ArticlePDF Available

Why do children take part in, and remain involved in sport?: implications for children’s sport coaches.

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Recent academic literature on sport coaching has acknowledged the importance of domain specificity, emphasising the significance of participant motives and the coaching environment in determining appropriate coaching practice. Although children's motivations for taking part in sport have been a popular research area, there has been no attempt, until now, to review the research literature that specifically addresses this issue. This review found that children's participation in sport is mediated by five primary factors: perception of competence; fun and enjoyment; parents; learning new skills; and friends and peers. These findings suggest that, in addition to the generally acknowledged psychological factors, the social-cultural context in which children play influences their motivations to participate. If children are to remain involved in sport, it is vital that coaches' behaviours and practices match the needs of the young participants. Coaches are responsible for creating a developmentally appropriate learning environment that ensures children maintain active sports participation. If this is to be achieved, coaches need to think carefully about the behaviours they use, and how they structure their coaching sessions. Coaches should use more positive than negative behaviours, and emphasise fun and enjoyment, teamwork and effort, over winning and competition.
Content may be subject to copyright.
56
Why do children take part in, and
remain involved in sport?
A literature review and discussion
of implications for sports coaches
Richard Bailey* RBES Ltd, Birmingham, UK
Edward J. Cope University of Bedfordshire, UK
Gemma Pearce Queen Mary University of London, UK
International Journal of
Coaching Science
Vol. 7 No. 1
January 2013.
pp.56-75
Abstract
Recent academic literature on sport coaching has acknowledged the importance of domain specificity,
emphasising the significance of participant motives and the coaching environment in determining appropriate
coaching practice. Although children’s motivations for taking part in sport have been a popular research
area, there has been no attempt, until now, to review the research literature that specifically addresses this
issue. This review found that children’s participation in sport is mediated by five primary factors:
perception of competence; fun and enjoyment; parents; learning new skills; and friends and peers. These
findings suggest that, in addition to the generally acknowledged psychological factors, the social-cultural
context in which children play influences their motivations to participate. If children are to remain involved
in sport, it is vital that coaches’ behaviours and practices match the needs of the young participants.
Coaches are responsible for creating a developmentally appropriate learning environment that ensures
children maintain active sports participation. If this is to be achieved, coaches need to think carefully about
the behaviours they use, and how they structure their coaching sessions. Coaches should use more positive
than negative behaviours, and emphasise fun and enjoyment, teamwork and effort, over winning and
competition.
Keywords: motivation, children, coaching, learning, sport
* Address for correspondence: Dr Richad Bailey, RBES Ltd, Birmingham, B29 4JS, United Kingdom, E-mail address:
info@richardbailey.net
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
57
Introduction
There is a broad consensus that the principal goals of children’s sport are to provide intrinsically
rewarding experiences, and to maintain interest so that they are inclined to continue playing throughout
their lives (Côté & Fraser-Thomas, 2007; Kirk, 2005; McNamee & Bailey, 2009). It is widely believed that
taking part in sport and other physical activities from an early age is important if children are to develop a
foundation for lifelong physical engagement in healthy sporting experiences (e.g., Bailey et al., 2009;
Keegan et al., 2009; Kirk, 2005). In order to achieve this, a social environment is needed that reflects
children’s motivations for taking part in, and remaining involved in, sport for these goals to be achieved.
Therefore, the purpose of this paper is to review children’s motivations for engaging in, and remaining
involved in sport, in order to inform the practice of coaches; more specifically, making recommendations
about coaches’ behaviour and practice that increase the likelihood that children will remain active sporting
participants.
Studies have identified a number of factors that facilitate participation in sport, including such things
as a supportive family, accessibility to local sports clubs, and the construction of appropriate learning
environments (Bailey & Toms, 2010). However, not all children are fortunate enough to be benefactors of
such opportunities. Evidence suggests that not all sporting contexts are equally valuable in supporting
children’s engagement in sport (Bailey & Toms, 2010). For example, a study of swimmers (Pelletier et al.,
2001) found that children had dropped out because they perceived their coaches as less encouraging and
supportive, and more controlling and autocratic than coaches of children who had maintained active
participation. Moreover, when coaches promoted intra-team rivalries, favour the most talented players, and
punish players for making mistakes, children become de-motivated (Newton, Duda, & Yin, 2000). Other
reasons identified as leading to children becoming de-motivated are when: 1) coaching sessions are too
serious, with an over-emphasis placed on winning by parents, coaches and peers (Orlick, 1973); 2) children
are judged in a competitive environment, resulting in a fear of failure due to being assessed against peers
(Sager, Lavellee, & Spray, 2007; 3) and, there is a lack of enjoyment (Weiss, 1993). These are scenarios
that coaches involved in children’s sport would want to avoid, given the negative affect they have on
children’s motivation to participate.
Coaching practice is likely to be ineffective, or even harmful, if the coaching environment is not
aligned to the needs of children (Bailey, 2012). From the limited research that has been undertaken on
children’s coaching, it seems that many coaches of children attempt to emulate the practices of those who
work in elite or professional sport (Muir et al., 2011). This is obviously problematic as these practices
might not align with children’s developmental needs (Wiersema, 2000). What results is developmentally
inappropriate coaching, which fails to consider children’s distinctive learning needs within the coaching
contexts. There is ample evidence to support the principle that effective coaching of children is premised
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
58
on the coach acknowledging and addressing the particular needs and wants of the young athlete within the
distinctive coaching context (Côté et al., 1995). Lyle (2002) suggests three distinct coaching domains;
development, participation, and performance, with each of these having its own contextual requirements. For
example in a performance domain, coaches will likely plan short, medium and long term objectives, with
coaching centred on preparation for competition. On the other hand, in participatory domains, coaching will
likely be characterised by short term episodic sessions, where the focus is on fun and enjoyment, and
inclusivity. Our primary concern in this paper is with the participatory domain.
Historically, the practices and processes of coaches of children have received relatively scant attention
in the coaching literature (Lemyre, Trudel, & Durand-Bush, 2007). Instead, researchers have been more
interested in situating themselves within performance sport-orientated contexts (e.g. Jones, Armour, & Potrac,
2004). This bias should be viewed with some anxiety given that the coaching workforce is predominantly
characterised by coaches working with children in varying capacities (North, 2009). In fact, fifty percent of
the entire coaching workforce operates with children between the ages of 5-12 (Sportscoach UK, 2011a).
These data provide further support for the need to increase the level of research activity in children’s
coaching contexts in order to better understand children’s developmental learning needs and motivations for
participating (Wall & Côté, 2007). Sportscoach UK (2007) has acknowledged the need to consider the
“common core of knowledge and skills required by all coaches working with children” (p.33). This is an
explicit call for children’s coaches to be better prepared to manage the complexities of their coaching role.
Policy initiatives by Sportscoach UK (2009) have led to the construction of parallel coach and participant
models, both of which locate children’s sport as a central component. Informing this participant model is a
participant development academic review (Bailey et al., 2010), which highlights the need to recognise the
distinctive biological, psychological, and social development of children and young people. The participant
development model is based on the argument that participants’ needs should be placed at the heart of the
coaching process (Sportscoach UK, 2009). It is, therefore, timely and necessary to examine the motivations
of children and their reasons for remaining as active sporting participants.
Method
A search for literature was first undertaken using the terms children and motivations, sport, and
coaching from the earliest record until June 2011. The databases searched included Academic Search
Complete, PsycARTICLES, PsycBOOKS, PsycINFO, and SPORTDiscus with Full Text. A hand search of
relevant articles and book chapters was also carried out to ensure that all relevant articles were obtained.
Finally, reference lists of the articles obtained were read through to make sure that articles that satisfied
the search criteria were included. Additional search criteria were that the articles were peer reviewed and
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
59
focussed on children who were coached IN sport between the ages of 5-12. Any child over the age of 12
was deemed to have moved into their adolescent years, and therefore any article, which focussed on the
motivations of young people in this age range, was not considered for review.
Figure 1. Flow chart of review process
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
60
Results and discussion
What motivates children to take part in sport?
Children’s motivations for participating in sport have received a steady flow of research interest and
continue to do so (e.g. Fredricks & Eccles, 2005; Light & Lemonie, 2010; Sapp & Haubenstricker, 1978;
Weiss & Petlickhoff, 1989). In this research literature, children report a number of different motives for
taking part in sport (Sit & Lindner, 2006). This section discusses the main themes that emerged from
reviewing the literature on children’s motivations for participating in sport.
Perceived competence Harter’s (1978) competence motivation theory professes that individuals are
motivated to be competent in sport in order to satisfy their aims of mastering a skill. When this results in
success, children get a sense of positivity, which increases their motivation levels to participate in sport
(Carroll & Loumidis, 2001). However, when children perceive that they are not competent, motivation
levels decrease, which increases their likelihood of dropping out of sport (May et al., 2007). Klint and
Weiss (1987) suggest that there are two forms of competence: physical and social. Children’s physical
competence is related to their performance of skills, whereas social competence is related to such things as
making friends. Children attempt to demonstrate their competency in line with what they perceive equates
to personal success (Roberts, Klieber & Duda, 1981). However, a study conducted by Weiss and Duncan
(1992) found there to be a relationship between a child’s perception of physical competency and their
perception of social acceptance amongst peers. This finding suggests that these two forms of competence
are related.
Fun and enjoyment It is commonly presumed that the primary motive for children taking part in
sport is to have fun and enjoyable experiences (Chalip & Green, 1998; Green, 2005; Wankel & Kreisal,
1985; Weiss & Amorose, 2008). However, different children understand fun in different ways. For example,
soccer players stated that fun was experiencing freedom and self-expression (Piggott, 2009), whereas
swimmers experienced fun when competing in activities which were inherently challenging (Light &
Lemonie, 2010). Dismore and Bailey (2010) found that young children tended to define fun as playing
games providing hedonic pleasure. Further findings from this study demonstrate how the activity being
participated in can affect fun and enjoyment levels. Some children perceived some activities to be boring
because of their repetitive nature. Weiss and Amorose (2008) present a conceptual model that suggests how
different sources contribute towards sporting enjoyment (Figure 2.0). This model highlights the
individualistic nature of sport enjoyment, as fun and enjoyable experiences will not be the same for all
children.
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
61
Figure 2. Sources and consequences of sport enjoyment
Parents Parents have considerable influence on their children’s motivation to take part in sport (e.g.
Brustad, 1992), as well as their overall development (Anderson, Hughes & Fuemmeler, 2009). The role of
parents and the behaviours they exhibit directly affect children’s perceived level of competence and
motivation to participate (McCarthy & Jones, 2007). Research suggests that when parents are physically
active they increased their children’s motivation to participate in sport (Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Fredricks &
Eccles, 2004). This view is supported by Light and Lemonie (2010), who reported that most of the
children in their study were influenced to join a swimming club because they wanted to follow in the steps
of older family members, who were active sporting participants.
The extent to which parents become involved in their child’s sports participation influences their
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
62
level of motivation (Byrne, 1999). Under-involved parents, who show no or little interest in their child’s
sporting endeavours, will have a de-motivating impact. Similarly, over-involved parents can have just as
detrimental influence by placing too much pressure on their children, thus making their sporting experience
less enjoyable (Light & Lemonie, 2010). From a coach’s perspective, parents who over-emphasised winning
are more likely to negatively affect their children’s motivation levels, whereas parents who emphasised
effort, teamwork, and fun were more likely to positively affect children’s motivation levels (Gould et al.,
2008). Consequently, a caring and supportive family environment where sport is not taken too seriously
best motivates a child to take part (Allender, Cowburn, & Foster, 2006).
Learning new skills One study of motivations for joining and remaining in a swimming club found
that the majority of children stated that their primary motive for participating was to learn new skills to
make them better swimmers (Light & Lemonie, 2010). MacPhail, Gorely and Kirk (2003) found that
children enjoyed learning new skills because of the inherent challenge it presented.
Friends and Peers Research suggests that social interaction is an important motivating factor driving
sports participation, including opportunities for working together, gaining social acceptance (Smith, Balaguer,
& Duda, 2006), making friendships (Light & Lemonie, 2010), and meeting new people (MacPhail et al.,
2003). Peers were found to influence motivation through competitive behaviours, working in collaboration
and evaluative communication (Keegan et al., 2009). Alternatively, being judged or compared against peers
determines why some children become de-motivated to participate in sport (Sagar et al., 2007). The
influence of peers on motivational levels has been found to be greatest during late childhood and
adolescence (McCarthy & Jones, 2007).
Explaining children’s motivations
The two most commonly used theories to explain children’s reasons for taking part in sport are
achievement goal theory (Nicholls, 1984), and self-determination theory (SDT) (Ryan & Deci, 2000).
Achievement goal theory (Nicholls, 1984) is based on the principle that children’s motivations are
situated along a task- or ego-orientated continuum. Motivation to take part in sport is dependent upon a
child’s orientation. Those who are primarily task-orientated are concerned with personal improvement though
the learning of new skills, whereas those of an ego-orientated disposition are more motivated by achieving
superiority in the form of winning competitions and gaining accolades (Quested & Duda, 2011). However,
just because a child has a high task-orientation does not necessarily mean that ego-orientation will be low
(Quested & Duda, 2011). Duda and Whitehead (1998) suggest that athletes can have either high task-and
ego-orientations, low task-and ego-orientations, or high in task, low in ego, and vice versa. Nonetheless, it
seems that children tend to be inherently task-orientated (Nicholls, 1989); in other words, they judge
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
63
success against the amount of effort that they exert, as opposed to proving themselves more competent than
their opponents. This has important implications for coaches, as the learning environment created will
largely influence children’s motivations.
SDT explains why the reasons given for participation are motivating factors. These reasons (i.e. fun,
learning new skills, making friends) are called children’s motivational regulations (Deci & Ryan, 2000).
Placed on a continuum, at one end is intrinsic motivation, which explains how children participate for their
love of playing sport, and at the other end of the continuum is a motivation, which describes the
experiences of children who cannot recognize why it is that they play sport, and therefore do not want to
take part (Quested & Duda, 2011). Located between intrinsic motivation and amotivation is extrinsic
motivation (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Extrinsic motivation comprises of two types: self-determined and
non-self-determined (Mallett, 2005). Within self-determined motivation is identified regulation, which
according to Deci and Ryan (2000) is achieved when the sport or activity undertaken is internalised, with
children realising how engagement will satisfy their reasons for participating. Non-self-determined motivation
is further broken down into introjected and external regulations. Introjected regulations describe instances
where children participate through feelings of guilt (i.e. their parents want them to play sport). For children
who are motivated by external regulations, such things as financial rewards, media attention and winning
will dictate their participation in sport. Introjected and external regulations are likely to result in many
children becoming disinterested, as these regulations do not align with their reasons for participating (Deci
& Ryan, 2000), or cannot be universally attained. Ensuring there is a positive alignment between the
coaching environment and motivation regulations is important if a child’s basic psychological needs are to
be met.
As has been highlighted, the extent of children’s self-determined motivation depends on what it is
that motivates participation. For example, a child who is motivated to play sport because he or she is
given the opportunity to interact and socialise with friends is intrinsically motivated. However, the same
child, if placed in an environment where they are not given the opportunity to socialise, but instead
competition is emphasised, will likely be amotivated and not want to take part (Quested & Duda, 2011),
unless there is some other inducement to take part. Therefore, children’s self-determined motivation to
participate in sport will be significantly influenced by the type of coaching environment created.
Ensuring that children remain motivated
Basic psychological needs are made up of three interrelated components: autonomy; competence; and
relatedness (Deci & Ryan, 2000). For children to remain motivated to participate, each one of these
components must be satisfying. Children’s autonomy is assured when they are given decision-making and
problem-solving responsibilities. If children feel as though the sport in which they are engaged allows them
to demonstrate their capabilities, they will achieve competency. Finally, relatedness occurs when children are
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
64
made to feel cared for and wanted. In this way, relatedness is directly associated with children’s affections.
Adhering to these principles creates a motivational climate which is task orientated, guaranteeing that
the coaching environment is aligned with most children’s motives for taking part (Cumming, et al., 2007).
As such, skill development, making friends, and putting in maximum effort is prioritised over competition
and winning (Smoll & Smith, 2006). Positive changes in attitudes toward coaches and sport have been
observed in children who played for coaches who were more reinforcing, encouraging, and autonomy
supportive than coaches who were critical of performance (Conroy & Coatsworth, 2007).
An autonomy supportive environment seems to be most suited to satisfying children’s basic
psychological needs (Mageau & Vallerand, 2003). To be autonomy supportive requires coaches to: (1)
provide as much choice as possible within specific limits and rules; (2) provide a rationale for tasks, limits
and rules; (3) inquire about, and acknowledge others’ feelings; (4) allow opportunities to take initiatives and
do independent work; (5) provide non-controlling competence feedback; (6) avoid overt control, guilt
inducing criticisms, controlling statements and tangible rewards; and (7) prevent ego-involvement from
taking place (Mageau & Vallerand, 2003, p. 886).
Many coaches of children have relatively low levels of qualifications and work on a voluntary basis
(Sportscoach UK, 2011b). Therefore, the extent to which they would be able to construct the coaching
environment that Mageau and Vallerand (2003) propose is questionable. While an autonomy supportive
environment provides a suitable template to which children’s sport coaches can refer, it is perhaps too far
detached from the reality of most practice. Cushion, Armour and Jones (2006) suggest how coaching
scholars need to represent the coaching process in a manner to which coaches can relate. They argue that
coaching lacks a conceptual base, as a result of an overrepresentation of models ‘for’ coaching practice,
rather than models ‘of’ coaching practice. The benchmarks highlighted by Mageau and Vallerand, as those
which constitute an autonomy supportive environment, are perhaps an unrealistic expectation for children’s
coaches, given their voluntary or part-time coaching roles (Sportscoach UK, 2011b).
A consequence of this is that the training and education of coaches needs to place greater emphasis
on the development of pedagogical knowledge and skills (Jones, Morgan, & Harris, in press). Only by
doing so, can children’s coaches attempt to provide an autonomy supportive coaching environment.
However, we also need to acknowledge the limited scope of coach education, and also recognise that the
coaching environment is not solely determined by the coach. Parental pressures and the expectations of
sports administrators and employers may reinforce a performance orientation in some children’s sport.
What can coaches do to promote positive learning environments?
Although it has been argued that an autonomy supportive coaching environment can be challenging
to achieve, and that children’s coaches need progressively to implement the key tenets of this approach, the
behaviours utilised by coaches in practice are an important factor in ensuring that children’s coaching needs
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
65
and wants are met (Mageau & Vallerand, 2003). A consistent finding within the literature is that coaches
who mostly use positive coaching behaviours increase effort, whereas coaches who mostly use negative
coaching behaviours reduce the effort of children (Amorose & Weiss, 1998; Barker & Graham, 1987;
Bartholomew, et al., 2011; Keegan et al., 2009). Controlling behaviours, where coaches fail to include
children in any decision-making processes, or appear to be overly authoritarian, have also been found to
have adverse effects on children’s motivation levels (Amorose & Horn, 2000; Mageau & Vallerand, 2003).
Research into coach’s behaviour using systematic observation has found that, regardless of context,
the predominant behaviours used are instructional (e.g. Cushion & Jones, 2001; Lacy & Darst, 1985; Tharp
& Gallimore, 1976). Ford, Yates and Williams (2010) contend that the overuse of instruction limits the
potential for children to be involved in problem solving tasks, which are critical for their psychological
development. Further to this, when coaches use high levels of instruction, they are giving children little
choice in their own learning (Hollembeck & Amorose, 2005), which might subsequently impact on the
motivation to remain active. Harvey, Cushion and Massa-Gonzalez (2010) suggest that coaches tend to use
these instructional pedagogies because they view them as ‘safe’ and ‘tried and tested’.
The traditional, coach-centred approach has been criticised for restricting children from being able
freely to express themselves when playing sport. Mageau and Vallerand’s (2003) description of an
autonomy-supportive environment in that children are given ownership, and thus feel in control of their
own learning, are set personally achievable goals which are not related to winning, and become self-aware
and self-sufficient which prevents them from relying on their coach, should serve as key indicators of
developmentally appropriate coaching for children.
Although adopting a facilitative approach is a challenging task, coaches can take practical steps
towards it by limiting the amount of instructional and negative behaviours that they use (Ford, Yates, &
Williams, 2010). Smith and Cushion (2006) suggest the use of silence as an effective coaching strategy: it
allows coaches the opportunity to observe their children practising, so that when they do intervene, the
messages have increased clarity and meaning; it gives children the opportunity to work problems out for
themselves without constant input from coaches. The use of questioning has also been recommended as an
effective coaching intervention strategy in the quest to increase children’s decision-making opportunities
(Jones & Turner, 2006). In practice, coaches of children invest only a small percentage of their time
questioning their participants (Smith & Cushion, 2006).
Structuring the learning environment: deliberate play and deliberate practice
The importance of what coaches do (their coaching behaviour) has been discussed in terms of best
ensuring that children remain motivated to continue participating in sport. However, how coaches structure
the learning environment also needs careful consideration. Côté and Hay (2002) proposed three distinct
developmental stages; sampling years, specialising years and investment years, and that each stage needs to
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
66
be structured differently in order most appropriately to meet the needs of sports participants. In the
sampling years (aged 6-12), it is recommended that children should get the opportunity to sample a range
of different activities and sports in a way that promotes fun and enjoyment (Côté, et al., 2007). As
children move into the specialising years (aged 13-16) they start to focus on two or three sports, with the
investment years (aged 17+) being characterised by a focus on only one sport in a competitive environment
(Côté & Fraser-Thomas, 2007). These age limits resemble the different transition points of the Canadian
school system leading Bailey et al. (2010) to question the literal accuracy of the ages and stages of
development in a UK context. Nonetheless, these developmental stages provide a useful framework with
which to consider developmentally appropriate coaching environments.
When children actively participate in a range of sports, with an emphasis on promoting intrinsic
motivation, deliberate play is said to be occurring (Côté, Baker, & Abernethy, 2003); engagement in
deliberate play during the sampling years thus promoting children’s self-determined motivation and endorsing
their continued participation in sport. Additionally, some studies have suggested that children described
activities that required little input from coaches to be most enjoyable (Strean, 2009). However, it is largely
agreed that regardless of context, the coach is a crucial component in ensuring optimal development (e.g.
Abraham & Collins, 2011). Certainly, an approach in which ‘success’ is defined by skill development over
winning is something which coaches need to become aware of and promote (Abbott, et al., 2002), given
that the weight of evidence supports such an approach
An alternative coaching approach, in which children do not participate in a range of sports, and
where fun and enjoyment are not primary initiatives, has been termed as deliberate practice. Ericsson,
Krampe and Tesch-Romer (1993) suggest deliberate practice to be highly structured activities the principal
goal of which is to improve performance rather than be inherently enjoyable. Because the focus of
deliberate practice is not primarily on children having fun and enjoyable experiences, this type of practice
structure has been associated with an increased risk of children dropping out of sport (Côté, Baker, &
Abernethy, 2007). We reiterate, one of the main purposes, if not the main purpose of children’s sport in a
participation domain is to ensure their continued participation. An overemphasis on deliberate practice could
potentially result in this not being achieved.
Sports that emphasise deliberate practice characteristics have been termed ‘early specialized’. Research
by Côté and colleagues and others with children of a different age (Baker, Cobley, & Fraser-Thomas,
2009) has found that early-specialised sports are more likely to have a negative effect on children’s well
being. Côté (2004) suggests that if specialization occurs at a developmentally inappropriate age, benefits
(e.g. improved skills) are potentially outweighed by physical, psychological, and social disadvantages (e.g.
overtraining, injury, failure to develop transferable skills, decreased enjoyment, burnout, depression,
decreased self-esteem, increased sensitivity to stress, fear of competition, sense of failure, missed social
opportunities).
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
67
Implications for coaches
While it is recommended that children’s coaches adopt deliberate play practices, it cannot be
assumed that this is an easy or straightforward process, or that it will suit all sports, coaches or, indeed,
children. Coaches’ practices are underpinned by a plethora of socio-cultural factors ranging from their own
personal beliefs and philosophies, to the expectations placed on them by external agents (see Jones, et al.,
2010). Therefore, it is important to develop an understanding as to what factors inform coaches practices,
and the ways in which they can learn to adopt developmentally appropriate approaches.
We suggest that for coaches to be able successfully to implement developmentally appropriate
practices requires a commitment to continued professional development (CPD). However, as we stated
earlier, coaches of children often work in a voluntary capacity and as such time and resources are likely to
be limited. Consequently, their ability to undertake CPD in the form of National Governing Body (NGB)
coach education awards may not be possible given the cost and time requirements to complete these awards
(Gilbert & Trudel, 2006). Although it is important that coaches are subjected to, and become accredited as
competent coaches through NGB awards, it has been consistently reported that these may not be the
leading contributor towards coach learning (e.g. Abraham & Collins, 1998; Cushion, Armour & Jones,
2003; Lemrye et al., 2007). Instead, it has been suggested that coaches prefer to learn mostly through
interacting with other coaches (e.g. Gould, Werthner & Trudel, 2009; Stephenson & Jowett, 2009), and
more specifically, when working alongside a mentor (Cushion, 2006). We propose that if the practices of
coaches of children are to improve to the point where they can be considered developmentally appropriate
then a greater consideration is needed of how coaches operating in these contexts are educated. There is of
course no easy solution to this problem, bearing in mind the issues we have already raised. However, we
do believe that one possibility is for coach educators to support coaches of children within the contexts
that these coaches are working. Therefore, rather than coaches going to coach educators, the opposite ought
to happen where coach educators work with coaches in their contexts over a longitudinal period. There is
no doubt that contextual learning is most effective, especially when dealing with complex and varied needs
like those of the coach of children (Bransford, Brown & Cocking, 2000). By doing so, individual coaching
practices can be better understood, with recommendations for practice given which are more meaningful to
the coach. In the first instance though, what is needed is a greater understanding of what it is coaches of
children do, and what knowledge it is that they require. We have argued throughout this paper that coaches
of children are distinctly different from coaches in any other coaching context. As such, a greater
appreciation is needed of what it is they are actually doing at present, and how they believe they can be
supported in their future development. While the purpose of this review is not to discuss coach learning
and development, we do believe that further research in this area is important in understanding how
coaches are able to adopt developmentally appropriate practices.
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
68
Conclusion
The purpose of this review was to summarise children’s motives for taking part in sport, and to
identify some of the practices that coaches of children adopt to ensure they remain active participants in
sport. Five key factors emerged as a result of the literature review: perceived competence, fun and
enjoyment, parental influence, learning new skills and influence of friends and peers. Achievement goal
theory and SDT were used as theories to explain how these factors influenced children’s motives. By
understanding what motivates children to take part in sport, a discussion emerged that provides suggestions
to improve the practices of coaches of children. An important finding from this review is that children’s
motivations to participate in sport should be considered within the socio-cultural contexts in which sport
takes place, as well as from a psychological perspective.
It should not be surprising that carefully constructed learning environments can have positive benefits
on the psychosocial development of participating children and youth. The weight of evidence from the
review of relevant literature suggests that coaches of children need to start to move away from the more
traditional technocratic methods of coaching to appreciating more holistic, child-centred methods of coaching
that are better aligned with children’s motives for taking part in sport. The challenge for coach educators
and policy makers is how they can support coaches of children to ensure their practices are suitable for the
young people they are coaching.
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
69
References
Abbott, A., Collins., D. Martindale., R., & Sowerby, K. (2002). Talent Identification and Development: An
Academic Review. Edinburgh: sportscotland.
Abraham, A., & Collins, D. (2011). Effective Skill Development: how should athletes’ skills be developed?
In D. Collins, H. Richards & A. Button, A. (Eds.), Performance psychology: a practitioners guide
(pp.207-230). Oxford: Elsevier.
Allender, S., Cowburn, G., & Foster, C. (2006). Understanding participation in sport and physical activity
among children and adults: a review of qualitative studies. Health Education Research, 21 (6), 826
- 835.
Amorose, A. J., & Weiss, M. R. (1998). Coaching feedback as a source of information about perceptions
of ability: a developmental examination..Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 395-420.
Amorose, A. J., & Horn, T. S. (2000)..Intrinsic motivation: relationships with collegiate athletes’ gender,
scholarship status, and perceptions of their coaches’ behaviour..Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 22, 63-84.
Anderson, C. B., Hughes..S. O., & Fuemmeler, B. F. (2009)..Parent-child attitude congruence on type and
intensity of physical activity: testing multiple mediators of sedentary behaviour in older
children..Health Psychology, 28(4), 428-438.
Babkes, M. L., & Weiss, M. R. (1999)..Parental influences on children’s cognitive and affective responses
to competitive soccer participation..Pediatric Exercise Science, 11, 44-62.
Bailey, R.P. (2005)..‘Physical growth and physical development’..In N. Salkind (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
Human Development..Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bailey, R., Armour, K., Kirk, D., Jess, M., Pickup, I., & Sandford, R. (2009)..The educational benefits
claimed for physical education and school sport: an academic review..Research Papers in
Education, 24, 1-27.
Bailey, R., Collins, D., Ford, P., MacNamara, A., Toms, M., & Pearce, G. (2010)..Participant Development
in Sport..Leeds: Sportscoach UK.
Bailey, R., & Toms, M. (2010) ‘Youth talent development in sport: rethinking luck and justice.’.In A.
Hardman & R. Jones (Eds.), The Ethics of Sports Coaching. London: Routledge.
Bailey, R.P. (2012). ‘So what is developmentally appropriate sport?”.Active and Healthy, 19(2), 21-24.
Baker, J., Cobley, S., & Fraser-Thomas, J. (2009)..What do we know about early sport specialization?.Not
much!.High Ability Studies, 20, 77-90.
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
70
Barker, G. P., & Graham, S. (1987)..Developmental study of praise and blame as attributional cues..Journal
of Educational Psychology, 79(1), 62-66.
Bartholomew, K., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2011)..Psychological need
thwarting in the sport context: assessing the darker side of athletic experience..Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 33(1), 75-102.
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L. & Cocking, R. (Eds) (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience
and school. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Brewer, C., & Jones, R. L. (2002)..A five-stage process for establishing contextually valid systematic
observation instrument: The case of rugby union..The Sport Psychologist, 16, 138-159.
Brockman, R., Jago, R., Fox, K. R., Thompson, J. L., Cartwright, K., & Page, A. S. (2009)..“Get off the
sofa and go and play”: Family and socioeconomic influences on the physical activity of 10-11 year
old children..BMC Public Health, 9, 253-259.
Brustad, R. J. (1992)..Integrating socialisation influences into the study of children’s motivation in
sport..Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 14, 59-77.
Byrne, J. (1999)..Health, wealth and honesty: perceptions of self-esteem in primary aged children..Health
Education, 99(3), 95-102.
Carroll, B., & Loumidis, J. (2001)..Children’s perceived competence and enjoyment in physical education
and physical activity outside school..European Physical Education Review, 7(1), 24-43.
Chalip, L., & Green, B. L. (1998)..Establishing and maintaining a modified youth sport program: lessons
from Hotellings location game..Sociology of Sport Journal, 15(4), 326-342.
Conroy, D. E., & Coatsworth, J. D. (2007)..Assessing autonomy-supportive coaching strategies in youth
sport..Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 671–684.
Côté, J., & Hay, J. (2002)..‘Children’s involvement in sport: a developmental perspective’..In J. M. Silva
and D. Stevens (Eds.), Psychological Foundations of Sport (pp. 484–502)..Boston, MA: Merrill.
Côté, J., Baker, J., & Abernethy, B. (2003)..From play to practice: A developmental framework for the
acquisition of expertise in team sports..In K.A. Ericsson & J.L. Starkes (Eds.)..Expert performance
in sports: Advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 89-113)..Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Côté, J. (2004)..Cost-benefit analysis of early specialization. In V. Klisouras., S. Kellis & I. Mouratidis
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 2004 Pre-Olympic Congress: Sport Science through the Ages. Aristotle
University of Thessaloniki, Greece (111).
Côté, J., Salmela, J., Trudel, P., Baria, A. & Russell, S. (1995). The coaching model: a grounded
assessment of expert gymnastics coaches’ knowledge. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
71
17, 1-17.
Côté, J., Baker, J. & Abernethy, B. (2007)..Play and practice in the development of sport expertise..In G.
Tenenbaum & R.C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of Sport Psychology (pp. 184-202)..Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley and Sons.
Côté, J., & Fraser-Thomas, J. (2007)..Youth involvement in sport..In P. Crocker (Ed.), Introduction to sport
psychology: A Canadian perspective (pp. 266-294)..Toronto: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Côté, J., Young, B., North, J., & Duffy, P. (2007)..Towards a definition of excellence in Sport
Coaching..International Journal of Coaching Science, 1(1), 3-18.
Cumming, S. P., Smoll, F. L., Smith, R. E., & Grossband, J. R. (2007)..Is winning everything?.The relative
contributions of motivational climate and won-lost percentage in youth sports..Journal of Applied
Sport Psychology, 19, 322-336.
Cushion, C. J., & Jones, R. L. (2001)..A systematic observation of professional top-level youth soccer
coaches..Journal of Sport Behaviour, 24, 354-376.
Cushion, C. J., Armour, K. M., & Jones, R. L. (2006)..Locating the coaching process in practice: models
‘for’ and ‘of’ coaching..Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 11(1), 83-99.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000)..The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: human needs and the
self-determination of behaviour..Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.
Dismore, H., & Bailey, R. (2010)..‘It’s been a bit of a rocky start’: attitude toward physical education
following transition. Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 15(2), 175-190.
Duda, J. L., & Whitehead, J. (1998)..Measurement of goal perspectives in the physical domain..In J. L.
Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 145-164)..Morgantown:
Fitness Information Technology.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993)..The role of deliberate practice in the
acquisition of expert performance..Psychological Review, 100(3), 363-406.
Ford, P. R., Yates, I., & Williams, M. A. (2010)..An analysis of practice activities and instructional
behaviours used by youth sport coaches during practice: Explaining the link between science and
application..Journal of Sports Sciences, 28(5), 483-486.
Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2004)..Parental influences on youth involvement in sports..In M. Weiss
(Ed.), Developmental Sport and Exercise Psychology: A Lifespan Perspective..Morgantown, WV:
Fitness Information Technology.
Fredicks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2005)..Family socialisation, gender, and sport motivation and
involvement..Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 3-31.
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
72
Gould, D., Lauer, L., Rolo, C., Jannes, C., & Pennisi, N. (2008)..The role of parents in tennis success:
focus group interviews with junior coaches..The Sport Psychologist, 22, 18-37.
Green, C. B. (2005)..Building sport programs to optimize athlete recruitment, retention and transition:
toward a normative theory of sport development..Journal of Sport Management, 19(3), 233-245.
Hamilton, S. F., Hamilton, M. A., & Pittman, K. (2004)..Principles for youth development. In S. F.
Hamilton, & M. A. Hamilton (Ed.) The youth development handbook: coming of age in American
communities (pp.3-22)..Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Harter, S. (1978)..Effectance motivation reconsidered: toward a developmental model..Human Development,
1, 34-64.
Harvey, S., Cushion, C, J., & Massa-Gonzalez, A. (2010)..Learning a new method: teaching Games for
Understanding in the coaches’ eyes..Physical Education & Sport Pedagogy, 15(4), 361-382.
Hollembeck, J., & Amorose, A.J. (2005)..Perceived coaching behaviours and college athletes’ intrinsic
motivation: A test of self-determination theory..Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 20-36.
Jones, R.L., Armour, K.M., & Potrac, P. (2004)..Sports coaching cultures: from practice to
theory..London:.Routledge.
Jones, R.L., & Turner, P. (2006). Teaching coaches to coach holistically: can problem-based learning (PBL)
help?.Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 11(2), 181-202.
Jones, R., Morgan, K., & Harris, K. (in press)..Developing coaching pedagogy: seeking a better integration
of theory and practice..Sport, Education and Society.
Keegan, R. J., Harwood, C. G., Spray, C. M., & Lavallee, D. E. (2009)..A qualitative investigation
exploring the motivational climate in early-career sports participants: coach, parent and peer
influences on sport motivation..Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 361-372.
Kirk, D. (2005)..Physical education, youth sport and lifelong participation: the importance of early learning
experiences..European Physical Education Review, 11(3), 239-255.
Klint, K.A., & Weiss, M.R. (1987)..Perceived competence and motives for participating in youth sports: A
test of Harter's competence motivation theory..Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 55-65.
Lacy, A. C., & Darst, P. W. (1985)..Systematic observation of behaviours of winning high school Head
football coaches..Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 4, 256-270.
Lemyre, F., Trudel, P., & Durand-Bush, N. (2007)..How youth-sport coaches learn to coach..The Sport
Psychologist, 21, 191-209.
Light, R., & Lemonie, Y. (2010)..A case study on children’s reasons for joining and remaining in a French
swimming club..Asian Journal of Exercise and Sports Science, 7(1), 27-33.
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
73
Lyle, J. (2002). Sports Coaching Concepts: A Framework for Coaches’ Behaviour. London: Routledge.
McNamee, M., & Bailey, R.P. (2009)..‘Physical Education and sport.’.In R.P. Bailey, D. Carr, R. Barrow,
& C. McCarthy (Eds.), Handbook of the Philosophy of Education..London: Sage.
MacPhail, A., Gorely, T., & Kirk, D. (2003)..Young people’s socialisation into sport: a case study of an
athletics club..Sport, Education and Society, 8(2), 251-267.
Mageau, G.A., & Vallerand, R.J. (2003)..The coach-athlete relationship: A motivational model..Journal of
Sports Sciences, 21, 883-904.
Mallett, C. J., (2005)..Self-Determination Theory: A case study of evidence-based coaching..The Sports
Psychologist, 19, 417-429.
May Woods, A., Bolton, K. N., Graber, K. C., & Crull, G. S. (2007)..Chapter 5: influences of perceived
motor competence and motives on children’s physical activity..Journal of Teaching in Physical
Education, 26, 390-403.
McCarthy, P. J., & Jones, M. V. (2007). A qualitative study of sport enjoyment in the sampling years. The
Sports Psychologist, 21, 400-416.
McCullagh, P., Matzkanin, K. T., Shaw, S. D., & Maldonado, M. (1993). Motivation for participation in
physical activity: a comparison of parent – child perceived competencies and participation motives.
Paediatric Exercise Science, 5, 224-233.
Mosston M., & Ashworth S. (2002). Teaching Physical Education, (5th edn). San Francisco: Benjamin
Cummings.
Muir, B., Morgan, G., Abraham, A., & Morley, D. (2011). Developmentally appropriate approaches to
coaching children. In I. Stafford (Ed.), Coaching Children in Sport (pp. 17-37). London: Routledge.
Newton, M., Duda, J. L., & Yin, Z. (2000). Examination of the psychometric properties of the Perceived
Motivational Climate in Sport Questionnaire – 2 in a sample of female athletes. Journal of Sport
Sciences, 18 (14), 275-290.
Nicholls, J. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and
performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328-346.
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA, Harvard
University Press.
North, J. (2009). The Coaching Workforce 2009-2016: The UK Coaching Framework. Leeds: Sportscoach
UK.
Orlick, T. D. (1973). Children’s sport—a revolution is coming. Canadian Association of Health, Physical
Education, and Recreation Journal, 12-14.
International Journal of Coaching Science Vol. 7 No. 1 January 2013
74
Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Briere, N. M. (2001). Associations among perceived
autonomy supportive, forms of self-regulation, and persistence: a prospective study. Motivation and
Emotion, 25(4), 279-306.
Piggott, D. (2009). Young people's experiences of football: a grounded theory. Unpublished Doctoral Thesis,
Loughborough University.
Roberts, G. C., Klieber, D. A., & Duda, J. L. (1981). An analysis of motivation in children’s sport: the
role of perceived competence in participation. Journal of Sport Psychology, 3, 206-216.
Quested, E., & Duda, J. L. (2011). Enhancing children’s positive sport experiences and personal
development: a motivational perspective. In I. Stafford (eds.) Coaching Children in Sport (pp.
123-138). London: Routledge.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Sagar, S. S., Lavallee, D., & Spray, C. M. (2007). Why young elite athletes fear failure: Consequences of
failure. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25, 1171-1185.
Sapp, M., & Haubenstricker, J. (1978). Motivation for joining and reasons for not continuing in youth sport
programs. Paper presented at AAHPERD Conference, Kansas City..
Sit, C. H. P., & Lindner, K. J. (2006). Situational state balances and participation motivation in youth
sport: A reversal theory perspective. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 369-384.
Smith, A. L., Balaguer, I., & Duda, J. L. (2006). Goal orientation profile differences on perceived
motivational climate, perceived peer relationships, and motivation related responses of youth
athletes. Journal of Sport Sciences, 24(12), 1315-1327.
Smith, M., & Cushion, C. (2006)..An investigation of the in-game behaviours of professional, top-level
youth soccer coaches..Journal of Sports Sciences, 24(4), 355-366.
Smoll, F. L., & Smith, R. E. (2006)..Enhancing coach-athlete relationships: cognitive- behavioural principles
and procedures..In J. Dosil (Ed.), The Sport Psychologist’s Handbook: A Guide for Sport-Specific
Performance Enhancement (pp. 19-37)..Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Sportscoach UK..(2007). Sports coaching in the UK II: main report..Leeds: Sportscoach UK.
Sportscoach UK..(2009). The UK Coaching Framework: The Coaching Workforce 2009-2016..Leeds:
Sportscoach UK.
Sportscoach UK..(2011a). Sports Coaching in the UK III: A statistical analysis of coaches and coaching in
the UK: Sportscoach UK.
Sportscoach UK..(2011b). UK Coach Tracking Study..Leeds: Sportscoach UK.
Richard Bailey, Edward J. Cope, Gemma Pearce
75
Strean, W. B. (2009)..Remembering instructors: play, pain and pedagogy..Qualitative Research in Sport and
Exercise, 1(3), 210-220.
Tharp, R.G., & Gallimore, R. (1976)..Basketball’s John Wooden: What a coach can teach a
teacher..Psychology Today, 9(8), 74-78.
Tinning, R. I. (1988)..Student teaching and the pedagogy of necessity..Journal of Teaching in Physical
Education, 7(2), 82-89.
Wall, M., & Côté, J. (2007)..Developmental activities that lead to dropout and investment in sport..Physical
Education and Sport Pedagogy, 12(1), 77-87.
Wankel, L. M., & Kreisal, P. S. J. (1985)..Factors underlying enjoyment of youth sports: sport and age
group comparisons..Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 51-64.
Weiss, M. R., & Petlichkoff, L. M. (1989)..Children’s motivation for participation in and withdrawal from
sport: identifying the missing links..Paediatric Exercise Science, 1, 195-211.
Weiss, M. R., & Duncan, S. C. (1992)..The relationship between physical competence and peer acceptance
in the context of children’s sport participation..Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 14(2),
177-192.
Weiss, M. R. (1993)..Children’s participation in physical activity: are we having fun yet?.Paediatric
Exercise Science, 5(3), 205-210.
Weiss, M. R., & Amorose, A. J. (2008)..Motivational orientations and sport behaviour..In T. S. Horn (Ed.),
Advances in Sport Psychology (3rd edn) (pp. 115 - 155)..Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Wiersema, L. D. (2000)..Risks and benefits of youth sport specialization: perspectives and
recommendations..Paediatric Exercise Science, 12, 13-22.
Copyright of International Journal of Coaching Science is the property of ICCE (International Council for
Coach Education) and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.
... Previous research has shown that children's involvement in sports is influenced by several factors, such as sense of competence, enjoyment and fun, parents, opportunity to learn new skills, and friends (Bailey, Cope, and Pearce 2013). However, research has also shown that children's sports face several challenges, such as excessive focus on performance, parental pressure, early specialisation, socioeconomic barriers, inequality, and accessibility (Côté and Hay 2002). ...
... Sports leaders for children are expected to have knowledge in several different areas such as pedagogy, physiology, psychology, and sociology (Armour 2011;Gilbert and Trudel 2001). Leaders must be able to create a developmentally appropriate environment that emphasises fun, teamwork, and effort over winning, using more positive behaviours and carefully structuring their sessions (Bailey, Cope, and Pearce 2013). Unfortunately, previous research indicates that there are often shortcomings in terms of leaders' regarding knowledge about children's development in the context of sports (Côté et al. 2010). ...
... Furthermore, the riding teachers' competence and attitudes are crucial in determining how they can act and structure the activities. This has been shown in previous research in other sports (see Bailey, Cope, and Pearce 2013;Côté and Hay 2002;Marsden and Weston 2007;Klaar and Öhman 2014). By organising and instructing the accompanying adults alongside the children, interactive learning can be fostered, irrespective of previous experiences (cf . ...
... 2,3 In particular, enjoyment, learning new skills and the perception of competence are children's intrapersonal motives for sport participation in this stage of development. 4 If the sporting environment does not address these motives, the probability of early drop-out from sport increases. 5,6 To enable positive experiences to encourage long-term participation, training practices and competitions should optimally address the children's developmental stages. ...
... 5,6 To enable positive experiences to encourage long-term participation, training practices and competitions should optimally address the children's developmental stages. 4,7,8 In recent years, concepts of scaling sport to the children's needs in terms of the equipment, play areas (e.g. field dimensions) or competitive rules have dedicated special attention within research (for an overview see Buszard et al. 9 ). ...
... Thus, the use of SSGs offers both high-and lowinvolved players more on-the-ball actions, which may help to prevent drop-out, especially of low-involved players, by enhancing the perception of competence and learning new skills. 4 In U7, however, dominant players achieved a greater dominance in dribbles, goal shots and goals. The playing dominance in these variables is even more evident in SSGs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Soccer competitions in childhood should ensure a high involvement of all players regardless of their talent. The purpose of the present study was to compare soccer players’ technical actions and playing time parameters in 7v7 in comparison to 3v3 (on mini-goals without goalkeepers; under-7) and 5v5 (on 1.65 × 5 m. goals with goalkeepers; under-9) game formats in a competitive context. Using a quasi-experimental within-subject design, tournaments with under-7 (n = 42) and under-9 (n = 43) players were conducted. Technical actions and playing time parameters were rated. Based on the total number of technical actions in 7v7, median splits of the samples were conducted to compare differences between high-involved (dominant) and low-involved (non-dominant) players. No differences in the effective playing time comparing game formats within the age groups were found. In total, 7884 technical actions were rated. In both age groups, the total number of technical actions was higher in the smaller game formats. In under-7, the number of technical actions in all variables was higher in 3v3. In under-9, the number of technical actions was also higher in 5v5 except for unsuccessful passes as well as successful 1v1 situations. More unsuccessful 1v1 situations were found in 7v7. Concerning the total number of technical actions, both dominant and non-dominant players in both age groups performed more technical actions in small-sided games. Due to the higher involvement of the children, better conditions for the acquisition of soccer-specific skills in the smaller game formats compared to 7v7 game formats can be concluded.
... The organizational structure of sport and the performance demands characteristic of sport training and competition provide a unique opportunity for participants to engage in health-enhancing physical activity of varied intensity, duration, and mode; and the opportunity to do so with other people as part of a team and/or club. Participation in individual and team sports have shown to be beneficial to physical, social, psychological, and cognitive health outcomes [1][2][3][4][5][6][7]. Often, the social and mental health benefits facilitated through participation in sport exceed those achieved through participation in other leisure-time or recreational activities [8][9][10]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Sport is a subset of physical activity that can be particularly beneficial for short-and-long-term physical and mental health, and social outcomes in adults. This study presents the results of an updated systematic review of the mental health and social outcomes of community and elite-level sport participation for adults. The findings have informed the development of the ‘Mental Health through Sport’ conceptual model for adults. Methods Nine electronic databases were searched, with studies published between 2012 and March 2020 screened for inclusion. Eligible qualitative and quantitative studies reported on the relationship between sport participation and mental health and/or social outcomes in adult populations. Risk of bias (ROB) was determined using the Quality Assessment Tool (quantitative studies) or Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (qualitative studies). Results The search strategy located 8528 articles, of which, 29 involving adults 18–84 years were included for analysis. Data was extracted for demographics, methodology, and study outcomes, and results presented according to study design. The evidence indicates that participation in sport (community and elite) is related to better mental health, including improved psychological well-being (for example, higher self-esteem and life satisfaction) and lower psychological ill-being (for example, reduced levels of depression, anxiety, and stress), and improved social outcomes (for example, improved self-control, pro-social behavior, interpersonal communication, and fostering a sense of belonging). Overall, adults participating in team sport had more favorable health outcomes than those participating in individual sport, and those participating in sports more often generally report the greatest benefits; however, some evidence suggests that adults in elite sport may experience higher levels of psychological distress. Low ROB was observed for qualitative studies, but quantitative studies demonstrated inconsistencies in methodological quality. Conclusions The findings of this review confirm that participation in sport of any form (team or individual) is beneficial for improving mental health and social outcomes amongst adults. Team sports, however, may provide more potent and additional benefits for mental and social outcomes across adulthood. This review also provides preliminary evidence for the Mental Health through Sport model, though further experimental and longitudinal evidence is needed to establish the mechanisms responsible for sports effect on mental health and moderators of intervention effects. Additional qualitative work is also required to gain a better understanding of the relationship between specific elements of the sporting environment and mental health and social outcomes in adult participants.
... Coaches are critical in supporting good youth development via sport, since they impact the sports atmosphere. Positive youth development is more likely when sport coaches intentionally establish mastery-oriented environments that strive to develop players beyond athletic skills and strategies (Feltz et al., 1999;Cope et al., 2013;Camiré, 2015;Strachan et al., 2016;Holt et al., 2017). Coaches must stress the purpose of sport as learning from mistakes while appreciating working hard for personal growth in order to create a mastery-oriented atmosphere favorable to good young development. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study focused on the role of program curriculum in building social skills and sports coaching in academic and career development in terms of sports humanities and sociology. Social skills coaching and sports coaching for the students are two significant factors that need to be considered by the universities around the globe to improve the organizational climate, which ultimately lead to better student’s career and academic development. This study utilized data from 308 members of the sports federation enrolled in different programs in the universities in China through a questionnaire for investigating such influencing factors. The data were analyzed using the structural equation modeling (SEM) technique using SmartPLS. This study found a substantial relationship of social skills with academic development and organizational climate. The relationship of sports coaching with organizational climate and career development is also discussed in this study. Furthermore, the organizational climate acts as a partial mediator between sports coaching and career development. However, this study revealed that organizational climate had an insignificant relationship with academic growth and organizational climate did not mediate between social skills coaching and educational development among students. While hiring the sports personnel, universities must consider the coach’s competency using effective methods such as physical tests, preassessments, and psychological tests. Second, the universities in China have to develop a culture of social skills and sports to improve academic and career development.
... This result shows that children who are less involved in 7v7 achieve greater benefits from modifications than highly involved players. Research indicates that less-involved players are particularly at risk of dropping out of their sport early, as they may have fewer possibilities to learn new skills and to perceive themselves as competent in playing soccer (Cope, Bailey, & Pearce, 2013;Schlesinger, Löbig, Ehnold, & Nagel, 2018). Therefore, the higher involvement of non-dominant players supports the use of SSGs in U9 soccer competitions, potentially leading to better performance and preventing dropout. ...
Article
The question of appropriate game formats for soccer competitions in childhood is intensively discussed in both research and practice. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to compare under-9 (U9) soccer players’ involvement and performance regarding on-the-ball decision-making and skill execution as well as off-the-ball support in “traditional” 7v7 and “modified” 5v5 game formats in a competitive context. A tournament with U9 soccer players (N = 43) was conducted where both 7v7 and 5v5 game formats were applied. The matches were analyzed using the Game Performance Assessment Instrument. The interrater reliability for decision-making and skill execution and appropriate support was good to excellent. Only fair coefficients were found for inappropriate support. In the 5v5 matches, a higher overall game involvement was found (d = 0.48). This higher involvement resulted from significantlymore appropriate and inappropriate decisions as well as from significantly more efficient and inefficient skill executions. No differences were found in the overall game performance. The performance of executed skills was significantly better in the 5v5 game format (ηp 2 = 0.102). Low-involved and low-performing players achieved greater benefits from the modified form in terms of a higher game involvement and a better performance. The 5v5 game format seems to offer advantages for soccer competitions in the U9 age group regarding game involvement and performance.
... One participant commented on the negative aspect of pressure as "being guilted into doing a sport I hated." Research is quite clear that as parental pressure increases, children's reported enjoyment and satisfaction decreases (Bailey et al., 2013;Knight et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine the perceptions of former youth sport athletes and their recalled experiences with their parents (mothers and fathers) during the time they were participating in youth sports. Participants included 270 college students from 12 states who participated in youth sports in various sized cities and communities. Participants completed a 14 item survey with an additional 28 questions from the Parental Involvement in Sport Questionnaire (PISQ). This paper presents results from the open-ended questions on the positive and negative experiences the participants recalled with their parents during their youth sport years and PISQ results.
... 6 Fun and enjoyment is commonly reported as the primary motive for children participating in sport (and lack of enjoyment is linked to dropping out) -but the individualistic nature of sport enjoyment, means that fun and enjoyable experiences may not be the same for all. 84,88 Furthermore, levels of enjoyment in sporting contexts might not be accurately captured using an existing standardised evaluation tool (as was used in this study). The moderate enjoyment scores resulting in this study ($3/5 for both groups) also indicate that players did not completely enjoy some elements of their sporting experience in netball. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this randomized controlled trial was to evaluate the impact of a novel 8-week coach development intervention (MASTER) on game-based coaching practices of netball coaches from one netball club (n = 16; 8 intervention, 8 active-control; NSW Australia), and player outcomes for the junior athletes being coached. The multi-component MASTER intervention aimed to educate coaches on how to design and implement high quality game-based coaching sessions. The core pillar of MASTER is ‘positive coaching,’ delivered through games-based coaching practices. At baseline and 10-weeks, two coaching sessions per coach were filmed and assessed using the MASTER assessment tool. Players (8-16yrs; n = 85) were videoed during structured gameplay, with three aspects of game play assessed using Game Performance Assessment Instrument. Coaches and players (n = 75; 12-16yrs) completed a perceptions questionnaire. Using linear mixed model ITT analysis, significant interventions effects were observed for the primary outcome: percentage of time spent performing playing-form activities [25.7% (95% CI (7.79-43.65), P = 0.008, d = 1.52]. Significant interventions effects were also observed for coach perceptions, player game skills, and player self-perceptions (P < 0.05). No significant changes were observed for player enjoyment, motivation, or wellbeing. The MASTER program was effective in improving coach and player self-perceptions, and coaching practices of netball coaches during training sessions.
... Research suggests that professional coach development can increase knowledge, particularly around aspects of positive youth development, focusing on positive experiences of youths as a way to build values and sustain long-term prevention of unhealthy and risky behaviours [53]. Development of excellent coaching not only makes sessions more enjoyable and structured for participants, but also motivates participants to return to subsequent sessions [54]. It is important that programme developers and coaching staff consider their approach to coaching multi-sport programmes in order to retain participants, while ensuring the outcomes of the intervention are met through the achievement of an acceptable level of fidelity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Community parks provide opportunities for physical activity (PA) and facilitate social interactions. This formative evaluation assesses the implementation of ‘Open Goals’ (OG), a novel multi-sport programme aiming to increase family PA and community cohesion, delivered weekly by Liverpool Football Club’s charitable foundation to local parks in Liverpool, North West England. Three Open Goals parks were chosen for the evaluation settings. Formative evaluation measures included: System for Observing Play and Recreation in Communities (SOPARC) observations (n = 10), direct session observations (n = 8), semi-structured interviews with Open Goals coaching staff (n = 3), and informal feedback from families (n = 5) about their experiences of Open Goals. Descriptive statistics and thematic analysis were applied to quantitative and qualitative data, respectively. Within the three evaluation parks, Open Goals reached 107 participants from May–July 2019, through 423 session attendances. Fidelity of the programme was high (M = 69% of session content delivered as intended). Overall park use when OG was offered compared to when it was not offered was not statistically significant (p = 0.051), however, target area use was significantly increased (p = 0.001). Overall physical activity levels in parks were significantly (p = 0.002) higher when Open Goals was being offered, compared to when it was not. Coaches reported that engagement in OG positively affected family co-participation and children’s behavioural development. Contextual issues included environmental and social barriers to programme engagement, including the co-participation element of the programme and criticism of the marketing of OG. It is evident that community-based multi-sport PA programmes endorsed by professional football clubs are well positioned to connect with local communities in deprived areas and to encourage PA and community engagement. This study suggests that such programmes may have the ability to improve park usage in specific areas, along with improving physical activity levels among families, although further research is required. Effective marketing strategies are needed for promotional purposes. Upskilling of coaches in the encouragement of family co-participation may support regular family engagement in PA in local parks.
Article
Full-text available
Background In recent years, a decline in sports behaviour among adolescents was observed, even though it is generally known that sports contribute to healthy development. According to the social cognitive theory of Bandura, outcome expectancies play an important role in the practice of health behaviour. Methods This study analysed the relationship between exercise-related outcome expectancies (EOEs) and exercise behaviour among adolescent athletes, and the differences of EOEs according to age, gender and type of sport played. In an online survey N = 223 (female = 140, male = 83) athletes between the age of 10–19 (Mage = 14.27, SD = 3.21) completed the Exercise-related Outcome Expectancies Questionnaire for Adolescents (EOEQ-A), as well as questions about their sociodemographic background and training behaviour. Results A small positive correlation of psychological EOEs with the overall amount of training was discovered while expectancies about negative consequences of sports were associated with lower training efforts. Athletes between 14 and 16 years reported significantly stronger negative EOEs than younger or older participants. Regarding gender, no significant differences were found. Furthermore, a positive correlation between social EOEs and participation in team sports, as well as training in a group was found. Conclusion These results could help with adapting exercise-related interventions so that the positive expectancies of the athletes can be fulfilled and exercise behaviour among adolescents can be promoted. Future studies should investigate the relationship between changes in expectancies and changes in exercise behaviour.
Article
Full-text available
The Frailty Myth proposes that the female body can be frozen, restricted by the ever present negative gendered narrative perpetuated by society. Embodiment occurs when the female body is thawed. The opposite can be argued for boys. Boys are taught to live their bodies, that is they have a sense of embodiment. Therefore, boys do not have to concern themselves with thawing their bodies as they already experience their bodies in strong and liberal ways. In this study, I compare how girls and boys live their bodies utilizing participant observation. Six themes emerged: being the instructor, gendered discourse in action, body proximity and movement, The Invincibility Effect, the grade six and seven/eight divide and lived body moments. The implications of these observations suggest how activities such as self-defense have the potential to create a lived body, that girls can work toward a lived body and gender can be observed through everyday lived experiences. Though research exists within the literature; it does not seem to address the performance of the lived body within this population, utilizing a comparative approach.
Article
Full-text available
Researchers have investigated how elite or expert coaches learn to coach, but very few have investigated this process with coaches at the recreational or developmental-performance levels. Thirty-six youth-sport coaches (ice hockey, soccer, and baseball) were each interviewed twice to document their learning situations. Results indicate that (a) formal programs are only one of the many opportunities to learn how to coach; (b) coaches' prior experiences as players, assistant coaches, or instructors provide them with some sport-specific knowledge and allow them to initiate socialization within the subculture of their respective sports; (c) coaches rarely interact with rival coaches; and (d) there are differences in coaches' learning situations between sports. Reflections on who could help coaches get the most out of their learning situations are provided.
Article
Full-text available
The theoretical framework presented in this article explains expert performance as the end result of individuals' prolonged efforts to improve performance while negotiating motivational and external constraints. In most domains of expertise, individuals begin in their childhood a regimen of effortful activities (deliberate practice) designed to optimize improvement. Individual differences, even among elite performers, are closely related to assessed amounts of deliberate practice. Many characteristics once believed to reflect innate talent are actually the result of intense practice extended for a minimum of 10 years. Analysis of expert performance provides unique evidence on the potential and limits of extreme environmental adaptation and learning.
Article
Student teaching as a significant part of the professional development of physical education teachers is implicated in the general failure of teacher education to adequately prepare teachers who can envision a world of schooling that is any different from the present one. This paper argues that the dominant pedagogy of student teaching is inherently conservative, is characterized by technical rationality, and embraces an outmoded view of professional knowledge. The adoption of a critical-inquiry perspective in student teaching is offered as a possible alternative.
Article
Children’s reasons for participating in sport as well as their reasons for discontinuing involvement have been extensively studied over the last decade. However, a complete understanding of the underlying processes influencing these phenomena has been clouded by failing to consider a number of individual difference and contextual factors related to sport participation. These missing links include participant status group differences, program type, level of intensity, type of sport, particular reasons for attrition, multiple assessments across a season, developmental differences, and the social structure surrounding the sport experience. Future research possibilities and practical implications for pediatric educators are provided.
Article
An important issue facing sport psychology researchers as well as practitioners is understanding the motives of children involved in youth sport programs. The present study extended previous work in this area by examining parental perceptions of their children’s motivations and perceived competencies in addition to the typically assessed variables of youth-reported motives and perceived competencies. Eighty-one children and one of their parents from a youth soccer league served as subjects for this study. Results indicated that children and parents alike ranked intrinsic motives such as feeling good and having fun as primary reasons for participation. In addition, both parents and children rated external reasons as the lowest priorities for participation. Multivariate analyses of variance indicated that children rated all the motive subscales more positively than their parents. No significant relationships were found between perceived competencies and motives.
Article
This study examined, from a developmental perspective, how coaching feedback serves as an ability cue. Boys and girls (N = 60) comprising 2 age groups (6-8, 12-14) viewed videotapes of youth athletes attempting to hit a baseball or softball, followed by a coach who provided evaluative, informational, or neutral feedback. Participants then rated each athlete's ability, effort, and future expectancy of success. Separate 2 × 2 × 3 (age × gender × feedback type) repeated measures MANOVAs were conducted for the successful and unsuccessful outcome conditions. Following successful attempts, both older and younger children rated praise higher than neutral and informational feedback as a source of ability information. Athletes receiving informational feedback following unsuccessful attempts were rated highest, followed by neutral feedback and criticism. Open-ended questions revealed some age-related differences in use of ability information. Results are discussed in relation to research on sources of competence information and coaching feedback.