ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

SUMMARY Over the past several decades, the field of pediatric pain has made impressive advances in our understanding of the pain experience of the developing child, as well as the devastating impact of inadequately managed pain early in life. It is now well recognized that, from infancy, children are capable of developing implicit memories of pain that can influence their subsequent reactions to pain. The present review provides a synthesis of selected studies that made a significant impact on this field of inquiry, with particular emphasis on recent clinical and laboratory-based experimental research examining children's explicit autobiographical memories for acute pain. Research has begun to move towards improving the precision with which children at risk for developing negatively estimated pain memories can be identified, given the adverse influence these memories can have on subsequent pain experiences. As such, several fear- and anxiety-related child and parent variables implicated in this process are discussed, and avenues for future research and clinical intervention are identified throughout. Finally, a preliminary empirically and theoretically derived model of acute pain memory development in childhood is presented to parsimoniously summarize the evidence accumulated to date and guide future investigation in this area.
Content may be subject to copyright.
part of
SUMMARY Over the past several decades, the eld of pediatric pain has made
impressive advances in our understanding of the pain experience of the developing child,
as well as the devastating impact of inadequately managed pain early in life. It is now well
recognized that, from infancy, children are capable of developing implicit memories of
pain that can inuence their subsequent reactions to pain. The present review provides
a synthesis of selected studies that made a signicant impact on this eld of inquiry,
with particular emphasis on recent clinical and laboratory-based experimental research
1Department o f Psychology, Dalhousie Universi ty, Life Sciences Centre, PO Box15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4R2, C anada
2Centre for Pediatric Pain Research, IWK Health Centre, 5850/5980, University Avenu e, Halifax, N ova Scotia, B3K 6R8, Canada
3Department of Pediatrics, Dalhousie Univer sity and the IWK Health Centre, 5850 Univer sity Avenue, PO Box9700, B3K 6R8, Canada
4Department of Psychiatr y, Dalhousie Universi ty, 5909 Veterans’ Memor ial Lane, 8th oor, Abbie J Lane Memorial Buildin g QEII Health
Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scot ia, B3H 2E2, Canada
5Research, IWK Heal th Centre, 5850/5980 Universit y Avenue, PO Box 9700, Halifa x, NS B3K 6R8, Canada
6Department of Communit y Health & Epidemiology, Centre for Clinic al Research, 5790 University Avenue, Hali fax, NS B3H 1V7, Canada
*Author for correspond ence: melanieenoel@gma il.com
The eld of pediatric pain has come to appreciate the importance of children’s early pain memories in
inuencing their subsequent pain experiences.
This review synthesizes selected studies that made a signicant impact on this eld of inquiry,
with particular emphasis on recent clinical and experimental research examining children’s explicit
autobiographical memories for acute pain.
Research is beginning to enhance the precision with which children at risk for developing negatively
estimated pain memories can be identied, in light of the adverse inuence that these memories can
have on children’s subsequent pain experience.
Although there is a dearth of research examining the eectiveness of memory reframing interventions,
these treatments hold promise for reducing the adverse impact of inadequately managed pain.
With the development of new assessment tools and the pediatric fear-avoidance model of chronic
pain, future research should examine the inuence of child and parent fear-avoidance variables in the
development of children’s memories for pain.
A preliminary empirically and theoretically derived model of children’s acute-pain memory development
is proposed, which outlines the factors and relationships implicated in the process by which children’s
pain memories develop, are reframed and inuence the pain experience over time.
Practice Points
Pain is not over when the needle ends:
a review and preliminary model of acute
pain memory development in
childhood
REVIEW
Melanie Noel*1,2, Christine T Chambers1–4, Mark Petter1,2, Patrick J
McGrath1–5, Raymond M Klein1 & Sherry H Stewart1,4,6
487
ISSN 1758-1869
10.2217/PMT.12.41 © 2012 Future Medicine Ltd Pain Mana ge. (2012) 2(5), 487–497
For reprint orders, please contact: reprints@futuremedicine.com
40 years ago, it was widely believed that infants
could not feel pain [1] , which led to inadequate
treatment and needless suffering [2] . Prior to
the 1980s, infants were also believed to be
incapable of encoding, storing and retrieving
memories of past autobiographical events [3].
Recognition of the importance of children’s
memories for pain was largely inspired by land-
mark studies that were published prior to the
turn of the millennium. The seminal research
by Taddio and colleagues provided evidence of
the adverse impact of poorly managed pain in
the first days of life on subsequent pain experi-
enced at 4 and 6 months [4] . Infants who were
circumcised without the use of topical local
anesthetic, as compared with infants who
were uncircumcised, displayed a greater pain
response and cried longer at a subsequent rou-
tine immunization. Furthermore, the infants
circumcised with placebo were observed to
experience a greater increase in pain during
the subsequent immun ization than infants cir-
cumcised with topical local anesthetic. These
researchers subsequently demonstrated that
infants of diabetic mothers who were exposed
to repeated heel lances in the first 2–3 days of
life, as compared with infants who were not
exposed to repeated painful procedures, exhib-
ited a greater pain response prior to (i.e., dur-
ing skin preparation and cleansing) and during
a subsequent venipuncture [5] . These studies
dramatically demonstrated how inadequately
managed pain in the rst days of life could
lead to sensitization to later pain experiences.
This research also raised the possibility that
these infants learned to anticipate pain during
a subsequent procedure and developed some
form of implicit pain memory that influenced
their distress during subsequent pain [6]. This
early work provided critical evidence for the
powerful role of children’s early memories for
pain in their subsequent pain experience and
established the importance of this area in the
field of pediatric pain.
Indeed, children’s memories for pain have
important implications for pain assessment,
treatment and health across the lifespan [6 ,7].
Many of the tools used to assess pain rely on the
child’s retrospective account of their psycholog-
ical and physical experience of pain (e.g., com-
parison between current and remembered pain
experience) and memory for pain is often used
to infer the effectiveness of current interven-
tions. Additionally, memories for pain may
play an important role in the transition of pain
from an acute to chronic state [8,101], through
operant and respondent learning processes as
well as altered processing within the CNS [8] .
Moreover, early memories for pain can have
important long-term consequences for subse-
quent health behaviors and pain perception.
Retrospective research suggests that early pain
memories influence fear and avoidance of medi-
cal care in adulthood [9]. Furthermore, among
both clinica l and healthy samples, children
who develop pain memories that are negatively
estimated (i.e., recalled pain is higher than
initial pain report) are at risk for experiencing
increases in distress and pain over time [10,11].
As such, children’s memories for pain are as
important to their subsequent health behaviors
as their actual experience of pain itself.
The purpose of this review is to offer a con-
temporary perspective on recent issues in the
field by providing a synthesis of selected studies
that have made significant contributions to our
understanding of children’s explicit memories
for acute pain, with particular emphasis on
recent clinical and laborator y-based experi-
mental research. In addition, a novel prelimi-
nary theoretical model of children’s acute pain
memory development, inspired by this research,
is presented to stimulate future research in this
area. Finally, avenues for clinical intervention
are highlighted throughout. Given that this
review paper is not intended to be a systematic
review or meta-ana lysis, a formal search strategy
was not applied.
examining children’s explicit autobiographical memories for acute pain. Research has
begun to move towards improving the precision with which children at risk for developing
negatively estimated pain memories can be identied, given the adverse inuence
these memories can have on subsequent pain experiences. As such, several fear- and
anxiety-related child and parent variables implicated in this process are discussed, and
avenues for future research and clinical intervention are identied throughout. Finally, a
preliminary empirically and theoretically derived model of acute pain memory development
in childhood is presented to parsimoniously summarize the evidence accumulated to date
and guide future i nvestigation in this area.
Pain Manage . (2012) 2(5) future science group
488
REVIEW Noel, Chambers, Petter, McGrath, Klein & Stewart
Children’s pain memories:
the state-of-the-science
Pain memories are – not unlike the pain experi-
ence [12]subjective and multidimensional, con-
sisting of sensory (e.g., pain intensity), affective
(e.g., fear) and contextual (e.g., people, place
and time) aspects [7]. Children’s memories for
pain are constructive and reconstructive, which
makes them susceptible to distortion over time.
The manner in which memories are framed is
influenced by a host of factors including: age
[13]; distress [10]; pain intensity [14]; trait anxi-
ety [15]; negative affectivity [16]; parent–child
discourse about the event following exposure
[17] ; duration of the interim between pain expe-
rience and memory assessment [18,19]; and state
anxiety at the time of memory elicitation [20].
As a result, empirical investigation in this area is
methodologically complex. Nevertheless, given
the importance of children’s pain memories in
shaping their subsequent pain experiences, gar-
nering understanding of the processes involved
in children’s pain memory development is of
critical importance.
Previous reviews on children’s memories
for pain
There is a substantial body of literature on the
development of children’s autobiographic al
memories for stressful and traumatic events (for
reviews, see [21, 22]); however, specific research
on children’s memories for pain is relatively
limited. To date, two reviews on the topic have
been conducted. The first, by Ornstein and col-
leagues, provided an overview of research that
was primarily conducted with children in medi-
cal procedure contexts [7]. The authors provided
an information-processing framework outlining
the various stages of memory (encoding, storage
and retrieval), informed by literature on chil-
dren’s autobiographical memory development
within which the acquisition, retention and dis-
tortion of information about pain experiences
can be organized and understood. Despite the
importance of this initial review, the majority of
studies reviewed assessed children’s memory for
contextual details as opposed to aspects of the
pain itself. As such, it was unclear whether the
same principles underlying children’s contextual
memory also applied to memory for somatosen-
sory and affective aspects of pain. The second
review, by von Baeyer and colleagues, provided
an overview of developments in the field since
the time of the original review, emphasized the
consequences of children’s pain memories for
subsequent pain and healthcare behaviors, and
discussed individual differences as well as situ-
ational and methodological influences in chil-
dren’s recall [6]. Importantly, this review high-
lighted the individual variability in children’s
pain memories and resulting health behaviors
as well as potential intervention targets that
could prevent the deleterious consequences of
n egatively estimated pain memories.
Fragility of pain memories
Children’s autobiographical memory – the type
of explicit memory of an event from one’s life
that is tied to a particular place and time (e.g., a
painful medical procedure) – has been described
as remarkably robust yet significantly fragile
[23,24 ]. Similar to memory for stressful events,
children have generally been found capable of
accurately recalling previous painful experi-
ences; indeed, children as young as 3 years of age
are fairly accurate at recalling contextual aspects
of painful medical procedures, especially in the
absence of specific or leading questions [25, 26].
Similarly, among both healthy and clinical sam-
ples of children undergoing experimental and
medically induced pain, children’s pain memo-
ries have been found to be accurate after short
(e.g., 1 week) and long (e.g., 1 year) delays [27–29] .
Nevertheless, the fragility of pain memories
is rooted in the fact that memory can become
distorted over time [10,14,15, 30]. Bruck and col-
leagues conducted an investigation that pro-
vided the rst and perhaps most compelling
demonstration of the reconstructive nature of
children’s pain memories [31]. They examined
children’s memories for contextual details of,
and pain experienced during, an immunization
as well as the effect of providing children with
dif ferent types of information following the
procedure on their subsequent recall. Following
immunization, children were randomly assigned
to receive either pain-affirming (e.g., ‘the shot
hurt’), pain-denying (e.g., ‘the shot did not
hurt’), or neutral (e.g., ‘the shot is over’) feed-
back. Children’s recalled pain and observer-
reported distress ratings (i.e., based on facial
features, verbalizations and degree of crying)
did not differ 1 week later; however, approxi-
mately 1 year later (range: 4–18 months) the
researchers conducted follow-up home visits on
three separate occasions to provide additional
pain-denying or neutral feedback and mis-
leading or non misleading (e.g., falsely stating
Review & preliminary model of acute pain memory development in childhood REVIEW
future science group www.futuremedicine.com 489
that the pediatrician read a story to the child)
information about the individuals involved in
the children’s care. At a fourth and final visit,
children who received pain-denying feedback
remembered crying less and experiencing less
pain than children who received neutral feed-
back. Furthermore, children who received
misleading information made more false alle-
gations regarding the individuals involved in
their care (e.g., reported that the research assis-
tant gave them the vaccination) than children
who did not receive this misleading informa-
tion. Interestingly, those children who exhibited
greater distress during the immunization were
found to be more susceptible to suggestibil-
ity 4–18 months later. Together, the findings
demonstrated the malleability of children’s pain
memories and the impact that discourse sur-
rounding the experience following the event can
have on subsequent recall.
Memory distortion & subsequent pain
experience
Once children’s pain memories become dis-
torted, they can have important implications
for distress experienced during subsequent pain
experiences. Chen and colleagues examined this
possibility among children diagnosed with leu-
kemia who were required to repeatedly undergo
lumbar punctures spaced 1 week apart as part
of their treatment protocol [10]. Although chil-
dren’s pain memories were generally accurate, a
subset of children later developed exaggerated
memories of the negative details of the lum-
bar puncture (described in more detail below).
Furthermore, children who developed more
negatively estimated, versus accurate or posi-
tively estimated, memories of pain and anxiety
exhibited greater increases in behavioral distress
(i.e., observer-rated behaviors that are indicative
of anxiety and/or pain such as crying, scream-
ing and verbalizations of anxiety), and reported
more pain over time. This was a robust nd-
ing that persisted even after controlling for age,
initial distress and number of previous lumbar
punctures. This research highlighted the impor-
tance of memory distortion for subsequent dis-
tress and identified children who are most at risk
for developing maladaptive responses to pain
versus those who may be buffered from such
outcomes.
A recent investigation by Noel and colleagues
extended this research among healthy children
undergoing a novel experimental pain task [11] .
Children completed the cold pressor task on
two laboratory visits spaced 1 month apart. At
2 weeks following the initial exposure, chil-
dren’s memories for the pain task and expec-
tancies about future pain were elicited. Similar
to healthy adults [16] , children’s memories of
pain intensity were a better predictor of subse-
quent reporting of pain intensity than was their
actual initial experience of pain; in fact, chil-
dren’s memories fully mediated the relationship
between pain reporting at the initial and the
subsequent pain experience. Similar to children
with chronic illness [10] , differences in children’s
memory style also influenced subsequent pain
reporting. Children who developed negatively
estimated, versus accurate or positively esti-
mated, memories of pain intensity and pain-
related fear developed more negative expecta-
tions of future pain and showed greater increases
in pain ratings over time.
Individual vulnerability & protective
factors
Given the potential malleability of children’s
pain memories and the importance of nega-
tive distortion for subsequent pain and distress,
research has moved towards identifying children
who are most at risk for developing negatively
estimated pain memories. Although the rela-
tionship between stress and children’s memories
for autobiographical events is controversial and
marked by inconsistencies across studies [13], in
the context of pain there is a clear link between
children’s level of pain and distress experienced
during a painful event, and the subsequent fram-
ing of their memories. Across a range of medical
procedures and among both clinical and healthy
samples, it has been demonstrated that children
who exhibit greater behaviora l distress and
report higher levels of pain, tend to develop neg-
atively estimated pain memories [10,14] . Chen and
colleagues examined pain memories of children
between the ages of 3 and 18 years who were
diagnosed with cancer and who were required
to repeatedly undergo lumbar punctures as part
of their treatment protocol [10]. Higher distress
at the initial lumbar punctures was associated
with more negatively estimated pain memories,
even when controlling for number of previous
lumbar punctures.
To ensure the generalizability of these find-
ings, Noel and colleagues examined the relation-
ship between venipuncture pain experienced
during routine blood draws and memories for
Pain Manage . (2012) 2(5) future science group
490
REVIEW Noel, Chambers, Petter, McGrath, Klein & Stewart
the somatosensory and affective aspects of the
pain experience [14]. Consistent with previous
research [10], children who reported higher lev-
els of pain intensity during venipuncture later
developed negatively estimated memories of
anxiety. Similarly, children undergoing hepatitis
vaccinations who received standard care (i.e., no
pain management) developed more negatively
estimated memories of pain intensity compared
with children who received pharmacological
(e.g., topical anesthetic) and nonpharmacologi-
cal (e.g., distraction) interventions. These chil-
dren also developed less accurate memories of
anxiety than children who received pharmaco-
logical intervention [30]. Furthermore, children
undergoing dental procedures who had higher
levels of trait anxiety tended to recall more pain
after a delay, suggesting that highly trait anx-
ious children may have developed n egatively
estimated pain memories [15].
Anxiety & children’s pain memories
In light of theoretical [32] and empirical [15] sup-
port for the role of anxiety in the development
of memory biases for threat and pain, Noel and
colleagues recently examined the causal impact
of state anxiety, and the influence of anxiety-
related variables, on the development of healthy
children’s memories for a novel pain experience
[33]. Children were randomly assigned to com-
plete either a laboratory state anxiety induction
task (i.e., anticipation of giving a speech in front
of judges and completing a difficult math task)
or a control task (i.e., anticipation of watching
a nature video) before completing the cold pres-
sor task. Children also completed measures of
anxiety-related variables (state/trait anxiety and
anxiety sensitivity) and provided pain ratings
immediately following the pain task and again
2 weeks later based on their memories. Results
did not reveal memory differences between chil-
dren in the state anxiety induction group and
those in the control group; however, irrespec-
tive of group assignment, state anxiety emerged
as a unique predictor of children’s memories of
pain intensity and pain-related fear, over and
above sex, stable anxiety-related variables, and
initial pain reporting. Moreover, anxiety sensi-
tivity and trait anxiety significantly predicted
children’s memories of the fearful aspect of the
pain experience. These predictive models had
substantial explanatory power accounting for
52 and 35% of the variance in recalled pain
intensit y and pain-related fear, respectively.
This research extends findings yielded among
healthy adults to earlier developmental peri-
ods [34] and further illustrates the importance
of both state anxiet y and trait-level anxiety
variables in influencing the development of
c hildren’s pain memories.
Memory reframing interventions
Given the powerful influence of pain memories
on subsequent pain experience, intervention
efforts should extend beyond the immediate
pain context; however, very few studies have
examined the effectiveness of memory refram-
ing interventions to date. Chen and colleagues
examined the influence of a brief memory
reframing intervention on subsequent distress
during repeated lumbar punctures among chil-
dren diagnosed with leukemia [35] . Children
in the treatment group’s memories were elic-
ited through a memor y inter view designed to
assess biases in children’s recall of threatening
aspects of the lumbar puncture. Subsequently,
a therapist who observed the procedure used
cognitive-behavioral strategies to reframe the
memory based on objective information yielded
during the procedure. Specifically, therapists
assisted children in re-evaluating their reac-
tions by reinforcing their beliefs about their
abilities to use coping strategies (e.g., increasing
their perceived self-efficacy), appraising their
responses to the procedure in a more realistic
manner and increasing the accuracy of their
recall. In this way, the intervention directly tar-
geted and aimed to reduce the catastrophic and
negatively estimated aspects of children’s recall,
and aimed to increase children’s perceptions
of the effectiveness of their coping abilities.
Compared with children in the control group,
those receiving the memory reframing inter-
vention showed significant reductions in dis-
tress at a subsequent lumbar puncture 1 week
later. Similarly, Pickrell and colleagues exam-Similarly, Pickrell and colleagues exam-
ined the effectiveness of a memory reframing
intervention among children (aged 6 –9 years)
undergoing repeated restorative dental treat-
ments involving local anesthesia injections [36].
At 2 weeks following the initial dental treat-
ment, children were randomized to receive
either a memory reframing intervention, which
was designed to positively reframe their memo-
ries of the previous procedure, or an interactive
neutral discussion (i.e., control condition) led
by an inter ventionist. Immediately following
the intervention or control procedure, children
Review & preliminary model of acute pain memory development in childhood REVIEW
future science group www.futuremedicine.com 491
once again underwent a similar dental treat-
ment. Findings revealed that children receiv-
ing the memory reframing intervention, as
compared with children in the control group,
remembered experiencing less pain and fear
than they actually reported at the initial pro-
cedure, reported less fear following injections
over time, and showed improvements in their
behavior (i.e., degree of cooperativeness) from
the first to the second dental treatment.
Given these findings, it is surprising that more
research has not examined the effectiveness of
similar memory reframing interventions for use
with other pediatric populations (e.g., healthy
children and children with anxiety disorders).
Given the powerful role of parents in shaping
children’s pain experience/expression [37–39], as
well as their autobiographical memories [3,40] ,
parent-led memory reframing interventions
may be particularly promising and cost effec-
tive. Moreover, given the frequency with which
parent–child interactions take place in children’s
naturalistic environments, there would be a
wealth of opportunities for verbal interactions
between parents and children aimed at positively
reconstructing pain memories.
In addition to the influence of postevent
information on the reconstruction of children’s
pain memories and subsequent pain experience,
there is a small body of experimental research
suggesting that provision of information prior
to a novel event (e.g., visiting a pretend zoo or
a pirate) can have a significant impact on the
accuracy and completeness of children’s auto-
biographical memories [41–43] . Moreover, there
is theoretical support for the role of informa-
tion provision prior to medical procedures in
promoting more accurate expectations and
schemasbased on children’s previous experi-
ences and memories – as well as optimal emo-
tion regulation and coping [44]. Given that more
accurate pain memories are linked to less pain
and anxiety during subsequent painful experi-
ences [10,11], future research should examine the
influence of pre-event information provision
and children’s pain memories and experiences
over time.
A preliminary model of acute pain
memory development in childhood
The results of the extant research on children’s
memories for pain have important theoreti-
cal and clinical implications and enhance our
understanding of the factors that influence
the development of children’s pain memories
from their inception, their reconstruction over
time, and the manner in which these memo-
ries shape subsequent pain experience. A pre-
liminary empirically and theoretically derived
model of how these factors are hypothesized to
influence pain memories, and how pain memo-
ries influence subsequent pain experiences, is
presented in Figure 1. Consistent with predic-
tions of theories of anxiety and memory biases
[32,45] , children with higher levels of anxiety
and danger-related schemata pertaining to their
general environments and somatic sensations
tend to develop pain memories that are char-
acterized by amplified estimations of sensory
and affective threat over time. Several theories
also predict that these memories develop as a
result of highly anxious children’s attentional
biases toward threatening aspects of the pain-
ful experience [32,46]; however, it is also pos-
sible that there are subtypes of highly anxious
children who differ in their individual atten-
tion style (e.g., hypervigilant versus avoidant
of threat cues) and who may also differ in the
degree to which their memories become nega-
tively estimated over time [47]. It is likely that
in addition to general anxiety-related constructs
(e.g., state/trait anxiety and anxiety sensitiv-
ity), the presence of fear and anxiety that is
specific to pain itself (e.g., fear of pain, pain
anxiety and pain catastrophizing) is strongly
related to memory biases. Furthermore, these
general anxiety-related variables are thought to
be predisposing factors that predict the devel-
opment and exacerbation of pain-specific fear
and anxiety (e.g., fear of pain and pain cata-
strophizing) [48–51] . Highly anxious children are
also likely to be characterized by a perceived
inabilit y to cope with pain [45] , which could
influence the construction and reconstruction
of pain memories at encoding and retrieval, as
well as the development of negative expectancies
of future pain. Indeed, negative memory biases,
such as the anxiety constructs described herein,
are characterized by amplified estimations of
threat; that is, highly anxious children recall
their experience as more painful and fear-induc-
ing than they initially report it to be. Once these
negative memory biases develop, they become
powerful predictors of children’s subsequent
pain experience. It has been suggested that
negative memory biases and pain expectancies
seen among highly anxious and fearful children
may become activated upon re-entry into the
Pain Manage . (2012) 2(5) future science group
492
REVIEW Noel, Chambers, Petter, McGrath, Klein & Stewart
pain context and then further exaggerated as
a result of increased arousal [10]. As a result,
negatively estimated memories and expectan-
cies likely become the experientia l context
through which subsequent pain is experienced.
Not surprisingly then, these children’s reactions
to pain, like their pain memories themselves,
come to involve even more distress over time.
Furthermore, this process likely exacerbates
and perpetuates the vicious cycle of increased
pain-specific anxiety and fear, distress, pain
during painful experiences, additional recon-
struction of negatively estimated pain memo-
ries and expectancies, and heightened subjective
r eactivity to pain over time.
To date, research on children’s memories
for pain has seldom examined the role of par-
ents. Nevertheless, children do not live and
experience pain in isolation; indeed, parents
exert a powerful influence on the socializa-
tion of children’s fear, anxiety, memory and
pain behaviors [3,3 7–4 0,52] . Children’s beliefs
and cognitions about the inherent threat value
of pain develop within the familial context.
Throu gh early vica rious and instrumental
learning about somatic symptoms (including
pain), individuals develop beliefs and cogni-
tions about the dangerousness of pain and the
appropriateness of associated behaviors [53–57] .
Similarly, children’s autobiographical memo-
ries develop within this familial context and
are highly influenced by the quality and content
of their verbal interactions with individuals in
their social environments, particularly parents
[3,40] . Cognitive affective parent factors shown
to increase child pain behaviors and anxiety,
such as parental catastrophizing about child
pain [5 8,59] and parenta l anxiety sensitivit y
[60], likely fuel the complex inter-relationships
between parent and child factors in influencing
the development of pain memories and subse-
quent response to pain. Moreover, the relation-
ships between the psychological characteristics
and behaviors of the parent and those of the
Pain experience
Pain memories
and expectations
Future pain
experience
Pain-specific child
characteristics:
– Fear of pain
– Pain anxiety
– Pain catastrophizing
– Accurate
– Exaggerated
– Minimized
Pain report
Distress
Pain report
Distress
General child
characteristics:
– Anxiety and danger
schemata: anxiety
sensitivity and
trait/state anxiety
– Attentional biases/style
Parent psychological characteristics
and behaviors:
– Parent catastrophizing about child pain
– Parent anxiety sensitivity
– Information provision: pain affirming
and pain denying
Encoding/
reframing
Figure1. A preliminary model of acute pain memory development in childhood outlining the
theoretically and empirically derived relationships between child and parent factors that are
hypothesized to inuence the development of children’s pain memories, expectations and pain
experiences over time.
Review & preliminary model of acute pain memory development in childhood REVIEW
future science group www.futuremedicine.com 493
child are likely reciprocal [61,62]; as such, par-
ents can exert influence at every stage in the
cycle. Accordingly, they have the potential to be
critical agents in intervention efforts aimed at
reducing children’s anxiety before, and fear and
pain during, painful experiences. Furthermore,
they have the potential to influence the refram-
ing of children’s pain memories and modifi-
cation of expectancies through parent–child
discourse following painful experiences.
Conclusion & future perspective
Researching children’s memories is not without
its methodological challenges (e.g., difficulty
disentangling memory of pain experience from
memory of pain reporting, necessity of reliance
on self-report and lack of control over factors
influencing memory reconstruction over time);
however, the importance of understanding the
development of pain memories and their influ-
ence on children’s experience of pain over time
cannot be overstated. The recent development
of child and parent report measures of pain-
related fear and anxiet y [48,63 ,6 4] will enable
more refined examinations of the role of anxiety
and fear in the development of children’s pain
memories. The development of the pediatric
fear-avoidance model of chronic pain [62]an
adaptation of Vlaeyen and Linton’s model to
explain the processes by which pain transitions
from an acute to a chronic state [65]provides
intriguing opportunities to examine the role
of cognitive and affective fear-avoidance fac-
tors in both the child’s pain experience and the
development of their pain memories over time.
Furthermore, given that the mediating role of
attention is at the core of theories put forth to
account for the relationship between anxiety
and memory biases [32], inclusion of measures
of attention in future investigations is war-
ranted. Finally, multi method investigations of
the reciprocal influences of parent and child
psychological factors in the development of
pain memories and coping over time will offer
valuable insights that will facilitate the ultimate
goal of improved pain management for chil-
dren. A lthough intriguing questions about the
nature of children’s pain memory development
exist, and research is needed to disentangle its
inherent complexity, such future research offers
promise for improved identification, assessment
and treatment of children at risk of develop-
ing pain memories that can have important
i mplications for their health across the lifespan.
Acknowledgements
Parts of this review paper are derived from unpublished
portions of the first author’s doctoral dissertation entitled,
‘Children’s memory for pain : experimental investigations
of the role of anxiety in children’s pain memories and the
influence of pain memories on subsequent pain experience’.
The authors greatly appreciate the generosity of the anony-
mous reviewer who made important contributions to the
development of the preliminary model. The authors would
also like to thank K Birnie and K Boerner for their valu-
able insights into the model. Finally, the authors would
like to sincerely thank the children and families who took
part in this dissertation research and inspired these ideas
and writing.
Financial & competing interests disclosure
M Noel and M Petter are Killam Scholars and are sup-
ported by Frederick Banting and Charles Best Canada
Gradua te S ch ol arships Doctoral Awards f rom the
Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR ). At the
time that this work was completed, M Noel was also sup-
ported by a CIHR Team in Children’s Pain Fellowship. M
Noel’s dissertation research was supported by a Marion and
Donald Routh Student Research Grant f rom the Society
of Pedia tric Psyc holog y (D ivision 54, Ame rican
Psychological Association), a Canadian Pain Society
Trainee Research Award from the Canadian Pain Society,
and a Category A Research Grant from the I WK Health
Centre awarded to M Noel. Funding for this research was
also provided by an operating grant from the CIHR and
infrastructure funding from the Canada Foundation for
Innovation awarded to CT Chambers. M Noel and M
Petter are trainee members of Pain in Child Health, a
Strategic Training Initiative in Health Research of CIHR.
CT Chambers and PJ McGrath are supported by Canada
Research Chairs. SH Stewart was supported through a
Killam Research P ro fessorship from the D alhousie
University Faculty of Science at the time this research was
conducted. The authors have no other relevant affiliations
or financial involvement with any organization or entity
with a financial interest in or financial conflict with the
subject matter or materials discussed in the manuscript
apart f rom those disclosed.
No writing assistance was utilized in the production of
this manuscript.
Ethical conduct of research
The authors state that they have obtained appropriate insti-
tutional review board approval or have followed the princi-
ples outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki for all human
or animal experimental investigations that they conducted
themselves. In addition, for investi gations involving human
subjects that they themselves conducted, informed consent
has been obtained from the participants involved.
Pain Manage . (2012) 2(5) future science group
494
REVIEW Noel, Chambers, Petter, McGrath, Klein & Stewart
References
Papers of special note have been highlighted as:
 of interest
 of considerable interest
1 Swafford LI, Allan D. Pain relief in the
pediatric patient. Med. Clin. North Am. 52,
131–136 (1968).
2 Schechter NL . The u nder treatment of pain
in children : an overview. Pediatr. Clin. North
Am. 36, 781–794 (1989).
3 Bauer PJ, L arkina M, Deocampo J. E arly
memory development. In: The Handbook of
Childhood Cognitive Development (Second
Edition). Goswami U (Ed.). Wiley-
Black well, NJ, USA , 153–179 (2010).
4 Taddio Q, Katz J, Ilersich A L, Koren G.
Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain
response during subsequent routine
vaccination. Lancet 349, 599–603 (1997).
 Seminal resea rch documenting the impact
of inadequately ma naged pain in t he first
days of life on subsequent distress at a
vaccination 4– 6 months later.
5 Taddio A, Shah V, Gilbert-MacLeod C ,
Katz J. Conditioning and hy pera lgesia in
newborns exposed to repeated heel lances.
JAMA 288, 857–861 (2002).
 Demonstrates that infants who expressed
more pain during repeated heel lances in
the first days of life ex hibited greater pa in
intensity prior to (i.e., during skin
preparation and cleansing) and du ring a
subsequent painful procedure, thereby
raising the possibilit y that infants
developed some type of implicit memory of
the pa in that shaped their subsequent
reactions to pain.
6 von Baeyer CL, Marche TA, Rocha EM,
Salmon K. Children’s memory for pain:
overview and implications for practice.
J. Pain 5, 241–249 (2004).
 Provides an excel lent overview of the
literature on children’s memory for pain,
emphasizing the consequences of children’s
pain memories for subsequent pain and
healthcare behaviors, exploring individual
differenc es as well a s situational and
methodological influences in children’s
reca ll, a nd highlighting avenues for clinical
inter vention.
7 Ornstein PA, Ma nning EL, Pelphrey KA.
Children’s memory for pain. J. Dev. Behav.
Pediatr. 20, 262–277 (1999).
 First revie w on children’s memory for pain
wherei n the authors provided an
information-processing
framework – informed by literature on
children’s autobiographical memor y
development – within which the
acquisition, retention and distortion of pain
information could be understood.
8 Flor H, Birbaumer N. Acqu isition of chronic
pain : psychophysiological mechanisms.
APS J. 3(2), 119–127 (1994).
9 Pate JT, Blount R L, Cohen LL, Smith
AJ. Childhood medical experience and
temperament as predictors of adult
functioning in medica l situations. Child
Health Care. 25 (4), 281–298 (1996).
10 Chen E, Zeltzer LK, Craske MG, K atz ER.
Children’s memories for painful ca ncer
treatment procedures: implications for
distress. Child Dev. 71(4), 933–947 (2000).
 First study to document that distress during
lumbar puncture influenced t he
development of negatively estimated pa in
memories among children with chronic
illness. Importantly, the study demonstrated
that children who developed negatively
estimated pain memories showed increases
in distress during subsequent lumba r
punct ures.
11 Noel M, Chambers CT, McGrath PJ, K lein
RM, Stewart SH. The impact of children’s
pain memories on subsequent pain
experience. Pain , 153, 1563–1572 (2012).
 This study extended models of memory a nd
subsequent pain reporting from adulthood
to earlier developmental periods and
demonstrated that healthy children’s pain
memories mediated t he relationship
between pain reported at an initial and a
subsequent pain experience. It a lso showed
that children who developed negatively
estimated pain memories showed greater
increases in pain during a subsequent pain
experience.
12 IASP Task Force on Taxonomy. Classification
of Chronic Pain (Second Edition). IASP Press,
WA, USA (1994).
13 Peterson C, Warren LK. Injuries, emergency
rooms, and children’s memory: factors
contributing to individual differences. In:
Emotion and Memory in Development:
Biological , Cognitive, and Social Considerations.
Quas J, Fivush R (Eds). Oxford University
Press, NY, USA, 60–85 (2009).
14 Noel M, McMurtry CM, Chambers CT,
McGrat h PJ. Chi ldren’s memor y for painf ul
procedures : the relationship of pain intensit y,
anxiety, and adult behaviors to subsequent
recall. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 35, 626– 636
(2010).
15 Rocha EM, Ma rche TA, von Baeyer CL .
Anxiety influences children’s memory for
procedural pain. Pain Res. Manag. 14(3),
233–237 (2009).
16 Gedne y JJ, Logan H. Pain related reca ll
predicts future pain report. Pain 121, 69–76
(2006).
17 Fivush R, Haden CA, Reese E. Elaborating
on elaborations: role of maternal reminiscing
style in cognitive and socioemotional
development. Child Dev. 77(6 ), 1568–1588
(2006).
18 Everts B, Karlson B, Währborg P, Abdon N,
Herlitz J, Hedner T. Pain recollection af ter
chest pain of card iac origin. Cardiology 92,
115 –120 (1999).
19 Gedne y JJ, Logan H, Baron RS. Predictors of
short-term and long-term memory of sensor y
and affective dimensions of pain. J. Pain 4,
47–55 (2003).
20 Kent G. Memory of dental experiences as
related to naturally occurring changes in
state a nxiety. Cog n. Emot. 3 (1), 45–53
(1989).
21 Howe ML . Children’s memor y for traumatic
experiences. Learn Individ. Differ. 9, 153–174
(1997).
22 Deffenbacher KA, Bornstein BH, Penrod
SD, McGorty EK . A meta-analytic rev iew of
the ef fects of high stress on eyewitness
memory. Law Human Behav. 28(6), 687–706
(2004).
23 Courage M L, Howe ML . Autobiographical
memory: individual differences and
developmenta l course. In: Handbook of
Individual Differences in Cognition :
Attention , Memory, and Cognitive Control .
Gruszka A, Matthe ws G, Szymura B (Eds).
Springer-Verlag, N Y, USA, 403–417
(2010).
24 Bauer PJ. Remembering the Times of our Lives:
Memory in Infancy and Beyond. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, NJ, USA (20 07).
25 Sjöberg RL, Lindholm T. A systematic
review of age-related errors in children’s
memories for voiding c ystourethrograms
(VCUG ). Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 14,
104–105 (2005).
26 Qua s JA , Goodman GS, Bidrose S,
Pipe ME, Craw S, Ablin DS. Emotion and
memor y: children’s long-term rememberi ng,
forgett ing, a nd suggest ibility. J. Exp. Chil d
Psychol . 72 , 235 –270 (1999).
27 Bada li MA, Pillai RR, Craig KD, Giesbrecht
K, Cha mbers CT. Accuracy of children’s and
parent’s memory for a novel pa inful
experience. Pain Res. Manag. 5, 161–168
(200 0).
future science group www.futuremedicine.com 495
Review & preliminary model of acute pain memory development in childhood REVIEW
28 Lander J, Hodgins M, Fowler-Kerry S.
Children’s pain predictions a nd memories.
Behav. Res. Ther. 30, 117–124 (1992).
29 Zonneveld LN, McGrath PJ, Reid GJ, Sorbi
MJ. Accuracy of ch ildren’s pain memories.
Pain 71, 297–302 (1997).
30 Cohen LL, Blount RL, Cohen RJ, Ba ll CM,
McClellan CB, Bernard R S. Children’s
expectations and memories of acute distress.
Short- and long-term efficacy of pain
mana gement interventions. J. Pediatr.
Psychol. 26 (6), 367–374 (2001).
31 Bruck M, Ceci SJ, Francoeur E , Barr R. “I
hard ly cried when I got my shot!”
Influencing children’s reports about a visit to
their pediatrician. Chil d Dev. 66, 193–208
(1995).
32 Beck AT, Clark DA. A n information
processing model of a nxiety: automatic and
strategic processes. Behav. Res. Ther. 35(1),
49–58 (1997).
33 Noel M, Chambers CT, McGrath PJ, K lein
RM, Stewart SH. The role of state a nxiety on
children’s memories for pain. J. Pediatr.
Psychol. 37(5), 567–579 (2012).
 Demonstrates the influence of state/tra it
anxiety and a nx iety sensit ivit y on the
development of children’s memories for the
somatosensory and affective aspects of a
novel pa in experience and presented models
of memor y development t hat had
substantial ex planatory power. Importantly,
the research highlighted the import ance of
anxiety as an individual difference variable
that may help to identify children most at
risk for developing negatively estimated
pain memories.
34 Gedne y JJ, Logan H. Memory for stress-
associated acute pain. J. Pain 5(2), 83–91
(2004).
35 Chen E, Zeltzer LK, Craske MG, K atz ER.
Alteration of memory in the reduction of
children’s dist ress during repeated aversive
medical procedures. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
67(4), 481–490 (1999).
 First study to exa mine and demonstrate the
effectiveness of a brief memory reframing
inter vention on subsequent distress.
Children with leukemia who received the
inter vention following lumbar puncture
showed less distress at a subsequent lumbar
punct ure t han children in the control
condit ion.
36 Pickrell JE , Heima M, Weinstein P et al.
Using memory re structuring strategy to
enhance dental behaviour. Int. J. Paediatr.
Dent. 17, 439 448 (2007).
37 Chambers CT, Cra ig K D, Bennett SM. The
impact of maternal behavior on children’s
pain experiences : an experimental ana lysis.
J. Pediatr. Psychol . 27, 293–301 (20 02).
38 Craig KD. T he social communications
model of pain. Can . Psychol. 50, 22–32
(2009).
39 McMurtry CM, Cha mbers CT, McGrath PJ,
Asp E . When “don’t worr y” com munic ates
fear: children’s perceptions of parental
reassurance and distraction during a painf ul
medical procedure. Pain 150, 52–58 (2010).
40 Peterson C, Sales JM, Rees M, Fivush R.
Parent– child talk and child ren’s memory for
stressful events. Appl . Cognitive Psych. 21,
1057–1075 (2007).
41 McGuigan F, Salmon K. Pre-e vent discussion
and recall of a novel event: how are children
best prepared? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 91,
342–366 (2005).
42 Sutherland R, Pipe ME , Schick K, Murray J,
Gobbo C. Knowing in advance: the impact
of prior event information on memor y and
event knowledge. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 84,
244–263 (20 03).
43 Salmon K, Mewton L, Pipe ME, McDonald
S. Asking parents to prepare children for an
event: altering parental instructions
influences children’s recall. J. Cogn. Dev. 12,
80–102 (2011).
44 Jaaniste T, Hayes B, von Baeyer CL.
Providing children with information about
forthcoming medical procedures: a review
and synthesis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Prac. 14,
124–143 (2007).
45 Beck AT, Emer y G, Greenberg RL. Anxiety
Disorders and Phobias: A Cognitive
Perspective. Basic Books, NY, USA (1985).
46 Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Sa ntos R, Calvo
MG. An xiet y and cognitive performance :
attentional control t heory. Emotion 7(2),
336–353 (2007).
47 Noel M, Taylor TL, Quinlan CK, Stewart
SH. The impact of attention style on directed
forget ting among high a nxiety sensitive
individuals. Cognit. Ther. Res. 36, 375–389
(2012).
48 Huguet A, McGrath PJ, Pardos
J. Development and preliminary testing of a
scale to assess pain-related fear in children
and adolescents. J. Pain 12, 840– 848 (2011).
49 Pagé MG, Campbell F, Isaac L, Stinson J,
Martin-Pichora AL, Katz J. Reliability and
validity of the Child Pain Anxiet y Symptoms
Scale (CPASS) in a clinical sample of
children and adolescents with acute
postsurgical pain. Pain 152, 1958–1965
(2011).
50 Tsao JCI, Allen LB, Evans S, Lu Q,
Myers CD, Zeltzer LK. An xiety sensitivit y
and c ata strophizing associations with
pain and somat ization in non- clinical
children. J. Health Psychol. 14, 1085–1094
(2009).
51 Vervoort T, Eccleston C, Goubert L, Buysse
A, Crombez G. Children’s catastrophic
thinking about their pain predicts pain and
disability 6 months later. Eur. J. Pain 14,
90–96 (2010).
52 Gerull FC, Rapee MM. Mother k nows best:
effects of maternal modeling on the
acquisition of fear a nd avoidance behaviour
in todd lers. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 279–287
(2002 ).
53 Watt MC, Stewart SH. A nxiety sensitivity
mediates the relationships between childhood
learning experiences and elevated
hypochondriacal concerns in young
adulthood. J. Psychosom. Res. 48, 1–12
(200 0).
54 Stewart SH, Taylor S, Jang K L et al. Causal
model ing of relations among learning
history, anxiety sensitivity, and pan ic
attacks. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 443 –456
(2001).
55 Watt MC, O’Connor R M, Stewart SH,
Moon EC, Terry L. Specificit y of childhood
learning experiences in relation to anxiety
sensitivity and il lness/injur y sensitivity:
implications for hea lth anx iety and pain.
J. Cognit. Psychother. 22, 128–142 (2008).
56 Watt MC, Stewart SH, Moon E, Terry LL.
Childhood learning history origins of adult
pain anxiety. J. Cognit. Psychother. 24,
198–212 (2010).
57 Watt MC, Stewart SH, Cox BJ. A
retrospective study of the learning histor y
origins of anxiety sensitivity. Behav. Res.
Ther. 36, 505–525 (1998).
58 Goubert L, E ccleston C, Vervoort T,
Jordan A, Crombez G. Pa rental
catastrophizing about their child ’s pain. The
parent version of the Pain Cata strophizing
Scale (PCS-P): a preliminary validation. Pain
123, 254–263 (2006).
59 Caes L, Vervoort T, Eccleston C,
Vandenhende M, Goubert L . Parental
catastrophizing about child ’s pain and its
relationship with activity restriction: the
mediating role of parental distress. Pain 152,
212–222 (2011).
60 Tsao JCI, Lu Q, Myers CD, Kim SC, Turk
N, Zeltzer LK. Parent and child anxiety
sensitivity: relationship to children’s
experimental pain responsivity. J. Pain 7(5),
319–326 (200 6).
Pain Manage . (2012) 2(5) future science group
496
REVIEW Noel, Chambers, Petter, McGrath, Klein & Stewart
61 Palermo TM, Chambers CT. Parent and
family factors in pediatric chronic pain and
disability: an integrative approach. Pain 119,
1–4 (2005).
62 Asmundson GJG, Noel M, Pet ter M,
Parkerson H. Pediatric fear-avoidance model
of chronic pain: foundation, application, and
future directions. Pain Res. Manag.
(In Press).
63 Pagé MG, Fuss S, Mart in AL, Escoba r EMR,
Katz J. Development and preliminary
validation of the Child Pain An xiet y
Symptoms Scale in a communit y sample.
J. Pediatr. Psychol. 35, 1071–1082 (2010).
64 Simons L E, Sieberg CB, Carpino E, Logan
D, Berde C. The Fear of Pain Questionnaire
(FOPQ ). Assessment of pa in-related fear
among children and adolescents w ith chronic
pain. J. Pain 12, 677–686 (2011).
65 Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and
its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal
pain: a state of the a rt. Pain 85, 317–332
(2000).
Website
101 Sun-Ok S, Carr D. Pain and memory.
Internationa l Association for the Study of
Pain. Pa in: Clinical Updates 7, 1–4 (1999).
ww w.iasp-pain.org/A M/AMTemplate.cfm ?S
ection=Home& SECTION=Home &CONT
ENTID=7640 &TE MPLATE =/CM/
ContentDisplay.cfm
future science group www.futuremedicine.com 497
Review & preliminary model of acute pain memory development in childhood REVIEW
... pain intensity) and affective components (i.e. pain-related fear/affect; Noel, Chambers, Petter, et al., 2012;Ornstein et al., 1999). As such, our results are in line with the findings of the study of Marche et al. (2016), who observed that children who were better able to forget the negative aspects of their pain experience, expected less anxiety from the upcoming CPT, but not less pain intensity. ...
... As such, our results are in line with the findings of the study of Marche et al. (2016), who observed that children who were better able to forget the negative aspects of their pain experience, expected less anxiety from the upcoming CPT, but not less pain intensity. Results can be interpreted within the model of acute pain memory development of Noel, Chambers, Petter, et al. (2012), which posits that especially children showing more anxiety and threat-related schema related to the pain are prone to encode their pain memories as more threatening/negative. Subsequently, they develop more threatening expectancies and greater distress during future pain experiences. ...
... Drawing upon the model of Noel, Chambers, Petter, et al. (2012), earlier research indicating the implication of certain child variables in the encoding of pain memories (Marche et al., 2016;Noel et al., 2015;Wauters et al., 2020;Wauters et al., 2021;Wauters et al., 2022), and the finding of Marche et al. (2016) that not all children showed the same ability to forget, it was investigated whether children's attention bias, attention-switching ability and pain catastrophizing could predict their ability to forget the negative details of a previous pain experience. Contrary to expectations, children's attention bias to pain did not predict their ability to forget. ...
Article
Background Children's inability to forget the negative aspects of a painful event is associated with more anticipatory anxiety at an upcoming pain task and lower pain thresholds; however, the impact of forgetting on children's pain outcomes has not been examined. Retrieval‐Induced Forgetting (RIF) was experimentally induced to investigate whether children would (1) forget more negative details of a previous painful autobiographic event and; (2) report better pain‐related outcomes for an unrelated pain task (i.e., cold pressor task; CPT). Additionally, it was investigated whether the success of RIF was dependent on child characteristics known to influence children's memories for pain (i.e., attention bias to pain, attention switching ability and pain catastrophizing). Methods Healthy school children ( N = 128; 9–16 years old) recalled and rehearsed memory details of two painful autobiographical events, while only children in the randomized RIF group rehearsed positive details. All children underwent two CPTs (before and after RIF) and reported pain‐related outcomes. Two weeks later, children recalled CPT pain and reported on future pain expectancies. Results Children in the RIF group remembered less negative details of their past autobiographical pain events, but also reported a greater reduction in pain‐related fear from the CPT 2 compared to their ratings for CPT 1, than children in the control group. They furthermore expected less pain‐related fear 2 weeks later for a future pain task. Discussion Findings suggest that RIF is a promising avenue in pediatric pain management that could be harnessed to foster more positive memories and better future pain experiences. Significance Statement Retrieval‐induced forgetting (RIF) makes children forget negative details of a past autobiographical pain experience, decreases experienced pain‐related fear for experimental pain and lowers future pain‐related fear expectancies. Results show a promising role for RIF‐ based memory interventions in the context of paediatric pain care.
... Moreover, children experiencing repeated painful procedures and conditions are at special risk since memories of previous experiences play a crucial role in managing future painful events. Whereby children with previous experiences of under or untreated pain have increased risk of experiencing psychological negative consequences and lower pain threshold (McMurtry et al., 2015;Noel, Chambers, Petter, et al., 2012), that might affect the child's response of pain negatively and influence their future health related help seeking behavior and health (Birnie et al., 2018). Hence, in managing procedural treatment and care-related pain in hospitalized children, there is an urgent need to reduce the experiences of pain and distress. ...
... Children often describe needle-related procedures as the worst pain (Friedrichsdorf et al., 2015;Walther-Larsen et al., 2017). Furthermore previous negative experiences may affect the child's response of pain in the current situation (McMurtry et al., 2015;Noel, Chambers, Petter, et al., 2012;Noel et al., 2018;Thompson, 2018). This study shows how the clown accommodates the children's previous experiences by asking the children how they felt during previous experiences, subsequently embracing the children's expression in the WE. ...
Article
Full-text available
Hospital clowns are widely used in pediatric settings. However, the field of pain and pain experience in children is complex and multifaceted. Therefore, the aim of this study was to expand knowledge on the impact and influence of hospital clowns on 4 to 15-year-old children’s experiences of managing painful procedures and conditions. A convergent mixed method study was conducted integrating findings from three individual studies: a prospective non-blinded study and two focused ethnographic studies. The findings illuminated a child—clown interaction characterized by a close relationship evolving during a continuous, mutually focused attention, conceptualized as WE. This WE represent a responsive interaction between child and clown, which supported the child during the procedures, and generated three integrated findings: an age-differentiated approach; understanding the child’s condition; and previous experiences as a part of the child’s history. The WE approach strengthened the child’s competences and ability to manage during painful procedures.
... Negatively biased pain memories (ie, recalling higher levels of pain and fear during a painful event than initially reported) have been theorized and identified as a risk factor for increased pain and distress during future painful experiences. 10,34,36,37,39,41,62 Indeed, how children remember past pain robustly predicts their future pain, anxiety, and distress, 10,17 more so than their actual initial experience of pain itself. 34 Pain memories have furthermore been linked to the development and maintenance of chronic pain. ...
Article
Negatively biased pain memories robustly predict maladaptive pain outcomes in children. Both attention bias to pain and parental narrative style have been linked with the development of these negative biases, with previous studies indicating that how parents talk to their child about the pain might buffer the influence of children's attention bias to pain on the development of such negatively biased pain memories. This study investigated the moderating role of parental narrative style in the relation between pain-related attention and memory biases in a pediatric chronic pain sample who underwent a cold pressor task. Participants were 85 youth-parent dyads who reminisced about youth's painful event. Eye-tracking technology was used to assess youth's attention bias to pain information, whereas youth's pain-related memories were elicited 1 month later through telephone interview. Results indicated that a parental narrative style using less repetitive yes–no questions, more emotion words, and less fear words buffered the influence of high levels of youth's attention bias to pain in the development of negatively biased pain memories. Opposite effects were observed for youth with low levels of attention bias to pain. Current findings corroborate earlier results on parental reminiscing in the context of pain (memories) but stress the importance of matching narrative style with child characteristics, such as child attention bias to pain, in the development of negatively biased pain memories. Future avenues for parent–child reminiscing and clinical implications for pediatric chronic pain are discussed.
Chapter
In this chapter we review the significance of age in pain management, with a particular focus on children and the elderly. Right from birth, our responses to disease-related or external sources of pain differ, necessitating tailored assessments and treatment strategies. Not too long ago, there was a prevailing belief that infants could not experience pain or that, since they wouldn’t remember it, the need for analgesics during painful procedures was diminished. Today, neurophysiological knowledge and clinical expertise confirm that babies possess at birth the neural pathways to process nociceptive inputs. Even if they might not form conscious memories of the pain, there exists a physiological memory that could negatively influence their future pain responses. Elderly individuals are at a higher risk of enduring painful experiences due to age-related physiological degeneration. Furthermore, these physiological changes can impact the response to pharmacological treatments, elevating the likelihood of significant side effects. However, it is essential that elderly individuals receive the same level of pain relief as young adults, even if the adjustments required may be more intricate.
Article
Background: Pain is one of the main causes of distress and suffering in children receiving care. Children who
Article
The current study investigated the influence of children's ability to flexibly shift attention towards and away from pain information on the development of negatively-biased pain memories, thereby employing a direct measure of attention control reliant on behavioral responses in the context of pain (i.e., an attention switching task). Direct influence of children's attention shifting ability and pain catastrophizing as well as the moderating role of this shifting ability in the relationship between pain catastrophizing and the development of negatively-biased pain memories was examined. Healthy school children (N = 41; 9 to 15 years old) received painful heat stimuli and completed measures of state and trait pain catastrophizing. They then performed an attention switching task wherein they had to shift attention between personally relevant pain-related and neutral cues. Two weeks after the painful task, children's pain-related memories were elicited via telephone. Findings indicated that children's reduced ability to disengage attention away from pain information predicted more fear memory bias 2 weeks later. Children's pain-related attention shifting ability did not moderate the relationship between children's pain catastrophizing and negatively-biased pain memories. Findings highlight the contribution of children's attention control skills in the development of negatively-biased pain memories. PERSPECTIVE: Results of the current study indicate that children with a reduced ability to shift attention away from pain information are at risk for developing negatively-biased pain memories. Findings can inform interventions to minimize the development of these maladaptive negatively-biased pain memories by targeting pain-relevant attention control skills in children.
Article
Full-text available
Children remember their memories of pain long after the painful experience is over. Those memories predict higher levels of future pain intensity. Young children’s memories can be reframed to be less distressing. Parents and the way they reminisce about past events with their children play a key role in the formation of pain memories. A novel parent-led memory-reframing intervention changed children’s memories of post-surgical pain to be less distressing. The intervention efficacy in the context of vaccine injections is unclear. This registered randomized controlled trial (NCT05217563) aimed to fill this gap. Seventy-four children aged 4.49 years (SD = 1.05) and scheduled to obtain two COVID-19 vaccine injections and one of their parents were randomized to receive: (1) standard care; (2) standard care and memory-reframing information; and (3) standard care and memory-reframing information with verbal instructions. Children reported their pain after vaccine injections. One week after the first vaccination, children reported memory of pain. Parents reported their use of memory-reframing strategies and intervention feasibility and acceptability. The intervention did not result in significant differences in children’s recalled or future pain. Parents rated the intervention as acceptable and feasible.
Article
Full-text available
The present study sought to reduce children's distress during aversive medical procedures using a brief, cost-effective intervention aimed at reframing memory. Fifty children diagnosed with leukemia (25 treatment, 25 attention control, aged 3–18) were observed as they underwent 3 consecutive lumbar punctures (LPs; baseline, postintervention, and follow-up). Self-report, physiological, and observable distress measures were collected before and after each LP. At posttreatment, children in the intervention group showed reductions in anticipatory physiological and self-report ratings relative to the control group. At follow-up, these effects generalized to reductions in procedural distress. These results suggest that (a) a simple memory-based intervention is efficacious at reducing children's distress and (b) benefits from this intervention are maintained over 1 week even without continued intervention.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To examine the effect of psychologic and pharmacologic interventions on children's expectations and 6-month recollections of painful procedures. Methods: A repeated measures design allowed examination of 22 fourth graders' expectations, experiences, and memories of distress across three conditions (typical care, distraction, topical anesthetic) for a three-injection vaccination series. All participants were African American and from urban, low-income families. Results: Across conditions, children's expectations of distress were significantly higher than their experience of distress. Distress ratings did not differ among conditions prior to or immediately following the injections; however, children later recalled that the treatment conditions were superior to control for distress relief. Analyses of recall accuracy suggest that the interventions buffered the children from forming negative recollections that occurred with typical care. Conclusions: Children have negative expectations prior to a procedure despite knowing that a distress management intervention will be employed. However, interventions may thwart the development of negative memories of distress.
Article
Full-text available
Despite recent progress in understanding memory of pain in adults, the validity of the assumption that these findings extend to children has not been established. Because treatment often is evaluated on the basis of pain recall, it is crucial that the accuracy of pain memories in children be established. OBJECTIVES: To examine children's ability to recall pain intensity and contextual details associated with a novel painful experience. Furthermore, children's memories were compared with those of their parents. SUBJECTS AND METHODS: Twenty-three parent-child dyads were recruited from a sample participating in an earlier study investigating children's responses to the cold pressor test and parents' characterization of the children's responses. Children (age five to 12 years) and parents independently had rated the child's pain using a seven-point Faces Pain Scale. Approximately one year later, they were asked to recall the experience and rate the pain again. Memory for contextual details associated with the event also was assessed through a series of open-ended questions. RESULTS: A 2 (rater) × 2 (time period) repeated measures ANOVA examined the reliability of ratings and differences between parent and child ratings of pain, both recorded at the time of the cold pressor and recalled one year later. There were no significant differences in ratings over time or between parent and child. CONCLUSIONS: The findings indicate that children's memories of a novel painful experience can be consistent over a long interval. Moreover, parents' ratings also reflect good recall and can be in agreement with those of their children.
Book
The purpose of Remembering the Times of Our Lives: Memory in Infancy and Beyond is to trace the development from infancy through adulthood in the capacity to form, retain, and later retrieve autobiographical or personal memories.
Article
Context Hospitalized infants undergo repeated invasive procedures. It is unknown whether cumulative experiences with pain lead to anticipatory pain behaviors and hyperalgesia.Objectives To determine whether newborns who are born to mothers with diabetes and undergo repeated pain learn to anticipate pain and exhibit more pain during a painful procedure than normal infants.Design, Setting, and Participants Prospective cohort study of 21 full-term newborns born to mothers with diabetes and 21 born to mothers with uneventful pregnancies, at a university teaching hospital between August 1999 and October 2000. Infants of diabetic mothers underwent repeated heel lances in the first 24 to 36 hours of life for monitoring of blood glucose concentrations. Pain responses of all infants undergoing a venipuncture on the dorsum of the hand to obtain blood for the newborn screening test after the first day of life were compared. In addition, from September through November 2001, 12 infants of diabetic mothers and 12 normal infants were compared for pain reactions to intramuscular vitamin K injection after birth.Main Outcome Measures Percentages of time observed grimacing and crying and visual analog scale (VAS) scores.Results Raters were blinded to exposure group. Median baseline scores for grimacing, crying time, and VAS did not differ significantly between groups (P = .27, P = .32, and P = .32, respectively). Median scores (interquartile range) for grimacing during skin cleansing were higher in infants of diabetic mothers (22.2% [77.5%] vs 0% [15%]; P = .03). The VAS scores for both groups were zero, but the distribution of the scores was significantly different (86% of normal infants vs 52% of infants of diabetic mothers had scores of zero) (P = .04). During venipuncture, infants of diabetic mothers had higher median scores for grimacing (81.7% [32.5%] vs 40% [73.4%]; P = .01), VAS (69% [27.5%] vs 5% [60.5%]; P = .002), and crying (40.2% [77%] vs 0% [54.8%]; P = .03) compared with normal infants. There were no differences between groups on any pain measure in response to intramuscular injection.Conclusions Newborns who had diabetic mothers and were exposed to repeated heel lances in the first 24 to 36 hours of life learned to anticipate pain and exhibited more intense pain responses during venipuncture than normal infants.
Article
In a study designed to explore the relevance of anxious mood on recall, patients about to see their dentist were asked to complete questionnaires measuring their anxiety before and after the appointment. The patients were also asked to describe a previous visit, but half were given this task before the appointment—when their state anxiety was high—and half afterwards—when their state anxiety had declined. Female patients rated the recalled experience as being significantly more unpleasant before the appointment than afterwards, a result not shown by the males. A judge's ratings of the descriptions indicated similar effects for vividness, but not for procedures, sensations or dentist's behaviour. The results support the notion that anxious mood can affect the description of previous experiences.
Article
Physical symptoms are a common cause of attendance at general hospital out-patient clinics. There is good evidence that cognitive therapy is effective in the management of such physical symptoms. This narrative review suggests that the assessment itself, without formal psychological therapy, may be used as a treatment, regardless of whether relevant pathology is absent or present. Changing patients' beliefs about their symptoms may improve a broad range of outcomes, including symptoms, disability, distress, and health-care resource use. The evidence for investigations as treatment is reviewed, along with potential for further development and possible pitfalls. A rationale is presented for a brief psychoeducational intervention that can be delivered in the clinic. This would be a logical extension of the kind of simple explanation and reassurance that occurs routinely today, but which is not explicitly used as, or regarded as, treatment. The dearth of relevant evidence is emphasized, and recommendations are made for future research.