Content uploaded by Dennis Grevenstein
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Dennis Grevenstein on Apr 12, 2018
Content may be subject to copyright.
Running Head: SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 1
Sense of Coherence and Substance Use: Examining Mutual Influences
Published 2014 in Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 52-57, doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2014.02.017
Dennis Grevensteina, Matthias Bluemkeb, Ede Nagya, Christina E. M. Wippermanna, Henrik
Kroeninger-Jungaberlea
a Institute of Medical Psychology, Centre for Psychosocial Medicine, University Hospital
Heidelberg, Bergheimer Str. 20, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany
b Psychological Institute, University of Heidelberg, Hauptstraße 47-51, 69117 Heidelberg,
Germany
Word count: 4999
Corresponding author:
Dennis Grevenstein
Institute of Medical Psychology, Centre for Psychosocial Medicine, University Hospital
Heidelberg, Bergheimer Str. 20, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany
Tel: +49 6221 568135
Email: dennis.grevenstein@med.uni-heidelberg.de
Declaration of interest: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments: This research is part of a longitudinal study on salutogenesis and drug
consumption patterns funded by the German Research Council (DFG) from 2002-2013 within its
Collaborative Research Centre (Sonderforschungsbereich) 619.
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 2
Abstract
Sense of coherence (SOC) is conceptualized as a mutable orientation to life, but has often been
found a stable attribute of individual differences. While several studies have documented the
relationship between SOC and substance use, nothing is known about mutual influences between
both variables over time in adolescence. The present study examines whether changes in SOC
predict changes in substance use, or whether changes in substance use predict changes in SOC. A
longitudinal cross-lagged panel design was used to inspect SOC and self-reported frequency of
substance use of tobacco, alcohol, and cannabis over the course of ten years. Participants were
318 German adolescents aged 14 to 15 at the beginning of the study. Structural equation
modeling indicated a single significant negative path from SOC to later cannabis use as well as
one significant positive path from cannabis use to SOC. Despite a general association – high SOC
corresponds to less substance use – SOC overall develops independently from substance use.
Keywords: Sense of coherence, alcohol, tobacco, cannabis, cross-lagged, longitudinal
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 3
1. Introduction
Sense of coherence (SOC) is the core aspect of Antonovsky’s salutogenic theory and is
conceptualized as a general resistance resource that promotes health (Antonovsky, 1987, 1998).
In this theory, health is not understood merely as an on/off state, but as a continuum between
health and disease. A stronger SOC should enable people to move towards the health-end of this
continuum.
According to Antonovsky, SOC represents a general “orientation-to-life”. It protects
people’s health in the face of adversities like critical life events and stress. Theoretically, three
major factors constitute SOC: comprehensibility, that is, an individual’s perception that situations
and events are structured and clear; manageability, that is, an individual’s belief that she has the
necessary skills to deal with the challenges of life; and meaningfulness, that is, an individual’s
belief that the demands and challenges of life are worthy of investment and engagement.
The positive influence of a SOC has been described numerous times. For example, SOC
has been linked to positive mental health and health-related behavioral outcomes (Eriksson &
Lindström, 2006; Togari, Yamazaki, Takayama, Yamaki, & Nakayama, 2008), general
psychological well-being (Nilsson, Leppert, Simonsson, & Starrin, 2010), depression (Haukkala
et al., 2013), and anxiety (Moksnes, Espnes, & Haugan, 2013). SOC has also received increasing
attention regarding the development of adolescent health issues (Rivera, García-Moya, Moreno,
& Ramos, 2013). Concerning substance use, high SOC was shown to predict reduced tobacco use
and lesser consumption of alcohol (Mattila et al., 2011) as well as less alcohol-related behavioral
problems (Nilsson, Starrin, Simonsson, & Leppert, 2007). This is especially important, as early
and high-frequent consumption has been associated with the development of later problematic
consumption styles (Behrendt, Wittchen, Höfler, Lieb, & Beesdo, 2009).
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 4
Antonovsky (1987) described the development of SOC as a dynamic process up to the age
of 30 that is supposedly influenced by external factors. Adolescence is seen as a particularly
important developmental phase for the development of SOC, and SOC is expected to be mutable
and fluctuant at this age. As such, SOC can be seen as part of an individual’s ontogenesis
characterized by several developmental tasks (Havighurst, 1972; Hurrelmann & Quenzel, 2012).
It has been documented that the use of psychoactive substances is an important aspect of
adolescent health behavior (Silbereisen, Noack, & Reitzle, 1987; Young et al., 2002) and, as
such, could also be considered a developmental task, for instance, an individual’s quest for
autonomy from the (adult or peer) mainstream, achieving peer-group acceptance, or the
development of coping strategies (Hurrelmann & Quenzel, 2012). SOC might either be a
protective factor for these tasks, yet it might as well be the result of successful coping.
The mutability and flexibility of SOC has received increasing attention in recent years.
Several studies have documented a surprising stability of SOC and a high test-retest reliability in
adulthood (Feldt, Leskinen, Kinnunen, & Mauno, 2000; Feldt, Leskinen, Kinnunen, & Ruoppila,
2003). In younger ages, SOC has also been shown to be rather stable, even through adolescence
(Honkinen et al., 2008). Similarly, SOC has been found not only to be a predictor, but also an
outcome of health. Longitudinally, psychological symptoms at the age of three (as reported by
parents) predicted lower SOC scores 15 years later at age 18 (Honkinen et al., 2009).
Theoretically, this association is astonishing, but it is not possible to control for the level of SOC
at such an early age. Contrasting these results on stability, SOC has been shown to increase after
positive life events, such as recovery from major depression (Skärsäter et al., 2009), social
intervention for unemployed people (Vastamäki, Moser, & Paul, 2009), therapeutic intervention
(Weissbecker et al., 2002), and even after clinical rehabilitation from cannabis abuse (Lundqvist,
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 5
1995). Consequently, SOC has been shown to decrease after negative life events (Lövheim,
Graneheim, Jonsén, Strandberg, & Lundman, 2013).
Given the astounding documented mutability of SOC, the following research aims to
examine mutual influences between SOC and substance use of tobacco, alcohol, and cannabis.
While the importance of SOC regarding substance use has been documented before, no study has
focused on mutual influences, that is, whether a change in one variable can predict a change in
the other variable at a later time. If SOC were truly flexible and considerably developed during
adolescence and early adulthood, one would expect an increase in health-benefitting behavior:
less substance use as SOC increases. Similarly, if SOC depended on experiences made during
adolescence (in this case with psychoactive substances), we would expect SOC to be predicted
over time by previous levels of substance use. To answer this question rigorously, a cross-lagged
panel design (structural equation modeling) is required.
2. Methods
2.1 Study sample
The following research is part of a ten-year-longitudinal study of drug use patterns
(RISA)1 conducted in the south of Germany from 2003 to 2012. The study comprised 14 data
collection events. Participants were 318 students (164 female; 51.6% and 154 male; 48.4%) with
a mean age of 14 at the beginning of the study. 65.4% of the participants (n = 208) grew up in a
traditional family, which was defined as living with both biological parents up to the age of 18
years. Level of education was also balanced across the three-tier German school system.
1 The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital Heidelberg (No. 218/2005).
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 6
While there was noticeable sample attrition (n = 134; 42.1%) over the course of ten years,
participant dropout was comparable to other studies on adolescents’ development (Honkinen et
al., 2009). There were some signs of systematic dropout. In comparison to participants remaining
in the study until the end, those who dropped out consumed moderately more tobacco at age 14 to
15, Ms = 3.59 vs. 2.70 (SDs = 2.49 vs. 2.11), t(248.14) = 3.30, p = .001, Cohen’s d = 0.39, and
more cannabis, Ms = 1.49 vs. 1.26 (SDs = 1.00 vs. 0.70), t(214.03) = 2.27, p = .024, d = 0.27.
2.2 Measures
2.2.1 SOC-13: Sense of coherence
SOC was measured using an abbreviated German 13-item adaptation of Antonovsky’s
original Orientation to Life questionnaire with five-point rating scales (most of the time ranging
from 0=very rarely to 4=very often) (Abel, Kohlmann, & Noack, 1995). For comparability with a
later authorative German version developed by Schumacher and colleagues (Schumacher,
Gunzelmann, & Brähler, 2000), scores were rescaled to a seven-point rating scale format using a
linear transformation.2 The scale includes four meaningfulness items (e.g., “Do you have the
feeling that you don’t really care about what goes on around you?”), five comprehensibility
items (e.g., “Has it happened in the past that you were surprised by the behavior of people whom
you thought you knew well?”) and four manageability items (e.g., “Has it happened that people
whom you counted on disappointed you?”). Replicating the three-factor structure of the SOC
scale has been empirically challenging (Klepp, Mastekaasa, Sørensen, Sandanger, & Kleiner,
2007; Zimprich, Allemand, & Hornung, 2006). As Antonovsky (1987) stressed the holistic nature
of the SOC scale and recommended not to use subscale scores, a sum score is commonly used. In
our sample, Cronbach’s Alpha of the scale increased from .80 to .92 over the course of ten years.
2 The rescaled values resemble the norms published by Schumacher et al. (2000) who reported a SOC sum score of
M = 67.31 (SD = 12.09) for men and M = 64.52 (SD = 11.61) for women spanning a wider age range from 18 to 40.
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 7
2.2.2 Substance use frequency
The substance use scale was adapted from the national survey on drug use among
adolescents (BZgA, 2004). It is similar to the brief self-report drug use frequency measure
provided by O'Farrell, Fals-Stewart and Murphy (2003). 6-month-substance use frequency was
measured using a single item question: “How often have you used this substance in the last 6
months?” Answers were given separately for tobacco, alcohol, and cannabis on seven-point
scales with the following options: (1) “not used in last 6 months”, (2) “1-2 times in the last 6
months”, (3) “3-5 times in the last 6 months”, (4) “1-3 times a month”, (5) “1-2 times a week”,
(6) “several times a week”, and (7) “several times a day”.
2.3 Statistical analysis
We used SPSS 21 for descriptive data analyses and Mplus 5.21 (Muthén, 1998-2007) for
Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). With SEM (Kline, 2011) multiple relationships among
several variables in a model can be inspected concurrently. Specifically, the cross-lagged panel
design allows to model unique predictive influence across time. It estimates the associations and
mutual influences among the variables. Hence it allows estimating the development of
psychological factors over time while controlling for interindividual differences in previous
behavior. The main focus is on the diagonal (longitudinal) paths from one type of variable to
another type of variable at the next time point. Vertical (cross-sectional) paths between variables,
and horizontal (autocorrelative longitudinal) paths within a variable are merely used for
controlling statistical covariation. Yet, the diagonal, cross-lagged paths represent partial
regressions that indicate the unique predictive influence of a variable at a given time.
SEM involves the estimation of variances of variables as well as of covariances between
variables (Kline, 2011). This approach usually requires larger samples as the number of variables
included in a model increases. Due to sample attrition after ten years, we had to reduce the
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 8
number of model parameters to be estimated and therefore aggregated data over time by
computing mean scores. The RISA study included 14 data collection events. In the first four
cases we aggregated three data collection events to single data points (T0, T1, T2, T3), whereas
the last data point (T4) comprised only two data collection events. The five data points over the
course of the ten-year study represented age 14–15 (T0), age 16–17 (T1), age 18–19 (T2), age
20–22 (T3), and age 23–24 (T4). We included cross-sectional covariation between SOC and
substance use at the beginning and at the end of the study to control for covariation between both
types of variables. For the longitudinal aspect, we regressed every type of variable at a given
point in time on the same type of variable at the preceding point in time (autocorrelations). The
models also include covaration paths with gender and family setting for both, substance use and
SOC, at the first data point to control for covariates.
The goodness-of-fit of the models was evaluated by (1) the—ideally non-significant—χ2
test (Bentler & Bonett, 1980) and as low as possible a χ2/df ratio, ideally as low as 2 (Tabachnick
& Fidell, 2007); (2) the comparative fit index (CFI) with values of .90/.95 and above indicating
appropriate/good model fit (Bentler, 1990; Hu & Bentler, 1999); (3) the root mean square error of
approximation (RMSEA) with values of .00–.05/.06–.08/.09–.10 indicating good/reasonable/poor
model fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993); and (4) the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR)
with values less than .08 considered to reflect good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). A robust Maximum
Likelihood (MLR) algorithm was used for parameter estimation and imputation of missing data
(a total of 25% of all cells across all variables and time points over the whole 10-year span).
3. Results
3.1 Descriptive data analysis
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 9
Several ANOVAs with a five-level repeated measurement factor (time) were conducted to
analyze longitudinal shifts of variable means. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of
sphericity had been violated for all ANOVAs. Consequently, Greenhouse-Geisser corrected
degrees of freedom will be reported. Similar to prior studies (Young et al., 2002), substance use
increased significantly from T0 to T4 for tobacco, F(2.47, 317.98) = 14.85, p < .001, η2 = .10
(Ms = 2.63, 2.89, 3.42, 3.53, 3.65), alcohol, F(2.97, 454.29) = 73.53, p < .001, η2 = .02 (Ms =
2.57, 3.40, 3.87, 3.91, 3.93), and cannabis, F(3.14, 314.44) = 6.86, p < .001, η2 = .33 (Ms = 1.20,
1.30, 1.53, 1.63, 1.62). Additionally, there was also a marginally significant increase in SOC
from T0 to T4, F(2.34, 370.22) = 2.67, p = .062, η2 = .02 (Ms = 63.65, 64.24, 65.37, 64.61,
65.88).
Gender differences were evident. Men tended to consume more alcohol and cannabis.
Concerning SOC, male adolescents scored significantly higher than female participants at the
beginning of the study, but this difference did not subsequently retain significance. Means for
SOC and substance use frequency can be seen in Figure 1.
Individual differences in SOC appeared to be moderately stable during adolescence. From
T0 to T4, autocorrelations amounted to r(190) = .46, p < .001. This means that SOC was
descriptively less stable than tobacco use, which correlated from T0 to T4 at r(167) = .54, p <
.001, but more stable than alcohol use, r(182) = .32, p < .001, and cannabis use, r(141) = .42, p <
.001, respectively. As such, the relative stability of SOC scores among people at a young age,
despite statistically significant developmental shifts of means of the sample, was remarkable, but
not overwhelming. There was still sufficient variability in SOC left to be explained by the
influence of developmental tasks such as handling one’s substance use.
3.2 Structural equation modeling (SEM)
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 10
3.2.1 Tobacco
Figure 2 shows the standardized estimates of the SEM paths for SOC and tobacco
consumption. The model fitted the data well, χ2(24) = 45.87, χ2/df = 1.91, p < .01, RMSEA =
.054, CFI = .981, SRMR = .032. Differences in individuals’ consumption and SOC scores were
quite stable over time. SOC correlated negatively (cross-sectionally) at T0 and T4. The path from
tobacco use at T2 to SOC at T3 turned marginally significant. No other paths were significant.
3.2.2 Alcohol
Figure 3 displays the standardized paths for SOC and alcohol. This model also fitted the
data well, χ2(24) = 46.95, χ2/df = 2.08, p < .01, RMSEA = .055, CFI = .974, SRMR = .040.
Again, the stability of SOC and alcohol use is evident. SOC had a significant negative correlation
at T0, replicating Mattila and colleagues’ (2011) as well as Nilsson and colleagues’ (2007) cross-
sectional findings. No other paths were significant.
3.2.3 Cannabis
The standardized paths for cannabis consumption and SOC are found in Figure 4. The
model also fitted the data well, χ2(24) = 61.90, χ2/df = 2.58, p < .001, RMSEA = .070, CFI = .953,
SRMR = .057. Again, SOC and cannabis use were relatively stable. Compared to the other
substances, this model revealed most mutual influences. There was a significant negative cross-
sectional correlation at T0. Yet, future Cannabis use at T1 was significantly reduced by high SOC
scores at baseline, even when controlling for prior substance use. Additionally, there was a
significant positive path from cannabis use at T2 to SOC at T3. No other paths were significant.
4. Discussion
One objective of the present research was to examine mutual influences between
substance use and sense of coherence (SOC). Whereas individual differences in SOC appeared
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 11
moderately stable (cf. Honkinen et al., 2008, 2009), our analysis revealed very little mutual
influence between SOC and substance use. With only one significant negative longitudinal path
from SOC to later cannabis use, for the most part, changes in SOC did not predict changes in
substance use. Notably, even while controlling for preexisting correlations between cannabis use
and SOC as well as for the respective autocorrelations of the two variables, at this young age
SOC was still a significant predictor of cannabis use two years later. This hints at a possible
causal protective influence of salutogenic resources for later frequency of cannabis consumption.
Regarding the mutability of SOC as an orientation-to-life, supposedly forming under
situational and contextual forces, there was only one significant path from cannabis use to later
SOC, where SOC was influenced by any substance use. This path in fact had a positive
regression weight, that is, an increase in substance use predicted an increase in SOC. This pattern
may support the hypothesis that dealing with substance use may – at the same time – represent
successful dealing with developmental tasks (Silbereisen et al., 1987). More research seems
necessary whether it is actually the successful completion of developmental challenges that
fosters such an increase.
Taken together, the previously documented associations between SOC and substance use
emerged, yet SOC, by and large, appears to exist or develop rather independently from substance
use. This is rather surprising, considering the presumed mutability of SOC in adolescence and the
significant role that the consumption of psychoactive substances plays at this age (Young et al.,
2002). According to theory, SOC develops throughout adolescence and early adulthood, shaped
by interpersonal relationships and personal experiences, until it stabilizes around the age of 30
(Antonovsky, 1987). Hence, our results are not in line with Antonovsky’s conception of SOC
being an externally shaped orientation to life rather than a temperamental personality trait. The
lack of mutual influences between SOC and substance use, may in fact be due to SOC resembling
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 12
emotional stability. Considerable overlap between SOC and Neuroticism has been reported
before (Feldt, Metsäpelto, Kinnunen, & Pulkkinen, 2007; Hochwälder, 2012). However, as our
own data show, SOC contains unique variance that incrementally explains substance use over and
above Neuroticism and other competing personality variables (Grevenstein, Bluemke, &
Kroeninger-Jungaberle, 2014). Hence SOC cannot simply be equated with a positively framed (or
reversed) version of Neuroticism. Rather the interplay between flexible and stable aspects of
SOC and their relationships to Neuroticism remain research topics for future investigations.
4.1 Limitations
As our sample size was just sufficiently large enough for structural equation modeling, it
was necessary to aggregate data collection events over time. While possibly even increasing the
reliability of the findings, this approach may have masked short-term changes. We also
documented hints at systematic dropout from the study. Participants who dropped out consumed
more tobacco and cannabis at age 14 to 15. As both models for tobacco use and cannabis use are
the ones with most significant paths, this seems only a minor issue, but dropout may imply
underestimated variance towards the end of the study. This potentially masked mutual influences
that would have emerged if the whole spectrum of individuals had been analyzed. Still, at earlier
time points, with less sample attrition, there were little signs for mutual influences.
4.2 Future research
Given some positive influence from substance use to later SOC, researchers may further
look into certain forms of substance use as a way of coping with developmental tasks and its
influence on the development of identity and personality. Furthermore, as substance use is in
many ways gender specific (Chen & Jacobson, 2012), and given that gender differences were
evident in our sample, the future use of multigroup SEM with larger samples might help to
investigate gender-specific hypotheses.
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 13
Finally, we assessed substance use by means of a frequency measure. While frequency of
substance consumption has been documented as a good predictor of health related outcomes
(Young et al., 2002), it does not allow us to discriminate between adolescents' perception of their
substance use as normative, damaging, helpful, or of little consequence. The motivations behind
substance use and the perceived effects cannot be uncovered by frequency scales. As such,
consumption may or may not be problematic, yet in a transitional phase it may simply reflect
normative behavior among peers. Successful coping with this challenge might teach one a lot,
contributing to positive personality changes; at least in retrospect such a phase might count as
valuable aspect in life (Mezquita, Stewart, & Ruipérez, 2010).
4.3 Conclusions
The present research examined mutual influence between substance use and SOC. So far
the evidence for this relationship has been inconclusive. Our findings clearly show that, in
absolute terms, both SOC and substance use change throughout adolescence. Yet, neither do
individual changes in SOC correspond to altered consumption patterns, nor does individual
substance use impair the natural development of one’s salutogenic resources, as would be implied
by salutogenic theory. Our results indicated that SOC develops by and large independently from
substance use. Given the overall association of high SOC with less substance use, SOC is a
reasonable general predictor, and individual differences in SOC at early adolescence are already
quite revealing. Yet, with some notable exceptions, SOC is not shaped by substance use.
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 14
References
Abel, T., Kohlmann, T., & Noack, H. (1995). Eine deutsche Übersetzung des SOC. Institut für
Sozial- und Präventivmedizin. Bern.
Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay
well. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Antonovsky, A. (1998). The sense of coherence: An historical and future perspective. In H. I.
McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson & J. E. Fromer (Eds.), Stress, coping, and
health in families: Sense of coherence and resiliency (pp. 3-20). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Behrendt, S., Wittchen, H. U., Höfler, M., Lieb, R., & Beesdo, K. (2009). Transitions from first
substance use to substance use disorders in adolescence: Is early onset associated with a
rapid escalation? Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 99, 68-78. doi:
10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.06.014
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107,
238-246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of
covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606. doi: 10.1037/0033-
2909.88.3.588
Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen &
J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage.
BZgA. (2004). Bundeszentrale für gesundheitliche Aufklärung. Die Drogenaffinität Jugendlicher
in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland. Köln: BZgA 2004.
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 15
Chen, P., & Jacobson, K. C. (2012). Developmental trajectories of substance use from early
adolescence to young adulthood: Gender and racial/ethnic differences. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 50, 154-163. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.05.013
Eriksson, M., & Lindström, B. (2006). Antonovsky's Sense of Coherence Scale and the relation
with health: A systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 60,
376-381. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.041616
Feldt, T., Leskinen, E., Kinnunen, U., & Mauno, S. (2000). Longitudinal factor analysis models
in the assessment of the stability of sense of coherence. Personality and Individual
Differences, 28, 239-257. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(99)00094-x
Feldt, T., Leskinen, E., Kinnunen, U., & Ruoppila, I. (2003). The stability of sense of coherence:
Comparing two age groups in a 5-year follow-up study. Personality and Individual
Differences, 35, 1151-1165. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00325-2
Feldt, T., Metsäpelto, R., Kinnunen, U., & Pulkkinen, L. (2007). Sense of coherence and five-
factor approach to personality: Conceptual relationships. European Psychologist, 12, 165-
172. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.12.3.165
Grevenstein, D., Bluemke, M., & Kroeninger-Jungaberle, H. (2014). Incremental validity of
sense of coherence, neuroticism, extraversion, and self-efficacy: Longitudinal prediction
of substance use frequency and mental health. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Haukkala, A., Konttinen, H., Lehto, E., Uutela, A., Kawachi, I., & Laatikainen, T. (2013). Sense
of Coherence, Depressive Symptoms, Cardiovascular Diseases, and All-Cause Mortality.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 75, 429-435. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31828c3fa4
Havighurst, R. J. (1972). Developmental tasks and education. New York: McKay.
Hochwälder, J. (2012). The contribution of the big five personality factors to sense of coherence.
Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 591-596. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.008
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 16
Honkinen, P., Aromaa, M., Suominen, S., Rautava, P., Sourander, A., Helenius, H., & Sillanpää,
M. (2009). Early childhood psychological problems predict a poor sense of coherence in
adolescents: A 15-year follow-up study. Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 587-600. doi:
10.1177/1359105309103578
Honkinen, P., Suominen, S., Helenius, H., Aromaa, M., Rautava, P., Sourander, A., & Sillanpää,
M. (2008). Stability of the sense of coherence in adolescence. International Journal of
Adolescent Medicine and Health, 20, 85-91. doi: 10.1515/ijamh.2008.20.1.85
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. doi:
10.1080/10705519909540118
Hurrelmann, K., & Quenzel, G. (2012). Lebensphase Jugend: eine Einführung in die
sozialwissenschaftliche Jugendforschung (11. ed.). Weinheim: Beltz.
Klepp, O. M., Mastekaasa, A., Sørensen, T., Sandanger, I., & Kleiner, R. (2007). Structure
analysis of Antonovsky's sense of coherence from an epidemiological mental health
survey with a brief nine-item sense of coherence scale. International Journal of Methods
in Psychiatric Research, 16, 11-22. doi: 10.1002/mpr.197
Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Lövheim, H., Graneheim, U. H., Jonsén, E., Strandberg, G., & Lundman, B. (2013). Changes in
sense of coherence in old age—A 5-year follow-up of the Umeå 85+ study. Scandinavian
Journal of Caring Sciences, 27, 13-19. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2012.00988.x
Lundqvist, T. (1995). Chronic cannabis use and the sense of coherence. Life Sciences, 56, 2145-
2150. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(95)00201-G
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 17
Mattila, M., Rautava, P., Honkinen, P., Ojanlatva, A., Jaakkola, S., Aromaa, M., . . . Sillanpää,
M. (2011). Sense of coherence and health behaviour in adolescence. Acta Paediatrica,
100, 1590-1595. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02376.x
Mezquita, L., Stewart, S. H., & Ruipérez, M. (2010). Big-five personality domains predict
internal drinking motives in young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 49,
240-245. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.043
Moksnes, U. K., Espnes, G. A., & Haugan, G. (2013). Stress, sense of coherence and emotional
symptoms in adolescents. Psychology & Health, 29, 32-49. doi:
10.1080/08870446.2013.822868
Muthén, L.K., Muthén, B.O. (1998-2007). Mplus User’s Guide. Fifth Edition. Los Angeles, CA:
Muthén & Muthén.
Nilsson, K. W., Leppert, J., Simonsson, B., & Starrin, B. (2010). Sense of coherence and
psychological well-being: Improvement with age. Journal of Epidemiology and
Community Health, 64, 347-352. doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.081174
Nilsson, K. W., Starrin, B., Simonsson, B., & Leppert, J. (2007). Alcohol-related problems
among adolescents and the role of a sense of coherence. International Journal of Social
Welfare, 16, 159-167. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2397.2006.00452.x
O'Farrell, T. J., Fals-Stewart, W., & Murphy, M. (2003). Concurrent validity of a brief self-report
drug use frequency measure. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 327-337. doi: 10.1016/S0306-
4603(01)00226-X
Rivera, F., García-Moya, I., Moreno, C., & Ramos, P. (2013). Developmental contexts and sense
of coherence in adolescence: A systematic review. Journal of Health Psychology, 18,
800-812.
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 18
Schumacher, J., Gunzelmann, T., & Brähler, E. (2000). Deutsche Normierung der Sense of
Coherence Scale von Antonovsky. Diagnostica, 46, 208-213. doi: 10.1026//0012-
1924.46.4.208
Silbereisen, R. K., Noack, P., & Reitzle, M. (1987). Developmental perspectives on problem
behavior and prevention in adolescence. In K. Hurrelmann, F.-X. Kaufmann & F. Lösel
(Eds.), Social intervention: Potential and constraints. (pp. 205-218). Oxford, UK: Walter
De Gruyter.
Skärsäter, I., Rayens, M. K., Peden, A., Hall, L., Zhang, M., Ågren, H., & Prochazka, H. (2009).
Sense of coherence and recovery from major depression: A 4-year follow-up. Archives of
Psychiatric Nursing, 23, 119-127. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2008.04.007
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA:
Allyn & Bacon/Pearson Education.
Togari, T., Yamazaki, Y., Takayama, T. S., Yamaki, C. K., & Nakayama, K. (2008). Follow-up
study on the effects of sense of coherence on well-being after two years in Japanese
university undergraduate students. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1335-1347.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.002
Vastamäki, J., Moser, K., & Paul, K. I. (2009). How stable is sense of coherence? Changes
following an intervention for unemployed individuals. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 50, 161-171. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00695.x
Weissbecker, I., Salmon, P., Studts, J. L., Floyd, A. R., Dedert, E. A., & Sephton, S. E. (2002).
Mindfulness-based stress reduction and sense of coherence among women with
fibromyalgia. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 9, 297-307. doi:
10.1023/A:1020786917988
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 19
Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., Stallings, M. C., Rhee, S. H., Crowley, T. J., & Hewitt, J. K. (2002).
Substance use, abuse and dependence in adolescence: Prevalence, symptom profiles and
correlates. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 68, 309-322. doi: 10.1016/s0376-
8716(02)00225-9
Zimprich, D., Allemand, M., & Hornung, R. (2006). Measurement invariance of the abridged
sense of coherence scale in adolescents. European Journal of Psychological Assessment,
22, 280-287. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.22.4.280
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 20
Figure 1: Means for study variables at T0 (age 14-15), T1 (age 16-17), T2 (age 18-19), T3 (age 20-22) and T4 (age 23-24).
Men and women differ significantly at †p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 (two-tailed).
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
T0
T1
T2
T3
T4
Tob acc o U se
total
men
woman
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
T0
T1†
T2**
T3***
T4***
Alcohol Use
total
men
woman
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
T0**
T1**
T2***
T3**
T4***
Cannabis Use
total
men
woman
58
60
62
64
66
68
T0**
T1
T2
T3
T4
Sense of Coherence
total
men
woman
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 21
Figure 2: Cross-lagged panel design for sense of coherence (SOC) and tobacco use frequency (TOB) with covariates gender and home
(traditional family setting). Paths are significant at †p < .10, *p < .05 and **p < .01 (two-tailed).
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 22
Figure 3: Cross-lagged panel design for sense of coherence (SOC) and alcohol use frequency (ALC) with covariates gender and home
(traditional family setting). Paths are significant at *p < .05 and **p < .01 (two-tailed).
SENSE OF COHERENCE AND SUBSTANCE USE 23
Figure 4: Cross-lagged panel design for sense of coherence (SOC) and cannabis use frequency (CAN) with covariates gender and home
(traditional family setting). Paths are significant at *p < .05 and **p < .01 (two-tailed).