Content uploaded by Bertram Opitz
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Bertram Opitz on May 15, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
Memory Function and the Hippocampus
Bertram Opitz
Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, UK
Short Title: Hippocampus and Memory
Abstract:
There has been a long tradition in memory research adopting the view of a vital role of the medial
temporal lobe and especially the hippocampus in declarative memory. Despite the broad support for
this notion, there is an on-going debate about what computations are performed by the different
substructures. The present chapter summarises several accounts of hippocampal functions in terms
of the cognitive processes subserved by these structures, the information processed and the
underlying neural operations. Firstly, the value of the distinction between recollection and familiarity
for the understanding of the role the hippocampus plays in memory is discussed. Then multiple lines
of evidence for the role of the hippocampus in memory are considered. Cumulating evidence
suggests that the hippocampus fosters the binding of disparate cortical representations of items and
their spatio-temporal context into a coherent representation by means of a sparse conjunctive
neural coding. This association of item and context will then lead to the phenomenological
experience of recollection. In contrast, surrounding cortical areas have broader neural coding that
provide a scalar signal of the similarity between two inputs (e.g., between the encoding and the
retrieval). By this they form the basis of a feeling of familiarity but also might encode the
commonalities between these different inputs. However, a more complete picture of the
importance of the hippocampus for declarative memories could only be drawn when the
interactions of the medial temporal lobe with other brain areas are also taken into account.
Bertram Opitz
Department of Psychology
University of Surrey
Guildford
GU2 7XH
UK
phone: +44 1483 689449
fax: +44 1483 689553
email: b.opitz@surrey.ac.uk
Introduction
Ever since the first report of profound amnesia following medial-temporal lobe (MTL) resection in
patient H.M. [1], there has been a large amount of research aiming at the functional role of the MTL
sub regions, especially of the hippocampus, in memory. This research includes all currently available
methods, including neuroimaging studies and electrophysiological recordings in humans, single cell
recordings in animals and neuropsychological studies of patients with brain injuries or of animals
with experimental lesions. Despite that any research method has its own strength and limitations
they all converge on the view that the hippocampus operates in the service of declarative memory.
One cognitive account assumes that the MTL is involved in the recognition of a previously
encountered event [2–4]. Despite the broad support for this notion, there is an on-going debate
about what computations are performed by different sub regions within the MTL. One prominent
view capitalizing on bidirectional interconnections between the hippocampus and the surrounding
medial-temporal lobe cortex (MTLC) proposes that the hippocampus is important for all forms of
declarative memory, including recognition memory [2]. A contrasting view emphasizes the
differences between the same structures within the MTL suggesting that the hippocampus and the
MTLC support different aspects of recognition memory[3; 5; 6]. In particular, the hippocampus and
the parahippocampal cortex were assumed to support recollection, i.e. recognition of an item on the
basis of the retrieval of specific contextual details of the previous learning experience, whereas the
perirhinal cortex subserves familiarity, i.e. item recognition on the basis of a scalar memory strength
but without retrieval of any specific detail about the study episode. These models, however, are
hard to reconcile with the notion of a highly integrated network connecting all MTLC structures with
each other and, most importantly, with the hippocampus [7]. Thus, more recent modifications
sought to overcome the explanatory limitations of models associating MTLC and hippocampal
functions in terms of the purely cognitive dichotomy between familiarity and recollection,
respectively, by focussing on the kind of information, i.e. item-specific and contextual information,
stored by the different substructures of the MTL [3; 8; 9]. Other models emphasize the putative
operational characteristics of specific brain regions to describe the role of these regions in memory
[7; 10]. The present chapter reviews evidence from animal research, neuropsychological studies with
patients suffering from amnesia and the growing body of neuroimaging studies that form the basis
of each of the different accounts of the role of the hippocampus in memory.
Cognitive Processes Accounts
Following a widely acknowledged cognitive view it has been argued that the hippocampus is vital for
recognition based on recollection but not for recognition based on familiarity (Figure 1A). These
models usually further argue that MTLC regions (especially the perirhinal cortex but not the
parahippocampal cortex) are essential for familiarity based recognition, and that this function is
independent of the hippocampus [3; 11]. Consistent with this view, patients with severe hypoxic
damage to the hippocampus exhibit disproportional large deficits in associative recognition (thought
to rely on recollection) as compared to simple item memory (relying on familiarity, e.g. [12]). An
almost identical pattern of impaired recollection and preserved familiarity has also been observed in
a patient with selective hippocampal atrophy caused by meningitis [13]. Most striking evidence in
favour of dual process models comes from a double dissociation between deficits in recollection or
associative memory following hippocampal damage compared to deficits in familiarity or item
memory following damage to the perirhinal cortex [14]. These observations are paralleled by several
animal studies demonstrating that rats, initially trained to recognize associations between an odour
(e.g. , cumin, lemon, thyme) and one of several digging media (e.g. , sand, wood chips, etc.) and then
retested after selective damage to the hippocampus, exhibited impaired recollection of the
associations while memory for the odours alone was spared indicating intact familiarity [15]. In the
same vein, neuroimaging studies in healthy participants employing associative recognition memory
task demonstrated greater hippocampal activity for successful as compared to failed source
recollection [16]. In this experiment participants studied a word list while alternating between a
pleasant/unpleasant decision and a concrete/abstract decision. At test, they were required to
discriminate between two simultaneously presented test words by selecting the member of the pair
previously associated with a particular encoding task. Successful source retrieval was associated with
increased activity in the left hippocampus.
Another method capitalizes on Receiver-Operating-Characteristics (ROC) assuming that recollection
is a threshold process whereas familiarity varies in a continuous manner with response confidence
[17]. A number of studies have, therefore, used linear and curvi-linear approximations of confidence
ratings (representing recollection and familiarity, respectively) to identify regions where
hemodynamic activity systematically varies with recognition confidence [18–20]. Such parametric
analyses consistently showed that hippocampal activity was related to recollection. While some
studies found increasing activity in the perirhinal cortex as perceived strength of familiarity
increased [18], others reported monotonic decreases in activity with increasing memory strength
not only in the perirhinal cortex but also in the anterior hippocampus [19]. Yet another result was
reported in a recent study [20] observing both decreasing and increasing activity as a function of
increasing familiarity in the anterior and posterior perirhinal cortex, respectively. This latter finding
emphasizes the contradictory results with respect to the role the MTLC plays in recognition memory.
A prominent alternative view explains the functional distinction between the hippocampus and the
adjacent MTLC structures described above with a single cognitive process in which differences in
memory strength account for the differential involvement of the hippocampus and the perirhinal
and parahippocampal cortices [2; 4]. While a weak memory trace seems sufficient to engage the
MTLC, strong memories are required to engage the more powerful computational properties of the
hippocampus. Evidence for this view is for example provided by studies demonstrating that amnesic
patients are similarly impaired in all kinds of declarative memory [21]. However, single process
models cannot account for the double dissociations in amnesia and neuroimaging studies cited
above, nor can single process theories account for the double dissociations between the role of the
hippocampus and the surrounding MTLC in recollection and familiarity in recent animal studies.
Information Based Accounts
More recent models move beyond the simple and rather phenomenological dichotomy between
recollection and familiarity towards an understanding of MTL functions in terms of the information
they store. As depicted in Figure 1B these models propose that the perirhinal and the
parahippocampal cortex support the encoding and retrieval of item-specific and contextual
information, respectively whereas the hippocampus stores representations of item–context
associations [3; 8; 9]. This view is based on increased hippocampal activity in tasks emphasizing such
conjunctive memory representations such as memorizing paired associates [22], source memory
tasks [23] and tasks requiring also the spatial location of a previously presented item to be
remembered [24]. Conversely, in many of those studies activity in the perirhinal cortex correlates
with item rather than conjunctive memory performance [22; 24]. Together these studies
demonstrated that an increase of activity of the hippocampus is essential for the process of relating
an item to contextual information during retrieval. This notion has gained further support from
neuropsychological studies in amnesic patients [25; 26]. For instance, it was demonstrated that
amnesic patients could well discriminate between old and new visual scenes but were unable to
distinguish between intact old scenes and manipulated old scenes (e.g. by left– right shifting of
particular elements within the scene) [26] indicating a deficit in processing the relations of items
within a specific context rather than a deficit in recollection, per se.
Parallel evidence has also been obtained from animal studies showing that hippocampal neurons
develop representations of the specific combination of stimulus elements (odours A and B) and the
context (room X and Y) in which they occur [27]. In the beginning of their training the rats’
hippocampal neurons responded selectively to a specific location in environment occupied by the
animals (so-called “place cells”). However, after several exposures to the same contextual
discrimination problem, i.e. odour A is only rewarded in room X, when the animals acquired the
item-context associations, some neurons began to fire selectively during the sampling of a specific
item in a particular context and these cells continued to exhibit item-context specificity after
learning. Similar firing patters for the combination of specific stimuli with a location or behavioural
context in which they occurred was also demonstrated for monkeys [28] and humans [29]. These
results indicate that hippocampal firing patterns reflect unique conjunctions of stimuli with the
places and contexts in which the stimuli occur. Extending this view it was proposed that also
associations of multiple items that share their cortical representations due to a substantial feature
overlap (within the same domain, e.g. two faces or two words) can be stored by the perirhinal cortex
and recognized based on their familiarity [30]. Evidence for this assumption comes from several
neuropsychological patients with selective hippocampal damage demonstrating severe impairments
in the recognition of e.g. object–location and face–voice associations, while they were relatively
unimpaired at recognizing pairs of words, non-words, unknown and famous faces after one or
several study trials [31].
Common to all the examples described above and more general to typical episodic memory tasks,
the item presented at the time of learning has to be associated with its specific study context. Later
during recognition this association must be retrieved. As argued above the hippocampus enables the
retrieval of the association of an item with its study context and, consequently, will lead to the
phenomenological experience of recollection. In contrast, the proposed role of perirhinal cortex in
retrieving item information alone is consistent view the dual process view of familiarity based
recognition. Thus, recollection and familiarity can be regarded as rather epiphenomenal to the
information processed within the hippocampus and the MTLC, respectively.
Neural Process Based Accounts
In a similar vein, others have proposed that functional differences between MTL sub-regions are
based on their key computational role in memory [7; 10; 32]. Despite their differences these views
about MTL functions converge on the opinion that the distinct properties of hippocampal neurons
and neurons in the surrounding MTLC subserve different memory processes (Figure 1C). It was
suggested that sparse neural coding within the hippocampus will (a) foster the convergence of
disparate cortical representations of items, actions, etc. and their spatiotemporal context that
compose a unique input into a bound representation of that input and will (b) reduce the probability
that the same neurons within the hippocampus are activated by two different inputs, thereby
leading to distinct (pattern-separated) representations [10; 32]. The process of binding mentioned in
(a) can be specified in terms of relational operations (e.g. identity, greater than or earlier than) that
link together and organize the individual elements of an experience. For example, during paired
associate learning two items provide relational information about their identity with respect to their
spatio-temporal context that is processed by the hippocampus. It is capable to organize any arbitrary
relations and by this very effortful but highly flexible. Consequently, it allows for the rearrangement
of the elements of individual experiences to deal with novel situations. Crucially, the hippocampal
circuitry possesses anatomical and computational characteristics to support these properties of
separated relational bindings (see [3], for a detailed discussion). Due to this separation of different
inputs mentioned in (b) the hippocampus is able to entirely reconstruct each single input (pattern
completion), e.g. an item bound to its study context. It thereby enables the retrieval of contextual
information. Thus, the process of relational binding will lead to recognition based on recollection.
This close connection between relational binding and recollection was corroborated by animal
studies and neuroimaging studies (extensively reviewed by [3; 9]).
In contrast, the neural activity of separate inputs to the MTLC is highly overlapping and therefore
allows processing the shared structure of these separate inputs (representational bindings). For
example, the first presentation of an item in a particular context, e.g. during encoding, weakly
activates a large number of MTLC neurons, whereas repeated and thus familiar stimuli although in a
different context, e.g. during a recognition test, activate only a subset of these neurons representing
the familiar stimuli but every neuron is activated to a stronger degree [32]. Thus, during recognition
the presentation of a studied item initiates a set of processes that may be described in a more
cognitive framework as a comparison between the neural activity associated with the short-lived
representation of the actual stimulus and the confined activity in the MTLC of the previous
encounter of that stimulus. As a result, a scalar familiarity signal is provided that tracks the global
similarity between the test probe and the studied items [33]. It should be noted that similar to the
information based accounts it is assumed that, due to the divergent neural connections of the MTLC
sub regions to neocortical areas, different structures within the MTLC bind different features of the
entire input [7, 34]. While the perirhinal cortex encodes information about objects, the
parahippocampal cortex represents the respective context of that input (Figure 1C).
As a consequence of this representational binding the MTLC is capable extracting the general
regularities inherent in the input over repeated exposures to that input. These regularities mainly
comprise frequency of co-occurrence but may also include transition probabilities or temporal
contingencies (e.g. red and green in a traffic light, or item positions in a list learning paradigm).
However, there are limitations to the ability of the MTLC to abstract the regularities inherent in the
recent input. As the MTLC receives the majority of its inputs from unimodal and polymodal
association areas [34], representational bindings within the MTLC are necessarily based on
superficial perceptual features. Consequently, the MTLC is hardly capable to create abstract
representations that are essential for goal-directed behaviour. However, the prefrontal cortex (PFC)
seems ideally suited for the abstraction of such behaviour-guiding representations [35]. Thus, while
the MTLC mainly binds the representation of the actual item/context to the representation of a
previous occurrence of that same item/context, the PFC mediates the binding of the actual event to
a more abstract or prototypical representation of invariant and non-accidental features of that
event.
This binding view is supported by recent studies demonstrating an impairment of patients with
anterior MTL lesions, including the perirhinal cortex, in perceptual discrimination of complex objects
with a large number of overlapping features [36]. More importantly, this impairment was largest for
objects with pre-existing semantic representations, e.g. beasts as compared to novel objects such as
bar codes. This is consistent with the present view, that representational bindings supported by the
perirhinal cortex link the actual appearance of a particular object to the mental representation of
previous experiences with that same object. In a similar vein, the parahippocampal cortex mediates
representational bindings of contextual features. For instance, it was demonstrated that the
parahippocampal cortex is more active for objects that are strongly associated with a specific
context (e.g. roulette wheel) than for objects that are very weakly associated with many possible
contexts (e.g. cherry) [37]. These examples underscore the important role of both cortices for
representational binding by demonstrating that readdressing object and/or contextual features of
object occurrence during the repeated processing of a particular event require the integrity/activity
of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex, respectively.
Summary
The present chapter summarized part of the recent evidence for the role of the hippocampus and
the surrounding MTLC in memory. Several accounts described hippocampal function in terms of the
cognitive processes subserved by different substructures within the MTL others have focused more
on the different information processed by these structures or on the underlying neural operations.
Although accounts based on the dichotomy between recollection and familiarity have a long
standing tradition in memory research, it is still an open question whether the brain actually
operates on this dichotomy. The two other accounts are more directly related to the different
structures and the respective neural processes. Despite the exact relation of the neural processes to
the assumed cognitive processes remains to be clarified it seems that the neural processes account
cuts across the boundaries inherent in the cognitive processes.
The list of accounts on hippocampal memory function is far from being complete. Other accounts
implicate the hippocampus in recent but not in remote memories. These are special cases of the
overarching issue of memory consolidation assuming that under certain circumstances memories
can become independent of the hippocampus. Although these circumstances are subject to current
debate they are of high importance for amnesia research. Yet others emphasize the role of the
hippocampus in spatial memory. This will be covered in the next chapter. More general, these highly
different views are suggestive of a more general role of MTL substructures in memory than
discussed in the present chapter.
It should be also noted that the hippocampus and the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices are
interconnected with multiple brain areas in the parietal and frontal lobes. As the focus of this
chapter was on the role of the hippocampus in memory the important contribution of these other
brain structures was not covered. However, only when their role was taken into account a more
complete picture of the importance of the medial temporal lobe for the formation of declarative
memories can be drawn.
References:
1. Scoville WB, Milner B: Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957; 20:11–21.
2. Wixted JT, Squire LR: The medial temporal lobe and the attributes of memory. Trends in
Cognitive Neuroscience 2011; 15(5):210–217.
3. Eichenbaum HB, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C: The medial temporal lobe and recognition
memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007; 30:123–152.
4. Squire LR, Wixted JT, Clark RE: Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new
perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007 Nov; 8(11):872–883.
5. Davachi L, Wagner AD: Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: insights from
relational and item-based learning. Journal of Neurophysiology 2002; 88:982–990.
6. Diana RA, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C: The effects of unitization on familiarity-based source
memory: testing a behavioral prediction derived from neuroimaging data. J Exp Psychol Learn
Mem Cogn 2008; 34(4):730–740.
7. Aggleton JP: Multiple anatomical systems embedded within the primate medial temporal
lobe: Implications for hippocampal function. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2012;
36(7):1579–96.
8. Davachi L: Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol
2006; 16(6):693–700.
9. Diana RA, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C: Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial
temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn Sci 2007 Sep;11(9):379–386.
10. Opitz B: Neural binding mechanisms in learning and memory. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews 2010; 34(7):1036–46.
11. Brown MW, Aggleton JP: Recognition memory: what are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and
hippocampus? Nat Rev Neurosci 2001 Jan; 2(1):51–61.
12. Yonelinas AP, Quamme JR, Widaman KF, Kroll NEA, Sauvéand MJ, Knight RT: Mild hypoxia
disrupts recollection,not familiarity. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2004; 4(3):393–400.
13. Aggleton JP, Vann SD, Denby C, Dix S, Mayes AR, Roberts N, Yonelinas AP: Sparing of the
familiarity component of recognition memory in a patient with hippocampal pathology.
Neuropsychologia 2005; 43(12):1810–1823.
14. Bowles B, Crupi C, Mirsattari SM, Pigott SE, Parrent AG, Pruessner JC, Yonelinas AP, Köhler S:
Impaired familiarity with preserved recollection after anterior temporal-lobe resection that
spares the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America 2007; 104(41):16382–7.
15. Fortin NJ, Wright SP, Eichenbaum H: Recollection-like memory retrieval in rats is dependent
on the hippocampus. Nature 2004; 431(7005):188–91.
16. Dobbins IG, Rice HJ, Wagner AD, Schacter DL: Memory orientation and success: separable
neurocognitive components underlying episodic recognition. Neuropsychologia 2003;
41(3):318–333.
17. Yonelinas AP: The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research.
Journal of Memry and Language 2002; 46:441–517.
18. Montaldi D, Spencer TJ, Roberts N, Mayes AR: The neural system that mediates familiarity
memory. Hippocampus 2006; 16(5):504–520.
19. Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Cabeza R: Triple dissociation in the medial temporal lobes:
recollection, familiarity, and novelty. J Neurophysiol 2006; 96(4):1902–1911.
20. Yassa MA, Stark CEL: Multiple signals of recognition memory in the medial temporal lobe.
Hippocampus 2008; 18(9):945–954.
21. Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Roberts N, Cezayirli E, Isaac CL, O’Reilly RC: Under what conditions is
recognition spared relative to recall after selective hippocampal damage in humans?
Hippocampus 2002; 12:341–351.
22. Kirwan CB, Stark CEL: Medial temporal lobe activation during encoding and retrieval of novel
face-name pairs. Hippocampus 2004; 14(7):919–930.
23. Davachi L, Mitchell JP, Wagner AD: Multiple routes to memory: distinct medial temporal lobe
processes build item and source memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003; 100(4):2157–2162.
24. Uncapher MR, Otten LJ, Rugg MD: Episodic encoding is more than the sum of its parts: an
{fMRI} investigation of multifeatural contextual encoding.s. Neuron 2006; 52(3):547–556.
25. Mayes AR, Holdstock JS, Isaac CL, Montaldi D, Grigor J, Gummer A, Cariga P, Downes JJ,
Tsivilis D, Gaffan D, Gong Q, Norman KA: Associative recognition in a patient with selective
hippocampal lesions and relatively normal item recognition. Hippocampus 2004; 14(6):763–
784.
26. Ryan JD, Althoff RR, Whitlow S, Cohen NJ: Amnesia Is a Deficit in Relational Memory. Psychol
Sci 2000; 11:454–461.
27. Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Eichenbaum H: Robust conjunctive item-place coding by
hippocampal neurons parallels learning what happens where. The Journal of Neuroscience
2009; 29(31):9918–29.
28. Wirth S, Yanike M, Frank LM, Smith AC, Brown EN, Suzuki WA: Single neurons in the
hippocampus and learning ofnew associations. Science 2003; 300:1578–1581.
29. Ekstrom AD, Kahana MJ, Caplan JB, Fields TA, Isham EA, Newman EL, Fried I: Cellular
networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature 2003; 425:184–187.
30. Mayes AR, Montaldi D, Migo E: Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends
Cogn Sci 2007; 11(3):126–135.
31. Mayes AR, Holdstock JS, Isaac CL, Hunkin NM, Roberts N: Relative sparing of item recognition
memory in a patient with adult-onset damage limited to the hippocampus. Hippocampus
2002; 12(3):325–340.
32. O’Reilly RC, Norman KA: Hippocampal and neocortical contributions to memory: advances in
the complementary learning systems approach. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2002; 6:505–
510.
33. Hintzman DL: Similarity, global matching, and judgments of frequency. Mem Cognit 2001;
29:547–556.
34. Suzuki WA, Amaral DG: Functional neuroanatomy of the medial temporal lobe memory
system. Cortex 2004; 40(1):220–222.
35. Simons JS, Spiers HJ: Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory.
Nature Rev Neurosci 2003; 4:637–648.
36. Barense MD, Gaffan D, Graham KS: The human medial temporal lobe processes online
representations of complex objects. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45(13):2963–2974.
37. Bar M, Aminoff E: Cortical Analysis of Visual Context. Neuron 2003; 38:258–347.
Figure 1. Schematic illustrations of the core assumptions of (A) Cognitive Processes Accounts, (B)
Information Based Accounts (cf. [3]) and (C) Neural Processes Based Accounts (cf. [10])
about the role of different substructures within the medial temporal lobes in memory.
PRC – perirhinal cortex, PHC – parahippocampal cortex, PFC – prefrontal cortex