ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Anthropogenic (man-made) noise has changed the acoustic environment both on land and underwater and is now recognized as a pollutant of international concern. Increasing numbers of studies are assessing how noise pollution affects animals across a range of scales, from individuals to communities, but the topic receiving the most research attention has been acoustic communication. Although there is now an extensive literature on how signalers might avoid potential masking from anthropogenic noise, the vast majority of the work has been conducted on birds and marine mammals. Fish represent more than half of all vertebrate species, are a valuable and increasingly utilized model taxa for understanding behavior, and provide the primary source of protein for >1 billion people and the principal livelihoods for hunderds of millions. Assessing the impacts of noise on fish is therefore of clear biological, ecological, and societal importance. Here, we begin by indicating why acoustic communication in fish is likely to be impacted by anthropogenic noise. We then use studies from other taxa to outline 5 main ways in which animals can alter their acoustic signaling behavior when there is potential masking due to anthropogenic noise and assess evidence of evolutionary adaptation and behavioral plasticity in response to abiotic and biotic noise sources to consider whether such changes are feasible in fish. Finally, we suggest directions for future study of fish acoustic behavior in this context and highlight why such research may allow important advances in our general understanding of the impact of this global pollutant.
© The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of
the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For
permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com
The ocial journal of the
ISBE
International Society for Behavioral Ecology
Behavioral Ecology (2014), 25(5), 1022–1030. doi:10.1093/beheco/aru029
Behavioral
Ecology
The ocial journal of the
ISBE
International Society for Behavioral Ecology
Behavioral
Ecology
Invited Review
Acoustic communication in a noisy world: can
fish compete with anthropogenicnoise?
Andrew N.Radford,a EmmaKerridge,a and Stephen D.Simpsonb
aSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol BS8 1UG, UK and bBiosciences,
College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Stocker Road, Exeter EX4 4QD, UK
Received 15 September 2013; revised 24 January 2014; accepted 12 February 2014; Advance Access publication 11 March 2014.
Anthropogenic (man-made) noise has changed the acoustic environment both on land and underwater and is now recognized as a
pollutant of international concern. Increasing numbers of studies are assessing how noise pollution affects animals across a range
of scales, from individuals to communities, but the topic receiving the most research attention has been acoustic communication.
Although there is now an extensive literature on how signalers might avoid potential masking from anthropogenic noise, the vast
majority of the work has been conducted on birds and marine mammals. Fish represent more than half of all vertebrate species, are
a valuable and increasingly utilized model taxa for understanding behavior, and provide the primary source of protein for >1 billion
people and the principal livelihoods for hunderds of millions. Assessing the impacts of noise on fish is therefore of clear biological,
ecological, and societal importance. Here, we begin by indicating why acoustic communication in fish is likely to be impacted by
anthropogenic noise. We then use studies from other taxa to outline 5 main ways in which animals can alter their acoustic signaling
behavior when there is potential masking due to anthropogenic noise and assess evidence of evolutionary adaptation and behavioral
plasticity in response to abiotic and biotic noise sources to consider whether such changes are feasible in fish. Finally, we suggest
directions for future study of fish acoustic behavior in this context and highlight why such research may allow important advances in
our general understanding of the impact of this global pollutant.
Key words: acoustic signaling, adaptation, anthropogenic noise, behavioral plasticity, fitness benefits, hearing, masking,
pollution.
INTRODUCTION
Anthropogenic (man-made) noise has changed the acoustic land-
scape of many areas around the globe and is now recognized as
a pollutant of international concern (e.g., inclusion in the US
National Environment Policy Act and the European Commission
Marine Strategy Framework Directive, and as a permanent item on
the International Maritime Organization Marine Environmental
Protection Committee agenda). Noise-generating human activities
in aquatic environments, such as commercial shipping, recreational
boating, pile-driving, seismic exploration, and energy production,
are widespread and occur with increasing frequency (McDonald
et al. 2006; Normandeau Associates 2012). In terrestrial environ-
ments, the prevalence of transportation networks, resource extrac-
tion, and urban development, for example, is similarly greater now
than ever before (Watts etal. 2007; Barber etal. 2009).
In addition to increasing the amount of noise, human activities
often generate sounds that are very dierent from those arising
from natural sources (Hildebrand 2009; Popper and Hastings 2009;
Normandeau Associates 2012). Common and consistent ambient
noises can exert a strong selective influence on the frequencies used
by species to communicate acoustically; adaptation to the acous-
tic environment can include utilization of available “windows” in
the background frequency range (see Brumm and Slabbekoorn
2005; Lugli 2010). Anthropogenic noises often have prominent
frequencies within those naturally occurring windows (McDonald
etal. 2006; Barber et al. 2009) and thus have the potential to dis-
rupt communication eciency. More generally, anthropogenic
noises may dier from abiotic or biotic sounds in such acoustic
characteristics as constancy, rise time, duty cycle, and impulsive-
ness (Hildebrand 2009; Popper and Hastings 2009; Normandeau
Associates 2012). For instance, pile-driving generates high-energy
impulsive sounds, which are characterized by a rapid rise time to
a maximal pressure value followed by a decay period during which
there is gradual reduction in the oscillating maximal and mini-
mal pressure fluctuations. Underwater transmission of explosions
includes an initial shock pulse followed by a succession of oscillat-
ing bubble pulses. Anthropogenic noise, therefore, presents a very
real, and often novel, challenge to animals.
It is well established in humans that anthropogenic noise can
cause physiological, neurological, and endocrinological problems;
cognitive impairment; sleep disruption; and an increased risk of
Address correspondence to A.N. Radford. E-mail: andy.radford@bristol.ac.uk.
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Radford etal. • Acoustic communication in a noisy world
coronary disease (World Health Organization 2011; Le Prell et al.
2012). In the last decade, there has also been a burgeoning research
interest in the potential impacts of noise on nonhuman animals;
a recent survey of the peer-reviewed literature showed that more
than 30% of studies published by the end of 2011 appeared within
that last year alone (Radford et al. 2012; see also Morley et al.
2014). Eects have been demonstrated in a variety of taxonomic
groups across a range of scales, from the physiology and behav-
ior of individuals to changes at the population and community
level (see Tyack 2008; Barber etal. 2009; Slabbekoorn et al. 2010;
Kight and Swaddle 2011 for reviews). Despite the overall breadth
of considered impacts, one topic—acoustic communication—has
dominated research attention; nearly 60% of terrestrial studies, for
example, have considered this aspect of behavior (Radford et al.
2012; Morley etal. 2014).
The most obvious way in which anthropogenic noise can dis-
rupt acoustic communication is through masking, whereby there
is an increase in the threshold for detection or discrimination of
one sound as a consequence of another (Fay and Megela-Simmons
1999; Brumm and Slabbekoorn 2005). Masking can be complete,
when the signal is not detected at all, or partial, when the signal
is detectable by the listener but the content is hard to understand
(Clark et al. 2009). Communication gets more dicult as back-
ground sounds increase for all vertebrates that have been studied,
including birds, marine mammals, fish, and amphibians (see Fay
and Megela-Simmons 1999; Brumm and Slabbekoorn 2005; Clark
etal. 2009; Dooling etal. 2009). Individual fitness can consequently
be compromised, either through eects on survival, if say signals
indicating a predation threat are unheard or altered (Lowry et al.
2012), or as a result of impacts on reproductive success, arising
from incorrect assessment of the quality of rivals or potential mates
(Halfwerk etal. 2011) or from disrupted communication between
parents and ospring (Leonard and Horn 2012). Numerous studies
have, therefore, investigated how eective acoustic communication
can be maintained despite rising levels of anthropogenic noise; in
particular, research has focused on how signalers may enhance the
likelihood of being heard and of conveying their intended message
accurately.
The vast majority of the work examining how anthropogenic
noise aects acoustic signaling behavior has been on birds and
marine mammals (e.g., Miller et al. 2000; Slabbekoorn and Peet
2003; but see Sun and Narins 2005; Rabin et al. 2006; Lampe
et al. 2012 for exceptions). Although it has become increasingly
apparent in recent decades that many fish species also communi-
cate acoustically (Ladich etal. 2006) and that noise has the poten-
tial to alter the likelihood of detection of these signals (Amoser
et al. 2004; Vasconcelos et al. 2007), the ways in which the
acoustic behavior of this taxonomic group is aected by anthro-
pogenic noise has received virtually no direct empirical attention
(see Picciulin et al. 2012 for an exception). Fishes represent more
than half of all vertebrate species, possess a broad range of hear-
ing and sound-production mechanisms, and exhibit a diverse array
of vocal, reproductive, and social traits (Bone and Moore 2008).
Consequently, fish are a valuable, and increasingly utilized, model
taxa for understanding behavior. Additionally, fish provide the pri-
mary source of protein for >1 billion people and the principal live-
lihoods for hunderds of millions (FAO 2012). Because the majority
of fish species live in coastal or freshwater environments, they are
exposed to many forms of anthropogenic noise. Therefore, assess-
ing the impacts of noise on fish is of clear biological, ecological,
and societal importance.
Here, we begin with brief overviews of acoustic communication
in fish and of the evidence that fish can indeed be aected by noise
generated from human activities (full reviews of these topics are
provided elsewhere). Combined, these bodies of work suggest that
fish acoustic communication is likely to be disrupted by anthropo-
genic noise. We then use studies on other taxa to outline 5 main
ways in which animals can alter their acoustic signaling behavior
when there is potential masking due to anthropogenic noise and
use evidence of evolutionary adaptation and behavioral plasticity
in response to abiotic and biotic noise sources to consider whether
such changes are feasible in fish. Finally, we suggest directions for
future study of fish acoustic behavior in this context and highlight
why such research may allow important advances in our general
understanding of the impact of this global pollutant.
WHY MIGHT THE IMPACTS OF
ANTHROPOGENIC NOISE ON ACOUSTIC
COMMUNICATION BE OF RELEVANCE TO
FISH?
Acoustic communication infish
More than 800 species of fish from over 100 families have been
documented to produce sounds, and many more are likely to do
so (detailed reviews in Tavolga 1971; Myrberg 1981; Hawkins and
Myrberg 1983; Ladich et al. 2006; Bass and Ladich 2008). As in
other taxa, acoustic characteristics of the sounds produced can
vary considerably between species and populations, in relation
to gender and size, and with fluctuations in motivation (Hawkins
and Rasmussen 1978; Myrberg et al. 1993; Parmentier et al.
2005; Verzijden etal. 2010). Sounds generated by fishes, therefore,
provide valuable information in a variety of dierent contexts,
including during territorial disputes and competition for food, pred-
atory attacks, courtship interactions, and spawning aggregations
(Myrberg et al. 1986; Hawkins and Amorim 2000; Amorim and
Neves 2008). Consequently, there is mounting evidence that acous-
tic communication can aect the survival and reproductive success
of fish (Rowe etal. 2008; Verzijden etal. 2010).
Fishes produce sounds in many varied ways, but they can be
broadly divided into those that arise incidentally from another
activity and those generated, often by specialized organs or struc-
tures, for communication (Tavolga 1971; Bass and Ladich 2008).
Unspecialized sounds include those resulting from feeding, move-
ment, or respiration. It is unlikely that these incidental sounds are
either under selection pressure or can be controlled flexibly by the
individual and thus they probably do not adapt across evolution-
ary time or exhibit plasticity in response to noise. Actively produced
acoustic signals include stridulation, drumming, and stringing.
Stridulation involves the rubbing together of mobile bony elements
such as teeth, jaws, fin rays, and vertebrae. For example, when
damselfish and clownfish open and close their mouths, bringing
into contact their pharyngeal teeth, sounds described as pops and
chirps are the result (Parmentier et al. 2007). Drumming sounds
arise from the high-frequency contraction and relaxation of sonic
muscles, which induces vibrations of the swim bladder wall. Cod
(Gadus morhua) and haddock (Melanogrammus aeglefinus), for instance,
use this mechanism, with their sounds described as knocks and
grunts (Hawkins and Rasmussen 1978). Some species, such as
croaking gouramis (genus Trichopsis), also generate sound through
vibrations of tendons within the pectoral fins (Henglmuller and
Ladich 1999). It is more plausible to expect that anthropogenic
1023
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
noise might result in changes in these specialized types of sounds
generated for communicatory purposes.
In general, sounds produced by fish for communication are
made up of pulses (Winn 1964; Myrberg and Spires 1972; Bass
and Ladich 2008). The wide interspecific diversity in sound char-
acteristics will influence the likelihood of a given species being
able to respond to noise created by human activities (see Francis
et al. 2011). Moreover, the type and extent of intraspecific varia-
tion will determine the capacity to minimize masking arising as a
consequence of anthropogenic noise. Although fish sounds most
commonly vary in temporal patterning, with receivers usually
extracting information from pulse number, duration, and rep-
etition rate, interindividual dierences in fundamental and domi-
nant frequency, bandwidth, harmonic structure, and amplitude
ratio have also been documented in various species (Hawkins and
Rasmussen 1978; Myrberg et al. 1993; Bass and Ladich 2008).
Thus, there is certainly the basis for adaptation across evolutionary
time. Intraindividual variation in acoustic structure is also known
to occur in a range of fishes, with fluctuations in relation to season,
time of day, context, level of competition, and motivation (see Bass
and Ladich 2008). Although some of this variation is due to dier-
ences in parameters such as temperature and circulating androgen
levels, which are not under the control of the individual, examples
of behavioral plasticity in sound production are becoming apparent
(Parmentier etal. 2010; Amorim etal. 2011).
Known impacts of anthropogenic noise onfish
Fish species dier greatly in their hearing abilities as a consequence
of dierences in ear structure and other anatomical features,
such as the presence of a swim bladder (Popper and Fay 1999;
Bone and Moore 2008; Popper and Schilt 2008). The most valu-
able measurements of fish hearing have considered both particle
motion and sound pressure (although all fishes can detect the for-
mer, only some are sensitive to the latter) and have been carried out
in the free field or at specialized acoustic facilities (e.g., Hawkins
and Chapman 1975; Popper et al. 2007; Halvorsen et al. 2012).
Many other studies have created audiograms (plots of the low-
est sound levels detectable at dierent frequencies) using auditory
evoked potential methods in standard tank conditions, which is an
approach that is now questioned (see Fay and Popper 2012 for a
full discussion). Accurate assessments of precise hearing abilities are
therefore relatively rare, but it is clear that although there are some
species that can hear above 100 kHz, and many with capabilities
above 3 kHz, the majority of fishes are likely to be able to detect
sounds from below 50 Hz up to at least 500–1500 Hz (Popper and
Fay 1999; Normandeau Associates 2012). Because most anthropo-
genic activities generate considerable noise at frequencies below
1 kHz (Normandeau Associates 2012), the potential for an impact
is readily apparent.
Conclusive empirical evidence for a negative impact of anthro-
pogenic noise on fish is rarer than for other taxa, most notably
birds. This is partly due to a smaller research eort to date, partly
because observational studies in natural conditions are often di-
cult to interpret, and partly because experimental work conducted
in captivity can indicate an eect of increased noise but lacks eco-
logical validity (see Normandeau Associates 2012; Slabbekoorn
2014 for a full discussion). However, there is sucient information
to suggest that the same range of impacts as found in other taxo-
nomic groups—behavioral responses furthest from the source, with
an increasing likelihood of physiological impacts, hearing dam-
age, injury, and death with increasing proximity (Dooling et al.
2009)—may be apparent in fish (see Popper and Hastings 2009;
Slabbekoorn etal. 2010; Normandeau Associates 2012 for reviews).
It is important to note, though, that dierent noises can have dif-
ferent eects, and the same or similar noise sources may not aect
all species in the same way; intraspecific dierences are also likely
(see Radford et al. 2014). For example, some fish species but not
others may suer injury or even death when very close to certain,
particularly impulsive, sound sources (Keevin and Hempen 1997;
Halvorsen et al. 2012). Likewise, some sources of anthropogenic
noise have been shown to cause, for instance, temporary threshold
shifts (transient reductions in hearing sensitivity) and stress, but only
in some of the tested species (Popper et al. 2005, 2007; Wysocki
et al. 2006, 2007). Catch rates, as indicators of movement away
from a sound source, have also produced contrasting results in dif-
ferent studies (Engås etal. 1996; Løkkeborg etal. 2012).
In general, because they can be caused by lower sound intensities
than other potential eects, the most important impacts might be
those on behavior, including acoustic communication. The acoustic
signals produced by many fish fall within a frequency band between
100 Hz and 1 kHz, making them vulnerable to anthropogenic noise
from a variety of sources (Ladich etal. 2006; Bass and Ladich 2008;
Normandeau Associates 2012). Several studies have suggested that
noise from boat trac, for example, could reduce the eective
range of communication signals and therefore the signaling e-
ciency between individuals (Amoser etal. 2004; Vasconcelos etal.
2007; Codarin et al. 2009). This is because detection distances are
reduced through masking (Codarin etal. 2009) and/or the auditory
sensitivities of receivers are diminished (Vasconcelos et al. 2007).
Only one study, however, has directly examined how noise might
impact fish acoustic behavior. Picciulin et al. (2012) found that the
mean pulse rate of brown meagres (Sciaena umbra) was higher follow-
ing repeated, though not single, boat passes compared with during
ambient conditions (it was assumed that the noise generated by the
boats was the causal eect, although this was not tested directly).
The observed increase in vocal activity could have arisen either
from an increased density of callers or from an increased acoustic
output by those individuals already calling (Picciulin etal. 2012).
HOW MIGHT FISH ACOUSTIC SIGNALERS
RESPOND TO ANTHROPOGENICNOISE?
Fish have not evolved in a quiet environment; as with most animals
that communicate acoustically, they face the problem of naturally
occurring, potentially masking, noise arising from abiotic sources
including wind, rain, and waves and biotic noise from chorus-
ing conspecifics or heterospecifics (see Luther and Gentry 2013).
Solutions to ensure the audibility of signals over background noise
could be manifested over 3 dierent time frames (see Brumm and
Slabbekoorn 2005). First, acoustic signals might be shaped by nat-
ural selection across evolutionary time. Long-term adaptations in
response to natural noise have been demonstrated in some fishes
(Lugli etal. 2003; Lugli 2010), with interspecific and interpopula-
tion dierences in frequency range recorded in a number of species
(Hawkins and Rasmussen 1978; Parmentier et al. 2005). Second,
an animal could potentially reduce the masking eects of habitat-
specific noise by making adjustments to signal properties during
its lifetime. Such ontogenetic changes are feasible in species that
exhibit vocal learning (e.g., passerine birds; Catchpole and Slater
2008) but are perhaps less likely in fish; although there is evidence
of changes in the acoustic characteristics of some fish signals with
age, vocal flexibility in this regard has not been documented (see
1024
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Radford etal. • Acoustic communication in a noisy world
Vasconcelos and Ladich 2008). Third, animals may exhibit behav-
ioral plasticity to temporary changes in background noise. Some
fishes are known to be capable of such acoustic flexibility in certain
contexts (Luczkovich etal. 2000; Parmentier et al. 2010; Amorim
et al. 2011) although this may be less common than evolutionary
adaptation.
Studies of the impact of anthropogenic noise on acoustic signal-
ing in other taxa, primarily in relation to mate choice and territory
defense (e.g., Miller etal. 2000; Slabbekoorn and Peet 2003; Luther
and Baptista 2010; Bermudez-Cuamatzin etal. 2011), but also par-
ent–ospring communication (Leonard and Horn 2012) and alarm
calling (Lowry etal. 2012), suggest 5 main ways in which fish might
alter their acoustic behavior. These are not necessarily mutually
exclusive—multiple adaptations for communication in noisy condi-
tions could occur (Brumm etal. 2004)—and there may be a trade-
os between them—for example, increasing amplitude may be
energetically costly and so result in a compromise in terms of sig-
nal duration (Fernandez-Juricic etal. 2005). Research investigating
responses to nonanthropogenic noise sources allows an assessment
of whether these putative changes in signaler behavior might be
feasible in fish. However, it is important to note that relevant studies
have been conducted on only a small minority of the >32 000 spe-
cies of known fish; given their great diversity, generalizations should
be avoided.
Avoidance ofnoise
The simplest means of avoiding the potential impacts of anthropo-
genic noise is to move away from the source. However, this is not
always possible if the source dominates certain frequencies, as is the
case with low-frequency shipping noise (Wright etal. 2007), or if
an entire area is aected, as might occur in harbors and estuaries
subjected to large amounts of shipping. Also, if a species is depen-
dent on a particular area because of crucial resources, such as food
or nesting sites, or is restricted by the geography of the region, then
there may be no option but to remain despite the noise. An alter-
native way to maximize signal transmission is through temporal
adjustments in communication, taking advantage of inherent gaps
or fluctuations in competing noise to enhance the likelihood of sig-
nal detection and discrimination. A number of diurnal bird spe-
cies have been shown to sing more at night when there is greater
daytime competition from similar sounding species (see La 2012)
and that adjustment in timing might also be selected for in response
to sources of anthropogenic noise that are variable over time. For
example, by singing at night in areas where there are high levels of
daily urban noise, European robins (Erithacus rubecula) may benefit
from minimizing acoustic competition or from an increase in the
clarity of their signal (Fuller etal. 2007).
A single study of silver perch (Bairdiella chrysoura) provides the only
suggestion to date that adaptive suppression of calling in response to
external stimuli might occur in fish. Following natural occurrences
or playbacks of whistles from bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops trunca-
tus), a major predator of the perch, sound levels recorded from the
chorusing fish were significantly reduced (Luczkovich etal. 2000).
This reduction is most likely due to the cessation of calling by indi-
viduals close to the sound source, although it is also possible that
fish in the vicinity moved away from the potential danger, causing
a decrease in the amplitude of the chorus (Luczkovich etal. 2000).
Either response would also aid in minimizing acoustic overlap with
a nonthreatening but potentially masking influence (i.e., anthropo-
genic noise) although that remains to be tested directly. Moreover,
it is currently unknown if the ability to adjust calling flexibly in this
way exists in other fish species.
Temporal adjustments
Perceptual studies have shown that the detectability of brief acous-
tic signals is considerably enhanced by increasing their duration, as
a consequence of the temporal summation of signal energy in the
peripheral auditory system of receivers (see Brumm and Slabbekoorn
2005). Some animals, such as common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)
and killer whales (Orcinus orca), appear to take advantage of this eect
by extending the duration of their calls in response to temporarily
elevated, man-made noise (Brumm etal. 2004; Foote etal. 2004). For
longer acoustic signals, an increase in duration cannot be explained
by exploitation of temporal summation but could increase the proba-
bility that some of the signal is given during a quieter period in terms
of the background noise; this could be the case with some whale
vocalizations (e.g., Fristrup et al. 2003; Di Iorio and Clark 2010).
The likelihood of detection can also be enhanced through increased
redundancy, achieved either by repeating the signal or through an
increase in the rate of calling. Various whale, frog, and bird species
have been shown to respond in this way to either playback or natu-
ral sources of anthropogenic noise (Lesage et al. 1999; Kaiser and
Hammers 2009; Diaz etal. 2011).
The potential for evolutionary changes in the temporal structure
of fish acoustic signaling is evidenced by the geographic variation
observed in the skunk clownfish Amphiprion akallopisos, with popula-
tions in Indonesia and Madagascar having dierent call character-
istics (Parmentier etal. 2005). The pulse duration of “short pops,”
1 of 3 call types produced, is longer in the Madagascan popula-
tion, but the number of peaks per pulse is higher in the Indonesian
population. For “long pops,” pulse period is longer, but the number
of peaks is lower in the Indonesian population compared with the
Madagascar population. It is not known whether the call param-
eters are dierent because of adaptation to dierent local abiotic or
biotic sources of noise or as a consequence of genetic drift in repro-
ductively isolated populations (Parmentier et al. 2005). However,
there clearly exists the capacity for changes in the duration and rate
of calling over evolutionary timescales, at least in this species.
There is also evidence that some fishes can respond flexibly
in terms of the temporal structure of their calling. For example,
certain damselfish (Pomacentridae) produce acoustic signals with
dierent pulse rates depending on whether they are interacting
agonistically with conspecifics or heterospecifics (Mann and Lobel
1998; Parmentier etal. 2010). Other species, such as the gulf toad-
fish (Opsanus beta), reduce their call rate when a predator is nearby
(Remage-Healey etal. 2006). Male gulf toadfish, which produce a
characteristic boatwhistle advertisement call to attract females, have
also been shown to increase their call rate to compete acoustically
with nearby rivals (Fine and Thorson 2008). Amore recent study
on Lusitanian toadfish (Halobatrachus didactylus) has demonstrated
that males reduce their call duration and pulse period at low tide
(Amorim etal. 2011), potentially because low-frequency sound rap-
idly attenuates in shallow water, so any calls produced would not
be detected by distant females (Mann 2006). Behavioral plasticity
in response to anthropogenic noise might, therefore, be feasible in
terms of such acoustic characteristics.
Amplitudeshifts
Animals experiencing elevated noise levels may increase the signal-
to-noise ratio during communication by raising the amplitude of
1025
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
their vocalizations, a response known as the “Lombard Eect”
(Brumm and Zollinger 2011). To date, the Lombard Eect in
response to anthropogenic noise has been demonstrated in a vari-
ety of species, including beluga whales (Lesage et al. 1999), killer
whales (Holt etal. 2009), common marmosets (Brumm etal. 2004),
domestic fowl (Gallus gallus domesticus: Brumm et al. 2009), and
nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos: Brumm and Todt 2002; Brumm
2004). Nightingales in noisier territories were neither bigger nor
heavier than those in quieter territories, eliminating the possibil-
ity that the ability to sing at higher amplitudes is only exhibited by
individuals that are big enough to enable them to do so (Brumm
2004). Brumm and Todt (2002) also demonstrated that nightingales
do not just sing at maximum amplitude but regulate vocal intensity
depending on the level of masking noise and whether it is within
the spectral region of their ownsongs.
To date, there is little evidence that fish adjust the amplitude of
their acoustic signaling in response to background noise. Whether
they have the capacity to do so is likely to be constrained by body
size as well as by the energetic costs of producing louder sounds
(see Oberweger and Goller 2001). It is notable that all the exist-
ing examples of the Lombard eect are from birds and mammals.
Although anurans, for example, are capable of varying the ampli-
tude of their calls, there is no evidence that they do so in response
to elevated noise levels (see Love and Bee 2010). It is also likely that
noise-dependent regulation of signal amplitude does not occur in
insects because there seems to be strong selection for increased
loudness in this group, meaning they are often signaling close to
their power capabilities anyway (Gerhardt and Huber 2002). As
with many insects and frogs, fishes often call in aggregations (e.g.,
Luczkovich et al. 2000; Amorim et al. 2011) where a Lombard
eect would quickly escalate and lead to all males signaling at max-
imum levels (Brumm and Zollinger 2011). Another potential reason
for a lack of control over the amplitude of acoustic output would
be if auditory feedback does not play a role in sound production;
this is known to be the case with stridulating insects (reviewed in
Gerhardt and Huber 2002), but whether it is also the case in fish
requires further research before conclusions can be drawn.
Frequencyshifts
Laboratory studies have shown that sounds with a greater band-
width and a higher rate of frequency modulation are harder to
detect from noise (Lohr etal. 2003), and animals in habitats with
high levels of natural noise converge on vocalizations with primar-
ily pure tones (e.g., Dubois and Martens 1984). To date, only one
study has documented such a change in response to anthropogenic
noise, with red-winged blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus) songs exhibit-
ing increased signal tonality due to an emphasis on lower frequen-
cies (Hanna et al. 2011). More commonly, birds and cetaceans
produce songs or calls with a higher average minimum or funda-
mental frequency at times or in areas with more low-frequency
noise from sources such as trac and seismic surveys (Lesage etal.
1999; Slabbekoorn and Peet 2003; Fernandez-Juricic etal. 2005;
Parks etal. 2007). There are also examples of animals adjusting the
relative amplitude of dierent frequency components: California
ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi), for instance, shift the peak
energy of their calls from lower to higher harmonics when there
is low-frequency noise from wind turbines (Rabin et al. 2006).
Frequency shifts in response to noise could arise through evolution-
ary adaptation, as evidenced by a 30-year study of white-crowned
sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys) songs: there was an increase in mini-
mum frequency as urban noise increased across time (Luther and
Baptista 2010). Afrequency change could also come about through
behavioral plasticity if individuals prioritize the use of higher fre-
quencies within their existing repertoire. Such acoustic flexibility
has been demonstrated experimentally in birds, with individual
house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) exposed to playback of urban
noise increasing the minimum frequency of their song elements
and also decreasing the frequency when lower levels of anthropo-
genic noise were transmitted (Bermudez-Cuamatzin etal. 2011).
Two Italian freshwater gobies (Padogobius martensii and Gobius
nigricans) provide evidence that some fishes can adapt their call fre-
quencies in response to abiotic noise sources. The gobies inhabit
environments where waterfalls produce low-frequency noise but
with a quiet window in the noise spectrum around 100 Hz (Lugli
etal. 2003). Both species exhibit the highest sensitivity to sound at
100 Hz and produce sounds with a fundamental frequency from
73 to 200 Hz (Lugli et al. 2003). Frequency dierences between
dierent populations of the same species of skunk clownfish
(A. akallopisos) (Parmentier et al. 2005) also suggest the possibility
of adjustments over evolutionary timescales. Although it was ini-
tially considered unlikely that fish could flexibly modulate their call
frequencies (see Bass and Ladich 2008), a recent study has shown
that male Lusitanian toadfish calling at low tide increase their fun-
damental frequency; as low-frequency sound rapidly attenuates in
shallow waters, this is likely to enhance the transmission of their
calls to females (Amorim et al. 2011). There is thus at least the
potential for such adjustments in response to anthropogenic noise.
Species that characteristically have higher frequencies within their
repertoire may be better able to respond to anthropogenic noise in
such fashion (see Francis etal. 2011). Dierent fish species produce
acoustic signals in dierent frequency ranges although they are
often structurally much simpler than, for example, birdsong, which
may provide reduced opportunities for immediate flexibility.
Change in signaling modality
Animals may also increase the ecacy of communication amid
noise by shifting emphasis to another modality (Brumm and
Slabbekoorn 2005; van der Sluijs etal. 2011). The “redundant sig-
naling” hypothesis proposes that animals have multiple signals so
that if one modality fails to transmit to the receiver, other signals
will successfully convey the message (Johnstone 1996). Many ani-
mal displays include various signal components, often in 2 or more
sensory modalities (e.g., Candolin 2003). However, to the best of
our knowledge, empirical testing of this possibility in response to
anthropogenic noise has yet to be undertaken in anytaxa.
Some fish species are certainly capable of shifting signaling
modalities depending on present conditions. For example, the rel-
ative importance of visual and olfactory cues in determining the
mate preference of female three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus acu-
leatus) diers in clear versus turbid water (Heuschele et al. 2009).
The potential for anthropogenic noise to alter the relative impor-
tance of dierent sensory modalities, therefore, seems likely in at
least some species. Mate selection in Lake Malawi rock-dwelling
cichlids, for instance, depends on visual, chemical, and acoustic
cues (Plenderleith etal. 2005; Amorim etal. 2008). If boat trac
noise were to increase the masking of the latter, then a shift toward
one or other modality might be expected. It is worth bearing in
mind that signals in dierent modalities have dierent advantages
and disadvantages. In most terrestrial habitats, for example, visual
displays act at a much shorter range than acoustic signals and
thus long-range aspects of acoustic cues may not be readily com-
pensated for by visual alternatives. However, the more intense the
1026
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Radford etal. • Acoustic communication in a noisy world
background noise (and, as a result, the shorter the communication
range of acoustic signals), the more important short-range visual
signals will become. For fishes, the trade-o may be somewhat dif-
ferent, as acoustic communication tends to be over relatively short
distances compared with, say, birdsong.
MOVING FORWARD
Although there is undoubtedly a rapidly increasing research interest
in the impacts of anthropogenic noise in general, and on acoustic
communication in particular, there are a number of steps that we
suggest would enhance considerably our current understanding.
Realistic masking experiments
Much early research examining the potential impact of anthropo-
genic noise used correlative, observational data and thus did not
provide suitable controls for potential confounding factors (see
Radford et al. 2012; Morley et al. 2014). If the eects of noise
at dierent natural sites are to be compared, ideally these should
be matched for other variables (see Francis et al. 2009, 2011).
Ultimately, however, it is carefully controlled experimental manip-
ulations (e.g., Ber mudez-Cuamatzin et al. 2011; Halfwerk et al.
2011) that are crucial to tease out the direct eect of noise and thus
provide the strongest possible conclusions.
High-quality noise-related experiments require accurate and
suitable characterization of the sound source (see Schaub et al.
2009; Morley et al. 2014). Underwater sound includes 2 compo-
nents: pressure (as detected by humans) and particle motion, which
results from the oscillatory displacement of particles back and forth
within a propagating sound wave. Although some fish species are
sensitive to sound pressure, all detect particle motion and thus mea-
suring this element of a sound source and determining the masking
impact are critical (see Normandeau Associates 2012). Moreover,
variation in characteristics beyond just absolute amplitude should
be considered; for instance, dierences in temporal patterns and
fluctuations in periodicity, frequency, and amplitude may well result
in dierent impacts. Relatively loud, continuous noise might be
expected to lead to long-term acoustic adjustments, whereas fluctu-
ating noise levels might result in acoustic plasticity (see Luther and
Gentry 2013 and references therein).
As with all experimental research, there are likely to be advan-
tages and disadvantages to dierent approaches (Slabbekoorn
2014). Captive studies often allow behaviors to be examined in
greater detail than those conducted in the wild, as well as oer-
ing potentially more control over the conditions and contexts of
the focal animals. However, aquatic tank setups result in complex
sound fields (e.g., Parvulescu 1964); although it is possible to deter-
mine the potential for an eect of additional noise, assessments of
absolute values for masking and the scale of impact are not fea-
sible. Moreover, captive animals are usually more constrained than
in the wild and may not exhibit their full behavioral repertoire.
Studies in the wild do not suer from these issues, thus providing
ecological validity, but they can be logistically much more challeng-
ing and do not normally allow the same level of control, provid-
ing more limited information in terms of subtle responses. Given
the current dearth of detailed knowledge relating to the impact of
anthropogenic noise in general, and on acoustic signaling in fish in
particular, we would advocate a complementary approach, includ-
ing full consideration of the relevant limitations where appropri-
ate (see also Slabbekoorn 2014). In general, masking experiments
should include readily controllable noise sources; the potential to
manipulate and measure the magnitudes, direction, and spatial
characteristics of both particle motion and sound pressure; and the
ability to collect and analyze response variables of most relevance.
Receiver’s perspective
Acoustic communication of course entails not only the actions of
the signaler, as considered in this review, but also signal perception
and discrimination on the part of the receiver. Sensory adaptations
and perceptual flexibility to enhance extraction of signals from
background noise include selective attention and auditory stream
segregation, mechanisms to counteract constraints on discrimina-
tion, and improvements in detection thresholds (see Brumm and
Slabbekoorn 2005). Such evolutionary adaptations and behavioral
plasticity are relevant not only to acoustic communication but also
to the use of natural sounds for habitat selection, settlement, preda-
tor avoidance, and prey detection (e.g., Simpson etal. 2005, 2011;
Schaub etal. 2009). However, although examples of these receiver
abilities in relation to natural noise sources are readily apparent in
a variety of taxa, including fish (Fay and Edds-Walton 1997; Lugli
etal. 2003), far less research attention has focused on how anthro-
pogenic noise aects receivers compared with signalers. Moreover,
assessments of fish hearing have rarely been conducted in acousti-
cal conditions that allow accurate measurement of the true sound
field in terms of both sound pressure and particle motion (Fay and
Popper 2012) and thus precise hearing thresholds for most species
are not yet known (see above). Use of current hearing–assessment
methods do potentially allow comparative research, as long as all
measurements are conducted in the same laboratory using the same
equipment, and thus provide an important tool when considering
the possibility of temporary or permanent threshold shifts; if the
hearing ability of the receiver is detrimentally aected by anthro-
pogenic noise, then acoustic communication will be compromised
(Vasconcelos etal. 2007).
Nonmasking effects ofnoise
In addition to masking signals, anthropogenic noise might also
aect acoustic communication in various indirect ways (Naguib
2013). For instance, studies have shown that noise can cause stress
in a range of taxa, with some preliminary indications in fish (e.g.,
Wysocki etal. 2006, but see Wysocki et al. 2007), and stress can
aect individual performance and decision making, with conse-
quences for any behavior, including communication (Kight and
Swaddle 2011). Moreover, noise may be distracting, shifting atten-
tion away from signals of relevance as well as impairing cognitive
performance; animals have a finite capacity to attend simultane-
ously to multiple stimuli (Chan and Blumstein 2011). Work in non-
communicative contexts has demonstrated that additional noise
in the environment can potentially distract fish and detrimentally
aect behavioral performance (Purser and Radford 2011). Noise
can also influence habitat choice, individual spacing, and popula-
tion density (Francis etal. 2009; Simpson etal. 2010); although less
is known about such eects in aquatic environments compared with
terrestrial species, there is evidence that anthropogenic noise can at
least temporarily aect space use by fish (Engås etal. 1996; Holles
et al. 2013). Alterations in the way conspecifics are distributed in
the environment could have consequences for the likelihood of
detecting particular signals, the number of rivals or potential mates
that can be assessed, and the eort and resources needed to acquire
that information; noise could influence not just individual com-
municative interactions but information flow through conspecific
1027
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
networks and whole communities (Naguib 2013). As yet, there is
hardly any empirical work assessing these indirect consequences of
anthropogenic noise on acoustic communication, and certainly not
in fish.
Fitness consequences
The vast majority of experimental studies investigating anthropo-
genic noise have, to date, considered relatively short-term eects
(see Radford etal. 2012; Morley etal. 2014 and references therein).
However, both for fundamental scientific progress in this field and
for successful policymaking and mitigation, assessments of the likely
impacts on individual survival and reproductive success are vital.
Some short-term eects (e.g., increased predation risk) can be trans-
lated relatively easily into ultimate fitness consequences; others,
including acoustic communication, need more careful consideration
because animals may be able to compensate in some way and thus
there may be no direct link between short- and long-term impacts
(Bejder etal. 2006). Although the masking of communication sig-
nals elicits demonstrable behavioral changes in many passerine bird
species and marine mammals, for instance, there is little evidence
to support resultant changes in fitness. That is not to say changes
in fitness do not arise, but rather that the experiments required to
determine them have rarely been conducted (but see Halfwerk etal.
2011; Kight et al. 2012). Moreover, adjustments in acoustic signal-
ing may result in greater energetic needs, an increased likelihood
of detection by predators, or the loss of vital information, but these
costs are rarely considered (see Read etal. 2014). Among aquatic
organisms, fish oer a more feasible opportunity than marine mam-
mals for a direct examination of the benefits and costs of acoustic
alterations made in response to anthropogenic noise and thus to
determine the impact on individual fitness and population viability.
CONCLUSIONS
The human population is projected to increase by 2.3 billion
between 2011 and 2050 (United Nations 2011) and thus noise
pollution is not just a pressing issue, but one of growing concern.
There is increasing recognition that sublethal impacts of anthro-
pogenic noise are perhaps the most important considerations for
populations of animals. Potentially, our greatest knowledge in
this regard currently relates to acoustic communication and par-
ticularly the way signalers minimize the risk of masking; certainly,
this is the research topic that has so far received the most atten-
tion. But, there is a strong taxonomic bias in those studies that have
been conducted; the majority focus on birds and marine mammals.
Although the range of sounds made by fishes is not as diverse as in
those taxa, their acoustic communication is also likely to be aected
by anthropogenic noise and their biological and societal value
mean that research into potential negative impacts is important.
The evidence available from other fields suggests that some spe-
cies of fish have the potential to compete with anthropogenic noise;
there are at least some of the same capabilities as birds and mam-
mals in terms of adaptation or flexible adjustment in their acoustic
signaling. Perhaps the most likely responses to anthropogenic noise
are changes in temporal patterning and signal modality although
alterations in frequency parameters and when to call may also be
feasible in some cases. The likelihood that a given species will be
able to respond in suitable fashion to a particular noise source, and
thus maintain ecient acoustic communication, will depend on the
mechanism of its sound production (not something that varies so
considerably in birds and mammals), the types of sound produced,
and the intraspecific and intraindividual levels of variation shown.
It is worth emphasizing that the body of relevant empirical work
is currently very limited, and the huge diversity among the more
than 32 000 species of fish means that extrapolations should be
treated with extreme care. The same noise may aect species dif-
ferently, dierent impacts can be expected at dierent spatial scales,
and there may also be variation between individuals of the same
species and even dierences by the same individual depending on
condition, state, and motivation. Clearly what is needed now are
experimental tests of these ideas, both to enhance our understand-
ing of how fish respond to anthropogenic noise and because studies
of this taxonomic group present an ideal opportunity to take the
whole field forward in exploring the ultimate eects of this global
pollutant on individual fitness, population viability, and community
structure.
FUNDING
A.N.R. and S.D.S. were partly supported by Department for
Environment, Food and Rural Aairs (DEFRA) contract ME5207;
S.D.S. was also supported by Natural Environment Research
Council (NE/J500616/2).
We are grateful to J.Purser, R.Bruintjes, E. Morley, I. Voellmy, S.Holles,
M.Wale, K.Everley, and T.Hawkins for fruitful discussions and to 2 anony-
mous referees for helpful comments on the manuscript.
Forum editor: Sue Healy
REFERENCES
Amorim MCP, Neves ASM. 2008. Male painted gobies vocalise to defend
territories. Behaviour. 145:1065–1083.
Amorim MCP, Simões JM, Almada VC, Fonseca PJ. 2011. Stereotypy and
variation of the mating call in the Lusitanian toadfish, Halobatrachus didac-
tylus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 65:707–716.
Amorim MCP, Simões JM, Fonseca PJ, Turner GF. 2008. Species dier-
ences in courtship acoustic signals among five Lake Malawi cichlid spe-
cies (Pseudotropheus spp.). J Fish Biol. 72:1355–1368.
Amoser S, Wysocki LE, Ladich F. 2004. Noise emission during the first
powerboat race in an Alpine lake and potential impact on fish communi-
ties. J Acoust Soc Am. 116:3789–3797.
Barber JR, Crooks KR, Fristrup KM. 2009. The costs of chronic noise
exposure for terrestrial organisms. Trends Ecol Evol. 25:180–189.
Bass AH, Ladich F. 2008. Vocal-acoustic communication: from neurons to
behavior. In: Webb JF, Fay RR, Popper AN, editors. Springer handbook
of auditory research. Vol. 32. New York: Springer. p. 253–278.
Bejder L, Samuels A, Whitehead H, Gales N. 2006. Interpreting short-term
behavioural responses to disturbance within a longitudinal perspective.
Anim Behav. 72:1149–1158.
Bermudez-Cuamatzin E, Ríos-Chelén AA, Gil D, Garcia CM. 2011.
Experimental evidence for real-time song frequency shift in response to
urban noise in a passerine bird. Biol Lett. 7:36–38.
Bone Q , Moore RH. 2008. Biology of fishes. 3rd ed. New York: Taylor &
Francis Group.
Brumm H. 2004. The impact of environmental noise on song amplitude in
a territorial bird. J Anim Ecol. 73:434–440.
Brumm H, Schmidt R, Schrader L. 2009. Noise-dependent vocal plasticity
in domestic fowl. Anim Behav. 78:741–746.
Brumm H, Slabbekoorn H. 2005. Acoustic communication in noise. Adv
Study Behav. 35:151–209.
Brumm H, Todt D. 2002. Noise-dependent song amplitude regulation in a
territorial songbird. Anim Behav. 63:891–897.
Brumm H, Voss K, Köllmer I, Todt D. 2004. Acoustic communication in
noise: regulation of call characteristics in a New World monkey. J Exp
Biol. 207:443–448.
Brumm H, Zollinger SA. 2011. The evolution of the Lombard eect:
100years of psychoacoustic research. Behaviour. 148:1173–1198.
1028
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Radford etal. • Acoustic communication in a noisy world
Candolin U. 2003. The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biol Rev.
78:575–595.
Catchpole CK, Slater PJB. 2008. Bird song: biological themes and varia-
tions. Cambridge (MA): Cambridge University Press.
Chan AAY-H, Blumstein DT. 2011. Attention, noise, and implications for
wildlife conservation and management. App Anim Behav Sci. 131:1–7.
Clark CW, Ellison WT, Southall BL, Hatch L, van Parijs SM, Frankel A,
Ponikaris D. 2009. Acoustic masking in marine ecosystems: intuitions,
analyses and implication. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 395:201–222.
Codarin A, Wysocki LE, Ladich F, Picciulin M. 2009. Eects of ambi-
ent and boat noise on hearing and communication in three fish species
living in a marine protected area (Miramare, Italy). Mar Pollut Bull.
58:1880–1887.
Diaz M, Parra A, Gallardo C. 2011. Serins respond to anthropogenic noise
by increasing vocal activity. Behav Ecol. 22:332–336.
Di Iorio L, Clark CW. 2010. Exposure to seismic survey alters blue whale
acoustic communication. Biol Lett. 6:51–54.
Dooling RJ, West, EW, Leek MR. 2009. Conceptual and computation mod-
els of the eects of anthropogenic sound on birds. Proc Inst Acoust. 31
(part 1).
Dubois A, Martens J. 1984. A case of possible vocal convergence between
frogs and a bird in Himalayan torrents. J Ornithol. 125:455–463.
Engås A, Løkkeborg S, Ona E, Soldal AV. 1996. Eects of seismic shooting
on local abundance and catch rates of cod (Gadus morhua) and haddock
(Melanogrammus aeglefinus). Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 53:2238–2249.
FAO. 2012. The State of World Fisheries and Aquaculture 2012. Rome
(Italy): FAO.
Fay RR, Edds-Walton PL. 1997. Directional response properties of saccular
aerents of the toadfish, Opsanus tau. Hear Res. 111:1–21.
Fay RR, Megela-Simmons A. 1999. The sense of hearing in fishes and
amphibians. In: Fay RR, Popper, AN, editors. Comparative hearing: fish
and amphibians. New York: Springer Verlag. p. 269–318.
Fay RR, Popper AN. 2012. Fish hearing: new perspectives from two ‘senior’
bioacousticians. Brain Behav Evol. 79:215–217.
Fernandez-Juricic E, Poston R, De Collibus K, Morgan T, Bastain B, Martin
C, Jones K, Treminio R. 2005. Microhabitat selection and singing behav-
ior patterns of male house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) in urban parks in a
heavily urbanized landscape in the western U.S. Urban Habitats. 3:49–69.
Fine ML, Thorson RF. 2008. Use of passive acoustics for assessing behav-
ioral interactions in individual toadfish. Trans Am Fish Soc. 137:627–637.
Foote AD, Osborne RW, Hoelzel AR. 2004. Whale-call response to masking
boat noise. Nature. 428:910.
Francis CD, Ortega CP, Cruz A. 2009. Noise pollution changes avian com-
munities and species interactions. Curr Biol. 19:1415–1419.
Francis CD, Ortega CP, Cruz A. 2011. Vocal frequency change reflects dif-
ferent responses to anthropogenic noise in two suboscine tyrant flycatch-
ers. Proc Biol Sci. 278:2025–2031.
Fristrup KM, Hatch LT, Clark CW. 2003. Variation in humpback whale
(Megaptera novaeangliae) song length in relation to low-frequency sound
broadcasts. J Acoust Soc Am. 113:3411–3424.
Fuller RA, Warren PH, Gaston KJ. 2007. Daytime noise predicts nocturnal
singing in urban robins. Biol Lett. 3:368–370.
Gerhardt H, Huber F. 2002. Acoustic communication in insects and
anurans. Chicago (IL): The University of Chicago Press.
Halfwerk W, Holleman LJM, Lessells CM, Slabbekoorn H. 2011. Negative
impact of trac noise on avian reproductive success. J Appl Ecol.
48:210–219.
Halvorsen MB, Casper BM, Matthews F, Carlson TJ, Popper AN. 2012.
Eects of exposure to pile-driving sounds on the lake sturgeon, Nile tila-
pia and hogchoker. Proc Biol Sci. 279:4705–4714.
Hanna D, Blouin-Demers G, Wilson DR, Mennill DJ. 2011. Anthropogenic
noise aects song structure in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). J
Exp Biol. 214:3549–3556.
Hawkins AD, Amorim MCP. 2000. Spawning sounds of the male haddock,
Melanogrammus aeglefinus. Environ Biol Fish. 59:29–41.
Hawkins AD, Chapman CJ. 1975. Masked auditory thresholds in the cod
(Gadus morhua L.). J Comp Physiol. 103:209–226.
Hawkins AD, Myrberg AA. 1983. Hearing and sound communication
underwater. In: Lewis B, editor. Bioacoustics, a comparative approach.
London: Academic Press. p. 347–405.
Hawkins AD, Rasmussen K. 1978. The calls of galoid fish. J Mar Biol Ass
Lond. 58:891–911.
Henglmuller SM, Ladich F. 1999. Development of agonistic behaviour and
vocalization in croaking gouramis. J Fish Biol. 54:380–395.
Heuschele J, Mannerla M, Gienapp P, Candolin U. 2009. Environment-
dependent use of mate choice cues in sticklebacks. Behav Ecol.
20:1223–1227.
Hildebrand JA. 2009. Anthropogenic and natural sources of ambient noise
in the ocean. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 395:4–20.
Holles S, Simpson SD, Radford AN, Berten L, Lecchini D. 2013. Boat noise
disrupts orientation behaviour in a coral reef fish. Mar Ecol Prog Ser.
485:295–300.
Holt MM, Noren DP, Veirs V, Emmons CK, Veirs S. 2009. Speaking up:
killer whales (Orcinus orca) increase their call amplitude in response to ves-
sel noise. J Acoust Soc Am. 125:EL27–EL32.
Johnstone RA. 1996. Multiple displays in animal communication: “Backup
signals” and “multiple messages”. Phil Trans R Soc B. 351:329–338.
Kaiser K, Hammers JL. 2009. The eect of anthropogenic noise on male
advertisement call rate in the neotropical treefrog, Dendropsophus triangu-
lum. Behaviour. 146:1053–1069.
Keevin TM, Hempen GL. 1997. The environmental eects of under-
water explosions with methods to mitigate impacts. St. Louis (MO):
U.S. Army Corps of Engineers. Available from: http://www.denix.osd.
mil/nr/upload/underwaterexplosions.pdf, last accessed on 20 January
2014.
Kight CR, Saha MS, Swaddle JP. 2012. Anthropogenic noise is associated
with reductions in the productivity of breeding Eastern Bluebirds (Sialia
sialis). Ecol Appl. 22:1989–1996.
Kight CR, Swaddle JP. 2011. How and why environmental noise impacts
animals: an integrative, mechanistic review. Ecol Lett. 14:1052–1061.
La VT. 2012. Diurnal and nocturnal birds vocalize at night: a review.
Condor. 114:245–257.
Ladich F, Collin SP, Moller P, Kapoor BG. 2006. Fish communication.
Enfield (CT): Science Publisher.
Lampe U, Schmoll T, Franzke A, Reinhold K. 2012. Staying tuned: grass-
hoppers from noisy roadside habitats produce courtship signals with ele-
vated frequency components. Funct Ecol. 26:1348–1354.
Leonard ML, Horn AG. 2012. Ambient noise increases missed detections in
nestling birds. Biol Lett. 8:530–532.
Le Prell CG, Henderson D, Fay RR, Popper AN, editors. 2012. Noise-
induced hearing loss: scientific advances. New York: Springer Science +
Business Media, LLC.
Lesage V, Kingsley MCS, Sjare B. 1999. The eect of vessel noise on the
vocal behavior of belugas in the St. Lawrence River estuary, Canada.
Mar Mamm Sci. 15:65–84.
Lohr B, Wright TF, Dooling RJ. 2003. Detection and discrimination of
natural calls in masking noise by birds: estimating the active space of a
signal. Anim Behav. 65:763–777.
Løkkeborg S, Ona E, Vold A, Salthaug A. 2012. Eects of sounds from seis-
mic air guns on fish behavior and catch rates. In: Popper AN, Hawkins
AD, editors. The eects of noise on aquatic life. New York: Springer. p.
415–419.
Love EK, Bee MA. 2010. An experimental test of noise-dependent voice
amplitude regulation in Cope’s grey treefrog (Hyla chrysoscelis). Anim
Behav. 80:509–515.
Lowry H, Lill A, Wong BB. 2012. How noisy does a noisy miner have to
be? Amplitude adjustments of alarm calls in an avian urban ‘adapter’.
PLoS One. 7:e29960.
Luczkovich JJ, Daniel HJ, Hutchinson M, Jenkins T, Johnson SE, Pullinger
RC, Sprague MW. 2000. Sounds of sex and death in the sea: bottlenose
dolphin whistles suppress mating choruses of silver perch. Bioacoustics.
10:323–334.
Lugli M. 2010. Sounds of shallow water fishes pitch within the quiet win-
dow of the habitat ambient noise. J Comp Physiol A. 196:439–451.
Lugli M, Yan HY, Fine ML. 2003. Acoustic communication in two fresh-
water gobies: the relationship between ambient noise, hearing thresholds
and sound spectrum. J Comp Phys A. 189:309–320.
Luther D, Baptista L. 2010. Urban noise and the cultural evolution of bird
songs. Proc Biol Sci. 277:469–473.
Luther D, Gentry K. 2013. Sources of background noise and their
influence on vertebrate acoustic communication. Behaviour.
150:1045–1068.
Mann DA. 2006. Propagation of fish sounds. In: Ladich F, Collin SP, Moller
P, Kapoor BG, editors. Communication in fishes. Vol. 1. Enfield (CT):
Science Publishers. p. 107–120.
Mann DA, Lobel PS. 1998. Acoustic behaviour of the damselfish Dascyllus
albisella: behavioural and geographic variation. Environ Biol Fish.
51:421–428.
1029
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology
McDonald MA, Hildebrand JA, Wiggins SM. 2006. Increases in deep
ocean ambient noise in the Northeast Pacific west of San Nicolas Island,
California. J Acoust Soc Am. 120:711–718.
Miller PJ, Biassoni N, Samuels A, Tyack PL. 2000. Whale songs lengthen in
response to sonar. Nature. 405:903.
Morley EL, Jones G, Radford AN. 2014. The importance of invertebrates
when considering the impacts of anthropogenic noise. Proc Biol Sci.
281:20132683.
Myrberg AA. 1981. Sound communication and interception in fishes. In
Tavolga WN, Popper, AN, Fay RR, editors. Hearing and sound commu-
nication in fishes. New York: Springer-Verlag. p. 395–426.
Myrberg AA, Ha SJ, Shamblott MJ. 1993. The sounds of bicolor damsel-
fish (Pomacentrus partitus)—predictors of body size and a spectral basis for
individual recognition and assessment. J Acoust Soc Am. 94:3067–3070.
Myrberg AA, Mohler M, Catala JD. 1986. Sound production by males of
a coral reef fish (Pomacentrus partitus): its significance to females. Anim
Behav. 34:913–923.
Myrberg AA, Spires JY. 1972. Sound discrimination by the bicolor damsel-
fish (Eupomacentrus partitus). J Exp Biol. 57:727–735.
Naguib M. 2013. Living in a noisy world: indirect eects of noise on animal
communication. Behaviour. 150:1069–1084.
Normandeau Associates, Inc. 2012. Eects of noise on fish, fisheries,
and invertebrates in the U.S. Atlantic and Arctic from energy indus-
try sound-generating activities. A literature synthesis for the U.S. Dept.
of the Interior, Bureau of Ocean Energy Management. Contract
#M11PC00031. Bedford (NH): U.S. Dept. of the Interior, Bureau of
Ocean Energy Management. 153 pp. Available from: http://www.data.
boem.gov/homepg/data_center/other/espis/espismaster.asp?appid=1,
last accessed on 30 February 2014 .
Oberweger K, Goller F. 2001. The metabolic cost of birdsong production.
J Exp Biol. 204:3379–3388.
Parks SE, Clark CW, Tyack PL. 2007. Short- and long-term changes in
right whale calling behavior: the potential eects of noise on acoustic
communication. J Acoust Soc Am. 122:3725–3731.
Parmentier E, Colleye O, Fine ML, Frédérich B, Vandewalle P, Herrel A. 2007.
Sound production in the clownfish Amphiprion clarkii. Science. 316:1006.
Parmentier E, Kèver L, Casadevall M, Lecchini D. 2010. Diversity and com-
plexity in the acoustic behaviour of Dascyllus flavicaudus (Pomacentridae).
Mar Biol. 157:2317–2327.
Parmentier E, Lagardère JP, Vandewalle P, Fine ML. 2005. Geographical
variation in sound production in the anemonefish Amphiprion akallopisos.
Proc Biol Sci. 272:1697–1703.
Parvulescu A. 1964. Problems of propagation and processing. In: Tavolga
WN, editor. Marine bioacoustics. Oxford: Pergamon. p. 87–100.
Picciulin M, Sebastianutto L, Codarin A, Calcagno G, Ferrero EA. 2012.
Brown meagre vocalization rate increases during repetitive boat noise
exposures: a possible case of vocal compensation. J Acoust Soc Am.
132:3118–3124.
Plenderleith M, van Oosterhout C, Robinson RL, Turner GF. 2005. Female
preference for conspecific males based on olfactory cues in a Lake Malawi
cichlid fish. Biol Lett. 1:411–414.
Popper AN, Fay RR. 1999. Comparative hearing: fish and amphibians.
New York: Springer Verlag.
Popper AN, Halvorsen MB, Kane A, Miller DL, Smith ME, Song J, Stein
P, Wysocki LE. 2007. The eects of high-intensity, low-frequency active
sonar on rainbow trout. J Acoust Soc Am. 122:623–635.
Popper AN, Hastings MC. 2009. The eects of anthropogenic sources of
sound on fishes. J Fish Biol. 75:455–489.
Popper AN, Schilt CR. 2008. Hearing and acoustic behavior (basic and
applied). In: Webb JF, Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. Fish bioacoustics.
New York: Springer. p. 17–48.
Popper AN, Smith ME, Cott PA, Hanna BW, MacGillivray AO, Austin ME,
Mann DA. 2005. Eects of exposure to seismic airgun use on hearing of
three fish species. J Acoust Soc Am. 117:3958–3971.
Purser J, Radford AN. 2011. Acoustic noise induces attention shifts and
reduces foraging performance in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus
aculeatus). PLoS One. 6:e17478.
Rabin LA, Coss RG, Owings DH. 2006. The eects of wind turbines
on antipredator behaviour in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus
beecheyi). Biol Conserv. 131:410–420.
Radford AN, Morley E, Jones G. 2012. The eects of noise on biodiversity.
Defra report NO0235.
Radford AN, Purser J, Bruintjes R, Voellmy IK, Everley KA, Wale MA,
Holles S, Simpson SD. Forthcoming 2014. Beyond a simple eect: vari-
able and changing responses to anthropogenic noise. In: Popper AN,
Hawkins AD, editors. Eects of noise on aquatic life II. New York:
Springer.
Read J, Jones G, Radford AN. 2014. Fitness costs are as important as ben-
efits when considering noise-induced behavioural responses. Behav Ecol.
25:4–7.
Remage-Healey L, Nowacek DP, Bass AH. 2006. Dolphin foraging sounds
suppress calling and elevate stress hormone levels in a prey species, the
Gulf toadfish. J Exp Biol. 209:4444–4451.
Rowe S, Hutchings JA, Skjaeraasen JE, Bezanson L. 2008. Morphological
and behavioural correlates of reproductive success in Atlantic cod Gadus
morhua. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 354:257–265.
Schaub A, Ostwald J, Siemers BM. 2009. Foraging bats avoid noise. J Exp
Biol. 211:3174–3180.
Simpson SD, Meekan MG, Larsen NJ, McCauley RD, Jes A. 2010.
Behavioural plasticity in larval reef fish: orientation is influenced by
recent acoustic experiences. Behav Ecol. 21:1098–1105.
Simpson SD, Meekan M, Montgomery J, McCauley R, Jes A. 2005.
Homeward sound. Science. 308:221.
Simpson SD, Radford AN, Tickle EJ, Meekan MG, Jes AG. 2011. Adaptive
avoidance of reef noise. PLoS One. 6:e16625.
Slabbekoorn H. Forthcoming 2014. Aiming for progress in understanding
underwater noise impact on fish: complementary need for indoor and
outdoor studies. In: Popper AN, Hawkins AD, editors. Eects of noise on
aquatic life II. New York: Springer.
Slabbekoorn H, Bouton N, van Opzeeland I, Coers A, ten Cate C, Popper
AN. 2010. A noisy spring: the impact of globally rising underwater sound
levels on fish. Trends Ecol Evol. 25:419–427.
Slabbekoorn H, Peet M. 2003. Birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise:
great tits hit the high notes to ensure their mating calls are heard above
the city’s din. Nature. 424:267.
Sun JWC, Narins PA. 2005. Anthropogenic sounds dierentially aect
amphibian call rate. Biol Cons. 121:419–427.
van der Sluijs I, Gray SM, Amorim MCP, Barber I, Candolin U, Hendry
AP, Krahe R, Maan ME, Utne-Palm AC, Wagner H-J, et al. 2011.
Communication in troubled waters: responses of fish communication sys-
tems to changing environments. Evol Ecol. 25:623–640.
Tavolga WN. 1971. Sound production and detection. In: Hoar WS, Randall
DJ, editors. Fish physiology. New York: Academic Press. p. 135–205.
Tyack P. 2008. Implications for marine mammals of large-scale changes in
the marine acoustic environment. J Mamm. 89:549–558.
United Nations. 2011. World urbanization prospects: the 2011 revision.
New York: United Nations.
Vasconcelos RO, Amorim MC, Ladich F. 2007. Eects of ship noise on the
detectability of communication signals in the Lusitanian toadfish. J Exp
Biol. 210:2104–2112.
Vasconcelos RO, Ladich F. 2008. Development of vocalization, audi-
tory sensitivity and acoustic communication in the Lusitanian toadfish
Halobatrachus didactylus. J Exp Biol. 211:502–509.
Verzijden MN, van Heusden J, Bouton N, Witte F, ten Cate C, Slabbekoorn
H. 2010. Sounds of male Lake Victoria cichlids vary within and between
species and aect female mate preferences. Behav Ecol. 21:548–555.
Watts RD, Compton RW, McCammon JH, Rich CL, Wright SM, Owens
T, Ouren DS. 2007. Roadless space of the conterminous United States.
Science. 316:736–738.
Winn H. 1964. The biological significance of fish sounds. In: Tavolga WN,
editor. Marine bioacoustics. Oxford: Pergamon Press. p. 213–231.
World Health Organization. 2011. Burden of disease from environmen-
tal noise: quantification of healthy life years lost in Europe. Geneva
(Switzerland): World Health Organization.
Wright AJ, Soto NA, Baldwin AL, Bateson M, Beale CM, Clark C, Deak
T, Edwards EF, Fernández A, Godinho A, et al. 2007. Anthropogenic
noise as a stressor in animals: a multidisciplinary perspective. Int J Comp
Psychol. 20:250–273.
Wysocki LE, Davidson JW, Smith ME, Frankel AS, Ellison WT, Mazik
PM, Popper AN, Bebak J. 2007. Eects of aquaculture production noise
on hearing, growth, and disease resistance of rainbow trout Oncohynchus
mykiss. Aquaculture. 272:687–697.
Wysocki LE, Dittami JP, Ladich F. 2006. Ship noise and corticol secretion in
European freshwater fishes. Biol Conserv. 128:501–508.
1030
at University Library on September 10, 2014http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
... Globally, the most dominant source of underwater anthropogenic noise is vessel noise, which propagates efficiently over long distances (Vasconcelos et al. 2007;Frisk 2012;Possenti et al. 2024). Underwater, vessel noise can overlap with fish communication (Radford et al. 2014), masking important acoustic signals (Popper and Hastings 2009;Casper et al. 2013) that are critical for fish survival and reproduction (Fay and Popper 2000;Rollo et al. 2007;Slabbekoorn et al. 2010;Ladich and Fay 2013). Intense sounds may also lead to direct damage to the sensory receptors and related structures responsible for sound detection (McCauley et al. 2003;Popper 2003;Kunc et al. 2016;Mickle and Higgs 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Exposure to acute noise sources can lead to negative behavioural outcomes and fitness deficits in fishes, but it is unknown whether fish can habituate to chronic exposures. As underwater noise increases globally, understanding how long-term exposures can affect behavioural, morphological, and transcriptional measures of stress in fish is critical. We tested responses in captive brook trout (Salvelinus fontinalis) immediately after acute exposure to a noise and after 2 weeks of chronic exposure. Behavioural tests quantified fish movements before, during, and after sound presentation, with morphological and transcriptional changes assessed through ears and whole brains samples, respectively. Pre-control and pre-experimental brook trout exhibited increased swimming distance and velocity, but after the 2-week exposure, post-experimental fish showed no response to noise while the post-control group remained responsive. Post-experimental fish showed significant differences in transcription levels of genes involved in neuroplastic, appetite, and stress responses relative to the other groups. Together these results suggest that while fish may appear unresponsive via behavioural metrics to anthropogenic noises after chronic exposure, they still show significant changes at the transcriptional level with possible long-term effects.
... Marine fish are negatively affected by anthropogenic noise in a variety of different ways (Irons et al., 2000;Kunc et al., 2016;Radford et al., 2014;Slabbekoorn et al., 2010;Williams et al., 2015). There is apparently a long history of research attempting to link disturbance and noise from fishery activity and vessels with avoidance in commercial fish species dating back to the late 1950s, but most of this research is inaccessible (see Buerkle, 1973 for references). ...
Article
Anthropogenic noise is a pervasive environmental pollutant that continues to expand and increase globally, especially in marine environments, affecting many marine animals, especially fish. Although interest and concern regarding the effects of noise on fish has increased, most studies still focus on the effects noise has on individual species, often overlooking wider system-level consequences. This is particularly true of trophically important species such as forage fish. We investigated how different types of anthropogenic noise affect the quality of an important forage fish species, Pacific sand lance, Ammodytes personatus, which could impact the many species that rely on them. We found that, compared to controls, fish in noisy environments had lower energy density and lower weight at a given length. These results suggest that even over shorter periods of time the anthropogenic noise could reduce sand lance quality, which in-turn could cascade up the food chain causing drastic ecosystem-level consequences.
... 14 This type of study is often used to parameterize the relationship between disturbance and response, and they have a long history in ecology dating back to early studies of bird songs. 15,16 In the marine realm, studies have often been used to measure the impact of sound on a variety of behaviors across taxa-including behavioral response in larval settlement, 17 the response of reef fish to boat noise, [18][19][20] and responses of marine mammals to myriad sounds like acoustic pingers 21 and military sonar. 6,22 Here, we consider one type of BRS, controlled exposure experiments (CEEs), and present results combining all available data collected along with sound propagation and movement modeling to estimate noise SPL received by focal animals. ...
Article
Marine mammals are known to respond to various human noises, including and in certain cases, strongly, to military active sonar. Responses include small and short-term changes in diving behavior, horizontal avoidance of an ensonified area, and mass strandings. Considerable research has been conducted using short-term biologging tags to understand these responses. Yet researchers and managers want a better understanding of responses to sound over longer periods of time in a variety of contexts. The Atlantic Behavioral Response Study examines responses across multiple spatial and temporal scales using vessel-based focal follows, short-term biologging tags, and medium-term satellite transmitting tags. Since the latter do not record sound, we must intersect positions with a sound propagation model to estimate received sound pressure levels. We use all available information from (1) the observed x,y positions from the tag(s) and from focal follow vessels; (2) the discrete depth bin data (z) from the tag; (3) ocean bathymetry; and (4) outputs from sound propagation models. All these disparate streams of data contain varying levels of error in x, y, or z. We account for as much uncertainty as possible and include here a refined approach to better estimate the range of sound levels received by animals.
... As Humpback whales are known to breed in the study area during the time of year in which surveys were conducted (Rosenbaum et al., 2014), the impacts of anthrophony may have consequences on their reproductive output and require further attention. A body of work has documented the response of cetaceans (Gomez et al., 2016a;Wisniewska et al., 2018;Williams et al., 2006;Rolland et al., 2012;Bailey et al., 2010;Koschinski et al., 2003;Parsons et al., 2008;Elliott et al., 2019;Nowacek et al., 2007) and teleost fish (Fewtrell and McCauley, 2012;Gerlotto et al., 2004;Holles et al., 2013;Simpson et al., 2015;Simpson et al., 2016;Radford et al., 2014) to anthrophony, but few studies have considered other taxa, or employed animals as acoustic profilers to map sound (e.g. (Mikkelsen et al., 2019;Tyson et al., 2017;Cazau et al., 2017)). ...
Article
Full-text available
Anthropogenic noise (anthrophony) is pervasive in natural soundscapes and has become an important aspect of conservation. While moored sound recorders have aided marine soundscape research, they do not capture the dynamic experiences of animals as they move through underwater soundscapes. This study used animal-borne acoustic recording tags to capture the marine soundscape near leatherback turtle nesting grounds in Gabon, Central Africa. Propeller noise was heard in 75 ± 14.7 % (mean ± SD) of recordings and peaks in sound intensity up to 146 dB re 1 μPa were detected in shipping noise frequency bands. Loud noise events (> 141 dB), detected in 10 % of recordings, were distributed throughout the turtle interesting habitat. An anthrophony map was created, identifying peaks in noise corresponding with the Komo estuary-gateway to the nation's main international port and near a key nesting area. The pervasive, loud anthrophony recorded in the study may have negative impacts on nesting leatherback turtles and other species of conservation concern found in the area, and warrants further monitoring and management action. This study offers one of the first spatio-temporal analyses of sound experienced by an endangered marine vertebrate through animal borne, multi-individual acoustic monitoring. It highlights the utility of animal borne acoustic tags in delineating underwater soundscapes and their applicability to studying concurrent biological phenomena and threats, while supporting the need for similar monitoring efforts in other critical sea turtle habitats.
Article
As intensive aquaculture continues to develop, substantial investments have been made in equipment to sustain high‐density farming. However, this has resulted in elevated noise levels within these aquaculture environments, particularly below 2000 Hz, matching the hearing range of most cultured fish species. Fish under noise conditions may experience hearing loss and physiological stress, which can negatively affect their growth, foraging efficiency, reproductive success, and increase their susceptibility to diseases. These adverse effects compromise the welfare of cultured fish, potentially decreasing production quality and increasing mortality rates. Despite these notable effects, the understanding and management of noise conditions in aquaculture systems lag behind other environmental parameters in terms of recognition and control. In this review, we cover the fundamentals of fish auditory systems, the hearing range of key cultured fish species, and the most common noise sources and levels prevalent in current intensive aquaculture systems. Additionally, we examine recent discoveries on the effects of anthropogenic noise on fish hearing, physiological responses, and behavior. Finally, we provide strategies for noise monitoring and management in the aquaculture industry, while also highlighting open questions for future research. Our goal is to assist researchers and practitioners in comprehending underwater noise and its effects on cultured fish species, providing a valuable resource for promoting the healthy and sustainable development of intensive aquaculture.
Article
Full-text available
Passive acoustic monitoring of fish choruses utilises the sounds produced by fish as natural acoustic tags to extract ecologically important information regarding these animals and their respective habitats. Fish produce sounds in association with life functions and many species make these sounds en masse, in choruses. Passive acoustic monitoring of fish choruses can provide data on the spatiotemporal distribution, habitat use, spawning activity, behaviour, and local abundance of fish populations. Research in this area of study has been rapidly advancing since the 1940s; however, a review has yet to be undertaken to understand the scope of our understanding of these ecologically significant phenomena and how monitoring these choruses may be used to inform management practices. We have reviewed the literature on fish choruses to provide a broad summary on several research topics including: (1) the current scientific understanding of the definition of a fish chorus, (2) the spatiotemporal distribution of these phenomena, (3) drivers of fish chorus activity, (4) measurement methodologies, (5) current applications of passive acoustic monitoring of fish choruses to management practices, and (6) research areas requiring targeted improvement. We have identified a series of key research gaps that require prioritisation in future research. Appropriately addressing these shortfalls will facilitate the improvement of monitoring fish choruses in conjunction with other mainstream monitoring tools to inform management practices and stock assessments of fish populations in marine, brackish, and freshwater habitats worldwide.
Chapter
In the past 100 years, human activities have increased dramatically on both local and global scales, and as a result, levels of anthropogenic noise have risen in many habitats. Elevated noise has adverse effects for animals, including avoidance, triggering of stress, acoustic masking of cues used to find prey and to avoid predators, and disrupting the exchange of acoustic signals. Many species use acoustic signals in survival and reproduction, for example, to avoid predators or to find mating partners. When noise masks these biologically important signals, it can negatively affect the animals’ fitness and thus the viability of populations. Many species are less abundant near anthropogenic noise sources, but researchers are only just beginning to identify the implications of increasing noise pollution for animal populations, communities, food webs, and ecosystems. We need to increase our understanding of how anthropogenic noise reduces the maximum distance at which one animal can recognize the acoustic signals of another in its natural habitat. Future studies that link anthropogenic noise with population biology are urgently needed to develop reasonable action plans for noise abatement before the voices of many animals vanish forever.
Article
Full-text available
Anthropogenic noise can be detrimental to aquatic life through physical harm and behavioral impact. Physical harm to fish only occurs very close to typically brief but high-power sources. Behavioral impact occurs at more moderate levels and is spatially and temporally much more widespread. More studies are needed to get a better understanding of the behavioral impact on fish. Indoor and outdoor studies vary in their acoustic and behavioral validity and in the amount of experimental control. Although each approach has its limitations, scientific progress and applied insights will depend on the exploitation of their complementary potential.
Article
Full-text available
Noise is a ubiquitous feature in natural as well as in urban habitats. The presence of noise can have multiple direct and indirect effects on communication. Noise can directly mask signals leading to reduced detection and recognition. Noise also affects internal physiological processes which can influence attention and decision rules and, thus, indirectly affect communication. Also community effects of noise leading to lower densities or different community composition with respect to copying style or personality will affect communication through different communication distances and different daily behavioural routines. All these direct and indirect effects of noise on communication have been well documented but often are treated separately. This review focusses on some of the indirect effects of noise on animal communication, considering spatial responses, attentional effects and differential effects of noise depending on an animal’s characteristics, its personality. It evaluates how noise can indirectly influence communication and, thus, social network structure and processes of sexual and natural selection.
Book
Exposure to loud noise continues to be one of the largest causes of hearing loss in the adult population, already affecting some 15 percent of Americans between the ages of 20 and 69. There have been a number of discoveries and advances that have increased our understanding of the mechanisms of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss (NIHL). These advances have the potential to impact how NIHL can be prevented and how our noise standards can be made more appropriate. Noise-Induced Hearing Loss describes the effect of environmental noise on hearing, provides important background on the subject, and also explores the broader issues currently arising on effects of noise on non-human vertebrates. Perspectives on Noise-Induced Hearing Loss Colleen G. Le Prell and Donald Henderson The Public Health Significance of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss Peter M. Rabinowitz Noise-Induced Hearing Loss and Tinnitus: Challenges for the Military Marjorie A. M. Grantham The Use of Kurtosis Measurement in the Assessment of Potential Noise Trauma Donald Henderson and Roger P. Hamernik Noise-Induced Structural Damage to the Cochlea Bohua Hu Neural Coding of Sound with Cochlear Damage Eric D. Young Suprathreshold Auditory Processing in Noise-Induced Hearing Loss Mini N. Shrivastav The Neurobiology of Noise-Induced Tinnitus James A. Kaltenbach and Ryan Manz Effects of Early Noise Exposure on Subsequent Age-Related Changes in Hearing Eric C. Bielefeld Effects of Exposure to Chemicals on Noise-Induced Hearing Loss Thais C. Morata and Ann-Christin Johnson Hearing Protection Devices: Regulation, Current Trends, and Emerging Technologies John G. Casali Prevention of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss: Potential Therapeutic Agents Colleen G. Le Prell and Jianxin Bao Frontiers in the Treatment of Hearing Loss Tatsuya Yamasoba, Josef M. Miller, Mats Ulfendahl, and Richard A. Altschuler About the Editors Colleen Le Prell is an Associate Professor in the Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences and Director of the Hearing Research Center at the University of Florida. Donald Henderson is a Professor in the Department of Communication Disorders and Sciences at State University of New York (SUNY) at Buffalo. Richard R. Fay is Distinguished Research Professor of Psychology at Loyola University Chicago. Arthur N. Popper is a Professor in the Department of Biology and Co-Director of the Center for Comparative and Evolutionary Biology of Hearing at the University of Maryland, College Park. About the Series: The Springer Handbook of Auditory Research presents a series of synthetic reviews of fundamental topics dealing with auditory systems. Each volume is independent and authoritative; taken as a set, this series is the definitive resource in the field.
Book
A major goal of hearing research is to explain how the human auditory system normally functions and to help identify the causes of and treatments for hearing impairment. Experimental approaches to this research make use of animal models that are developed, evaluated and validated to determine what can be generalized from one species to another. By investigating the structures, physiological functions and hearing capabilities of various species, comparative hearing research establishes the biological and evolutionary context for such models. This volume brings together our current understanding of the auditory systems of two of the major vertebrate classes, fish and amphibians. It overcomes the differing theoretical and experimental paradigms that underlie most work on these groups and treats both fish and amphibians together in most chapters in order to address broader comparative issues.