ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Mindfulness is defined as sustained, non-judgmental attention towards the world and the self. Several recent studies are bringing this concept into cutting-edge research and creating important contributions for the clinical context. In spite of this fact, there is a promising but unmapped field of research around specific contributions of mindfulness to music, and more specifically, for musicians. This article reviews this topic in order to show the state of the art of mindfulness related to music and help further advancement. A total of 27 publications were reviewed. The main conclusion is that despite its youth, this field is yielding some promising results in several dimensions: (1) effectiveness in mindfulness-based intervention programs for professional and hobby performers, (2) attention and concentration boost because of mindfulness induction in music audience, (3) effective induction of mindfulness states by music, and (4) promising contributions of mindfulness and music therapies. Finally, some guidelines and future lines are suggested for expanding and improving results, models, and methods. Keywords:
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 27
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology (IJBRP)
ISSN 2332-3000
Mind fulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field
Research Article
Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz1, Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal2*
1 Clinical Psychology Student, Faculty of Psychology, Autonoma University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain.
2 Associate Professor, Faculty of Psychology, Department of Biological and Health Psychology, Division of Personality, Evaluation and
Treatment, Autonoma University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
*Corresponding Author:
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal,
Faculty of Psychology, Department of Biological and Health Psychol-
ogy, Division of Personality, Evaluation and Treatment, Autonomous
University of Madrid, Madrid, 28049, Spain.
Tel: +34 91 497 5122; Fax: +34 91 497 6409
E-mail: raquel.rodriguez@uam.es
Received: March 22, 2014
Accepted: April 11, 2014
Published: April 12, 2014
Citation: Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014)
Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field . Int
J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.19070/2332-
3000-140006
Copyright: Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal © 2014 This is an open-access
article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in
any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Introduction
Many musicians could describe performing as a touching, intense,
and high concentrated experience. The performer’s mind is get-
ting through the music piece with an intense attention and con-
centration to every aspect to assure the best outcome. Also, the
performer’s mind is aware of the audience remaining silent and
listening to the performance, activating the stress-response for a
social stimulus. As many musicians report, this intense situation
of perceived scrutiny may lead to a stressful response and threat-
ing experience [1]. In this situation, many musicians develop the
so-called stage fright, or music performance anxiety (MPA)[2], a
disrupting group of symptoms that could stop or difcult a per-
former’s career. On the opposite side, many performers describe
this experience as full, not painful, joyful and emotional, hence
meeting ow experiences [3]. At the same time, the audience is
putting all their senses into the performance, listening and feeling
what the performer has to express with intense attention. This
experience of intense present-orientation and focus is compared
in scientic literature with “peak experiences”, or ow [4].
Could this intense, emotional, demanding and present-centered
peak experiences have something in common with mindfulness?
This review focuses on these topics. For that purpose, several
sources and scientic databases were consulted. The rst conclu-
sion was that although promising contributions, there was not a
general overview or “map” to get oriented in this topic literature.
Hence, more awareness of contributions and systematization of
knowledge seem necessary. This paper tries to help in satisfying
this necessity.
Mindfulness
Mindfulness is usually dened as “paying attention in a particular
way: On purpose, in the present moment, and non-judgmentally”
[5]. Based on this denition, empirical research on mindfulness
began importing Eastern Buddhist practices, such as meditation,
into Western culture. On this process some modications and
conversions were performed, mainly depriving them of religious
or specic moral backgrounds [6]. This western translation has
encountered its main development within Health Sciences, es-
pecially Psychology and Behavioral Medicine, originating several
contributions at diverse levels.
Several intervention programs have been designed and tested.
Perhaps, one of the most widespread is the Mindfulness Based
Stress Reduction (MBSR), developed by Jon Kabat-Zinn and col-
leagues. It focuses on using mindfulness to damper stress, reduc-
ing suffering and increasing psychological well-being. Numerous
programs were implemented in several populations like pupils
with chronic pain, cancer, arthritis, or bromyalgia [1, 3]. Another
example of intervention program is the Mindfulness Based Cog-
nitive Therapy (MBCT). It focuses on using both mindfulness
Abstract
Mindfulness is dened as sustained, non-judgmental attention towards the world and the self. Several recent studies are bringing this
concept into cutting-edge research and creating important contributions for the clinical context. In spite of this fact, there is a promising
but unmapped eld of research around specic contributions of mindfulness to music, and more specically, for musicians. This article
reviews this topic in order to show the state of the art of mindfulness related to music and help further advancement. A total of 27 pub-
lications were reviewed. The main conclusion is that despite its youth, this eld is yielding some promising results in several dimensions:
(1) effectiveness in mindfulness-based intervention programs for professional and hobby performers, (2) attention and concentration
boost because of mindfulness induction in music audience, (3) effective induction of mindfulness states by music, and (4) promising
contributions of mindfulness and music therapies. Finally, some guidelines and future lines are suggested for expanding and improving
results, models, and methods.
Keywords: Mindfulness; Meditation; Music; Performers; Audience; Musicians; Music Performance Anxiety.
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 28
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
and cognitive behavioral therapy to treat different syndromes like
depression [8], generalized anxiety disorder [9], generalized social
anxiety disorder [10], social anxiety disorder [11-13], bipolar dis-
order [14], panic disorder [15], or hypochondriasis [16]. Evidence
has been found even in heterogeneous psychiatric samples [17,
18]. Mindfulness has also been associated with greater physical
health, in terms of less somatization, and an increased awareness
and attention to somatic experience (e.g., increased skin clearing
in psoriasis disease), as well as decreased ambulatory measures,
less blood overpressure, and a better immune system response
to inuenza or other hazards [19]. In spite of being vanguard re-
search, it can be assumed that mindfulness is an effective tool for
many clinical populations, either within mental health or medical
contexts (e.g., tinnitus [20]).
In other contexts, mindfulness practice has been found to en-
hance spirituality, and as a couple, both factors (mindfulness and
spirituality) have inuence in decreasing reported psychological
and medical symptoms [21]. Also, mindfulness seems to contrib-
ute to self-care to therapists in training [22].
Mindfulness abroad
Mindfulness has also been associated with psychological health
and well-being in general population. Specically, research relates
dispositional mindfulness (as a general disposition in the indi-
vidual) with increases in psychological well-being [23], as well as
decreased neuroticism and psychopathological indexes [24]. More
specically, it seems that dispositional mindfulness is positively as-
sociated with self-esteem, optimism, positive emotional states, life
satisfaction and eudemonic well-being, as well as it is moderately
associated with emotional intelligence. In contrast, it is inversely
associated with social anxiety, rumination, anxiety, depression,
negative emotional states, and neuroticism. In a more neuro-
logical context, several contributions have been done to account
the effects of mindfulness in the brain and the central nervous
system. Some results indicate functional modications in several
brain networks, but also, and surprisingly, structural modications
of brain areas because of the effect of mindfulness [27-29].
It also seems that dispositional mindfulness is an effective predic-
tor of diary self-regulation and well-being, both in clinical and
general context. This is supported by self-report and also by lab
evidence, particularly in extent to the inuence of mindfulness
and self-concordance between implicit and explicit processes [21].
Also, it seems to be enhanced by practice since signicant dif-
ferences were found between practitioners and non-practitioners
[21]. Expanding this idea, it was specically found that there was
less dissociation, alexithymia, rumination, general psychological
distress and mood disturbance [14, 15].
Beyond those basic frameworks, some contributions were made
taking into account mindfulness and other aspects of well-being.
In a more social context, dispositional mindfulness has been as-
sociated with marital relationship health and, in a complementary
way, situational mindfulness was related with better communica-
tion during a discussion [31]. Another example is in mindfulness
and intrinsic value orientation, were signicant associates to both
subjective well-being (e.g. life satisfaction) and ecological well-
being (e.g. respect to nature), which leads to a conclusion towards
compatibility of both ways of personal and collective well-being
[32]. From an economical and nancial perspective, mindfulness
seems to play a role into diminishing nancial desire discrepancy
(distance between real and desired nancial state) and increas-
ing, therefore, subjective well-being [33]. In other words, “having
enough with what one has”. Also, mindfulness seems to enhance
moral decision making in a cheating context [34]
In summary, mindfulness appears to be an expanding topic of
research with non-frequent but strong contributions of its thera-
peutic effectiveness for many clinical populations, and also of its
effects in enhancing psychological health and self-regulation.
Music Performance Anxiety
Although there is a lot of evidence on mindfulness and an in-
crease of positive variables and diminishment of negative ones,
we question: Could musicians be a population sensitive for be-
ing beneted from mindfulness? Do musicians suffer from sig-
nicant psychology-related problems susceptible to be solved
through a mindfulness intervention? Would it be even possible
to increase musicians’ performance with a mindfulness training?
It seems that these questions could have an afrmative answer.
The music profession appears to be a competitive, demanding
and high-task occupational eld, with great personal demands on
performers [42, 43]. The career begins with an extended, time
consuming training and continues with a demanding context of
social evaluation. This process might induce performance anxi-
ety, and utterly stage fright [1]. As Kenny and Steptoe defend [1,
35], performance anxiety is a common phenomenon in several ac-
tivities such as test-taking, public speaking, sports, and perform-
ing arts. Thus, it can occur in a continuum from normal healthy
anxiety and stress, to the problematic, debilitating and disrupt-
ing symptoms of stage fright. This phenomenon seems to be the
most frequent non-musculoskeletal medical problem among mu-
sicians [1].
Stage fright is dened as a subtype of social phobia [37], and
therefore considered a clinical disorder in the area of mental
health. This concept has a more broad basic process, namely “mu-
sic performance anxiety” (MPA), and dened by Steptoe [35]. It
appears to present a deviation towards women [1], with the largest
proportion of prevalence of music performance anxiety (MPA).
Also it seems to be an association with trait anxiety and MPA,
while not with occupational stress [2]. Cognitive models of social
phobia make a valid account for the cognitive processes of MPA,
mainly focused on social evaluation of the audience: audience
view of the performance, audience standards, discrepancy be-
tween audience view and standards, perceived likelihood of nega-
tive evaluation, and importance of consequences of that nega-
tive evaluation. Those types of cognitive schemes (core beliefs
and assumptions), associated with diathesis factors (critical past
aversive events, low self-esteem or self-efcacy, etc.) may induce
to maladaptive cognitions and survival rules (e.g. “I can’t make
mistakes”) producing, therefore, MPA [38]. Treatments for MPA
and stage fright were imported from, mainly, cognitive-behavioral
therapy (CBT) and combined treatments with CBT and pharma-
cologic therapy. There has been some considerable contributions
with modest efcacy, but lack of strong methodologies and larger
scales of study bring need of rigorous, evidence-based, scientic
contributions to asses MPA with the best efcacy [41].
Up to the knowledge of the authors, there are no systematic re-
views analyzing if mindfulness and music performing (e.g. MPA)
or listening are related or can interact. Neither there is any study
reporting a methodologically strong efcacy of a mindfulness-
based intervention program with MPA. To this point, some clues
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 29
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
we follow could justify this possible interactions between mind-
fulness and music: (1) MBSR seems to be effective treating social
anxiety disorder [10-13], showing a positive and good inuence
compared with cognitive-behavioral therapy, the actual treatment
of choice. (2) Also, trait mindfulness seems to modulate neuroen-
docrine response to social stressors [40], concretely with cortisol.
Consequently, it can be inferred that mindfulness could be related
to music performance anxiety, due to its relation to the broad
branch of social anxiety. (3) In addition, there are peer-reviewed
studies reporting efcacy of meditation-based intervention pro-
grams (in this case, Zen meditation) with specically MPA [48-
49]. (4) Regarding music listening, mindfulness seems to interact
with music audience [43]. (5) In a music therapy context, mindful-
ness seems to play an active role [44]. From all of this, this review
article focuses on the broad topic of the possible interactions of
mindfulness and music as a stimulus (e.g. to the audience) or as a
response (e.g. to musicians).
Materials and Methods
Computer and manual searches were conducted to nd research
papers regarding empirical and theoretical ndings of mindful-
ness specically applied to the eld of music, either of three pos-
sibilities: (1) as a stimulus (e.g. effects on the audience), (2) as a
response (e.g. effects on performers or in composers), or (3) as a
complement to mindfulness (e.g. mindful listening). The search
was conducted for publications from the past ten years (2003-
2013). The combinations of keywords were introduced into three
databases: PsychINFO, PubMed, and Google Scholar. The key-
words were mindfulness, mindfulness meditation, music, musical,
musicians, music performers, music performing, music perfor-
mance anxiety, music listening, audience, and ow. Afterwards,
peer-reviewed articles, theses, dissertations, and books were cho-
sen as a primary selection. To nd older relevant publications,
a retrospective secondary search was conducted based on refer-
ences of the retrieved high impact scientic journals, selecting
the most cited ones and related publications within those primary
Figure 1: Flowchart of inclusion/exclusion procedures for reviewed articles.
Table 1: Types and number of publications found
Type of publication Number of publications
Peer-reviewed articles 15
Book Sections 1
Theses and Dissertations 8
Conference Proceedings 3
TOTAL 27 publications
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 30
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
selected. A owchart of the inclusion procedure is displayed in
Figure 1.
Results And Discussions
The results of the review were organized as four main parts in the
following lines. First of all, in Section 3.1, we focus on music as a
response and on to what extent mindfulness and meditation inu-
ence that response in music performance. In this section the main
interaction investigated is the benets of meditation in MPA,
while some attention is devoted to musical performance quality.
Afterwards, in Section 3.2, we describe the effects of music as a
stimulus and its possible interactions with mindfulness medita-
tion. In this section, some interactions are reported, specically
the inuence of mindfulness within the audience, and the inu-
ence of mindfulness and music to cope with other issues (e.g.
depression). Then, in Section 3.3, we focus on music as a therapy
and on to what extent mindfulness can inuence that therapeu-
tic process. Lastly, Section 3.4, provides a selection of webpages
about mindfulness and music in order to show the popular impact
of mindfulness in music.
As an overview of the methodology used in the studies found
and hereinafter reviewed, Table 2 shows the main methodological
designs. To explore a deeper methodological assessment of quan-
titative studies, we have separated those studies into two tables:
Table 3 reports intervention studies with their methodological
considerations, and Table 4 reports experimental and cross-sec-
Table 2: Main methodological scope
Designs Quantitative Qualitative Theoretical
Studies
[43] Diaz (2010, 2011) X
[45] Bakker (2005) X
[46] Langer et al (2009) X X
[47] Farnsworth-Grodd (2012) X
[42] Cope et al (2009) X
[48] Chang et al (2003) X
[49] Khalsa et al (2013) X
[41] Lin et al (2008) X
[50] Monti et al (2006) X
[51] Stern et al (2012) X
[52] Robarts (2009) X
[53] Wolfe (2009) X
[54] Steyn (2013) X
[55-58] Vidyarthi et al (2012a, 2012b, 2013, in press) X
[59] Coholic (2011) X
[60] Fidelibus (2004) X
[61] Brown (2011) X
[44] Eckhardt, Dinsmore (2012) X
[62] De Felice (2004) X
[63] Oyan (2006) X
[64]Xu (2010) X
Table 3: Intervention studies with their methodological designs
Designs Control Group Randomized Follow-Up Blind / Double blind Others (case studies)
Studies
[42] Cope et al
(2009)
X X X
[48] Chang et al
(2003)
X
[49] Khalsa et al
(2013)
X
[41] Lin et al (2008) X X
[50] Monti (2006) X X
[51] Stern (2012) X
[52] Robarts (2009) X
[54] Steyn (2013) X X
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 31
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
tional studies and their methodological considerations.
As Table 2 shows, the majority of the studies reviewed adopt a
quantitative methodological scope, with few mainly qualitative
scopes or theoretical essays. The only exception is [46], with a
mixture of quantitative-qualitative scopes.
As Table 3 shows, the majority of intervention studies have
control group, although only three of them have a randomized
group assignment and only two have follow-up measures. Only
one study [42] accomplishes those three methodological condi-
tions. None of the studies reviewed adopts blind or double blind
designs. Finally, Table 4 shows only one experimental study and
three cross-sectional studies. Although the cross-sectional studies
have strong methodological analyses (modelling, regression mod-
elling, structural equation modelling, etc.) this evidence suggest
lack of empirical, lab-based studies, thus empirical and theoretical
need for well-driven studies.
Mindfulness and Music Performing
The effects of meditation on MPA were rst reported in 2003,
showing a signicant decreased of trait anxiety in music perform-
ers[48] as the mainly outcome, but no effects were found in music
performance concentration or state anxiety. From this, point, a
rst wave of studies reported promising results to meditation and
MPA. This rst wave of studies had an exploratory nature of the
possible positive effects of mindfulness in MPA. Although one
had an empirical scope [48], the most prevalent body of work was
theoretical.
In 2008, through Zen meditation, evidence suggested consistency
with the damping effect of meditation and MPA at a dispositional
level, but not within a situational level. In this study, Music Perfor-
mance Quality (MPQ) was negatively associated with MPA in the
control group, but was positively associated in the experimental
group. In addition, these associations were stronger when scoring
post-performance than in general punctuations [41,64]. Authors
concluded, rst of all, that meditation could be useful to damper
general MPA and trait anxiety. Secondly, Zen meditation might
work as a resource to change a possible hindrance like MPA to a
challenge in order to use MPA as a means to enrich their inter-
pretation. That is to say, MPA was not inhibited in interpretations
but maintained, and meditation skills empowered performers to
foster their interpretation and increase their quality. Subsequently,
we could hypothesize that mindfulness training could increase
MPQ. One study might support this assumption [46], report-
ing the same effect and assessed by performers and audience. In
this study the authors claimed that having musicians mindfully
inserted in a performance leads them to insert subtle, spontane-
ous modications and enjoy the performance. Consequently, this
effect was reected by the audience, making them enjoying the
performing even more. These two afrmations were evaluated
with a self-report methodology: (1) Performers were instructed
to perform the same piece like “the nest performance of this
piece you can remember” (control/xed goal condition) and like
“the nest way you can, offering subtle new nuances to your per-
formance” (experimental condition). To ensure a mindfulness
induction, a qualitative questionnaire for state mindfulness was
developed and applied with positive results. (2) Moreover, mem-
bers of a chorus were assembled and listened to both recordings.
Then, they were asked if they noted a difference and if they had
a preference to one of the recordings. Effects were consistent in
replication, and controlled for a practice bias (performers) and for
an order bias (audience).
This new understanding led to other studies to explore this regu-
latory effect, suggesting general emotion regulation through yoga
intervention [49, 51] and mindfulness meditation [54]. In this last
study, exploring the regulating function of mindfulness in MPA,
Farnsworth-Grodd developed a theoretical model, stating medi-
ating mechanisms in mindfulness and MPA. As it could be hy-
pothesized, acting with awareness was the greatest predictor of
lower MPA. Also, this variable, essential in mindfulness, received
other inuences in dispositional (e.g. studying or practicing music)
and situational (e.g. concerts and shows) contexts. Such predictors
were: higher hope, lower avoidance, and increased goal-orientated
strategy in a dispositional level (practicing music); and positive
focus, self-kindness, and self-acceptance in a situational level (dur-
ing a performance). It could be possible to link this self-regulatory
and positive effect with some exploratory evidence: from (1) an
increased psychological well-being and mental performance in
music students after a mindfulness intervention [54], to (2) an
increased ow response linked to mindfulness states while per-
forming [61].
In another trend, some research was performed with yoga in-
terventions analyzing its effects in Performance-Related Mus-
culoskeletal Disorders (PRMD), besides MPA. While one study
reports no changes in PRMDs with yoga interventions [42], an-
other study reveals inconsistent results [49]. The authors of the
last study report a signicant effect in one sample compared to
the control group while non-signicant in another. Some possi-
ble reasons for this inconsistency were explained by the authors:
(1) Low baseline scores in PRMDs, revealing a healthy musicians
sample and a possible oor effect, and (2) not long enough dura-
tion of the program to produce signicant stable effects [49]. A
general conclusion is an uncertain issue that needs to be explored
Table 4: Experimental and cross-sectional studies with their methodological designs
Designs Experimental
(lab-based)
Cross-Sectional
(self-report-based)
Studies
[43] Diaz (2010, 2011) X
[53] Wolfe (2009) X
[45] Bakker (2005) X
[46] Langer (2009) X
[47] Farnsworth-Grodd
(2012)
X
[54] Steyn (2013) X
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 32
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
and explained, hence a deeper study is necessary.
Mindfulness and Music Listening
Attending to music as a stimulus, only a few contributions have
been found. The most directly related contribution is a research
project by Frank Diaz [4, 43], analyzing effects of mindfulness
and ow in the audience. Using Continuous Response Digital
Format (CRDI) and self-reports, music pieces were exposed to
a selected sample audience. Participants were randomly assigned
into three groups: (1) mindfulness induction and aesthetic re-
sponse, (2) mindfulness induction and ow response, (3) aes-
thetic response only, and (4) ow response only. The procedure
to induce mindfulness was a 15 minute recorded guided induc-
tion, centered in a body scan meditation procedure. Results of
this work indicate no signicant differences in attention, which
were based on summative accounts. However, self-reports and
verbal responses revealed differences between groups, highlight-
ing benets of mindfulness induction in the attention task leading
to a decreased mental distraction, increased awareness of music
characteristics, and an enhanced focus. In addition, it seems that
magnitude response was also behaving like attention or attentive-
ness. Summative accounts report no signicant differences in par-
ticipants’ level of magnitude, but real time data analysis brings up
differences regarding mindfulness. It seems that mindfulness has
a differential effect on ow and aesthetic responses. As regards
to ow, magnitude tends to have an overall decrease, whereas for
aesthetic response, an overall increase. This differential inuence
could be interpreted as mindfulness having an empowering role
for the participants’ goal: if they want to concentrate and not be
distracted, mindfulness helps decreasing affective response; and
if they want to enjoy and get immersed in the most powerful way,
mindfulness can boost their peak affective experiences, and there-
fore, increase response magnitude (intensity). And both effects
might be explained by the increase of awareness, the difference
lays on the goal of the subject.
In a different way, a research project proposed music listening (or
even sound listening) as a feedback source to the subject in order
to cultivate mindfulness, namely “The Sonic Cradle Project”. It is
based on a device that is dened as “a chamber of complete dark-
ness where users shape a peaceful soundscape using only their
respiration” [55]. The subject lies in a suspended hammock hang-
ing from the ceiling of the chamber and surrounded by darkness
(to avoid visual or interoceptive distractions). Then, the real-time
measurements of the subject’s breathing modulates the musical
stimulus (type of instrument, pitch, rhythm, etc.), thus closing the
feedback loop [56]. Through this biofeedback device, the authors
pretend to foster meditation and mindfulness skills to the users
of the device and to divulgate and facilitate mindfulness mindsets
to non-practitioners [57]. Evidence suggests that its effects are
really similar to mindfulness meditation [57, 58] reporting loss of
intention, relaxation, positive emotional responses, reduced think-
ing (“emptiness”) and self-awareness. Meditators compared the
experience highly with ordinary meditations [58].
In this context, a theoretical framework is established based on
the salutary effects of music listening, but also proposing atten-
tional and metacognitive mechanisms (and thus, mindfulness)
to cause these positive effects. Moreover, experimental evidence
shows an attentional improvement (one key factor of mindful-
ness) because of musical content in a vigilance task with auditory
distractors in kindergarten children [53].
Thus, it seems that music listening can induce mindfulness states,
with consequential benets, e.g. improved attention or improved
peak response intensity.
Mindfulness and Music Therapy
Some studies explored the conjunction of mindfulness and music
as an intervention for applied contexts (e.g. clinical or education-
al). Some explorations have been carried out with interesting con-
clusions. For instance, Coholic [59] proposes Arts-Based Mind-
fulness-Based Practices for young children in need. This program
pretends to train mindfulness skills with art practices, seeking
clinical goals, such as emotion regulation, social and coping skills,
self-esteem, resilience, and self-awareness. These ndings seem
to be similar in another studies [52]. The mindfulness effects on
music therapists when performing clinical music therapy improvi-
sation has also been studied, showing a qualitative prevalence of
mindfulness states in the music therapists [65-67].
In another clinical context, Monti et al [50] proposed a Mindful-
ness-Based Art Therapy (MBAT) for women with cancer. This
program, hybridized between the MBSR protocol and art thera-
pies, focuses on training mindfulness skills using art practices. The
MBAT has shown efcacy in reducing psychological distress and
enhancing quality of life. One particularity of the MBAT could
be its own name, because attending to its main curriculum the
great majority of art-related exercises are of plastic art (e.g. paint-
ing) practices. Nevertheless, the nal exercises are open to any
form of art, including music, and the evidence exposed underpins
promising results when in conjunction applying mindfulness and
music for clinical or educational purposes.
In a parallel way as in music performing, music listening and
mindfulness could be studied in conjunction for applied contexts.
One study explored this possibility [44]. Using their appointed
Mindful Music Listening (MML), the authors applied music lis-
tening practices combined with mindfulness training to patients
with depression. Results show an enhanced emotion regulation,
greater self-awareness and a strengthened therapeutic relation-
ship. Thus, listening to music with a mindful disposition leads
to positive outcomes (e.g. enhanced emotion regulation or self-
reference).
In conclusion, it seems that mindfulness can contribute to musi-
cal therapy programs to enhance their efcacy. And, to achieve
this goal, music can be employed by both performing or listening
dimensions.
Conclusions and guideslines for future research
The present review encourages research in mindfulness and mu-
sic towards a better understanding of its object of study. With a
better mapping of the state of the art, and a condensed view of
the latest results, researchers can get their job done quicker. This
seems to be a little explored eld (according to results in Table 1),
but shows promising results and thus seemingly offering poten-
tial. In general it seems that mindfulness can be characterized by
an important interaction with the art of music in many contexts,
thus deserving research and exploratory applications: (1) mindful-
ness could improve MPQ by mediating the positive relationship
between MPA and MPQ, thus turning MPA into performance-
facilitating, apart from enhancing psychological well-being in
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 33
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
musicians; (2) mindfulness could enhance music listening, raising
the intensity of peak responses; (3) music listening could con-
tribute to mindfulness induction (4) music performing could be
for therapeutic use in relation with mindfulness to foster clinical
treatments.
Based on these interactions, a possible map of the existing con-
tributions is displayed in Fig. 2, showing each possible interaction
between mindfulness and music. Bold connections are referred to
actually researched interactions, whereas dotted connections are
allocated for general coherence of the model.
As it can be seen in the present review, a substantial part of the
domains of music has been explored, but with parts still in an
unexplored state and with much progress awaiting and research
to be done in the already explored elds. More theoretical de-
velopment seems necessary, as well as further examination on
conceptual denitions and hypothesis fabrication. Also, as it was
stated in the initial results section, although the majority of stud-
ies reviewed have quantitative methodologies, there seems to be
still few empirical studies in the eld. Furthermore, there is a
lesser amount with experimental designs, remaining quantitative
studies just mainly cross-sectional or intervention designs. Thus,
the methodological strength of these ndings remains somehow
unstable as a consequence of the youth of the eld, in opinion
of the authors.
Future research in this eld could not only well theoretical found-
ed studies are needed but also to improve the accuracy of designs,
increasing longitudinal, diary and lab-based experimental studies.
Also, some dimensions seemsstill unexplored. One example is the
exploration of mindfulness in what relates to music creation. For
example, composing or improvising could be also inuenced by
related processes, like ow or creativity, both related to mindful-
ness. Furthermore, music could be studied as a stimulus inducer
to mindfulness, which has been only scouted initially. Research
also could explore possible roots of western culture within mind-
fulness and music from an anthropological or musicological ap-
proach, hence enhancing theoretical frameworks.
References
[1] Kenny DT and Osborne MS (2006) Music performance anxiety: New
insights from young musicians. Advances in Cognitive Psychology 2 (2):
103–112, Jan.
[2] Kenny DT, Davis P, and Oates J (2004) Music performance anxiety and
occupational stress amongst opera chorus artists and their relationship with
state and trait anxiety and perfectionism., Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18
(6): 757–777, Jan.
[3] Wrigley WJ and Emmerson SB (2011) e experience of the ow state in
live music performance. Psychology of Music 41 (3): 292-305, May. DOI
10.1177/0305735611425903.
[4] Díaz FM (2011) Mindfulness , attention , and ow during music listening :
An empirical investigation, Psychology of Music, 41 (1): 42–58.
[5] Kabat-zinn J (1990) Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body
and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. Bantam Dell, New York, USA.
[6] Snyder CR and Lopez SJ (Eds.) (2002) Oxford Handbook of Positive Psy-
Figure 2: Proposed map of interactions between music and mindfulness.
MPA = Music Perfomance Anxiety
Sit = Situational
Disp = Dispositional
MPQ = Music Performance Quality
PWB = Psychological Well-Being
PH = Psychological Health.
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 34
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
chology. Oxford university Press, New York, USA, p. 829.
[7] Veehof MM, Ten Klooster PM, Taal E, Westerhof GJ, and Bohlmeijer ET
(2011) Psychometric properties of the Dutch Five Facet Mindfulness Ques-
tionnaire (FFMQ) in patients with bromyalgia. Clinical Rheumatology
30(8): 1045–1054 Aug.
[8] Kuyken W, Byford S, Taylor RS, Watkins E, Holden E et al (2008) Mind-
fulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent relapse in recurrent depression..
Journal of Consulting. and Clinical Psychology 76 (6): 966–978 Dec.
[9] Evans S, Ferrando S, FindlerM, Stowell C, Smart C et al (2008) Mindful-
ness-based cognitive therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders 22 (4): 716–721 May.
[10] Koszycki D, Benger M, Shlik J, and Bradwejn J (2007) Randomized trial of
a meditation-based stress reduction program and cognitive behavior therapy
in generalized social anxiety disorder. Behavior Research and erapy 45
(10): 2518–2526 Oct.
[11] Goldin PR, Ramel W, and Gross J (2009) Mindfulness Meditation Training
and Self-Referential Processing in Social Anxiety Disorder: Behavioral and
Neural Eects. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 23: 242–257.
[12] Goldin PR and Gross JJ (2010) Eects of mindfulness-based stress reduction
(MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion 10 (1):
83–91, Feb.
[13] Kocovski NL, Fleming JE and Rector NA (2009) Mindfulness and Accept-
ance-Based Group erapy for Social Anxiety Disorder: An Open Trial.
Cognitive Behavioral Practice 16 (3): 276–289, Aug.
[14] Williams JMG, Alatiq Y, Crane C, Barnhofer T, Fennell MJV et al (2008)
Mindfulness-based Cognitive erapy (MBCT) in bipolar disorder: prelimi-
nary evaluation of immediate eects on between-episode functioning. Jour-
nal of Aective Disorders 107 (1–3): 275–279 Apr.
[15] Kim B, Lee S-H, Kim YW, Choi TK, Yook K et al (2010) Eectiveness of
a mindfulness-based cognitive therapy program as an adjunct to pharmaco-
therapy in patients with panic disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 24 (6):
590–595 Aug..
[16] Lovas DA and Barsky AJ (2010) Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for
hypochondriasis, or severe health anxiety: a pilot study. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders 24 (8): 931–935 Dec.
[17] Green SM and Bieling PJ (2012) Expanding the Scope of Mindfulness-
Based Cognitive erapy: Evidence for Eectiveness in a Heterogeneous
Psychiatric Sample. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 19 (1): 174–180 Feb.
[18] Chiesa A and Serretti A (2011) Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for psy-
chiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research
187 (3): 441–453 May.
[19] Brown KW, Ryan RM, and Creswell JD (2007) Mindfulness: eoretical
Foundations and Evidence for its Salutary Eects. Psychological Inquiry 18
(4): 211–237 Oct.
[20] Philippot P, Nef F, Clauw L, de Romrée M, and Segal Z (2012) A rand-
omized controlled trial of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for treating
tinnitus. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 19 (5): 411–419 Sep.
[21] Carmody J, Reed G, Kristeller J, and Merriam P (2008) Mindfulness, spir-
ituality, and health-related symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 64
(4): 393–403 Apr.
[22] Shapiro SL, Brown KW, and Biegel GM (2007) Teaching self-care to car-
egivers: Eects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health
of therapists in training. Training and Education in Proesional Psychology
1 (2): 105–115.
[23] Brown KW and Ryan RY (2003) e benets of being present: Mindfulness
and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 84 (4): 822–848.
[24] Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, and Toney L (2006) Using
self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment
13 (1): 27–45 Mar.
[25] Kilpatrick LA, Suyenobu BY, Smith SR, Bueller JA, Goodman T et al (2011)
Impact of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction training on intrinsic brain
connectivity. Neuroimage 56 (1): 290–298 May.
[26] Davidson RJ (2003) Alterations in Brain and Immune Function Produced
by Mindfulness Meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine 65 (4): 564–570 Jul.
[27] Brewer JA, Worhunsky PD, Gray JR, Tang Y-Y, Weber J et al (2011) Medita-
tion experience is associated with dierences in default mode network activ-
ity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America 108 (50): 20254–20259 Dec. 2011.
[28] Brefczynski-Lewis JA, Lutz A, Schaefer HS, Levinson DB, and Davidson
RJ (2007) Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term medita-
tion practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America 104 (27): 11483–11488 Jul.
[29] Farb NAS, Segal Z, Mayberg H, Bean J, McKeon D et al (2007) Attending
to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-
reference. Social, Cognivite and Aective Neuroscience 2 (4): 313–322 Dec.
[30] Shapiro SL, Oman D, oresen CE, Plante TG, and Flinders T (2008) Cul-
tivating Mindfulness : Eects on Well-Being. Journal of Clinical Psychology
64 (7): 840–863.
[31] Barnes S, Brown KW, Krusemark E, Campbell WK, and Rogge RD (2007)
e role of mindfulness in romantic relationship satisfaction and responses
to relationship stress. Journal of Marital and Family erapy 33 (4): 482–
500 Oct.
[32] Brown KW and Kasser T (2005) Are Psychological and Ecological Well-
being Compatible? e Role of Values, Mindfulness, and Lifestyle. Social
Indicators Research 74 (2): 349–368 Nov.
[33] Brown KW, Kasser T, Ryan RM, Alex Linley P, and Orzech K (2009) When
what one has is enough: Mindfulness, nancial desire discrepancy, and sub-
jective well-being. Journal of Research in Personality 43 (5): 727–736 Oct.
[34] Shapiro SL, Jazaieri H, and Goldin PR (2012) Mindfulness-based stress re-
duction eects on moral reasoning and decision making. Journal of Positive
Psychology 7 (6): 504–515 Nov.
[35] Steptoe A and Fidler H (1987) Stage fright in orchestral musicians: A study
of cognitive and behavioural strategies in performance anxiety. British Jour-
nal of Psychology, 78: 241–249.
[36] Yoshie M, Kudo K, Murakoshi T, and Ohtsuki T (2009) Music performance
anxiety in skilled pianists: eects of social-evaluative performance situation
on subjective, autonomic, and electromyographic reactions. Experimantal
Brain Research, 199, (2): 117–126, Nov.
[37] American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2013) DSM-5: Diagnostic and
Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington DC: APA.
[38] Osborne MS and Franklin J (2002) Cognitive processes in music perfor-
mance anxiety, Australian Journal of Psychology, 54 (2): 86–93, Aug.
[39] Kenny DT (2005) A Systematic Review of Treatments for Music Perfor-
mance Anxiety. Anxiety, Stress and Coping 18 (3): 183–208, Sep.
[40] Brown KW, Weinstein N, and Creswell JD (2012) Trait mindfulness modu-
lates neuroendocrine and aective responses to social evaluative threat. Psy-
choneuroendocrinology 37 (12): 2037–2041, Dec.
[41] Lin P, Chang JC, Zemon V, and Midlarsky E (2008) Silent illumination: a
study on Chan (Zen) meditation, anxiety, and musical performance quality.
Psychology of Music 36 (2): 139–155.
[42] Cope S,. Wyshak ÆG, and. Sklar ÆE (2003) Yoga Ameliorates Performance
Anxiety and Mood Disturbance in Young Professional Musicians. Applied
Psychophysiological Biofeedback 34 (4): 279–289.
[43] Díaz FM (2010) A Preliminary Investigation into Eects of a brief Mindu-
ness induction on Perceptions of Attention, Aesthetic response, and Flow
during music listening. Florida State university.
[44] Eckhardt KJ and Dinsmore JA (2012) Mindful music listening as a potential
treatment for depression., Journal of Creativity in Mental Health, 7 (2):
176–186.
[45] Bakker AB (2005) Flow among music teachers and their students: e cross-
over of peak experiences, Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66 (1): 26–44,
Feb.
[46] Langer E, Russel T, and Eisenkraft N (2009) Orchestral performance and
the footprint of mindfulness. Psychology of Music 37 (2): 125–136, Apr.
[47] Farnsworth-Grodd VA (2012) Mindfulness and the Self-Regulation of Mu-
sic performance Anxiety. University of Auckland.
[48] Chang JC, Midlarsky E, and Lin P (2003) e eects of meditation on
music performance anxiety, Medical Problems Performing Artists, 18(3):
126-130.
[49] Khalsa SBS, Butzer B, Shorter SM, Reinhardt KM, and Cope S (2013) Yoga
reduces performance anxiety in adolescent musicians., Alternative erapies
in Health and Medicine 19 (2): 34–45.
[50] Monti DA, Peterson C, Kunkel EJS, Hauck WW, Pequignot E et al (2006)
A randomized, controlled trial of mindfulness-based art therapy (MBAT) for
women with cancer., Psychooncology 15 (5): 363–373.
[51] Stern JRS, Khalsa SBS, and Hofmann SG (2012) A yoga intervention for
music performance anxiety in conservatory students. Medical Problems Per-
forming Artists 27 (3): 123–8, Sep.
[52] Robarts J (2009) Supporting the development of mindfulness and meaning:
Clinical pathways in music therapy with a sexually abused child.. In Com-
municative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, Mal-
loch S and Trevarthen C, Eds. Oxford University Press, New York: 377–400.
[53] Wolfe DE and Noguchi LK (2009) e use of music with young children to
improve sustained attention during a vigilance task in the presence of audi-
tory distractions. Journal of Music erapy 46 (1): 69–82, Jan.
[54] Steyn MH, e impact of psychological skills and mindfulness training on
the psychological well-being of undergraduate music students, University of
Pretoria, 2013.
[55] Vidyarthi J (2012) Sonic Cradle : Evoking Mindfulness through ‘ Immersive
’ Interaction Design. Simon Fraiser University.
[56] Vidyarthi J, Riecke BE, and Gromala D (2012) Sonic Cradle : Designing
for an Immersive Experience of Meditation by Connecting Respiration to
Music. Proceedings in DIS 2012.
[57] Vidyarthi J and Riecke BE (2013) Mediated Meditation: Cultivating Mind-
fulness with Sonic Cradle. CHI 2013 Extended Abstracts.
[58] Vidyarthi J and Riecke BE (in press) Interactively Mediating Experiences of
Mindfulness Meditation, International Journal of Human-Computer Stud-
ies, Feb. DOI 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2014.01.006
[59] Coholic DA (2011) Exploring the Feasibility and Benets of Arts-Based
International Journal of Behavioral Research & Psychology, 2014 © 35
Raquel Rodríguez-Carvajal, Oscar Lecuona de la Cruz (2014) Mindfulness and Music: A Promising Subject of an Unmapped Field. Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2(3), 27-35
Mindfulness-Based Practices with Young People in Need: Aiming to Improve
Aspects of Self-Awareness and Resilience. Child & Youth Care Forum, 40
(4): 303–317, Jan.
[60] Fidelibus JF (2004) Mindfulness and music therapy clinical improvisation:
When the music ows. New York University.
[61] Brown J (2011) In the zone : An autoethnographic study exploring the links
between ow and mindfulness for a piano accompanist. Studies in Learning,
Evaluation, Innovation and Development, 8 (2): 83–95, 2011.
[62] De Felice M (2004) Mindfulness Meditation: A new tool for understand-
ing and regulating musical performance anxiety: An aective neuroscientic
perspective. Dissertations from from ProQuest, Paper 2178.
[63] Oyan S (2006) Mindfulness Meditation: Creative Musical Performance
rough Awareness. George Mason University.
[64] Xu H (2010) Application of Zen Techniques to overcome Performance Anx-
iety. Florida State University.
[65] Grant A, Langer E, Falk E, and Capodilupo C (2004) Mindful Creativity:
Drawing to Draw Distinctions. Creativity Research Journal 16: 261–265.
[66] Wright JJ, Sadlo G, and Stew G (2006) Challenge-skills and mindfulness:
An exploration of the conundrum of ow process. OTJR: Occupation, Par-
ticipation and Health 26 (1): 25–32.
[67] Reid D (2011) Mindfulness and ow in occupational engagement: presence
in doing. Canadian Journal of Occupational erapy 78: 50–56.
... The remainder of the studies were literature-based research (Baer, 2003;Oyan, 2006;Rodríguez-Carvajal & de la Cruz, 2014;Xu, 2010). Sample sizes in the selected studies were varied, ranging from one (Robarts, 2009) to 203 participants (Langer et al., 2009). ...
... There are many ways to cultivate happiness and health; MBP and music therapy, whether experienced independently or integrated, can benefit health and wellbeing (Diaz, 2010(Diaz, , 2013Hwang, 2018;Lin et al., 2008;Medcalf, 2017;Rodríguez-Carvajal & de la Cruz, 2014;Vidyarthi et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
With increasing recognition of the advantage of working within a multidisciplinary team and interdisciplinary study for health, the incorporation of music and mindfulness across healthcare disciplines has become more common. The aim of this study was to explore the integrating theory and practice, key principles, and psychodynamic perspectives with respect to music therapy and mindfulness. Thirty articles were selected from electronic databases and grey literature. Conference abstracts and informal literature reviews were excluded. The articles were categorised and analysed according to methods, interventions, outcome measures and key messages. Key outcomes from the studies revealed that integrating mindfulness and music can enhance the musical experience, facilitate the music therapy process (e.g. Guided Imagery and Music), and contribute to mental wellness (e.g. stress reduction, emotional support, and self awareness). Based on the data analysis, two core themes were identified: a) psychodynamic perspectives of mindfulness and music therapy; and b) here and now, letting go, nonself, nonattachment and being non-judgmental. The link between music and mindfulness has been recognised during recent decades, and combining music and mindfulness demonstrated positive outcomes in the literature. The findings revealed several key perspectives and approaches between mindfulness-based practice (MBP) and music therapy. These findings can offer a new outlook to the therapeutic relationship and can give a practical and theoretical framework of combining mindfulness and music therapy.
... An important aim of the sound interaction is to encourage the user in a playful use of the tool and to encourage exploratory listening that can provide a positive experience with the sound algorithms [18]. The involvement and user engagement paradigm of this objective roots in the creation of performative installations where a user is engaged without having to be an expert of the tool [14]. ...
... The sound algorithms are implemented using the We-bAudio API. 17 The visual environment and visualisation is implemented using the WebXR API. 18 Both APIs use the positional data from movement detection. In the current version we have two possible sound algorithms, the classical Theremin sound and a sample-based Theremin granular synthesis algorithm. ...
Article
This paper presents a first prototype of a virtual Theremin instrument for accompanying film scenes with sound. The virtual Theremin is implemented as a hybrid application for the web. Sound control is achieved by capturing user gestures with a webcam and mapping the gestures to the corresponding virtual Theremin parameters pitch and volume. Different sound types can be selected. The application’s underlying research is part of the multi-modal digital heritage project KOLLISIONEN which targets to open up the private archive of the Russian film maker Sergej Eisenstein to a broader public in digital form. Eisenstein, a film theorist and pioneer of film montage, was particularly intrigued by the Theremin as an instrument for film sound design. The virtual Theremin presented here is therefore linked to a film scene from the 1929 Soviet drama “The General Line” by Sergej Eisenstein which was never set to music originally. In its first implementation state, the application connects music interaction design with digital heritage in a modular, flexible and playful way and uses contemporary web technologies to enable easy operation and the greatest possible accessibility.
... Music and meditation interventions have each gained recognition for their benefits to health and wellness, and healthcare practitioners have carried out a great deal of work in this area [1,47,[109][110][111][112][113][114]. There are various possibilities for modifying the MM programme for future research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mental wellness can be related to healthier living, the learning process and working environments for people in the university community. A wide range of mental wellness programmes have been explored to provide students with pleasant and satisfying experiences. The purpose of this study is to explore the effects of integrating Zen meditation and music listening on the mindfulness and happiness levels of university music therapy students. A qualitative methodology was used, and data were collected through surveys and semi-structured interviews. To investigate various aspects of data regarding the role of the meditation and music (MM) programme for mindfulness and happiness, this study used thematic analysis within a qualitative research design. The findings of this study suggest that the 8-week Mindfulness Meditation (MM) programme is a potential approach for enhancing mindfulness, happiness and stress management. These results carry broader implications, particularly in terms of supporting mental health resources in higher education. Furthermore, the study contributes to the ongoing discussion regarding the positive impact of combining meditation and music to promote mental well-being. This integrated approach has the potential to strengthen coping strategies and further promote the integration of music and meditation practices in various contexts, including higher education.
... Despite the important role that developing mindfulness skills could play to benefit higher education student musicians' health and well-being and performance, the research evidence in this particular field remains scant (Rodríguez-Carvajal & Lecuona, 2014) with only a few exploratory studies available. In a quasi-experimental study, Steyn et al. (2016) examined the effect of a 7-week mixed program, which integrated mindfulness and sport-related psychological skills, across various psychological and well-being measures on a convenience sample of 36 undergraduate music students allocated to either an experimental or a control group. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Higher education student musicians face high physical, psychological, and emotional demands affecting their well-being and academic experience. This study examined the feasibility and preliminary effectiveness of the so-called CRAFT program, based on mindfulness, yoga, positive psychology, and emotional intelligence, to improve psychological well-being, psychological distress, emotional regulation, and physical flexibility amongst tertiary education student musicians. Methods Using a single-arm pre-post study design, student musicians (n = 25) at a royal conservatory of music in Spain followed a 25-week CRAFT program that was curricularly implemented during the academic year 2018/2019, once a week for 50 min. The outcome measures included were the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ), the Subjective Psychological Well-Being Subscale (SPWS), the Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21), and the Sit and Reach Test (SRT). Results Paired samples t-test and practical significance analyses revealed significant improvements for the total scale of the FFMQ (g = 0.28), the Observe (g = 0.44) and Describe (g = 0.38) subscales of the FFMQ, the SPWS (g = 0.32), the Reappraisal subscale of the ERQ (g = 0.43), and the SRT (g = 0.39). A similar pattern of results was observed in a filtered sample (n = 15) when excluding participants simultaneously engaged in yoga/meditation activities other than the CRAFT program. Conclusions These results indicated that the CRAFT program is a promising intervention for improving mindfulness skills and health and well-being states and abilities amongst higher education student musicians. Further research is needed to substantiate these findings and extend them to similar settings and populations with complex psychophysical concerns.
Article
Full-text available
Music performance anxiety (MPA) is a common damaging phenomenon in musicians' careers. Mindfulness stands as a promising construct to prevent MPA. However, the relationships between mindfulness and MPA are scarcely explored, alongside other relevant attention-based (e.g., self-consciousness) or emotion-based constructs (e.g., negative affect). This study explores the relationships between these constructs. A sample of 151 musicians was assessed to explore the relationships between these constructs. Self-reports of mindfulness, MPA, negative affect, and self-consciousness were applied. We implemented network analysis following a general (second-order) and specific (first-order) framework. Networks showed dispositional mindfulness as negatively associated with negative affect and MPA in both general and facet levels, while mindfulness in past performances was only negatively associated with negative affect. MPA was positively associated with negative affect and self-consciousness. Mindfulness displayed light or no associations with self-consciousness. Therefore, mindfulness seems to be a relevant construct toward MPA. We propose a preliminary model to improve mindfulness research and interventions applied to music performers. We also outline limitations and future directions.
Thesis
Full-text available
Music performance anxiety (MPA) seriously affects nearly all musicians at some point in their lives and may cause musicians to abandon their careers or develop maladaptive coping mechanisms to manage their symptoms. While peer-reviewed studies have explored a broad array of psychological treatments for MPA, a paucity of research exists regarding expert classical guitarists’ recommendations for MPA symptom management. Since all post-secondary guitar instructors at the expert level interact with MPA in themselves and their students, and each instrumental discipline has idiosyncratic presentations of MPA, this dissertation seeks to understand common approaches and MPA management strategies from the perspective of classical guitar instructors at the post-secondary level—a cohort which includes the researcher. The project compares guitar experts’ recommendations with existing treatment protocols from psychology to understand MPA from a deeper scientific and heuristic perspective while providing pathways for novel research in this topic. This research concluded that guitarists’ approaches prioritized performance excellence and complete mental and physical preparation to manage MPA, while psychological treatments prioritized cognitive components of MPA, musician’s well-being, and symptom reduction. An impressive convergence of approaches appeared in the guitar and psychology literature, particularly regarding the use of mindfulness strategies and strategies from sports psychology.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Research shows that mindfulness interventions for test anxiety in a college student population are beneficial (Lothes, Mochrie, Wilson, & Hakan, 2021). This study expands on the existing literature by examining how distance learning mindfulness practices may affect anxiety in college students. Aim: This study assessed the effects of online mindfulness practices over a five-week period on test anxiety in college students. Method: Participants included 31 college students that were randomly assigned to either a sitting meditation or music meditation condition. The two groups were also split in half to add a wait list control condition. A weekly schedule of mindfulness practices was given to participants to complete on their own. Results: Participants in the sitting meditation condition showed significant within-group reductions in test anxiety, overall anxiety, and mindfulness from start to finish. The music meditation group showed no changes in test anxiety. However, overall anxiety showed decreases in scores, and overall mindfulness did show significant increases for this group. Conclusion: Mindfulness may play a role in the reduction of anxiety and test anxiety. Further research is needed to more definitively assess how music meditation may impact anxiety.
Article
Full-text available
In a cross-sectional study, associations of global affect with two ways of listening to music – attentive-analytical listening (AL) and emotional listening (EL) – were examined. Based on a two-dimensional model of general affect, we focused on the degrees to which AL and EL are differentially correlated with positive and negative affect. In addition to bivariate relationships, the interactions between different states of general affect were tested. In Study 1, a sample of 1,291 individuals responded to questionnaires on listening to music, positive affect (PA), and negative affect (NA). We used the PANAS, which measures PA and NA as high arousal dimensions. AL was positively correlated with PA, EL with NA. Moderation analyses showed stronger associations between PA and AL when NA was low. Study 2 (N = 499 participants) differentiated between three facets of affect and focused, in addition to PA and NA, on the role of relaxation. Similar to the findings of Study 1, AL was correlated with PA, EL with NA and PA. Moderation analyses indicated that the degree to which PA was associated with an individual´s tendency to listen to music attentively depended on their degree of relaxation. In addition, the correlation between PA and EL was stronger for individuals who were more relaxed; for individuals who were less relaxed, the correlation between NA and EL was stronger. In sum, the results demonstrate not only simple bivariate correlations, but also that the expected associations vary, depending on the different affective states. We argue that the results reflect a dual function of listening to music, which includes emotional regulation and information processing.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Mindfulness has been explored in the clinical and educational fields but has rarely been studied in the music domain. This study investigated the effects of teaching eight-week Mindfulness for Singers courses on vocalists’ music education and performance. Methods A mixed methods approach was utilized, which included controlled and randomized controlled trials using standardized and novel mindfulness measures pre- and post-intervention, interviews post-intervention and three months later, concurrent diaries, and a blinded teacher study. Participants included singing students (total n=52) and their teachers ( n=11) from a university and a music college over a period of two years. Results Levels of mindfulness increased over the intervention for experimental participants in comparison to controls. Considering their total student cohort, teachers identified 61% of eligible mindfulness singing participants as having completed the mindfulness intervention. Experimental participants reported that learning mindfulness had positive effects in lessons, solo and group instrumental practices, and when performing on stage. They described more focus and attention, positive effects of increased body awareness on singing technique, enhanced socio-collaborative relationships, reductions in performance anxiety, and beneficial effects whilst performing, such as more expressivity and enjoyment. Conclusions Learning mindfulness had positive holistic effects on vocal students and was well received by their mindfulness-naïve singing teachers. Findings suggest that it would be highly beneficial for mindfulness to be made available in music conservatoires and university music departments alongside singing lessons for singers to enhance their present experience as vocal students and their futures as performers and teachers.
Article
Full-text available
This study explored the efficacy of psychological skills and mindfulness training intervention on the psychological wellbeing of undergraduate music students. Participants were undergraduate music students (n = 36) from the Department of Music at a South African university, 21 of whom were elected to take the psychological skills and mindfulness training intervention. Data on their self-reported psychological wellbeing, psychological skills, mindfulness and performance anxiety levels were collected pre-and post-intervention. The analysis applied non-parametric procedures to determine changes in students’ psychological wellbeing after the seven-week intervention programme. Findings suggest improvements in psychological wellbeing, psychological skills, mindfulness and performance anxiety with training. Psychological skills and mindfulness training may have benefits to the psychological wellbeing of music students.
Article
Full-text available
The process of flow, a psychological state that seems to occur during optimal human experience, is currently unclear. This exploratory study examines how flow begins and what happens during and after a flow experience. A phenomenological approach was taken to examine the flow experiences of an artist, a musician, and a horticulturist. Participants kept journals and participated in semi-structured interviews. The results suggest that two phenomena, “challenge-skills” and “mindfulness,” were identified as being “flow” experiences. Challenge-skills and mindfulness had some common features. Both involved living in the present moment, not worrying, and performing activities because they were intrinsically rewarding. They were distinctly different experiences in regard to the effort involved, the perception of time, and the consequences of the experience. Understanding the process of challenge-skills and mindfulness may have implications for our understanding of the relationship between occupation, consciousness, and health and for occupational therapy practice.
Chapter
Full-text available
Communicative Musicality’ explores the intrinsic musical nature of human interaction. The theory of communicative musicality was developed from groundbreaking studies showing how in mother/infant communication there exist noticeable patterns of timing, pulse, voice timbre, and gesture. Without intending to, the exchange between a mother and her infant follow many of the rules of musical performance, including rhythm and timing. This is the first book to be devoted to this topic. In a collection of cutting-edge chapters, encompassing brain science, human evolution, psychology, acoustics and music performance, it focuses on the rhythm and sympathy of musical expression in human communication from infancy. It demonstrates how speaking and moving in rhythmic musical ways is the essential foundation for all forms of communication, even the most refined and technically elaborated, just as it is for parenting, good teaching, creative work in the arts, and therapy to help handicapped or emotionally distressed persons. A landmark in the literature, ‘Communicative Musicality’ is a valuable text for all those in the fields of developmental, educational, and music psychology, as well as those in the field of music therapy.
Article
Full-text available
Sonic Cradle is a human-computer interaction paradigm designed to foster meditative attentional patterns. A user's body is suspended comfortably in a completely dark sound chamber while the interaction paradigm subtly encourages them to focus on their breathing to summon and progressively shape an abstract immersive sound experience. Basic interpretive qualitative methods with a purposive sample of 39 participants were used to systematically analyze interview data after a 15-minute experience of the system. Results suggest that this persuasive medium can pleasantly encourage an experience comparable to mindfulness by consistently inducing a calm mental clarity and loss of intention. Surprisingly, participants also reported perceptual illusions, feelings of floating, and emotional responses. Mounting evidence implies mindfulness meditation as an effective practice for self-regulation; this study represents a first step toward realizing technology's potential to increase wellbeing by introducing people to this psychologically beneficial contemplative practice.
Article
This study investigated the effect of meditation on music performance anxiety. Participants were 19 students between the ages of 18 and 41 yrs, who were recruited from the Manhattan School of Music, Mannes College of Music, Yale University School of Music, and State University of New York at Purchase. The experimental group received a series of eight meditation classes, and the control group received no meditation training. After the 8-week training period, all performed in a concert. Pretests and post-tests of music performance anxiety were given and post-tests of state anxiety and of performance concentration. Performance anxiety decreased among participants in the meditation group, in contrast to participants in the control group, whose performance anxiety did not decrease. Differences in regard to post-test state anxiety and performance concentration were not significant. An additional benefit of meditation was a reported increase in relaxation pleasure even in the period immediately before the performance. Results indicate that meditation may be a useful tool for aiding performers to combat performance anxiety.
Book
The Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology studies the burgeoning field of positive psychology, which, in recent years, has transcended academia to capture the imagination of the general public. The book provides a roadmap for the psychology needed by the majority of the population-those who don't need treatment, but want to achieve the lives to which they aspire. The articles summarize all of the relevant literature in the field, and each is essentially defining a lifetime of research. The content's breadth and depth provide a cross-disciplinary look at positive psychology from diverse fields and all branches of psychology, including social, clinical, personality, counseling, school, and developmental psychology. Topics include not only happiness-which has been perhaps misrepresented in the popular media as the entirety of the field-but also hope, strengths, positive emotions, life longings, creativity, emotional creativity, courage, and more, plus guidelines for applying what has worked for people across time and cultures.
Article
This study examined the optimal psychological state of flow in a live music performance context at an Australian tertiary music institution in order to advance understanding of this under-researched experience in music performance and education. The Flow State Scale-2 (FSS-2) was administered to 236 students from five instrument families immediately after their performance examinations. A further aim was to examine the psychometric properties of the FSS-2 in order to determine its suitability for use as a measure of flow in music performance domains. The findings provided the first empirical confirmation of the validity and reliability of the flow model in live music performance. The flow experience was found to be consistent with findings from sport performance and did not vary substantially according to instrument type, year level, or gender. Most students in the sample did not believe they were sufficiently skilled to meet the challenge of the performance and most did not experience it as absorbing or enjoyable. The implications of the findings for the enhancement of teaching and learning methods were examined. Future research directions were discussed, particularly in regards to psychological skills training to help improve the music performance experience.
Article
This study investigated the effects of a brief mindfulness meditation induction technique on perceived attention, aesthetic response, and flow during music listening as measured by Continuous Response Digital Interface (CRDI) and questionnaire. Participants were students enrolled in music classes or ensembles at a comprehensive university in the southeastern United States (N = 132), and were randomly assigned to one of four groups: mindfulness induction paired with aesthetic response (n = 34), mindfulness induction paired with flow response (n = 35), aesthetic response (n = 32), or flow response (n = 31). Responses to questionnaire items suggests that participants experienced a subjective ‘heightening’ of attention during music listening compared to baseline in all conditions, with no specific modification attributable to the mindfulness task. However, a majority of respondents in the mindfulness groups reported that the task had modified their listening experience by increasing their ability to focus on the music without distraction. Composite CRDI graphs suggest unique response patterns between groups based on both the presence of a mindfulness task as well as the construct for focus of attention. Additionally, verbal accounts suggest phenomenological differences between flow and aesthetic responses, with each accounting for a unique type of heightened and positively valenced affective experience.