ArticlePDF Available

The Relationships Among Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction Among Undergraduate Students

Springer Nature
Journal of Happiness Studies
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Gratitude is a positive psychological characteristic that is connected to well-being. The aim of this study is to examine the effects of both social support and self-esteem in the association between gratitude and life satisfaction among undergraduate students. Four hundred and twenty-seven Chinese undergraduate participants were asked to complete the Gratitude Questionnaire, the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale, the Multi-Dimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support and the Satisfaction with Life Scale. Path analysis indicated that social support acted as a full mediator of the association between gratitude and life satisfaction. The identified model also revealed a significant path from gratitude through social support and self-esteem to life satisfaction. Furthermore, a multi-group analysis indicated that males with high gratitude scores are more likely to get greater social support than females, while females with high social support scores tended to report greater life satisfaction than males. The present findings provide valuable guidance for how to implement psychological interventions aimed at enhancing undergraduates’ well-being.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1 23
Journal of Happiness Studies
An Interdisciplinary Forum on
Subjective Well-Being
ISSN 1389-4978
J Happiness Stud
DOI 10.1007/s10902-014-9519-2
The Relationships Among Gratitude, Self-
esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction
Among Undergraduate Students
Feng Kong, Ke Ding & Jingjing Zhao
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and all
rights are held exclusively by Springer Science
+Business Media Dordrecht. This e-offprint
is for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you wish
to self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.
RESEARCH PAPER
The Relationships Among Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social
Support and Life Satisfaction Among Undergraduate
Students
Feng Kong Ke Ding Jingjing Zhao
ÓSpringer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014
Abstract Gratitude is a positive psychological characteristic that is connected to well-
being. The aim of this study is to examine the effects of both social support and self-
esteem in the association between gratitude and life satisfaction among undergraduate
students. Four hundred and twenty-seven Chinese undergraduate participants were asked
to complete the Gratitude Questionnaire, the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale, the Multi-
Dimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support and the Satisfaction with Life Scale. Path
analysis indicated that social support acted as a full mediator of the association between
gratitude and life satisfaction. The identified model also revealed a significant path from
gratitude through social support and self-esteem to life satisfaction. Furthermore, a multi-
group analysis indicated that males with high gratitude scores are more likely to get
greater social support than females, while females with high social support scores tended
to report greater life satisfaction than males. The present findings provide valuable
guidance for how to implement psychological interventions aimed at enhancing under-
graduates’ well-being.
Keywords Gratitude Social support Self-esteem
Satisfaction with life
F. Kong (&)K. Ding
State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing,
China
e-mail: kongfeng87@126.com
J. Zhao (&)
School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi’an, China
e-mail: zhaojingjing_31@126.com
123
J Happiness Stud
DOI 10.1007/s10902-014-9519-2
Author's personal copy
1 Introduction
With the advent of the positive psychology movement, gratitude has drawn much attention
among psychologists from personality, social, clinical, developmental perspectives (e.g.,
Emmons and McCullough 2003; Froh et al. 2008). Gratitude has been defined as a state
and a trait in the literature (e.g. Chen and Kee 2008; Emmons et al. 2003; Watkins et al.
2003). As a state, gratitude can be conceptualized as a subjective feeling of wonder,
thankfulness, and appreciation for life (Emmons and Shelton 2002). It has also been
considered as a complex, higher-level emotion since it requires cognitive sophistication
(Emmons and Shelton 2002). As a dispositional trait, gratitude is understood as a ‘‘virtue’
or characteristic of people, and can vary in intensity, frequency, and span (McCullough
et al. 2002; Wood et al. 2008). People high in dispositional gratitude feel more grateful
following a positive emotion, and experience gratitude more times per day and across a
wider array of life circumstances than those low in dispositional gratitude. Previous studies
have demonstrated that higher levels of trait gratitude are highly related to more intense
experiences of state gratitude in daily life (McCullough et al. 2002). The purposes of the
current study were to replicate the relation between gratitude and life satisfaction and to
expand previous literature by investigating the self-esteem and social support in this
relationship.
1.1 Gratitude and Life Satisfaction
There is a great body of research investigating the relationship between gratitude and life
satisfaction that refers to is defined as a global evaluation of the quality of one’s life and is
regarded as an important component of subjective well-being (Diener et al. 2003). Grat-
itude is thought to have a positive emotional valence, which may be prone to well-being
(McCullough et al. 2002). Numerous empirical studies have shown positive correlations
between gratitude and life satisfaction (e.g., Froh et al. 2009; McCullough et al. 2002; Sun
and Kong 2013; Toussaint and Friedman 2009; Watkins et al. 2003). Moreover, the
positive relationships between gratitude and life satisfaction remain significant after con-
trolling for personality characteristics such as social desirability and affectivity (McCul-
lough et al. 2002). Using two-step hierarchical multiple regression analyses, Wood et al.
(2009) found that gratitude explained an additional 9 % of the variance in life satisfaction
after controlling for the Big Five personality traits. These results support the notion that
gratitude is uniquely important to well-being, above and beyond the effects of personality
characteristics alone. Further, gratitude interventions have been developed to increase
gratitude (e.g., Emmons and McCullough 2003). For example, to induce gratitude, par-
ticipants were instructed to engage in self-guided exercises involving counting their
blessings on a weekly basis for 10 weeks (Emmons and McCullough 2003). Numerous
studies suggest that with an increase in gratitude through interventions, individuals’ life
satisfaction also tended to increase (e.g., Emmons and McCullough 2003; Froh et al. 2008).
1.2 Gratitude, Social Support, Self-esteem and Life Satisfaction
The previous literature is clear that gratitude is associated with life satisfaction, but the
extent to which intervening variables mediate their relationship is relatively less studied
(e.g., Wood et al. 2010).
A review of the literature has identified one promising mediator between gratitude and
life satisfaction is social support. Like other positive emotions, gratitude is believed to
F. Kong et al.
123
Author's personal copy
reflect, motivate, and reinforce social actions in both the giver and gift recipient (Fred-
rickson 2004). Thus, gratitude can build social resources, especially social support from
others. Some theorists have further asserted that individuals with high levels of gratitude
are more likely to perceive, receive, and appreciate social support from others and thus to
experience greater well-being (e.g., McCullough et al. 2002; Wood et al. 2008). This is
supported by a series of empirical studies. On the one hand, grateful people are likely to
perceive greater social support (Froh et al. 2009; Wood et al. 2008). On the other hand,
those who receive more social support from family and friends report greater life satis-
faction (e.g., Kong and You 2013; Kong et al. 2012a,b). Several studies have found that
social support acted as a mediator between gratitude and depression in undergraduate
students, as well as between gratitude and well-being in adolescent athletes (Chen et al.
2012; Wood et al. 2008). Thus, gratitude might be associated with higher levels of life
satisfaction by more social support from others.
Similarly, self-esteem, one’s general sense of his or her value or worth (Rosenberg
1979), may be hypothesized to act as a mediator of the relationship. According to Fred-
rickson’s (2001) ‘‘broaden and build’’ model of positive emotions, positive emotions
broaden people’s momentary repertoires of cognition and behavior, and build their
enduring personal resources. Thus, gratitude could possibly enhance their levels of self-
esteem. Some theorists have claimed that grateful people are more likely to have higher
self-evaluations and thus to experience greater well-being (e.g., McCullough et al. 2002).
Numerous empirical studies have shown that grateful people have a propensity to have
higher levels of self-esteem (Li et al. 2012; Kashdan et al. 2006; Strelan 2007). Moreover,
self-esteem theoretically (e.g., Hermans 1992; Mack 1983) and empirically (e.g., Kong and
You 2013; Kong et al. 2012b; Sedikides et al. 2004) contributes to life satisfaction. Thus,
gratitude is likely to be associated with higher levels of life satisfaction by greater sense of
self-esteem.
In addition, there is a long history in psychological research which has examined the
relationship between social support and self-esteem. Theoretical work has emphasized the
importance of social support as determinants of self-esteem (Mruk 1995; Rosenberg 1979).
Numerous empirical studies have shown that social support significantly predicted one’s
level of self-esteem (e.g., Hoffman et al. 1988; Kong and You 2013; Kong et al. 2012a).
Furthermore, past studies have demonstrated that social support seems to exert an influence
on life satisfaction both directly and indirectly through self-esteem (i.e., Kong and You
2013; Kong et al. 2012a; Yarcheski, Mahon, & Yarcheski, 2001). Therefore, we expected
that in this study self-esteem should mediate the association between social support and life
satisfaction.
1.3 The Current Study
The goal of this study is to test the concurrent mediation effects of social support and
self-esteem on the relationship between gratitude and life satisfaction within personal
and interpersonal frameworks. Considering the link of life satisfaction to social support
(e.g., Kong and You 2013; Kong et al. 2012a,b), and to self-esteem (e.g., Kong and
You 2013; Kong et al. 2012a; Sedikides et al. 2004), and the important role of grat-
itude in life satisfaction (e.g., Emmons and McCullough 2003; Froh et al. 2009;
McCullough et al. 2002; Toussaint and Friedman 2009; Watkins et al. 2003), we
predicted that social support and self-esteem might act as mediators of the relationship.
Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction
123
Author's personal copy
Further, previous research has shown that a multi-mediator model may be more
meaningful than a single-mediator model, because it may provide our relative impor-
tance of these mediators. For instance, Park et al. (2010) found that only maladaptive
coping might directly mediate between perfectionism and psychological distress, even
though the mediating effects of maladaptive coping and self-esteem have been exam-
ined separately in the previous literature (Dunn et al. 2006; Rice et al. 1998).
In summary, the current study tested the mediation effects of social support and self-
esteem on the relationship between gratitude and life satisfaction among university stu-
dents. Based on the previous studies, we proposed the following hypotheses: (1) Gratitude
predicted significantly life satisfaction. (2) Social support and self-esteem mediated the
association between gratitude and life satisfaction. (3) Social support mediated the asso-
ciation between gratitude and self-esteem. (4) Self-esteem mediated the association
between gratitude and life satisfaction.
2 Method
2.1 Participants and Procedure
Four hundred and twenty-seven Chinese undergraduates from Shaanxi Normal University
in Mainland China volunteered to participate in the study (mean age =21.01 years,
SD =1.42 years, age range =18–27 years). In the sample, 267 were females, 159 were
males and 1 did not report his/her gender; 225 majored in literal arts, and 196 in science
and 6 did not report his/her major.
A multi-section questionnaire was administered to the participants in a quiet classroom
environment. All the questionnaires administrated in this study were in Chinese language.
The entire study project was completed from mid-November 2012 to mid-December 2012.
We used the cluster sampling method to select the participants from grade one to grade
three in college. Researchers instructed the students who took part in the study, and then
gave the students a set of questionnaires containing the items of the scales. The participants
did not place their names on the measures and the confidentiality of their responses were
assured. It took approximately 15 min for the students to complete all the instruments.
2.2 Measures
2.2.1 Gratitude
Gratitude was measured by the Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ-6, McCullough et al. 2002). It
includes 6 items such as, ‘‘I feel thankful for what I have received in life’’, ‘‘Long amounts
of time can go by before I feel grateful to something or someone’’, and ‘‘I sometimes feel
grateful for the smallest things’’. Each item is answered on a 7-point Likert type scale
ranging from 1 =strongly disagree to 7 =strongly agree. Scale scores are the sum of
items with reverse coding of relevant items. Scores can range from 6–42, with higher
scores indicating higher levels of gratitude. The GQ-6 exhibits excellent psychometric
properties and has been shown to explain unique associations with relevant constructs even
after controlling for Big Five domains and social desirability (McCullough et al. 2002).
The Chinese version of the GQ-6 has been demonstrated to be a reliable and valid mea-
surement in assessing trait gratitude in Chinese populations (e.g., Chen and Kee 2008). In
the present study, the Cronbach alpha coefficient for the GQ-6 was .71.
F. Kong et al.
123
Author's personal copy
2.2.2 Social Support
Social support was measured by the Multi-Dimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support
(MSPSS: Zimet et al. 1988). The MSPSS consists of 12 items, for example ‘‘My family
really tries to help me’’, and ‘‘There is a special person who is around when I am in need’’.
Each item is answered on a 7-point Likert type scale ranging from 1 =strongly disagree to
7=strongly agree. The scale scores for the MSPSS can range from 12 to 84, with higher
scores indicating higher perceived levels of social support. Three separate scores can be
calculated for the sources of support; Significant Other, Family and Friends. In addition,
the total of all items indicates total perceived social support grades. The Chinese version of
the MSPSS has been demonstrated to be a reliable and valid measurement in assessing
social support in Chinese populations (e.g., Kong and You 2013; Kong and Zhao 2013;
Kong et al. 2012a). In the present study, the Cronbach alpha coefficients for the internal
consistency of MSPSS were calculated as .88 for the overall scale, .73 for family, .75 for
friends and .81 for significant other.
2.2.3 Self-esteem
Self-esteem was measured by the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg 1965),
which is a 10-item self-report measure of global self-esteem. Each item is answered on a
7-point Likert type scale ranging from 1 =strongly disagree to 4 =strongly agree. It
includes items such as, ‘‘I am able to do things as well as most other people.’’ and ‘‘I take a
positive attitude toward myself’’. Scale scores are the sum of items with reverse coding of
relevant items. The scores can range from 10 (low level of self-esteem) to 40 (high level of
self-esteem). A meta-analysis of the scale by Schmitt and Allik (2005) found that the scale
was a very popular test whose validity and reliability tests are used in 53 countries. The
Chinese version of the RSES has been found to be a reliable and valid measurement in
assessing self-esteem in Chinese populations (Kong and You 2013; Zhao et al. 2012,
2013). In the present study, the Cronbach alpha coefficient for the RSES was .83.
2.2.4 Life Satisfaction
Life satisfaction (the cognitive component of subjective well-being) was assessed by the
Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS, Diener et al. 1985) consisting of five items. Each item
is answered on a 7-point Likert type scale ranging from 1 =strongly disagree to
7=strongly agree. It includes items such as, ‘‘I am satisfied with my life’’ and ‘‘In most
ways my life is close to my ideal’’. A total score was obtained by adding up participants’
responses. Scores can range from 5–35, with higher scores indicating more satisfaction
with life. The scale has been proved to exhibit excellent psychometric properties in Chi-
nese populations (e.g., Kong and You 2013; Kong and Zhao 2013; Kong et al. 2014). In the
present study, the Cronbach alpha coefficient for the SWLS was .79.
2.3 Data Analysis
The two-step procedure recommended by Anderson and Gerbing (1988) was adopted to
analyze the mediation effects. The measurement model was first tested to assess the extent
to which each of the latent variables was represented by its indicators. If the measurement
model was accepted, then test the structural model via the maximum likelihood estimation
in AMOS 17.0 program. In order to control for inflated measurement errors due to multiple
Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction
123
Author's personal copy
items for the latent variable, we divided the items for each of each of the gratitude, the self-
esteem and life satisfaction factors into three parcels to serve as indicators of the factors
using an item-to-construct balance approach (i.e., successively assigning highest and
lowest loading items across parcels; Little et al. 2002).
The following four indices were used to evaluate the goodness of fit of the model (Hu
and Bentler 1999): Chi square statistics, standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR)
of .08 or less, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) of .06 or less, and
comparative fit index (CFI) above .95. Two indices that are proposed to measure the
parsimony of the model are also reported: parsimony goodness of fit index (PGFI) and
parsimony normed fit index (PNFI). Values of PGFI and PNFI range from 0 to 1.00 with a
value above .70 indicating a good fit (a parsimonious model) (Jo
¨reskog and Sorbom 1989).
To compare two or more models, we additionally examined Akaike Information Criterion
(AIC: Akaike 1987) with smaller values representing a better fit of the hypothesized model
and expected cross-validation index (ECVI: Browne and Cudeck 1993) with the smallest
values exhibiting the greatest potential for replication.
3 Results
3.1 Measurement Model
The measurement model consisted of four latent factors (gratitude, social support, self-
esteem, and life satisfaction) and 12 observed variables. An initial test of the measurement
model revealed a satisfactory fit to the data: v
2
(48, N =427) =65.11, p[.05;
v
2
/df =1.36; RMSEA =.029 [90 CI: .002–.046]; SRMR =.029; and CFI =.99. All the
factor loadings for the indicators on the latent variables were significant (p\.001),
indicating that all the latent factors were well represented by their respective indicators.
Means, standard deviations, and correlations between gratitude, social support, self-
esteem, and life satisfaction are presented in Table 1. As Table 1indicated, higher levels
of gratitude were positively associated with more perceived social support, greater self-
esteem and greater life satisfaction, which is consistent with previous studies (e.g., Em-
mons and McCullough 2003; Froh et al. 2009; Kashdan et al. 2006; Li et al. 2012;
McCullough et al. 2002; Toussaint and Friedman 2009; Watkins et al. 2003; Wood et al.
2008). Higher levels of life satisfaction were associated with and more social support and
greater self-esteem which is in line with previous studies (e.g., Kong et al. 2012a,b; Kong
and You 2013; Sedikides et al. 2004).
3.2 Test of the Mediation Model
The direct path coefficient from the predictor (gratitude) to the criterion (life satisfaction,
b=.23, p\.01) in the absence of mediators was significant. A partially-mediated model
(Model 1) with two mediators and a direct path from gratitude to life satisfaction revealed a
good fit to the data (Table 2). However, the standardized path coefficient from gratitude to
life satisfaction became non-significant (b=-.14, p[.05). Thus, a fully-mediated model
(Model 2) was tested subsequently with this path constrained to zero, which revealed a
satisfactory fit to the data: v
2
(50, N =427) =112.96, p\.001; v
2
/df =2.26;
RMSEA =.054 [90 CI: .039–.066]; SRMR =.074; and CFI =.96. No significant Chi
square difference existed when comparing Model 1 to Model 2, Dv2 (1, N =427) =3.20,
p[.05. Although there was no great difference between both the models according to the
F. Kong et al.
123
Author's personal copy
fit indices, the parsimony of Model 2 suggested that the fit of Model 2 was more
satisfactory.
To find the best model, a path from social support to self-esteem was added to the fully-
mediated model (Model 3) based on the previous studies reporting self-esteem acted as a
mediator between social support and well-being (Kong and You 2013; Kong et al. 2012, b)
and the results showed a very good fit to the data (Table 2). The significant Chi square
difference was obtained when comparing Model 2 to Model 3, Dv
2
(1, N =427) =45.77,
p\.001, as well as the smaller AIC and ECVI, indicated that this additional path sig-
nificantly contributed to the model. However, the standardized path coefficient from
gratitude to self-esteem became non-significant (b=.12, p[.05). Thus, the path was
removed and Model 4 was tested. No significant Chi square difference was found when
comparing Model 3 to Model 4, Dv
2
(1, N =427) =3.01, p[.05. Although there was no
great difference between Model 3 and Model 4 according to the fit indices, the parsimony
of Model 4 suggested that Model 4 was more satisfactory. The significant Chi square
difference was obtained when comparing Model 4 to Model 2, Dv
2
(1, N =427) =42.76,
p\.001, as well as the smaller AIC and ECVI, indicated that Model 4 was more satis-
factory. Taken together, Model 4 was selected as the best model (Fig. 1).
Bootstrapping procedures were used to test the significance of the partially-mediated
model. Bootstrapping has the advantage that it does not impose distributional assumptions,
since the assumption that the indirect effect is normally distributed is often violated (e.g.,
Shrout and Bolger 2002). Bootstrapping is a procedure in which a number of samples (e.g.
1,000) are taken from the original data by random sampling with replacement. The indirect
effect in each of these bootstrap samples is computed. We generated 1,000 bootstrapping
samples from the original data set (N =427) by random sampling. Table 3displays the
indirect effects and their associated 95 % confidence intervals. As shown in Table 3,grat-
itude exerted significant indirect effect on life satisfaction via social support and self-esteem.
Self-esteem exerted significant indirect effect on life satisfaction via social support.
3.3 Gender Differences
We found no statistically significant gender differences in life satisfaction, social support
and self-esteem but females significantly scored higher than males on gratitude.
Multi-group analysis was used to identify whether the path coefficients differ signifi-
cantly between females and males. As Byrne (2001) recommended, we first tested for
Table 1 Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations for all measures
Measure M SD Range 1 2 3 4 5 6
Gender
a
1
Major -.26** 1
Gratitude 33.02 5.27 6–42 -.12* -.09 1
RSES 30.21 4.45 10–40 -.01 -.11* .33** 1
MSPSS 62.33 12.44 12–84 .07 -.16** .47** .50** 1
SWLS 18.03 5.85 5–35 -.08 -.09 .19** .44** .52** 1
MSPSS Multi-dimensional scale of perceived social support, RSES Rosenberg self-esteem scale, SWLS Satisfaction
with life scale
a
Gender is coded 1 =male, 2 =female. Major is coded 1 =arts, 2 =science
*p\.05; ** p\.001
Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction
123
Author's personal copy
invariance of the measurement models across gender before constraining path coefficients,
and found non-significant Chi square differences between the two models, Dv
2
(8,
N=427) =15.06, p[.05. Further, we compared the first model, which allows the
Table 2 Fit Indices among competing models
v
2
df RMSEA SRMR CFI PGFI PNFI AIC ECVI
Model 1 109.76 49 .054 .071 .97 .72 .70 191.76 .45
Model 2 112.96 50 .054 .074 .96 .73 .70 192.96 .45
Model 3 67.19 49 .030 .031 .99 .74 .71 149.19 .35
Model 4 70.20 50 .031 .034 .99 .74 .71 150.20 .35
N=351. RMSEA root-mean-square error of approximation, SRMR, standardized root-mean-square resid-
ual, CFI comparative fit index, PGFI parsimony goodness of fit index, PNFI parsimony normed fit index,
AIC Akaike information criterion, ECVI expected cross-validation index
Fig. 1 The mediation model (N =351) Note. Factor loadings are standardized. GQ1–GQ3 are three
parcels of gratitude; SE1–SE3 = three parcels of self-esteem; LS1–LS3 are three indices of life satisfaction;
SS_F,SS_O and SS_FA are the subscales of the Multi-Dimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. All
the path coefficients are significant at the .001 level
Table 3 Standardized indirect effects and 95 % confidence intervals
Model pathways Estimated 95 % CI
Lower Upper
Gratitude?Social support?life satisfaction .17
a
.07 .31
Gratitude?Social support?Self-esteem .18
a
.07 .32
Social support?Self-esteem?life satisfaction .10
a
.02 .23
a
Empirical 95 % confidence interval does not overlap with zero
F. Kong et al.
123
Author's personal copy
structural paths to vary across sexes, with the second model, which constrains the structural
paths between females and males to be equal to examine the gender differences. All the
other paths (i.e., factor loadings, error variances and structure covariances) were con-
strained to be equal. We found non-significant Chi square differences between the two
models, Dv
2
(4, N =427) =8.65, p[.05, indicating that the final model was not found
to differ by gender. We also calculated the critical ratios of differences (CRD) by dividing
the difference between two estimates by an estimate of the standard error of the difference
(Arbuckle 2003). A CRD greater than 1.96 indicates that there is a significant difference
between the two parameter estimates at p\.05. The structural path from gratitude to
social support was identified to be significantly different; CRD =1.99, p\.05. The path
coefficient for males (b=.59, p\.001) was greater than the coefficient for females
(b=.41, p\.001), indicating that males with high gratitude scores are more likely to get
greater social support than females. The structural path from social support to life satis-
faction was identified to be significantly different; CRD =-2.25, p\.05. The path
coefficient for females (b=.49, p\.001) was greater than the coefficient for females
(b=.26, p\.05), indicating that females with high social support scores are more likely
to get greater life satisfaction than males.
3.4 Major Differences
We found no statistically significant major differences in gratitude but males significantly
scored higher than females on social support and self-esteem. Multi-group analysis was
also used to identify whether the path coefficients differ significantly between students
majoring in the arts and science. We found non-significant Chi square differences between
the two models, Dv
2
(4, N =427) =4.70, p[.05. Further CRD analysis indicated that no
structural path was identified to be significantly different.
4 Discussion
The present study aimed at testing the important role of social support and self-esteem in
the association between gratitude and life satisfaction during emerging adulthood. In line
with our expectations, the specific indirect effect of gratitude on life satisfaction via social
support was significant in emerging adults. That is, emerging adults with higher levels of
gratitude had a propensity to perceive greater social support from others, which may
contribute to an increase in their life satisfaction. This is in line with the previously
reported mediating model of social support. For example, Wood et al. (2008) found social
support mediated the association between gratitude and depression in undergraduate stu-
dents. Chen et al. (2012) found that gratitude might enhance athletes’ well-being by getting
more social support from their coaches and teammates. Fredrickson (2001) ‘‘broaden and
build’’ model of positive emotions provides a framework for understanding how gratitude
might have beneficial effects on well-being. This model hypothesizes that positive emo-
tions broaden people’s momentary repertoires of cognition and behavior, build their
enduring social and personal resources, and ‘‘undo’’ the adverse physiological effects of
negative emotions.
The best model from the current study did not support the mediating effect of self-
esteem between gratitude and life satisfaction. One clue that might help to explain this
result is the significant path of gratitude?social support?self-esteem?life satisfaction.
This path indicates that grateful people are prone to perceive high levels of social support
Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction
123
Author's personal copy
from others, which may enhance their self-esteem and in turn, may result in a satisfactory
life. In other words, self-esteem is a mediator between social support and life satisfaction
rather than a mediator between gratitude and life satisfaction, and social support is a
mediator between gratitude and self-esteem. The mediating effect of social support is in
line with the previously reported findings. Numerous studies have shown that grateful
people are likely to perceive greater social support from others (e.g., Wood et al. 2008);
those who perceive more social support tend to have higher levels of self-esteem (Kong
and You 2013; Kong et al. 2012a,b). The mediating effect of self-esteem is consistent with
recently reported self-esteem as a mediator of the association between social support and
life satisfaction in Chinese university students (Kong and You 2013; Kong et al. 2012 b).
We also indicated that females were more grateful than males. This is in accordance
with the previous studies that report females had higher scores than males on tests of
gratitude (Chen 2013; Froh et al. 2009; Kashdan et al. 2009). This is probably because
males consider the experience and expression of gratitude as evidence of vulnerability and
weakness, which may threaten their masculinity and social status (Levant and Kopecky
1995). In addition, the final model did not differ by gender, indicating that males and
females have the same mechanism underlying the relationship between gratitude and life
satisfaction. However, we found males with high gratitude scores were more apt to get
more social support than females. This is consistent with the study by Froh et al. (2009).
This may be partially account for by the androgyny hypothesis. According to the
hypothesis, psychologically androgynous individuals who possess both masculine and
feminine characteristics are optimally healthy than sex-typed individuals who possess
mainly masculine or feminine characteristics (Lefkowitz and Zeldow 2006). Thus, males
might get more benefit from the experience and expression of gratitude that is viewed as a
feminine characteristic. In addition, females with high levels of social support are more
likely to perceive greater life satisfaction than males. This is consistent with the findings of
Cheng and Chan (2006). They found that the effect of social support on self-rated health
was stronger for females than for males. The gender difference may reflect cultural dif-
ferences. According to Chinese general traditional ideas, because of social role expecta-
tions and practice, men pay more attention to and spend more time on their career
development whereas women pay more attention to and spend more time on their family
and interpersonal relations.
Several important limitations of the present study must be mentioned. The first limi-
tation is that the study was a cross-sectional design such that the temporal sequence of the
independent variables, mediator, and dependent variables cannot be verified (Maxwell and
Cole 2007; Maxwell et al. 2011). Longitudinal studies should be needed in order to draw
any conclusions about the causality or directionality of the relationships between gratitude,
self-esteem, social support and life satisfaction. The second limitation is that the data relied
exclusively on self-report measures. Although the measures were selected for their good
reliability and validity, self-report measures are subjective by nature and vulnerable to bias,
e.g., social desirability. The use of multiple methods for evaluation (e.g., peer reports) may
decrease the influence of subjectivity. The third limitation is that this sample was drawn
from a university population, which limits the extent to which these findings can be
generalized within the Chinese culture.
Despite these limitations, there are several important contributions in this study. The
current study substantially extended our insight into a complicated interplay among
gratitude, social support, self-esteem and life satisfaction among Chinese university stu-
dents (McCullough et al. 2002). The findings provide external validity for the social
support mediated model in China, underscoring the key role of social support. The
F. Kong et al.
123
Author's personal copy
significant path from gratitude through social support and self-esteem to life satisfaction
shed light on the underlying mechanisms between gratitude and life satisfaction. The
present findings may reflect the cultural differences (i.e., collectivist and individualist), so
future research need use cross-cultural samples to confirm the findings. In consideration of
the probable mechanisms, these findings may provide valuable guidance for how to
implement positive psychological interventions aimed at enhancement of human well-
being. The cultivation of gratitude may work as a preventive therapy to help individuals
increase their well-being in the future. It may also function as an active therapy by helping
them gain social support from others and enhance their self-esteem. Moreover, although no
significant differences in the final model was obtained, the gender differences in some
structural paths suggest that distinct types of positive psychology interventions for grati-
tude among males or females may be considered for use in the psychological services.
References
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and rec-
ommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411–423.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2003). AMOS 5.0 update to the AMOS user’s guide. Chicago, IL: Smallwaters.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Byrne, B. (2001). Structural equation modeling with amos: Basic concepts, applications and programming.
New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chen, L. H. (2013). Gratitude and adolescent athletes’ well-being: The multiple mediating roles of perceived
social support from coaches and teammates. Social Indicators Research, 114, 273–285.
Chen, L. H., Chen, M. Y., & Tsai, Y. M. (2012). Does gratitude always work? Ambivalence over emotional
expression inhibits the beneficial effect of gratitude on well-being. International Journal of Psy-
chology, 47, 381–392.
Chen, L. H., & Kee, Y. H. (2008). Gratitude and adolescent athletes well-being. Social Indicators Research,
89, 361–373.
Cheng, S. T., & Chan, A. (2006). Social support and self-rated health revisited: Is there a gender difference
in later life? Social Science and Medicine, 63, 118–122.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and
cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403–425.
Dunn, J. C., Whelton, W. J., & Sharpe, D. (2006). Maladaptive perfectionism, hassles, coping, and psy-
chological distress in university professors. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 511–523.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental
investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 377–389.
Emmons, R. A., & Shelton, C. M. (2002). Gratitude and the science of positive psychology. In C. R. Snyder &
S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 459–471). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build
theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Gratitude, like other positive emotions, broadens and builds. In R. A. Emmons &
M. E. McCullough (Eds.), The psychology of gratitude (pp. 145–166). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Froh, J. J., Sefick, W. J., & Emmons, R. A. (2008). Counting blessings in early adolescents: An experimental
study of gratitude and subjective well-being. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 213–233.
Froh, J. J., Yurkewicz, C., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Gratitude and subjective well-being in early adoles-
cence: Examining gender differences. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 633–650.
Hermans, H. J. M. (1992). Unhappy self-esteem: A meaningful exception to the rule. Journal of Psychology,
126, 555–570.
Hoffman, M. A., Ushpiz, V., & Levy-Shiff, R. (1988). Social support and self-esteem in adolescence.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 307–316.
Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction
123
Author's personal copy
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Con-
ventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Jo
¨reskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: A guide to the program and applications. Chicago: SPSS.
Kashdan, T. B., Mishra, A., Breen, W. E., & Froh, J. J. (2009). Gender differences in gratitude: Examining
appraisals, narratives, the willingness to express emotions, and changes in psychological needs.
Journal of Personality, 77, 691–730.
Kashdan, T. B., Uswatte, G., & Julian, T. (2006). Gratitude and hedonic and eudaemonic well-being in
Vietnam war veterans. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 177–199.
Kong, F., Wang, X., & Zhao, J. (2014). Dispositional mindfulness and life satisfaction: The role of core self-
evaluations. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 165–169.
Kong, F., & You, X. (2013). Loneliness and self-esteem as mediators between social support and life
satisfaction in late adolescence. Social Indicators Research, 110, 271–279.
Kong, F., & Zhao, J. (2013). Affective mediators of the relationship between trait emotional intelligence and
life satisfaction in young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 197–201.
Kong, F., Zhao, J., & You, X. (2012a). Social support mediates the influence of emotional intelligence on
mental distress and life satisfaction in Chinese young adults. Personality and Individual Differences,
53, 513–517.
Kong, F., Zhao, J., & You, X. (2012b). Emotional intelligence and life satisfaction in Chinese university
students: The mediating role of self-esteem and social support. Personality and Individual Differences,
53, 1039–1043.
Lefkowitz, E. S., & Zeldow, P. B. (2006). Masculinity and femininity predict optimal mental health: A
belated test of the androgyny hypothesis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 95–101.
Levant, R. F., & Kopecky, G. (1995). Masculinity, reconstructed. New York: Dutton.
Li, D., Zhang, W., Li, X., Li, N., & Ye, B. (2012). Gratitude and suicidal ideation and suicide attempts
among Chinese Adolescents: Direct, mediated, and moderated effects. Journal of adolescence, 35,
55–66.
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel:
Exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary
Journal, 9, 151–173.
Mack, J. E. (1983). Self-esteem and its development: An overview. In J. E. Mack & S. L. Ablon (Eds.), The
development and sustenance of self-esteem in childhood (pp. 1–42). New York: International Uni-
versities Press.
Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psy-
chological Methods, 12, 23–44.
Maxwell, S. E., Cole, D. A., & Mitchell, M. A. (2011). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal
mediation: Partial and complete mediation under an autoregressive model. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 46, 816–841.
McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and
empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 112–127.
Mruk, C. (1995). Self-esteem: Research, theory, and practice. New York: Springer.
Park, H., Paul Heppner, P., & Lee, D. (2010). Maladaptive coping and self-esteem as mediators between
perfectionism and psychological distress. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 469–474.
Rice, K. G., Ashby, J. S., & Slaney, R. (1998). Self-esteem as a mediator between perfectionism and
depression: A structural equations analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 304–314.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
Schmitt, D. P., & Allik, J. (2005). Simultaneous administration of the Rosenberg self-esteem scale in 53
nations: exploring the universal and culture-specific features of global self-esteem. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 89, 623–642.
Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal narcissists
psychologically healthy?: Self-esteem matters. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
400–416.
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures
and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 442–445.
Strelan, P. (2007). The prosocial, adaptive qualities of just world beliefs: Implications for the relationship
between justice and forgiveness. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 881–890.
Sun, P., & Kong, F. (2013). Affective mediators of the influence of gratitude on life satisfaction in late
adolescence. Social Indicators Research,. doi:10.1007/s11205-013-0333-8.
Toussaint, L., & Friedman, P. (2009). Forgiveness, gratitude, and well-being: The mediating role of affect
and beliefs. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10, 635–654.
F. Kong et al.
123
Author's personal copy
Watkins, P. C., Woodward, K., Stone, T., & Kolts, R. L. (2003). Gratitude and happiness: Development of a
measure of gratitude, and relationships with subjective well-being. Social Behavior and Personality,
31, 431–451.
Wood, A. M., Froh, J. J., & Geraghty, A. W. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical
integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 890–905.
Wood, A. M., Joseph, S., & Maltby, J. (2009). Gratitude predicts psychological well-being above the Big
Five facets. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 443–447.
Wood, A. M., Maltby, J., Gillett, R., Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2008). The role of gratitude in the
development of social support, stress, and depression: Two longitudinal studies. Journal of Research in
Personality, 42, 854–871.
Yarcheski, A., Mahon, N. E., & Yarcheski, T. J. (2001). Social support and well-being in early adolescents:
The role of mediating variables. Clinical Nursing Research, 10, 163–181.
Zhao, J., Kong, F., & Wang, Y. (2012). Self-esteem and humor style as mediators of the effects of shyness
on loneliness among Chinese college students. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 686–690.
Zhao, J., Kong, F., & Wang, Y. (2013). The role of social support and self-esteem in the relationship
between shyness and loneliness. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 577–581.
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived
social support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30–41.
Gratitude, Self-esteem, Social Support and Life Satisfaction
123
Author's personal copy
... There is a must to enhance the comprehension of how social support beyond the workplace influences individuals' work experiences, thus necessitating an expansion of the research scope on social support 10 . For instance, social support derived from family, friends, and significant others significantly influences life satisfaction 11 . That means the implications of the concept should also be examined within the work environment. ...
... Support from family, friends, and significant others can also influence work life. These resources affect individuals' life satisfaction 11 . ...
... A study indicated that females tend to report higher amounts of social support compared to males 14 . Another study demonstrated that females (ß = 0.49, p < 0.001) with higher social support are more likely to experience higher life satisfaction compared to males (ß = 0.26, p < 0.05) 11 . Accordingly, the utilization of social support and the impact of work experiences on quality of life may vary based on gender, and these differences can affect the relationship between perceived social support and WRQoL. ...
Article
Full-text available
Work-related quality of life has emerged as a central focus in the field of occupational health due to its impact on individual well-being and overall quality of life. So, it is crucial to enhance the current theory by conducting a study on the concept across different sectors. Also, the specific mechanisms through which perceived social support influences work-related quality of life remain underexplored. The role of psychological distress as a potential mediator in this relationship has not been investigated. Thus, the study aimed to explore the mediating role of psychological distress in this relationship, while also examining potential gender differences. The study, conducted with 401 teachers through a questionnaire, utilized the AMOS program for data analysis. The findings supported the direct effects among the variables and the mediating role of psychological distress. Additionally, the impact of psychological distress on work-related quality of life was found to be higher for females, but no differences for other paths. The study contributes to the literature by addressing the gap in understanding the mechanisms regarding work-related quality of life. The results highlight the importance of considering psychological distress and gender differences in interventions aimed at improving work-related quality of life.
... A gratidão tem sido, frequentemente, definida como uma disposição que indica "uma tendência geral de reconhecer e responder com gratidão aos papéis de benevolência de outras pessoas ou eventos em experiências pessoais e resultados positivos obtidos" (McCullough et al., 2002, p. 112;tradução nossa). Muitos estudos mostram que a gratidão está positivamente relacionada à autoestima, ao apoio social e ao bem-estar subjetivo, e negativamente relacionada a sintomas depressivos (Emmons & McCullough, 2003;Wood et al., 2010;Hill et al., 2013;Kong et al., 2015). A literatura aponta para a gratidão como um fenômeno importante para o aumento do bem-estar, o que justifica a relevância de estudá-la e medi-la. ...
... Outra questão importante ao usar o instrumento GQ-6 é a invariância da medida em relação ao sexo. Por exemplo, estudos anteriores compararam as diferenças de escores no GQ-6 entre homens e mulheres Sun & Kong, 2013;Kong et al., 2015). Kashdan et al. (2009) descobriram que as mulheres adultas pontuaram mais no GQ-6 do que homens adultos Sun & Kong, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Análises de invariância permitem a comparação das propriedades psicométricas de um instrumento (e.g., cargas fatoriais, interceptos e erros de medida) entre grupos ou momentos. O presente estudo analisou a invariância entre grupos (e.g., homens e mulheres) e ao longo do tempo (intervalo de 5 meses entre as coletas) do Questionário de Gratidão. A amostra utilizada entre os grupos foi composta por 1850 participantes com idade média de 25,13 anos (DP = 5,36), sendo 50% do sexo feminino e 50% do sexo masculino. A amostra utilizada ao longo do tempo contou com 617 participantes, todas do sexo feminino, com média de idade de 25,4 anos (DP = 5,6). Comparações de modelos aninhados e hierarquicamente restritivos indicaram alta invariância (e.g., configural, métrico, escalar e residual) para o questionário, indicando robustez na medida. Ademais, este estudo apresentou e exemplificou a importância das análises de invariância para a pesquisa em Psicologia.
... Various studies have shown that expressions of gratitude can increase happiness, reduce symptoms of depression and strengthen social relationships (Armenta, Fritz & Lyubomirsky 2017;Froh, Bono & Emmons 2010;Zhu et al. 2024). In an educational context, students who are able to develop gratitude tend to have higher levels of well-being and are better able to cope with academic stress (Garg & Sarkar 2020;Kong, Ding & Zhao 2015;Sapmaz et al. 2015). Previous research has largely focussed on quantitative analyses, exploring the correlation between gratitude and psychological well-being, often neglecting the more in-depth qualitative aspects of how individuals experience and express gratitude in specific contexts (Hahn, Brzoska & Kiessling 2024;Hu et al. 2024;Zhen et al. 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the dynamics of emotional expression of freshmen students at a public university in Indonesia, focussing on the role of gratitude in helping them overcome challenges and maintain academic vigour. Using qualitative methods with open-ended questionnaires and in-depth interviews, this study involved 20 freshmen from one public university. The results showed that the majority of students expressed gratitude to God as the main expression, while a minority also valued the role of social support and personal achievement. The findings also revealed that despite uncertainties, fears and disappointments, gratitude remains a fundamental and culminating emotion. Gratitude not only helps students overcome challenges but also enhances their resilience and motivation. Spiritual Meaning Construction Theory serves as an analytical tool, highlighting that spiritual experiences and expressions of gratitude are strongly influenced by an individual’s social and cultural context. The theory emphasises that the experience of religion or spirituality has no universal truth, but rather varies according to each individual’s interpretative framework. The findings suggest that the expression of gratitude in freshmen not only reflects universally inculcated spiritual beliefs but is also a result of the meaning-making processes by individuals within their social and cultural context. Implications for higher education institutions include the importance of developing orientation, counselling and academic support programmes that support students’ expression of gratitude and resilience. Programmes that promote resilience and gratitude can help students overcome challenges and stay motivated throughout their academic journey.Contribution: This research contributes to a deeper understanding of the complexity of emotions experienced by freshmen as well as the important role gratitude plays in helping them overcome challenges and maintain enthusiasm in pursuing their academic goals.
... Additionally, scholars conducted a questionnaire survey of 427 Chinese college students to explore the relationships among social support, self-esteem, gratitude, and life satisfaction. The results indicate that there is a strong correlation between gratitude and life satisfaction, with self-esteem playing a significant mediating role between them [42]. Expressing gratitude not only helps improve individuals' subjective well-being and life satisfaction [43], but also has a positive effect on physical health [44]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The academic community is increasingly interested in understanding the mechanisms through which gratitude influences coping strategies. In addition, the role of gratitude in fostering long-term resilience and mental health outcomes has garnered significant attention. This study explores the mechanisms through which gratitude affects problem-focused coping strategies and emotion-focused coping strategies by constructing models involving gratitude, perceived social support, self-esteem, and problem-focused coping strategies, as well as models involving gratitude, perceived social support, self-esteem, and emotion-focused coping strategies. Methods A longitudinal survey was conducted on 1666 Chinese university students using highly reliable and valid scales, including the Gratitude Scale, Perceived Social Support Scale, Self-Esteem Scale, and Brief Coping Strategies Scale. To examine whether perceived social support and self-esteem play a significant indirect role in the relationship between gratitude and problem-focused coping strategies, as well as between gratitude and emotion-focused coping strategies. Differences in variables based on demographic variables: We used one-way ANOVA to test the differences in gratitude, perceived social support, self-esteem, problem-focused coping strategies, and emotion-focused coping strategies among students of different grades and ages. Additionally, independent samples t-tests were used to examine the differences between students of different genders and household registrations. Results The study found that (1) Gratitude significantly positively predicted perceived social support (β = 0.661, p < 0.001), self-esteem (β = 0.234, p < 0.001), and problem-focused coping strategies (β = 0.130, p < 0.001); (2) Perceived social support significantly positively predicted self-esteem (β = 0.440, p < 0.001; β = 0.439, p < 0.001), problem-focused coping strategies (β = 0.443, p < 0.001), and emotion-focused coping strategies (β = 0.279, p < 0.001); (3) Self-esteem significantly positively predicted problem-focused coping strategies (β = 0.172, p < 0.001) and significantly negatively predicted emotion-focused coping strategies (β = −0.205, p < 0.001); (4) Gratitude can influence problem-focused coping strategies through the dual indirect effect of two mediating variables. After the inclusion of the mediating variables, the effect of problem-focused coping strategies in the indirect model was further strengthened. (5) Gratitude can influence emotion-focused coping strategies through a completely indirect effect on perceived social support and self-esteem. After inserting the mediating variables, the effect of emotion-focused coping strategies in the mediating model is enhanced. Conclusion Gratitude can directly and positively predict problem-focused coping strategies, and it can also positively predict problem-focused coping strategies through the dual indirect effect of two mediating variables. Gratitude does not significantly predict emotion-focused coping strategies directly, but it can influence emotion-focused coping strategies via a double indirect pathway.
... This implies that when parents receive this positive emotion from their children, they feel a stronger emotional connection with them, making them more willing to trust, support, and respect their children's ideas, thereby providing greater autonomy support. Some indirect evidence has also found that gratitude is associated with positive aspects of relationships, such as increased perceptions of support (Kong et al., 2015;Lin, 2014). Additionally, experiences of gratitude may also increase social support (Deichert et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Abundant evidence highlights the benefits of self-determined motivation (e.g., parental autonomy support as internalized extrinsic motivation, gratitude as intrinsic motivation) on academic engagement during adolescence, yet the potential mutual relations remain relatively unexplored. This study investigated the bidirectional relations and potential mechanisms among parental autonomy support, gratitude, and academic engagement using a traditional cross-lag-panel model (CLPM) and a within-person CLPM with random intercept (RI-CLPM) in a sample of Chinese youth (N = 1214; Mage = 15.46, SDage = 0.71; 39.30% girls) across three time points with 6-month intervals. The results indicated that the bidirectional relation between parental autonomy support and academic engagement was present in CLPM but not in RI-CLPM. However, the bidirectional relations between gratitude and academic engagement, and between gratitude and parental autonomy support, existed at both levels. Furthermore, in CLPM, parental autonomy support influenced academic engagement through gratitude, and gratitude, in turn, affected academic engagement through parental autonomy support. Academic engagement impacted gratitude via parental autonomy support, and simultaneously, academic engagement influenced parental autonomy support through gratitude. Academic engagement served as a mediator between parental autonomy support and gratitude, as well as between gratitude and parental autonomy support. Five self-enhancing loops were identified in CLPM. These findings reveal a virtuous cycle of mutual influence between parental autonomy support, gratitude, and adolescent academic engagement, highlighting the important role of academic engagement in strengthening autonomous motivation.
... Social support serves as an important predictor directly linked to life satisfaction and subjective wellbeing (Kong, Ding, and Zhao 2015;Kong and You 2013;Oh, Ozkaya, and LaRose 2014). For instance, Jun, Galambos, and Lee (2021) examined the impact of smartphone use on the psychological health of Korean immigrant elders, confirming a positive correlation between smartphone use and social support from family and friends, as well as life satisfaction. ...
Article
Full-text available
A fervent debate exists among researchers and social critics about whether smartphone use strengthens or undermines community participation and social relationships. This study investigates the dynamics between smartphone use and psychological well-being among university students over a one-year period, employing a two-wave longitudinal analysis to examine this relationship across various mobile applications. Our findings reveal that the direction and magnitude of the interplay depend on the specific purposes of smartphone engagement. Specifically, there is a bidirectional relationship between smartphone use (either overall use or specific mobile application use) and psychological well-being: higher levels of perceived social support and loneliness reduce subsequent smartphone use, while increased smartphone use boosts perceived social support and heightens loneliness. The impact of life satisfaction on different mobile applications varies: life satisfaction negatively affects the subsequent use of social media, mobile gaming, and mobile health applications, while its effects on mobile shopping and overall smartphone use are insignificant. Additionally, increased overall smartphone use decreases life satisfaction, whereas increased use of mobile shopping and mobile health applications significantly improves life satisfaction, but the others—social media and mobile gaming—do not. These findings hold noteworthy implications for research and practices.
... 14 PSS is linked to a decrease in hazardous conduct, 15 increased healthful behaviors, 16 physical health, 17 and life satisfaction. 18 NSS encompasses various aspects of social relationships that can undermine mental health, linking it to poor well-being and psychological distress. Key NSS domains include rejection or neglect, unsympathetic behavior, lack of help, excessive criticism, over-involvement, and unreliability, all of which contribute to feelings of isolation, inadequacy, frustration, and stress. ...
... Informal support begins to take over the function of family support as individuals transition to adolescence (Lee & Goldstein, 2016). In the study of Kong et al. (2015), it was shown that high social support is associated with more life satisfaction, which in turn supports the Theory of Cohen and Wills stating that having high social support will help buffer against emotional distress. The findings contradict the study conducted by Wu and Tu (2019), which concluded that social support acts as a buffer, enabling students to manage setbacks more effectively and reducing the likelihood of learned helplessness. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Teenage is a critical period of development, marked by rapid cognitive changes and transitions. Environmental factors play a significant role during this stage, providing support and facilitating socialization, which influences teenagers’ attitudes, standards, and behaviors. However, societal pressure, like high expectations, is often unavoidable and can result in emotional stress. This stress may lead teenagers to struggle with minor challenges, potentially escalating into cognitive distortions like catastrophizing. On the other hand, Gautam et al. (2024) stated that access to adequate support and resources often varies with socioeconomic status (SES) significantly influencing opportunities and mental health outcomes. The study utilized the Catastrophizing Questionnaire and the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) with a sample of one hundred (165) teenage participants. Results indicated that the majority of participants were female, and most participants were from low socioeconomic status. Most participants reported high levels of both catastrophizing (52.73%) and perceived social support (49.09%). The study revealed that there is a weak and insignificant correlation between catastrophizing and perceived social support. Regardless of the perceived social support of a teenager, catastrophizing may occur. It is also revealed that there was no significant difference in catastrophizing between male and female participants, although females showed higher levels of rumination. Also, socioeconomic status did not significantly impact levels of catastrophizing, indicating that catastrophizing does not vary across socioeconomic backgrounds of a teenager. In conclusion, the study highlights that catastrophizing can be experienced by individuals regardless of sex and socioeconomic background. These findings emphasize the need for interventions to help teenagers manage stress and avoid maladaptive coping mechanisms, promoting better mental health outcomes
Article
Full-text available
In four studies, the authors examined the correlates of the disposition toward gratitude. Study 1 revealed that self-ratings and observer ratings of the grateful disposition are associated with positive affect and well-being prosocial behaviors and traits, and religiousness/spirituality. Study 2 replicated these findings in a large nonstudent sample. Study 3 yielded similar results to Studies 1 and 2 and provided evidence that gratitude is negatively associated with envy and materialistic attitudes. Study 4 yielded evidence that these associations persist after controlling for Extraversion/positive affectivity, Neuroticism/negative affectivity, and Agreeableness. The development of the Gratitude Questionnaire, a unidimensional measure with good psychometric properties, is also described.
Chapter
Psychology after World War II became a science largely devoted to healing. It concentrated on repairing damage using a disease model of human functioning. This almost exclusive attention to pathology neglected the idea of a fulfilled individual and a thriving community, and it neglected the possibility that building strength is the most potent weapon in the arsenal of therapy. The aim of positive psychology is to catalyze a change in psychology from a preoccupation only with repairing the worst things in life to also building the best qualities in life. To redress the previous imbalance, we must bring the building of strength to the forefront in the treatment and prevention of mental illness.
Article
The effect of a grateful outlook on psychological and physical well-being was examined. In Studies 1 and 2, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 experimental conditions (hassles, gratitude listing, and either neutral life events or social comparison); they then kept weekly (Study 1) or daily (Study 2) records of their moods, coping behaviors, health behaviors, physical symptoms, and overall life appraisals. In a 3rd study, persons with neuromuscular disease were randomly assigned to either the gratitude condition or to a control condition. The gratitude-outlook groups exhibited heightened well-being across several, though not all, of the outcome measures across the 3 studies, relative to the comparison groups. The effect on positive affect appeared to be the most robust finding. Results suggest that a conscious focus on blessings may have emotional and interpersonal benefits.
Article
In this article, we provide guidance for substantive researchers on the use of structural equation modeling in practice for theory testing and development. We present a comprehensive, two-step modeling approach that employs a series of nested models and sequential chi-square difference tests. We discuss the comparative advantages of this approach over a one-step approach. Considerations in specification, assessment of fit, and respecification of measurement models using confirmatory factor analysis are reviewed. As background to the two-step approach, the distinction between exploratory and confirmatory analysis, the distinction between complementary approaches for theory testing versus predictive application, and some developments in estimation methods also are discussed.
Chapter
This chapter examines the feeling of being grateful. It suggests feeling grateful is similar to other positive emotions that help build a person's enduring personal resources and broaden an individual's thinking. It describes various ways by which gratitude can transform individuals, organizations, and communities in positive and sustaining ways. It discusses the specific benefits of gratitude including personal and social development, community strength and individual health and well-being.
Article
This article reports the development and validation of a scale to measure global life satisfaction, the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS). Among the various components of subjective well-being, the SWLS is narrowly focused to assess global life satisfaction and does not tap related constructs such as positive affect or loneliness. The SWLS is shown to have favorable psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and high temporal reliability. Scores on the SWLS correlate moderately to highly with other measures of subjective well-being, and correlate predictably with specific personality characteristics. It is noted that the SWLS is suited for use with different age groups, and other potential uses of the scale are discussed.