ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Abstract Recent research on spatial number representations suggest that the number space is not necessarily horizontally organized and might also be affected by acquired associations between magnitude and sensory experiences in vertical space. Evidence for this claim is however controversial. The present study now aims to compare vertical and horizontal spatial associations in mental arithmetic. In Experiment 1, participants solved addition and subtraction problems and indicated the result verbally while moving their outstretched right arm continuously either left-, right-, up- or downwards. The analysis of the problem solving performances revealed a motion-arithmetic compatibility effect for spatial actions along both the horizontal and vertical axes. Performances in additions was impaired while making downward compared to upward movements as well as when moving left compared to right and vice versa in subtractions. In Experiment 2, instead of instructing to perform active body movements, participants calculated while the problems moved in one of the four relative directions on the screen. For visual motions, only the motion-arithmetic compatibility effect for the vertical dimension could be replicated. Taken together, our findings provide first evidence for an impact of spatial processing on mental arithmetic. Moreover, the stronger effect of the vertical dimension supports the idea that mental calculations operate on representations of numerical magnitude that are grounded in a vertically organized mental number space.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by: [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam]
On: 27 July 2014, At: 04:28
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:
Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription
information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20
Spatial interferences in mental arithmetic:
Evidence from the motion–arithmetic
compatibility effect
Michael Wiemersa, Harold Bekkeringa & Oliver Lindemannab
a Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University
Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
b Division of Cognitive Science, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
Accepted author version posted online: 31 Jan 2014.Published online: 05
Mar 2014.
To cite this article: Michael Wiemers, Harold Bekkering & Oliver Lindemann (2014) Spatial interferences
in mental arithmetic: Evidence from the motion–arithmetic compatibility effect, The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 67:8, 1557-1570, DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2014.889180
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.889180
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our
licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or
suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication
are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor &
Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently
verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any
losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial
or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use
can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Spatial interferences in mental arithmetic: Evidence from
the motionarithmetic compatibility effect
Michael Wiemers
1
, Harold Bekkering
1
, and Oliver Lindemann
1,2
1
Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen,
The Netherlands
2
Division of Cognitive Science, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
Recent research on spatial number representations suggests that the number space is not necessarily
horizontally organized and might also be affected by acquired associations between magnitude and
sensory experiences in vertical space. Evidence for this claim is, however, controversial. The present
study now aims to compare vertical and horizontal spatial associations in mental arithmetic. In
Experiment 1, participants solved addition and subtraction problems and indicated the result verbally
while moving their outstretched right arm continuously left-, right-, up-, or downwards. The analysis of
the problem-solving performances revealed a motionarithmetic compatibility effect for spatial actions
along both the horizontal and the vertical axes. Performances in additions was impaired while making
downward compared to upward movements as well as when moving left compared to right and vice
versa in subtractions. In Experiment 2, instead of being instructed to perform active body movements,
participants calculated while the problems moved in one of the four relative directions on the screen. For
visual motions, only the motionarithmetic compatibility effect for the vertical dimension could be
replicated. Taken together, our ndings provide rst evidence for an impact of spatial processing on
mental arithmetic. Moreover, the stronger effect of the vertical dimension supports the idea that
mental calculations operate on representations of numerical magnitude that are grounded in a vertically
organized mental number space.
Keywords: Mental arithmetic; Numerical cognition; Spatialnumerical associations; Embodied
cognition.
Classical research on mathematical cognition has
emphasized the importance of spatial codes
for the representation of numerical magnitude
(see e.g., Fias & Fischer, 2005; Hubbard, Piazza,
Pinel, & Dehaene, 2005). Very recently, the idea
of spatialnumerical associations has been utilized
to formulate theories of mental arithmetic, which
basically hold that adding or subtracting numbers
involves a cognitive process that can be described
as right- or leftward attentional shifts on a spatial
representation of numerical magnitude (Knops,
Viarouge, & Dehaene, 2009; McCrink, Dehaene,
& Dehaene-Lambertz, 2007; Pinhas & Fischer,
2008).
Studies on spatial associations in number
processing have traditionally focused on numerical
judgements and interference effects between
numerical information and horizontal space. The
most prominent example of this coupling is the
effect of spatialnumerical association of response
codes (SNARC; Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux,
1993), which reects the tendency to react faster
Correspondence should be addressed to: Michael Wiemers, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, P.O. Box 9104,
6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands. E-mail: m.wiemers@donders.ru.nl
© 2014 The Experimental Psychology Society 1557
THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014
Vol. 67, No. 8, 15571570, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.889180
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
with right-side responses to large numbers and
with left-side responses to small numbers. This
numberresponse compatibility effect indicates
that numerical magnitudes are associated with pos-
itions along the horizontal axis with small numbers
on the left and large number on the right (so-called
mental number line hypothesis; Dehaene et al.,
1993; Fias & Fischer, 2005).
Interestingly, research has shown that the inter-
action between number and space also extends to
the vertical dimension (Holmes & Lourenco,
2012; Ito & Hatta, 2004; Schwarz & Keus, 2004;
Shaki & Fischer, 2012). Schwarz and Keus (2004)
were amongst the rst to report a vertical number
response compatibility effect and demonstrated
that upward saccades were initiated faster in
response to large numbers, while downwardsaccades
were initiated faster in response to small numbers.
However, studies on number representation that
compared vertical and horizontal spatialnumerical
couplings provide so far no consistent evidence on
whether the vertical or horizontal organization is
predominant (Holmes & Lourenco, 2012; Sell &
Kaschak, 2012; Shaki & Fischer, 2012; Winter &
Matlock, 2013). One the one hand, Shaki and
Fischer (2012) reported that Hebrew-speaking
participants who do not show a SNARC effect
with horizontally aligned responses due to incon-
sistent processing habits for words and numbers
exhibit a vertical SNARC effect. This nding
suggests a more robust and culturally independent
mapping between vertical space and magnitude.
Also, Winter and Matlock (2013) provided
support in favour of a strong vertical organization
of number space by showing that the production
of random numbers is affected stronger by vertical
than by horizontal head movements. On the
other hand, a predominance of vertical number
associations has been challenged by Holmes and
Lourenco (2012). The authors tested SNARC-
like effects in a parity judgement task with different
spatial response button layouts and found that
when pitting both dimensions directly against
each other, only a horizontal but not a vertical
numberspace mapping could be observed.
Although the empirical evidence for a predomi-
nantly vertical organization of number space is
ambiguous at this point, we argue that this idea is
in line with recent theoretical developments in psy-
chology of knowledge representations and the
general hypothesis that any conceptual represen-
tation is somehow grounded in sensorimotor
experiences (e.g., Barsalou, 2008; Lakoff, 1987).
Following such an embodied view on cognition,
Fischer and Brugger (2011) pointed out that
especially sensorimotor experience in vertical
space play a crucial role in the development of
abstract number concepts. They stated that due to
a strong correlation between vertical space and
numerical magnitude in the worldfor example,
through piling objects when comparing the numer-
osity of two sets of objects and the fact that stacks
extend upwards as they growan association
between these two dimensions emerges from early
life on. However, in the action of piling objects,
numerical magnitude is confounded with stack
height. As a consequence, vertical space should
become associated with numerical magnitude and
magnitude at an abstract level. Therefore, it is
more accurate to speak of an association of vertical
space and magnitude. Crucially, no systematic
relationship between horizontal space and
numerical magnitude exists when arranging
objects horizontally. One might therefore assume
that mappings of numbers onto vertical space
provide the grounding for any type of spatial
numerical mapping (henceforth referred to as
the grounding hypothesis of spatialnumerical
associations).
As mentioned above, the activation of spatial
codes is not restricted to the representation of
single numbers but also seems to play a crucial
role for mental arithmetic (McCrink et al.,
2007; Pinhas & Fischer, 2008). Until now,
studies of the interplay between mental arithmetic
and spatial processing have merely focused on the
horizontal dimension. McCrink et al. (2007) were
amongst the rst to discuss the interaction
between spatial processing and mental arithmetic.
The authors reported that when adding or sub-
tracting the numerosities of two dot clouds, par-
ticipants tend to overestimate the result of
additions and underestimate the result of subtrac-
tions (see also Knops, Viarouge, et al., 2009).
1558 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8)
WIEMERS ET AL.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
This so-called operational momentum effect has
been interpreted as evidence for the idea that
addition and subtraction are accompanied by
attentional shifts to the right (addition) and left
(subtraction) on the mental number line, inducing
an over- or underestimation, respectively (Knops,
Viarouge, et al., 2009; McCrink et al, 2007).
Similar effects of the operation have also been
observed in symbolic arithmetic. For instance,
Pinhas and Fischer (2008), who instructed par-
ticipants to indicate the outcome of symbolic
additions and subtractions by pointing to the cor-
responding location on the horizontal number
line, found a systematic response bias towards
larger numbers (i.e., the right side) after adding
and towards smaller numbers (i.e., the left side)
after subtracting.
The notion that shifts on a spatial or spatial
numerical continuum are causing the operational
momentum effect received so far only support
from two ndings. First, Knops, Viarouge, et al.
(2009) reported a space-to-arithmetic mapping.
That is, participants preferred to choose the upper
right dot cloud for additions and the upper left
dot cloud for subtractions. Second, neuroimaging
research (Knops, Thirion, Hubbard, Michel, &
Dehaene, 2009) demonstrated that activity in the
posterior superior parietal cortex related to horizon-
tal eye movements was correlated with mental
additions and subtractions. This evidence is
however rather indirect, and a strong test of the
coupling of space and arithmetic, such as an effect
of spatial processes on the performances in arith-
metic tasks, is still missing.
Taken together, the grounding hypothesis of
spatialnumerical associations predicts predomi-
nant vertical organization of number space and
stronger interference effects for vertical rather
than horizontal spatial information in number pro-
cessing. Since research on number representations
provided only inconclusive evidence for this idea
and since vertical mappings have been neglected
in studies on mental arithmetic so far, the present
study aims to compare vertical and horizontal
spatial associations while solving addition and
subtraction problems.
We addressed these issues by employing a dual-
task-like paradigm, in which participants per-
formed slow continuous arm movements in the
horizontal (left- or rightward) or vertical dimen-
sion (up- or downwards) while solving addition
or subtraction problems. We expected an effect
of the compatibility between the performed move-
ment and the mental arithmetic task (motion
arithmetic compatibility effect). That is, based on
studies on the operational momentum effect
(e.g., Knops, Viarouge, et al., 2009), a motion
arithmetic compatibility effect should be reected
in the horizontal dimension by better performance
for addition for a rightward movement than for a
leftward movement and better performance in sub-
traction for leftward than for rightward move-
ments. Importantly, the grounding hypothesis of
spatialnumerical associations predicts further-
more a stronger motionarithmetic compatibility
effect along the vertical dimension, with facilitated
processing of additions while moving upward and
facilitated processing of subtractions for downward
movements.
EXPERIMENT 1
Participants were instructed to perform constant-
speed arm movements with the fully extended
right arm, starting pointing straight forward and
moving left-, right-, up-, or downwards. Arm
movements had to be initiated at the presentation
of an arithmetic (addition or subtraction) problem
on the screen and had to be continued until an
answer was given verbally. Assuming that the
spatial coding of numerical magnitudes is function-
ally involved in mental arithmetic, it is expected
that the execution of arm movements in space has
a systematic impact on solving simple additions
and subtraction problems. In particular, we expect
that the processing of arithmetic problems is
more efcient while performing a compatible
action than while performing an incompatible
action (motionarithmetic compatibility effect).
Moreover, we expect the motionarithmetic
THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8) 1559
SPATIAL INTERFERENCE IN MENTAL ARITHMETIC
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
compatibility effect for vertical arm movements to
be stronger than that for horizontal arm
movements.
METHOD
Participants
A total of 38 students from the Radboud University
Nijmegen (11 males, mean age of 20.49 years, 20
native Dutch and 18 native German speakers)
with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, naive
to the purpose of the study, took part in the study
in exchange for 15 euros or credit points.
Experimental set-up and apparatus
Participants were standing at approximately 3 m
distance from a 17′′ CRT monitor holding the
Wii®-controller (Nintendo) in their right hand,
which was used to record the hand position with
a sampling rate of 100 Hz. To detect the voice
onset, a microphone was placed in front of the
head of the subject. The set-up consisted of two
interconnected computers, one for the recording
of the Wii controller position data and one for pre-
sentations of the stimuli and the voice onset detec-
tion. The experiment was controlled using the
Python software package Expyriment (Krause &
Lindemann, 2013). The experimenter registered
the verbally given answers and the correctness of
the executed movements.
Stimuli
All problems consisted of two operands and one
operator (plus or minus) and were presented cen-
trally in black Arial font on a light grey background
(see Table 1). At a viewing distance of 3 metres, the
problems were spanning approximately 4° (hori-
zontal) ×2° (vertical) in visual angle. Subtraction
and addition problems were matched for overall
problem size (i.e., average of all numbers in the
correct equation) by inverting addition into sub-
traction problems (e.g., 34 +58 =92, 92 58 =
34). The arithmetic problems in the experimental
block consisted of 15 small-size addition and sub-
traction problems (problem size ranging from 22
to 34, mean result: 13.2 for additions and 6.73
for subtractions) and 15 large-size addition and
Table 1. Arithmetic problems used for the experimental phase
Problem number Operand 1 Operation Operand 2 Result
19+817
27+916
39+615
45+914
59+413
63+912
78+715
86+814
98+513
10 3 +811
11 7 +613
12 5 +712
13 7 +411
14 6 +511
15 9 +211
16 27 +36 63
17 39 +25 64
18 29 +38 67
19 27 +44 71
20 49 +23 72
21 28 +53 81
22 37 +45 82
23 44 +39 83
24 56 +28 84
25 47 +38 85
26 57 +29 86
27 25 +66 91
28 38 +54 92
29 26 +67 93
30 36 +59 95
31 17 89
32 16 97
33 15 69
34 14 95
35 13 49
36 12 93
37 15 78
38 14 86
39 13 58
40 11 83
41 13 67
42 12 75
43 11 47
44 11 56
45 11 29
46 63 36 27
47 64 25 39
48 67 38 29
49 71 44 27
50 72 23 49
(Continued overleaf)
1560 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8)
WIEMERS ET AL.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
subtraction problems (problem size ranging 124 to
190, mean result: 80.60 for additions and 37.67 for
subtractions). A different set of arithmetic pro-
blems was used in the training phase.
A black square frame of 2° ×2° in visual angle
was used as a starting position of each arm move-
ment. Block arrows pointing to the left, to the
right, up, or down served as movement cues. A
black diamond (3° by 3° visual angle) indicated
that no movement had to be executed.
Procedure
The experiment started with two practice blocks.
First, participants were required to verbally report
the correct result of visually presented arithmetic
problems. The second practice block was identical
to the subsequent experimental block (see below).
The experiment started when participants were
able to perform both tasks correctly simultaneously.
In the experimental block, every trial started
with a movement instruction, indicated by an
arrow pointing in one of the four relative directions
or a black lled diamond symbol indicating that no
movement had to be performed in this trial.
Participants had to bring the arm to a centre pos-
ition by moving a red dot into a centrally presented
square frame (2° ×2°of visual angle). After a
random interval of 10001500 ms, the arithmetic
problem was presented. With problem onset, par-
ticipants were required to start the instructed arm
movement and to continue with a steady speed of
approximately 10° angle/s until an answer was
given.
If the arm movement did not start within 1000
ms, an error signal was presented, and the trial was
interrupted. Participants were instructed to report
the result of the presented problem as soon as poss-
ible by rst saying isfollowed by the result (in
their native language). The experimenter moni-
tored the movements and the verbal response and
entered both into the computer. If a movement
was stopped before an answer was given or the
answer was not given in the instructed manner,
the trial was scored as erroneous. After every trial,
feedback messages were presented saying Correct
Movementor Correct Answerwith green font
colour or Wrong Movementor Wrong
Answerin red font colour. Afterwards, the exper-
imenter started the next trial.
Design
The experimental block comprised 30 addition and
30 subtraction problems each presented once in
each of the ve movement conditions (left, right,
up, down, no movement), resulting in 300 trials
in total. All trials were presented in a randomized
order. The experiment lasted approximately
90 min.
Data analysis
The response time (RT) was dened as the time
between problem presentation and onset of the
vocal response.
For the analysis, all trials with anticipation
responses (RTs ,400 ms), missing responses (no
responses and RTs .10,000 ms), trials in which
triggering of the voice key was not due to the
verbal response of the participant, and trials with
incorrectly performed arm movements (e.g., move-
ments in the wrong direction, movements that
stopped before verbal response, very slowly per-
formed movements) were excluded (7.8% of the
trials).
To control for a possible trade-off between
speed and accuracy of responding or different strat-
egies of the participantthat is, an emphasis on
speed or accuracy depending on the experimental
conditionwe analysed efciency scores.
Efciency scores integrate the two performance
Table 1. Continued.
Problem number Operand 1 Operation Operand 2 Result
51 81 53 28
52 82 45 37
53 83 39 44
54 84 28 56
55 85 38 47
56 86 29 57
57 91 66 25
58 92 54 38
59 93 67 26
60 95 59 36
THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8) 1561
SPATIAL INTERFERENCE IN MENTAL ARITHMETIC
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
indices speed and accuracy of responding by divid-
ing the accuracy rate by response time in seconds.
Motionarithmetic compatibility was dened as
follows. In addition trials, right- and upward
motions were dened as motionarithmetic com-
patible. Left- and downward movements were con-
sidered as incompatible. For subtractions,
compatible and incompatible motions were in the
opposite direction to that for additions.
Results
The average error rate was 3.23% for problems
with small size and 17.67% for large-size problems,
t(41) =−10.02, p,.001.
Efciency scores (ES) were submitted to a four-
way repeated measures analysis of variance
(ANOVA) with the within-subject factors problem
size (small, large), operation (addition, subtraction),
spatial dimension (horizontal, vertical), and motion
arithmetic compatibility (compatible, incompatible).
The analysis revealed two main effects of the factors
problem size, F(1, 37) =344.20, MSE =0.10,
p,.001, η
2
=.90, and operation, F(1, 37) =
129.59, MSE =0.02, p,.001, η
2
=.78. That is,
performance was better for additions (0.575 effect
size, ES) than for subtraction problems (0.453 ES)
and better for small-size problems (0.749 ES) than
for large-size problems (0.279 ES). Both effects
reect two well-established ndings in the eld of
mental arithmetic. Moreover, there was a signicant
interaction between the factors operation and
problem size, F(1, 37) =37.03, MSE =0.01,
p,.001, η
2
=.50, reecting that the difference in
performance between addition and subtraction
trials was more pronounced for small- (0.176 ES)
than for large-size problems (0.072 ES).
Importantly, there was a signicant effect of
motionarithmetic compatibility, F(1, 37) =8.43,
MSE =0.03, p,.01, η
2
=.19, suggesting that
problem-solving performance was better when
adding and moving up or to the right or subtracting
and moving down or to the left (motionarithmetic
compatible; 0.521 ES) than when adding and
moving down or left or subtracting and moving up
or right (motionarithmetic incompatible; 0.507
ES). Interestingly, there was no interaction
between the factors motionarithmetic compatibility
and spatial dimension, F(1, 37) =1.14, MSE =
0.003, p=.29, η
2
=.03, suggesting that the
motionarithmetic compatibility effects for the hori-
zontal and vertical dimensions did not differ from
each other. However, post hoc ttests revealed that
only for the vertical dimension was the interaction
between movement direction and arithmetic
operation signicant, t(37) =2.00, p,.05. The
motionarithmetic compatibility effect for the hori-
zontal dimension did not reach signicance, t(37) =
1.10, p=.14 (see Figure 1). This pattern suggests a
predominant role of vertical space for numerical pro-
cessing in the context of mental arithmetic.
In order to specify whether the vertical motion
arithmetic compatibility effect reects an interfer-
ence and/or facilitation effect, the compatible and
incompatible conditions were compared with the
no-movement condition. The ttests indicated sig-
nicantly lower performance in both the motion
arithmetic compatible (0.523 ES) and incompatible
conditions (0.505 ES) than in the no-movement
condition (0.534 ES), t(37) =2.81, p,.05, and
t(37) =3.57,p,.01. This pattern suggests inter-
ference of motion, which is incompatible with the
arithmetic operation, as well as greater overall cog-
nitive demands for the movement than for the no-
movement conditions.
Since the average result size in additions was
larger than that in subtractions (13.2 for additions
and 6.73 for subtractions in small-size problems
and 80.60 for additions and 37.67 for subtractions
in large-size problems), it might be objected that
Figure 1.Efciency scores (accuracy rate/response time, acc/RT, in s)
as a function of the factors spatial dimension and motionarithmetic
compatibility in Experiment 1. The asterisk indicates a signicant
difference between conditions, p,.05.
1562 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8)
WIEMERS ET AL.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
the interaction between movement direction and
arithmetic operation is driven by a difference in
result size and a sort of effect of spatialnumerical
compatibility (larger results might be associated
with right- and upward motions and smaller with
left- and downward motions). In order to rule out
that the difference in result size for additions and
subtractions is driving the compatibility effects,
we performed an additional three-way repeated
measures ANOVA with the within-subject
factors problem size (small, large), spatial dimen-
sion (horizontal, vertical), and spatialnumerical
compatibility (compatible, incompatible). The
analysis did not reveal a main effect of spatial
numerical compatibility, F(1, 37) ,1, an inter-
action between spatialnumerical compatibility
and spatial dimension, F(1, 37) ,1, an interaction
between spatialnumerical compatibility and
problem size, F(1, 37) =1.14, MSE =0.002,
p=.29,η
2
=.03, or a three-way interaction
between spatialnumerical compatibility, spatial
dimension, and problem size, F(1, 37) ,1.
To further explore the effects from the efciency
analyses, we performed repeated measures
ANOVAs for mean RT data and error rates with
the factors operation (Table 3) (addition, subtrac-
tion), spatial dimension (horizontal, vertical), and
motionarithmetic compatibility (compatible,
incompatible). For the analysis of RT data, all
trials with a RT 2.5 standard deviations below or
above the grand mean were excluded. Similar to
the efciency score data, the analyses revealed a sig-
nicant effect for motionarithmetic compatibility,
F(1, 37) =8.36, MSE =0.43, p,01, η
2
=.18,
and no interaction between spatial dimension and
motionarithmetic compatibility, F(1, 37) ,1.
The analyses of error rates did not show a signi-
cant effect for motionarithmetic compatibility or
an interaction between motionarithmetic compat-
ibility and another factor.
Discussion
Experiment 1 provides evidence for an impact of
spatial processing on mental arithmetic by
showing a systematic effect of movement direction
on problem-solving performance of arithmetic
problems. That is, participantsarithmetic perform-
ance was impaired while moving the arm in an
incompatible direction with respect to the required
arithmetic operation (i.e., adding while moving left
or down, or subtracting while moving right or up)
as compared to the compatible movement con-
dition (adding while moving right or up, or sub-
tracting while moving left or down). The data
support the notion that spatial representations are
functionally involved in mental arithmetic (cf.
McCrink et al., 2007; Pinhas & Fischer, 2008).
Most importantly, the demonstration that arith-
metic operations are systematically affected by
movements along the vertical axis indicates that a
vertically organized number space is involved in
mental arithmetic (Fischer & Brugger, 2011).
It has to be emphasized that the grounding
hypothesis of spatialnumerical association predicts
a predominant role for the vertical associations.
While Experiment 1 revealed clear evidence for a
coupling of arithmetic operations and vertical
space, it provided only mixed support for a difference
in the motionarithmetic compatibility effects along
the horizontal and vertical dimensions. The lack of
interaction between the factors motionarithmetic
compatibility and spatial dimension in the
ANOVA suggests that compatibility effects are
not modulated by the spatial dimension.
The simple effects analysis, however, is in line
with the grounding hypothesis and supports our
assumption that motionarithmetic compatibility
effects emerge predominantly for vertical move-
ments. The results of Experiment 1 are thus not
fully conclusive, and we interpret the outcome
therefore parsimoniously as a failure to nd evi-
dence for a difference between horizontal and ver-
tical motionarithmetic compatibility effects.
Another problem of Experiment 1 relates to the
physical effort associated with arm movements in
different directions. Due to the forces of gravity,
moving the arm upwards is more effortful than
moving it downwards. This difference in effort
for vertical arm movements has a potentially con-
founding effect, since the motionarithmetic com-
patibility for the vertical dimension could also be
due to a more-is-moremapping (i.e., linking
physical effort with arithmetic operations, cf.
THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8) 1563
SPATIAL INTERFERENCE IN MENTAL ARITHMETIC
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
Stavy & Tirosh, 2000) instead of a grounding in
terms of mental space. In contrast, horizontal
arm movements do not suffer from the same
confound.
We conducted a second experiment in order to
address the issue of an asymmetry in the required
energy for up- and downward arm movements
and to provide a more conclusive additional test
of a potential difference between the effect of hori-
zontal and vertical spatial representations on arith-
metic problem-solving performance.
EXPERIMENT 2
Experiment 2 further investigated the two motion
arithmetic compatibility effects and aimed to rule
out the potential confound of a difference in effort
for the upward compared to the downward move-
ment condition being responsible for the compat-
ibility between vertical motion and arithmetic.
Whereas a difference in the effort for up- and
downward arm movements is clearly noticeable by
the participant, we believe that the difference in
the effort of up- and downward smooth pursuit
eye movements is only marginal and will not be
mentally represented. As a study by Collewijn,
Erkelens, and Steinman (1988) indicates, the brain
does not sufciently take into account gravity and
the related difference in inertial forces for the plan-
ning of vertical eye movements. The authors
showed that upward saccades undershoot, while
downward saccades systematically overshoot relative
to target position. This nding supports our
assumption that the difference in the applied
energy for eye movements is small and negligible.
Experiment 2 also served to provide an
additional test of a difference between the impact
of horizontal and vertical spatial information on
arithmetic problem-solving performance, since the
results from Experiment 1 were inconclusive in
this regard.
In order to induce slow smooth pursuit eye
movements, we presented arithmetic problems
moving from screen centre in one of the four
relative directions with a rate of about 1.7° visual
angle/s.
Method
Participants
A total of 44 students of the Radboud University
Nijmegen (10 males, mean age of 21.77 years) par-
ticipated in the experiment in return for 10 euros or
course credits.
Experimental set-up and apparatus
Participants were sitting at about 70 cm viewing
distance from a 17′′ CRT monitor. As in
Experiment 1, response onset was detected via
voice key. Responses were classied and scored by
the experimenter.
Stimuli
The same arithmetic problems as those in
Experiment 1 were presented. Problems were
moving after appearance at the screen centre to
the left, to the right, up, or down at approximately
1.7° visual angle/s. The problems were presented in
smaller size, spanning only 4° ×2° visual angle.
A black square (1° ×1° of visual angle) served as a
xation point.
Procedure
Again, the experiment started with a practice block.
In the experimental block, each trial started with a
central presentation of the xation point. After a
random interval of 1,000 to 1,500 ms, the addition
or subtraction problem was presented at the centre
of the screen and started moving in one of the four
directions or remained at screen centre. The par-
ticipantstask was to indicate the correct result as
fast as possible by rst saying isfollowed by the
result. If an answer was not provided before
the arithmetic problem reached the border of the
screen, or within 6 s in the no-motion condition,
the trial was counted as erroneous. Messages
saying either Correct Answerin a green font or
Wrong Answerin a red font were presented at
the end of each trial as feedback.
Design and data analysis
The experimental design and data analysis were
identical to those in Experiment 1. The experiment
lasted about 60 min.
1564 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8)
WIEMERS ET AL.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
Results
The average error rate was 3.22% for small-size
problems and 17.46% for large-size problems,
t(41) =−10.05, p,.001.
Efciency scores were submitted to a repeated
measures ANOVA with the within-subject factors
problem size (small/large), operation (addition/sub-
traction), spatial dimension (horizontal/vertical),
and motionarithmetic compatibility (compatible/
incompatible). As in the previous analyses, the
same pattern of effects for problem size, operation,
and the interaction between operation and problem
size was shown. The effect of motionarithmetic
compatibility was not signicant, F(1, 42) =2.63,
MSE =0.02, p=.11, η
2
=.06.
Most importantly, there was a signicant
interaction between spatial dimension and
motionarithmetic compatibility, F(1, 42) =4.50,
MSE =0.04, p,.05, η
2
=.10 (see Figure 2).
Post hoc ttests revealed that only vertical spatial
motion showed a signicant motionarithmetic
compatibility effect, with higher efciency scores
for compatible (0.504 ES) than for incompatible
(0.493 ES) visual motion, t(42) =7.48, p,.01.
For the horizontal dimension, movement direction
did not modulate the performance in the
mental arithmetic task (0.498 ES vs. 0.500 ES),
t(42) ,1. Thus, arithmetic problem-solving per-
formance was better when adding and moving up
or subtracting and moving down (0.504 ES) than
when adding and moving down or subtracting
and moving up (0.493 ES). In order to further
specify the compatibility effect for the vertical
dimension, the compatible and incompatible con-
ditions were compared against the no-movement
condition. The ttests indicated signicantly lower
performance in the motionarithmetic incompati-
ble (0.493 ES) than in the no-movement condition
(0.510 ES), t(41) =3.70, p,.01. The motion
arithmetic compatible condition did not differ
from the no-movement condition,t(41) =1.29,
p=.20, which suggests an interference of motion,
which is incompatible with the arithmetic
operation.
In order to rule out that differences in result size
for additions and subtractions are underlying the
motionarithmetic compatibility effect, we per-
formed an additional ANOVA with the within-
subject factors problem size (small, large), spatial
dimension (horizontal, vertical), and spatial
numerical compatibility (compatible, incompati-
ble). As in Experiment 1, the analysis revealed
neither a signicant effect of spatialnumerical
compatibility nor a signicant interaction between
spatialnumerical compatibility and any other
factor, all Fs,1.
As a supplement to the efciency score analysis,
we also performed analyses for RT and error data
with the factors operation (addition/subtraction),
spatial dimension (horizontal/vertical), and motion
arithmetic compatibility (compatible/incompatible)
(see Table 4).
For Experiment 2 the analyses of mean RTs
merely revealed a trend for an effect of motion
arithmetic compatibility, F(1, 41) =1.06, MSE =
0.012, p=.06.
The error rate analyses showed a trend for an
interaction between spatial dimension and
motionarithmetic compatibility, F(1, 41) =3.73,
MSE =226.19, p=.06, with a descriptive differ-
ence in the a motionarithmetic compatibility for
the vertical dimension (9.87%, compatible vs.
11.07%, incompatible) and an inverted difference
for the horizontal dimension (10.99%, compatible
vs. 9.87%, incompatible). However, further ttests
failed to prove the signicance of these differences
[t(41) =1.61, p=.06, and t(41) =1.56, p=.06,
for the vertical and horizontal spatial dimension,
respectively].
Figure 2.Efciency scores (accuracy rate/response time, acc/RT, in s)
as a function of the factors spatial dimension and motionarithmetic
compatibility in Experiment 2. The double asterisk indicates a
signicant difference between conditions, p,.01.
THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8) 1565
SPATIAL INTERFERENCE IN MENTAL ARITHMETIC
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
Discussion
Experiment 2 addressed the problem of a potential
confound of a difference of effort or required energy
for upward compared to downward movements,
underlying the compatibility between vertical
motion and arithmetic. In order to control for
this, we presented addition and subtraction pro-
blems moving from screen centre in one of the
four directions. The continuous motion induced
smooth pursuit eye movements, for which the
asymmetry in effort for up- and downward move-
ments is marginal or not even taken into account
by the brain (Collewijn et al., 1988). Importantly,
we found a compatibility between vertical motion
and mental arithmeticthat is, performance was
impaired for additions when moving downwards
(compared to upward movements) and for subtrac-
tions when moving upwards (compared to down-
ward movements). The replication of the vertical
motionarithmetic compatibility effect in this
setting suggests that representations of space are
driving the compatibility effect in the vertical
dimension. Moreover, as in Experiment 1, the
motionarithmetic compatibility for the horizontal
dimension was not signicant, suggesting a stron-
ger coupling of vertical space and mental arithmetic
than the linkage with horizontal space. Together,
Experiment 2 supports our hypothesis of a stronger
coupling between mental arithmetic and the verti-
cal than with the horizontal dimension and sup-
ports the notion that mental addition and
Table 2. Mean efciency scores in Experiment 2 as a function of the factors problem size, operation, spatial dimension, and motionarithmetic
compatibility
Spatial dimension MAC
Problems
Small-size Large-size
Addition Subtraction Addition Subtraction
Horizontal dimension Compatible 0.8045 0.6469 0.2900 0.2524
Incompatible 0.8019 0.6512 0.2907 0.2550
Vertical dimension Compatible 0.8126 0.6602 0.2907 0.2469
Incompatible 0.7988 0.6419 0.2857 0.2436
Note: MAC =motionarithmetic compatibility.
Table 3. Mean response time and error rates in Experiment 1 as a function of the factors problem size, operation, spatial dimension, and
motionarithmetic compatibility
Dependent variable Spatial dimension MAC compatibility
Problems
Small-size Large-size
Addition Subtraction Addition Subtraction
Response time (s) Horizontal Compatible 1.235 1.602 3.095 3.483
Incompatible 1.282 1.581 3.125 3.652
Vertical Compatible 1.230 1.531 3.110 3.568
Incompatible 1.277 1.619 3.083 3.587
Error rate (%) Horizontal Compatible 2.76 3.67 8.43 13.91
Incompatible 2.90 3.10 7.87 12.65
Vertical Compatible 2.96 4.68 8.20 10.72
Incompatible 3.37 2.58 7.29 15.23
Note: MAC =motionarithmetic compatibility.
1566 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8)
WIEMERS ET AL.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
subtraction are grounded in vertical spatial
numerical associations.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Two experiments investigated the involvement of
spatial representations in mental arithmetic and
demonstrated for the rst time an impact of
spatial information on the performances to solve
addition and subtraction problems. Interestingly,
the observed motionarithmetic compatibility
effect is more reliable for the vertical than for the
horizontal dimension, supporting the idea that
the mapping between numbers and vertical space
has a predominant function in mental arithmetic.
If participants were required to move their arm
while solving arithmetic problems (Experiment
1), we observed impaired performance in addition
when moving left- or downwards than when
moving right- or upwards and vice versa in subtrac-
tion. Experiment 1 furthermore revealed a descrip-
tive difference between the compatibility effects
along the two dimensions, suggesting a stronger
association for the vertical dimension. However,
the interaction between the factors motionarith-
metic compatibility and dimension did not reach
signicance. The second experiment provided an
additional test of differences in the impact of verti-
cal and horizontal spatial information and
controlled for the potential confound of higher
effort associated with upward than with downward
arm movements as an alternative explanation for
the motionarithmetic compatibility effect for the
vertical dimension. Experiment 2 revealed only a
vertical but not a horizontal motionarithmetic
compatibility effect and this way conrmed our
hypothesis that the coupling of mental arithmetic
is more robust for the vertical dimension.
Together our data support the idea that spatial pro-
cesses are functionally linked to mental arithmetic
(Pinhas & Fischer, 2008) and indicate that both
horizontal and vertical spatial information are
involved in mental arithmetic. Importantly, the
greater robustness of the motionarithmetic com-
patibility effect for the vertical than for the horizon-
tal dimension argues for a predominant role of the
vertical dimension in the coupling of space and
arithmetic operations.
The observed motionarithmetic compatibility
effect is in line with the idea that mental arithmetic
is accompanied by attentional shifts in mental
number space (McCrink et al., 2007; Pinhas &
Fischer, 2008). The previous support for a spatial
origin of the operational momentum effect in
approximate arithmetic was exclusively based on
spatial response biases for symbolic and nonsym-
bolic arithmetic (Knops, Viarouge, et al., 2009;
Pinhas & Fischer, 2008) and the predictability of
addition and subtraction from neural correlates of
Table 4. Mean response time and error rates in Experiment 2 as a function of the factors problem size, operation, spatial dimension, and
motionarithmetic compatibility
Dependent variable Spatial dimension MAC compatibility
Problems
Small-size problems Large-size problems
Addition Subtraction Addition Subtraction
Response time (s) Horizontal Compatible 1.300 1.650 3.106 3.518
Incompatible 1.330 1.628 3.168 3.497
Vertical Compatible 1.299 1.622 3.162 3.526
Incompatible 1.330 1.646 3.153 3.555
Error rate (%) Horizontal Compatible 3.23 4.28 16.50 19.95
Incompatible 2.23 3.64 13.64 19.98
Vertical Compatible 2.71 2.97 12.68 21.11
Incompatible 2.25 4.51 15.42 22.11
Note: MAC =motionarithmetic compatibility.
THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8) 1567
SPATIAL INTERFERENCE IN MENTAL ARITHMETIC
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
horizontal saccades (Knops, Thirion et al., 2009).
Motor response biases, however, do not provide a
strict test for the spatial processes and can be alter-
natively explained by the simple response heuristic
to point more toward smaller numbers after sub-
tracting and more towards large numbers after
additions in situations of uncertainty (see e.g.,
Knops, Zitzmann, & McCrink, 2013 for a discus-
sion of a similar alternative account). The present
study now sheds new light on the origin of the
operational momentum effect by showing that the
mere activation of spatial codes affects the overall
efciency to solve arithmetic problems. The non-
spatial accounts of response heuristics predict a
systematic over- or underestimation or distortions
in pointing trajectories but do not account for the
inuence reported here of spatial codes on arith-
metic performances. Therefore, the present
nding of an impact of spatial information on
arithmetic problem-solving performance also pro-
vides support for the previously made notions that
biases in nonsymbolic (McCrink et al., 2007) and
symbolic arithmetic (Pinhas & Fischer, 2008) orig-
inate from attentional shifts in mental numerical
space (Knops, Thirion, et al., 2009).
The present study substantially extends the lit-
erature on number processing by showing that
associations with vertical space are not restricted
to the processing of single numbers (Sell &
Kaschak, 2012; Shaki & Fischer, 2012; Winter &
Matlock, 2013) but are also present in mental arith-
metic. Importantly, the greater impact of vertical
spatial information on arithmetic provides direct
empirical support for the grounding hypothesis of
spatialnumerical associations (cf. Fischer &
Brugger, 2011; Lakoff, 1987). According to this
notion, sensory experiences with numbers will be
primarily associated with the vertical dimension
due to a correlation between vertical space and
magnitude in the world. Therefore, vertical space
becomes an important and maybe even the predo-
minant dimension in the organization of number
space. Following this line of reasoning, it has also
been argued that vertical spatial associations
provide the conceptual basis for the development
of any numerical knowledge (Fischer & Brugger,
2011). The presented motion arithmetic
compatibility effect and more robust effect of verti-
cal spatial information can therefore also be con-
ceived as support for the recently discussed
general hypothesis that sensorimotor experiences
shape the development of any kind of conceptual
knowledge (Barsalou, 2008).
One fundamental question concerning the
interference between spatial processing and
mental arithmetic is the direction of the inuence.
Importantly, in Experiment 2 participants merely
had to follow the moving problem with their eyes.
We regard it as rather unlikely that mental arith-
metic has a strong impact on the control of eye
movements, since previous studies could show
that the control of such movements is highly auto-
matized and robust against cognitive interferences
(e.g., Kathmann, Hochrein, & Uwer, 1999;
Theeuwes, 2010). The motionarithmetic compat-
ibility effect for visual motions therefore most likely
reects an impact of spatial processing on mental
arithmetic. However, more research is needed to
determine the causality between space and
arithmetic.
Since problem size was controlled between
addition and subtraction trials by matching overall
numerical size (i.e., the sum of the operands and
the correct solution), it might be objected that
our ndings are confounded by a difference in the
size of solutions for addition and subtraction
trials. If this was true, an interaction between
motion direction and problem size would be
expected. That is, independent of the arithmetic
operation performance should be enhanced for
large-solution trials when moving right- or
upwards compared to when moving left- or down-
wards. Similarly, small-solution problems should
be facilitated when moving left- or downwards
compared to when moving right- or upwards.
Additional analyses demonstrated that the data
provided no evidence for such an interaction
effect, which demonstrates that the effect of the
motion direction was driven by the compatibility
with the arithmetic operation.
As discussed above, vertical movements per
denition suffer from a confound of effort, since a
movement working against gravity requires more
energy than a movement going with the forces of
1568 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8)
WIEMERS ET AL.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
gravity. This difference is, however, marginal for
smooth pursuit eye movements and not even fully
taken into account by the brain (see Collewijn
et al., 1988). We therefore argue that asymmetries
in effort do not account for the predominance of
the vertical mapping in the present study.
Nevertheless we acknowledge that in principle it
cannot be exclude that the stronger vertical
motion arithmetic compatibility effect is the result
of a more-is-moreassociation (Stavy & Tirosh,
2000) between effort and numerical size, since a
difference in physically required energy for up and
downward eye movements does clearly exist.
The present nding of a motionarithmetic
compatibility effect for horizontal and vertical
space might have implications for maths education
in elementary school or for remedial educational
programmes for dyscalculic individuals. As it has
already been shown that gestures can increase
mathematical learning (Goldin-Meadow, Cook,
& Mitchell, 2009), a systematic involvement of
both horizontal and vertical spatial mappings
might support the learning of numerical concepts
and arithmetic.
In conclusion, the present study shows that both
horizontal and vertical organizations of mental
numerical space are activated during mental arith-
metic and suggests a functional involvement of
both dimensions. Our nding of a more robust
effect of vertical space furthermore suggests a pre-
dominant role of the vertical organization of the
mental number space in mental calculations.
Original manuscript received 13 August 2013
Accepted revision received 19 December 2013
First published online 6 March 2014
REFERENCES
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual
Review of Psychology,59, 617645.
Collewijn, H., Erkelens, C. J., & Steinman, R. (1988).
Binocular Co-ordination of human vertical saccadic
eye movements. Journal of Physioloogy,404, 183197.
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The
mental representation of parity and number
magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Genera,122, 371396.
Fias, W., & Fischer, M. H. (2005). Spatial represen-
tation of numbers. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.),
Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 4354).
New York: Psychology Press.
Fischer, M., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help
digits: Spatialnumerical associations point to nger
counting as prime example of embodied cognition.
Frontiers in Psychology,2, 260
Goldin-Meadow, S., Cook, S. W., & Mitchell, Z. A.
(2009). Gesturing gives children new ideas about
math. Psychological Science,20(3), 267272.
Holmes, K. J., & Lourenco, S. F. (2012). Orienting
numbers in mental space: Horizontal organization
trumps vertical. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology,65, 10441051.
Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S.
(2005). Interactions between number and space in par-
ietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,6,435448.
Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quanti-
tative representation of numbers: Evidence from the
SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition,32, 662673.
Kathmann, N., Hochrein, A., & Uwer, R. (1999).
Effects of dual task demands on the accuracy of
smooth pursuit eye movements. Psychophysiology,36
(2), 158163.
Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., &
Dehaene, S. (2009). Recruitment of an area involved
in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science,
324, 15831585.
Knops, A., Viarouge, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009).
Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and
nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the oper-
ational momentum effect. Attention Percepion &
Psychophysics,71, 803821.
Knops, A., Zitzmann, S., & McCrink, K. (2013).
Examining the presence and determinants of oper-
ational momentum in childhood. Frontiers in
Psychology,4, 325.
Krause, F., & Lindemann, O. (2013). Expyriment: A
Python library for cognitive and neuroscientic exper-
iments. Behavior Research Methods.doi: 10.3758/
s13428-013-0390-6
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, re, and dangerous things:
What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G.
(2007). Moving along the number line: Operational
momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception
and Psychophysics,69(8), 13241333.
THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8) 1569
SPATIAL INTERFERENCE IN MENTAL ARITHMETIC
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
Pinhas, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Mental movements
without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in sym-
bolic arithmetic. Cognition,109,408415.
Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. (2004). Moving the eyes along
the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects
with manual and saccadic responses. Perception &
Psychophysics,66, 651664.
Sell, A. J., & Kaschak, M. P. (2012). The comprehension
of sentences involving quantity information affects
responses on the updown axis. Psychonomic Bulletin
and Review,19, 708714.
Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. (2012). Multiple spatial mappings
in numerical cognition. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,38(3),
804809.
Stavy, R., & Tirosh, D. (2000). How students (mis-)
understand science and mathematics: Intuitive rules.
New York: Teachers College Press.
Theeuwes, J.(2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of
visual selection. Acta Psychologica,135(2), 7799.
Winter, B., & Matlock, T. (2013). More is upand
right: Random number generation along two axes.
In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I.
Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3789
3974). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
1570 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 67 (8)
WIEMERS ET AL.
Downloaded by [Universitaetsbibliothek Potsdam] at 04:28 27 July 2014
... The employment of the motor system in the metaphorical understanding of abstract concepts in terms of motion events could be particularly relevant to the results of Experiment ISSN 1980-4415 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980 1. As mentioned, although numbers are not inherently spatial concepts, they are metaphorically understood in terms of spatial concepts, and some evidence suggests that subtractions between numbers are metaphorically understood as leftward movements (e.g., Li et al., 2018;Fischer, 2014) or downward movements (Wiemers;Bekkering;Lindemann, 2014). According to the strong versions of embodied cognition (Gallese;Lakoff, 2005), this could mean that neural networks that are involved in a real leftward movement of hands are activated when someone performs subtractions between numbers. ...
... The employment of the motor system in the metaphorical understanding of abstract concepts in terms of motion events could be particularly relevant to the results of Experiment ISSN 1980-4415 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980 1. As mentioned, although numbers are not inherently spatial concepts, they are metaphorically understood in terms of spatial concepts, and some evidence suggests that subtractions between numbers are metaphorically understood as leftward movements (e.g., Li et al., 2018;Fischer, 2014) or downward movements (Wiemers;Bekkering;Lindemann, 2014). According to the strong versions of embodied cognition (Gallese;Lakoff, 2005), this could mean that neural networks that are involved in a real leftward movement of hands are activated when someone performs subtractions between numbers. ...
... The employment of the motor system in the metaphorical understanding of abstract concepts in terms of motion events could be particularly relevant to the results of Experiment ISSN 1980-4415 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980 1. As mentioned, although numbers are not inherently spatial concepts, they are metaphorically understood in terms of spatial concepts, and some evidence suggests that subtractions between numbers are metaphorically understood as leftward movements (e.g., Li et al., 2018;Fischer, 2014) or downward movements (Wiemers;Bekkering;Lindemann, 2014). According to the strong versions of embodied cognition (Gallese;Lakoff, 2005), this could mean that neural networks that are involved in a real leftward movement of hands are activated when someone performs subtractions between numbers. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this study, we investigated the embodied nature of vector as one of the fundamental concepts in mathematics. Our aim was to examine the role of gesture in finding directions of vectors and magnitudes of vectors on the basis of coordinates of initial points and end points of vectors. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to find directions of vectors on the basis of coordinates of initial points and end points of the vectors. While participants of one group were allowed to gesture, participants of another group were prohibited from gesturing. In Experiment 2, participants were asked to find directions and relative magnitudes of x-coordinates and y-coordinates of vectors on the basis of coordinates of initial points and end points of the vectors. In both experiments, participants of gesture-allowed group had a better performance in answering the questions. Specifically, participants performed better in finding the directions of those vectors which had a left-down direction. Based on these results, it can be concluded that the process of finding the direction and magnitude of a vector on the basis of its initial and end points is mainly embodied as a combination of leftward and downward movements (left-down direction).
... Hartmann et al., 2015Hartmann et al., , 2016Klein et al., 2014;Masson et al., 2018a, b;Salvaggio et al., 2022b), showing that the solving procedure is somehow related to visuo-spatial processes. Also, drawing participants' attention to the left while they mentally solved arithmetic problems, for example by showing them flickering lateralized targets (Masson & Pesenti, 2016) or asking them to move their arm to the left or to the right (Wiemers et al., 2014), has also been shown to influence response speed and/or quality. Finally, neuropsychological studies with neglect patients unable to orient their attention towards the side of space contralateral to their lesion have also shown that left neglect is associated with impairments in processing small-magnitude numbers and solving subtraction problems (Dormal et al., 2014;Vuilleumier et al., 2004;Zorzi et al., 2012), while right neglect is associated with impairments in processing largemagnitude numbers and solving addition problems (Masson et al., 2018b). ...
... Finally, while horizontal SNAs are the most investigated, vertical SNAs are also sometimes reported, with upward shifts of attention related to additions and downward shifts of attention to subtractions (Blini et al., 2019;Hartmann et al., 2015;Salvaggio et al., 2022;Wiemers et al., 2014). However, their role and mechanisms and how they interact or complement horizontal SNAs remain to be understood. ...
Article
Full-text available
Solving subtraction and addition problems is accompanied by spontaneous leftward and rightward gaze shifts, respectively. These shifts have been related to attentional processes involved in mental arithmetic, but whether these processes induce overt attentional shifts mediated by the activation of the motor programs underlying lateral eye movements or covert shifts only is still unknown. Here, we used the abducted eye paradigm to selectively disrupt activation of the oculomotor system and prevent oculomotor preparation, which affects overt but not covert attentional shifts. Participants had to mentally solve addition and subtraction problems while fixating a screen positioned either in front of them or laterally to their left or right such that they were physically unable to programme and execute saccades further into their temporal field while they still could do so in their nasal field. In comparison to the frontal condition, rightward eye abduction impaired additions (with carrying), and leftward eye abduction impaired subtractions (with borrowing) showing that at least some arithmetic problems rely on processes dedicated to overt attentional shifts. We propose that when solving arithmetic problems requires procedures such as carrying and borrowing, oculomotor mechanisms operating on a mental space transiently built in working memory are recruited to represent one numerical magnitude in relation to another (e.g. the first operand and the result).
... This association is supported by the SNARC (spatial-numerical association of response codes) effect, which refers to the phenomenon by which small-number responses are faster with the left hand and large-number responses are faster with the right hand (Dehaene et al., 1993). Plenty of studies have also addressed the possibility of spatialnumerical association taking place in a three-dimensional space, with smaller numbers being associated with the lower part and the near side of space, and larger numbers with the upper part and far side of space (Aleotti et al., 2020;Greenacre et al., 2022;Wiemers et al., 2014;. The space-number association is also conventionally used when visualising numerical quantities in graphs, in which numerical quantities typically increase from the bottom to the top along the y-axis and from left to right along the x-axis (Hartmann et al., 2014;Winter & Matlock, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the differences in the expression of numerical motion metaphors in English and Spanish. We evaluated 1472 English-to-Spanish translations in which a manner of motion verb (e.g., skyrocket, plummet ) was used to metaphorically express numerical change (e.g., unemployment is skyrocketing ). For each of the translations, we annotated (1) the type of metaphor used in Spanish, (2) whether the manner of motion and path information was present in Spanish, and (3) whether the path and manner information in Spanish were conflated in a single word or indicated via adjuncts. There were three main findings. First, Spanish translations shifted from the motion domain to a quantity domain in almost half of the translations (e.g., skyrocket translated as aumentar , Eng. increase ). Second, Spanish translations omitted manner of motion in half of the cases (e.g., prices surging translated as alza de los precios , Eng. rise in prices ). Third, the path of motion was always present in the Spanish translations. This translation analysis provides evidence that the typological differences reported for the encoding of literal motion are also observed in the expression of numerical, metaphorical motion and that the choice of metaphorical mappings depends on language typology.
... It was also found that the control of the steering of horizontal and vertical arm movements and the realisation of mental calculation tasks might involve similar attentional mechanisms (e.g. Khayat et al., 2021;Wiemers et al., 2014). On the other hand, Goldin-Meadow et al. (2009) found that certain gestures may be useful for solving a mathematical problem. ...
Article
The digital revolution has sparked the development and diffusion of digital tools (computers, tablets, smartphones, etc.) that are extensively used by children from many countries (Kowert et al., Citation2014; Odgers & Jensen, Citation2020). Such tools have also been increasingly employed for teaching purposes (Hrastinski, Citation2021) and have contributed to in-depth changes in the educational field (e.g. Haleem et al., Citation2022; Zeng et al., Citation2020). In this context, the possible use of video games at school has been debated, which has led studies to highlight both the opportunities (e.g. Zeng et al., Citation2020) and the limits (Sala et al., Citation2018) of these games for learning. The development of active video games (AVGs), also named exergames or active digital games, has renewed this issue. The practice of AVGs requires the repetitive performance of cognitive and motor tasks (Huber et al., Citation2021), and it has been well established that repetition may foster learning (e.g. Musfeld et al., Citation2023). Consequently, the same AVG might contribute to success not only in physical education (PE) but also in other school subjects, especially in mathematics, considering that physical activity may positively affect mathematics achievement (for a review, see Sneck et al., Citation2019). However, to our knowledge, such a possibility remains to be studied. Thus, the present study aims to examine the possible effects of the practice of AVGs with motor and mathematics content on mathematics learning and PE.
... Bien que leur nature exacte fasse encore l'objet de débats, ces associations spatio-numériques sont généralement attribuées à des déplacements de l'attention visuospatiale. En effet, il a été démontré que le fait d'attirer l'attention des participants vers la gauche ou vers la droite, par exemple en leur montrant des cibles latéralisées clignotantes (Masson & Pesenti, 2016) ou en leur demandant de déplacer leurs bras (Wiemers et al., 2014) ou leur corps (Anelli et al., 2014), pendant qu'ils résolvaient mentalement des problèmes arithmétiques affectait la vitesse et/ou la qualité des réponses. Cette interférence est plus importante lorsque les cibles distractrices sont présentées du côté de l'espace où l'attention est censée être dirigée par l'opération : les distracteurs du côté gauche interfèrent davantage avec les soustractions, les distracteurs du côté droit avec les additions (Masson & Pesenti, 2016). ...
... Numbers are metaphorically described as points on a horizontal line. This horizontal line can be perceived as the trace of a moving object on that line, and simple arithmetic operations such as addition and subtraction can be understood as rightward and leftward movements on this line (e.g., Pinhas et al., 2014;Pinhas & Fischer, 2008;Wiemers, Bekkering, & Lindemann, 2014;Zhu, You, Gan, & Wang, 2019), although they are basically defined as equations. In fact, the strong association between straight lines and movement of objects could explain why we have the tendency to understand numbers and arithmetic operations in terms of the movements of objects on a straight line. ...
Article
This article discusses two mechanisms through which understanding static mathematical concepts (basic and more advanced mathematical concepts) in terms of fictive motions or motion events enhance our understanding of these concepts. It is suggested that at least two mechanisms are involved in this enhancing process. The first mechanism enables us to employ both the motor system and the visual system as two contributing cognitive resources to process the static concept. When one representation of a mathematical concept is transformed into another representation, there is a shift in the mode of processing. This shift facilitates the process of employing new cognitive resources such as the motor and visual systems. The second mechanism, which is a special form of mental simulation, enables us to simulate the process of formation of the static concept, which, in turn, makes it easier for us to understand the structure and properties of the static concept.
Article
We review the evidence for the conceptual association between arithmetic and space and quantify the effect size in meta-analyses. We focus on three effects: (a) the operational momentum effect (OME), which has been defined as participants’ tendency to overestimate results of addition problems and underestimate results of subtraction problems; (b) the arithmetic cueing effect, in which arithmetic problems serve as spatial cues in target detection or temporal order judgment tasks; and (c) the associations between arithmetic and space observed with eye- and hand-tracking studies. The OME was consistently found in paradigms that provided the participants with numerical response alternatives. The OME shows a large effect size, driven by an underestimation during subtraction while addition was unbiased. In contrast, paradigms in which participants indicated their estimate by transcoding their final estimate to a spatial reference frame revealed no consistent OME. Arithmetic cueing studies show a reliable small to medium effect size, driven by a rightward bias for addition. Finally, eye- and hand-tracking studies point to replicable associations between arithmetic and eye or hand movements. To account for the complexity of the observed pattern, we introduce the Adaptive Pathways in Mental Arithmetic (APiMA) framework. The model accommodates central notions of numerical and arithmetic processing and helps identifying which pathway a given paradigm operates on. It proposes that the divergence between OME and arithmetic cueing studies comes from the predominant use of non-symbolic versus symbolic stimuli, respectively. Overall, our review and findings clearly support an association between arithmetic and spatial processing.
Article
Full-text available
Spatial orientation is one of the key capabilities of spatial navigation. Orientation in physical space, or large-scale spatial orientation, refers to the process by which an individual locates and navigates in a large-scale environment. Various geographic environments influence how individuals represent spatial orientation during navigation. Based on spatial reference frame theory, this study used a desktop virtual environment navigation task to explore the regional differences in large-scale spatial orientation abilities and their causes. The study findings offer valuable insights for designing navigation in different geographic areas to avoid safety accidents arising from navigation errors. Experiment 1 employed desktop virtual reality technology to clarify potential differences in large-scale spatial orientation abilities using the Route-repetition and Route-retracing tasks. Experiment 2 explored the underlying causes of regional disparities by utilizing the directional approach task, which assessed the flexibility of spatial reference frame transformation. Experiment 3 aimed to improve the large-scale spatial orientation abilities among participants from the southern region by activating the environmental spatial reference frame prior to the task. The results indicated significant differences between participants from the northern and southern regions in Experiment 1, with the former showing significantly higher performance in the Route-retracing task. It was observed that participants from the northern region preferred to utilize the environmental reference frame, while participants from the southern region demonstrated an advantage in using the egocentric reference frame. Additionally, participants performed better in the Route-repetition task compared to the Route-retracing task. In Experiment 2, the correct response rate for the same direction in the directional approach task was higher than for different directions. Furthermore, in the directional asymptote task, participants from the northern region achieved higher correct response rates compared to their southern counterparts. Experiment 3 revealed that the group with the activated environmental reference frame demonstrated higher performance compared to the control group. This study encompassed three experiments, yielding the following findings: (1) Spatial orientation abilities varied among participants from different regions. Participants from the northern region displayed superior performance in the Route-retracing task that required an environmental reference frame, while participants from the southern region preferred to utilize the egocentric reference frame. (2) These differences were attributed to disparities in the use and flexibility of spatial reference frames. Performance variations observed in the Route-retracing task between participants from different regions were linked to their capacity for flexible spatial reference frame switching during navigation tasks. (3) Activating the environmental reference frame for participants from the southern region enhanced their performance of large-scale spatial orientation tasks effectively. Specifically, incorporating a first-person perspective of the surrounding landmark structures in the navigation design facilitated the formation of an environmental reference frame for users. This study supports the spatial reference frame theory and embodied spatial transformation theory, offering recommendations for differentiated navigation interface design.
Article
Adults shift their attention to the right or to the left along a spatial continuum when solving additions and subtractions, respectively. Studies suggest that these shifts not only support the exact computation of the results but also anticipatively narrow down the range of plausible answers when processing the operands. However, little is known on when and how these attentional shifts arise in childhood during the acquisition of arithmetic. Here, an eye‐tracker with high spatio‐temporal resolution was used to measure spontaneous eye movements, used as a proxy for attentional shifts, while children of 2nd (8 y‐o; N = 50) and 4th (10 y‐o; N = 48) Grade solved simple additions (e.g., 4+3) and subtractions (e.g., 3‐2). Gaze patterns revealed horizontal and vertical attentional shifts in both groups. Critically, horizontal eye movements were observed in 4th Graders as soon as the first operand and the operator were presented and thus before the beginning of the exact computation. In 2nd Graders, attentional shifts were only observed after the presentation of the second operand just before the response was made. This demonstrates that spatial attention is recruited when children solve arithmetic problems, even in the early stages of learning mathematics. The time course of these attentional shifts suggests that with practice in arithmetic children start to use spatial attention to anticipatively guide the search for the answer and facilitate the implementation of solving procedures. Research Highlights Additions and subtractions are associated to right and left attentional shifts in adults, but it is unknown when these mechanisms arise in childhood. Children of 8–10 years old solved single‐digit additions and subtractions while looking at a blank screen. Eye movements showed that children of 8 years old already show spatial biases possibly to represent the response when knowing both operands. Children of 10 years old shift attention before knowing the second operand to anticipatively guide the search for plausible answers.
Article
In Western cultures, small-left and large-right spatial-numerical associations are constantly found in various simple number processing tasks. It has recently been suggested that spatial associations are also involved in more complex number processing, for example that individuals make rightward or upward “mental” movements along the number line during addition, and leftward or downward movements during subtraction. In line with this, it has been shown that participants' spontaneous eye movements on a blank screen during upward and downward counting follow such associations. The present research investigated whether eye movements along the number line are simply an epiphenomenon of the recruitment of a spatial reference frame, or whether they play a functional role for the arithmetic computation. This question was addressed by using multi-step problems (e.g., 59 + 5 + 4 + 3) that show a larger proportion of computation (vs. retrieval) when compared to single-step problems (e.g., 59 + 5), as confirmed in Pretest 1. Moreover, the question was addressed only for addition problems and vertical eye movements, because spatial-arithmetic associations were not found in the other conditions (subtraction, horizontal eye movements) in Pretest 2. In the main experiment, participants (n = 29) solved addition problems while following a moving dot with their eyes (smooth pursuit) either in a congruent (upward) or incongruent (downward) direction, or while keeping their eyes fixated on to the center of the screen, or while moving their eyes freely on a blank screen. Arithmetic performance was faster in the congruent condition (upward eye movements) when compared to the other conditions (downward eye movements, central fixation, free viewing). These results suggest that vertical shifts in spatial attention along the mental number line are functionally involved in addition. The results support the view of shared mechanisms for directing spatial attention in external (visual) and representational (number space). Implications for embodied views of number processing are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Expyriment is an open-source and platform-independent lightweight Python library for designing and conducting timing-critical behavioral and neuroimaging experiments. The major goal is to provide a well-structured Python library for script-based experiment development, with a high priority being the readability of the resulting program code. Expyriment has been tested extensively under Linux and Windows and is an all-in-one solution, as it handles stimulus presentation, the recording of input/output events, communication with other devices, and the collection and preprocessing of data. Furthermore, it offers a hierarchical design structure, which allows for an intuitive transition from the experimental design to a running program. It is therefore also suited for students, as well as for experimental psychologists and neuroscientists with little programming experience.
Article
Full-text available
The operational momentum (OM) effect describes a systematic bias in estimating the outcomes of simple addition and subtraction problems. Outcomes of addition problems are overestimated while outcomes of subtraction problems are underestimated. The origin of OM remains debated. First, a flawed uncompression of numerical information during the course of mental arithmetic is supposed to cause OM due to linear arithmetic operations on a compressed magnitude code. Second, attentional shifts along the mental number line are thought to cause OM. A third hypothesis explains OM in 9-month olds by a cognitive heuristic of accepting more (less) than the original operand in addition (subtraction) problems. The current study attempts to disentangle these alternatives and systematically examines potential determinants of OM, such as reading fluency which has been found to modulate numerical-spatial associations. A group of 32 6- and 7-year-old children was tested in non-symbolic addition and subtraction problems, in which they had to choose the correct outcome from an array of several possible outcomes. Reading capacity was assessed for half of the children while attentional measures were assessed in the other half. Thirty-two adults were tested with the identical paradigm to validate its potential of revealing OM. Children (and adults) were readily able to solve the problems. We replicated previous findings of OM in the adults group. Using a Bayesian framework we observed an inverse OM effect in children, i.e., larger overestimations for subtraction compared to addition. A significant correlation between children's level of attentional control and their propensity to exhibit OM was observed. The observed pattern of results, in particular the inverse OM in children is hard to reconcile with the previously proposed theoretical frameworks. The observed link between OM and the attentional system might be interpreted as evidence partially supporting the attentional shift hypothesis.
Article
Full-text available
Nine experiments of timed odd–even judgments examined how parity and number magnitude are accessed from Arabic and verbal numerals. With Arabic numerals, Ss used the rightmost digit to access a store of semantic number knowledge. Verbal numerals went through an additional stage of transcoding to base 10. Magnitude information was automatically accessed from Arabic numerals. Large numbers preferentially elicited a rightward response, and small numbers a leftward response. The Spatial–Numerical Association of Response Codes effect depended only on relative number magnitude and was weaker or absent with letters or verbal numerals. Direction did not vary with handedness or hemispheric dominance but was linked to the direction of writing, as it faded or even reversed in right-to-left writing Iranian Ss. The results supported a modular architecture for number processing, with distinct but interconnected Arabic, verbal, and magnitude representations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
A recent cross-cultural comparison (Shaki, Fischer, & Petrusic, 2009) suggested that spatially consistent processing habits for words and numbers are a necessary condition for the spatial representation of numbers (Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes; SNARC effect). Here we reexamine the SNARC in Israelis who read text from right to left but numbers from left to right. We show that, despite these spatially inconsistent processing habits, a SNARC effect still emerges when the response dimension is spatially orthogonal to the conflicting processing dimension. These results clarify the cognitive conditions for spatial-numerical mappings.
Article
Full-text available
Nine experiments of timed odd-even judgments examined how parity and number magnitude are accessed from Arabic and verbal numerals. With Arabic numerals, Ss used the rightmost digit to access a store of semantic number knowledge. Verbal numerals went through an additional stage of transcoding to base 10. Magnitude information was automatically accessed from Arabic numerals. Large numbers preferentially elicited a rightward response, and small numbers a leftward response. The Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) effect depended only on relative number magnitude and was weaker or absent with letters or verbal numerals. Direction did not vary with handedness or hemispheric dominance but was linked to the direction of writing, as it faded or even reversed in right-to-left writing Iranian Ss. The results supported a modular architecture for number processing, with distinct but interconnected Arabic, verbal, and magnitude representations.
Article
While research on the spatial representation of number has provided substantial evidence for a horizontally oriented mental number line, recent studies suggest vertical organization as well. Directly comparing the relative strength of horizontal and vertical organization, however, we found no evidence of spontaneous vertical orientation (upward or downward), and horizontal trumped vertical when pitted against each other (Experiment 1). Only when numbers were conceptualized as magnitudes (as opposed to nonmagnitude ordinal sequences) did reliable vertical organization emerge, with upward orientation preferred (Experiment 2). Altogether, these findings suggest that horizontal representations predominate, and that vertical representations, when elicited, may be relatively inflexible. Implications for spatial organization beyond number, and its ontogenetic basis, are discussed.
Article
We investigated the use of space in the comprehension of the concept of quantity in text. Previous work has suggested that the right-left axis is useful in spatial representations of number and quantity, while linguistic evidence points toward use of the up-down axis. In Experiment 1, participants read sentences containing quantity information and pressed buttons in either (1) an up and a down position or (2) a left and a right position. In Experiment 2, the participants pressed buttons in either (1) up and down positions or (2) left and right positions, but heard the sentences rather than reading them. We found spatial compatibility effects for the up-down axis, but not for the right-left axis. Additionally, the spatial compatibility effect was observed whether or not the participants moved to make their responses. We discussed the results in the context of embodied approaches to the comprehension of quantity information.