) [6] and physiological
(muscle fatigue) [7] manipulations. Consequently, it seems
important to mention its existence in a review on decision-
making theories relevant to self-regulation of pacing.
Therefore, the main aim of this letter is to briefly present
the psychobiological model and its sensitivity to internal
and external factors known to alter self-paced endurance
performance. This letter will also attempt to provide to the
reader a brief alternative interpretation of the role of per-
ception of effort in endurance performance.
The psychobiological model is an effort-based decision-
making model [2] based on motivational intensity theory
[8], and postulates that the conscious regulation of pace is
determined primarily by five different cognitive/motiva-
tional factors:
1. Perception of effort
2. Potential motivation
3. Knowledge of the distance/time to cover
4. Knowledge of the distance/time remaining
5. Previous experience/memory of perception of effort
during exercise of varying intensity and duration
Factor 2 (potential motivation) refers to the maximum
effort an individual is willing to exert to satisfy a motive,
and could be easily influenced by external factors (e.g.
higher motivation during an event with competitors than
during laboratory testing). Factors 3 to 5 are self-explan-
atory and can explain the end-spurt phenomenon [9]or
why athletes start different races at different paces [10].
Perception of effort (factor 1) could be defined as ‘‘the
conscious sensation of how hard, heavy and strenuous a
physical task is’’ [2], and is the key determinant of this
model. Indeed, according to this model, the conscious
regulation of pace is primarily determined by the effort
perceived by the athlete. Therefore, when perception of
effort is increased by muscle [7] or mental [6] fatigue, or
reduced (same perception of effort for a higher power
output) by pharmacological manipulation [11], the athlete
will consciously change its pace to compensate for the
negative/positive effect of the experimental manipulation
B. Pageaux (&)
Endurance Research Group, School of Sport and Exercise
Sciences, University of Kent at Medway, Chatham Maritime,
Kent ME4 4AG, UK
e-mail: bp89@kent.ac.uk
123
Sports Med
DOI 10.1007/s40279-014-0198-2
Author's personal copy
on perception of effort, thus leading to an improvement (if
decreased perception of effort [11]) or impairment (if
increased perception of effort [6,7]) in self-paced endur-
ance performance. Because the five factors mentioned
above are sensitive to external and/or physiological factors
known to impact endurance performance, the psychobio-
logical model could be considered as a tool to explain
regulation of self-paced endurance performance.
Contrary to the models presented by Renfree and col-
leagues, the psychobiological model of endurance perfor-
mance postulates that the sensory signal processed by the
brain to generate perception of effort is not the afferent
feedback from skeletal muscles and other interoceptors
[12]. Perception of effort is thought to result from the
central processing of the corollary discharge associated
with the central motor command [12,13], thus explaining
the alteration of perception of effort and performance when
central motor command is increased to compensate for
muscle fatigue [7] or central processing of the corollary
discharge is altered by mental fatigue [3,6]. Despite this
theoretical difference in the underlying sensory signals
thought to generate perception of effort, the models pre-
sented by Renfree and colleagues and the psychobiological
model agree on the crucial role of perception of effort in
the self-regulation of pacing. Therefore, it is important to
understand the neurocognitive link between perception of
effort and the regulation of endurance performance during
self-paced exercise. Recently, a strong link between the
response inhibition process (a main component of decision-
making in human volition [14]) and perception of effort
was suggested [6]. In this study, subjects performed 30 min
of either incongruent (involving response inhibition) or
congruent (non involving response inhibition) Stroop task
followed by a five kilometres running time trial. Interest-
ingly, endurance performance following completion of the
incongruent Stroop task was decreased in association with
an increased perception of effort. One plausible explana-
tion provided by the authors is the similarity in brain areas
involved in both mechanisms. Indeed, perception of effort,
response inhibition and consequently decision-making
process are known to be associated with activity in the
anterior cingulate cortex and the pre supplementary motor
areas [3,4,6]. Therefore, independently of the model/
theory used to explain endurance performance, further
researches on the neurophysiology of perception of effort
are required to provide a better understanding of the reg-
ulation of endurance performance during self-paced
exercise.
Acknowledgments I would like to thank Romuald Lepers and
Samuele Marcora for our numerous discussions on this topic and their
feedback on this manuscript.
Conflict of interest The author has no potential conflicts of interest
that are directly relevant to the content of this letter.
References
1. Renfree A, Martin L, Micklewright D, et al. Application of
decision-making theory to the regulation of muscular work rate
during self-paced competitive endurance activity. Sports Med.
2014;44(2):147–58. doi:10.1007/s40279-013-0107-0.
2. Marcora S. Counterpoint: afferent feedback from fatigued loco-
motor muscles is not an important determinant of endurance
exercise performance. J Appl Physiol. 2010;108(2):454–6 (dis-
cussion 6–7). doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00976.2009a108/2/454.
3. Pageaux B, Marcora SM, Lepers R. Prolonged mental exertion
does not alter neuromuscular function of the knee extensors. Med
Sci Sports Exerc. 2013;45(12):2254–64. doi:10.1249/MSS.
0b013e31829b504a.
4. Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs
physical performance in humans. J Appl Physiol.
2009;106(3):857–64. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008.
5. Marcora SM, Bosio A, de Morree HM. Locomotor muscle fatigue
increases cardiorespiratory responses and reduces performance
during intense cycling exercise independently from metabolic
stress. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol.
2008;294(3):R874–83. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00678.2007.
6. Pageaux B, Lepers R, Dietz KC, et al. Response inhibition
impairs subsequent self-paced endurance performance. Eur J
Appl Physiol. 2014. doi:10.1007/s00421-014-2838-5.
7. de Morree HM, Marcora SM. Effects of isolated locomotor
muscle fatigue on pacing and time trial performance. Eur J Appl
Physiol. 2013;113(9):2371–80. doi:10.1007/s00421-013-2673-0.
8. Brehm JW, Self EA. The intensity of motivation. Annu Rev
Psychol. 1989;40:109–31. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.
000545.
9. Marcora SM. The end-spurt does not require a subconscious
intelligent system, just our conscious brain. BJSM, BMJ Group
Blogs [Internet]; 2008. http://blogs.bmj.com/bjsm/the-end-spurt-
does-not-require-a-subconscious-intelligent-system/. Accessed 30
Apr 2014
10. Joseph T, Johnson B, Battista RA, et al. Perception of fatigue
during simulated competition. Med Sci Sports Exerc.
2008;40(2):381–6. doi:10.1249/mss.0b013e31815a83f6.
11. Watson P, Hasegawa H, Roelands B, et al. Acute dopamine/
noradrenaline reuptake inhibition enhances human exercise per-
formance in warm, but not temperate conditions. J Physiol.
2005;565(Pt 3):873–83. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2004.079202.
12. Marcora S. Perception of effort during exercise is independent of
afferent feedback from skeletal muscles, heart, and lungs. J Appl
Physiol. 2009;106(6):2060–2. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.90378.
2008.
13. de Morree HM, Klein C, Marcora SM. Perception of effort
reflects central motor command during movement execution.
Psychophysiology. 2012;49:1242–53. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.
2012.01399.x.
14. Haggard P. Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nat
Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(12):934–46. doi:10.1038/nrn2497.
B. Pageaux
123
Author's personal copy