ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Does the language people use to refer to the self during introspection influence how they think, feel, and behave under social stress? If so, do these effects extend to socially anxious people who are particularly vulnerable to such stress? Seven studies explored these questions (total N = 585). Studies 1a and 1b were proof-of-principle studies. They demonstrated that using non-first-person pronouns and one's own name (rather than first-person pronouns) during introspection enhances self-distancing. Studies 2 and 3 examined the implications of these different types of self-talk for regulating stress surrounding making good first impressions (Study 2) and public speaking (Study 3). Compared with the first-person group, the non-first-person group performed better according to objective raters in both studies. They also displayed less distress (Studies 2 and 3) and engaged in less maladaptive postevent processing (Study 3). Studies 4 and 5 examined how these different forms of self-talk influence the way people appraise social-anxiety-provoking events. They demonstrated that non-first-person language use (compared with first-person language use) leads people to appraise future stressors in more challenging and less threatening terms. Finally, a meta-analysis (Study 6) indicated that none of these findings were moderated by trait social anxiety, highlighting their translational potential. Together, these findings demonstrate that small shifts in the language people use to refer to the self during introspection consequentially influence their ability to regulate their thoughts, feelings, and behavior under social stress, even for vulnerable individuals. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Self-Talk as a Regulatory Mechanism: How You Do It Matters
Ethan Kross
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor Emma Bruehlman-Senecal
University of California, Berkeley
Jiyoung Park, Aleah Burson, Adrienne Dougherty,
Holly Shablack, and Ryan Bremner
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Jason Moser
Michigan State University
Ozlem Ayduk
University of California, Berkeley
Does the language people use to refer to the self during introspection influence how they think, feel, and
behave under social stress? If so, do these effects extend to socially anxious people who are particularly
vulnerable to such stress? Seven studies explored these questions (total N!585). Studies 1a and 1b were
proof-of-principle studies. They demonstrated that using non-first-person pronouns and one’s own name
(rather than first-person pronouns) during introspection enhances self-distancing. Studies 2 and 3
examined the implications of these different types of self-talk for regulating stress surrounding making
good first impressions (Study 2) and public speaking (Study 3). Compared with the first-person group,
the non-first-person group performed better according to objective raters in both studies. They also
displayed less distress (Studies 2 and 3) and engaged in less maladaptive postevent processing (Study 3).
Studies 4 and 5 examined how these different forms of self-talk influence the way people appraise
social-anxiety-provoking events. They demonstrated that non-first-person language use (compared with
first-person language use) leads people to appraise future stressors in more challenging and less
threatening terms. Finally, a meta-analysis (Study 6) indicated that none of these findings were
moderated by trait social anxiety, highlighting their translational potential. Together, these findings
demonstrate that small shifts in the language people use to refer to the self during introspection
consequentially influence their ability to regulate their thoughts, feelings, and behavior under social
stress, even for vulnerable individuals.
Keywords: distancing, stress, social anxiety, rumination, self-regulation, emotion regulation
During the summer of 2010, LeBron James, a future Hall-of-
Fame basketball player, faced a tough decision. Should he stay
with the small market team that nurtured his career from its
inception, or move to a larger city market? Shortly after making his
choice (he joined the larger market team), he described his
decision-making process in an interview noting, “One thing I
didn’t want to do was make an emotional decision. Iwanted to do
what’s best for LeBron James and to do what makes LeBron James
happy” (Greenberg, 2010).
Notice how James begins by referring to himself using the
pronoun I, but then quickly switches to using his own name after
indicating that he does not want to make an emotional decision.
Does this shift from Ito James represent a mere quirk of speech?
Or could it represent something more—a process, for example,
that consequentially influences people’s capacity to control their
thoughts, feelings, and behavior? Here we suggest that it is the
latter. Specifically, we hypothesize that using one’s own name and
other non-first-person pronouns to refer to the self during intro-
spection is a form of self-distancing that enhances self-regulation.
1
Harnessing Language to Promote Self-Regulation
Several lines of research motivate this prediction. First, con-
verging evidence suggests that enhancing psychological distance,
1
People can focus on a variety of stimuli from a psychologically
distanced perspective (e.g., the self, other people, other objects; Trope &
Liberman, 2010). We use the term self-distance to refer to instances in
which people focus specifically on the self from a distanced perspective.
Ethan Kross, Psychology Department, University of Michigan, Ann
Arbor; Emma Bruehlman-Senecal, Psychology Department, University of
California, Berkeley; Jiyoung Park, Aleah Burson, Adrienne Dougherty,
Holly Shablack, and Ryan Bremner, Psychology Department, University of
Michigan, Ann Arbor; Jason Moser, Psychology Department, Michigan
State University, East Lansing; Ozlem Ayduk, Psychology Department,
University of California, Berkeley.
We thank the many research assistants at Michigan and Berkeley for
their assistance conducting the studies, and Vivian Zayas, Robin Edelstein,
Phoebe Ellsworth, and Oscar Ybarra for their feedback.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ethan
Kross, Psychology Department, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI
48109, or to Ozlem Ayduk, Psychology Department, University of Cali-
fornia, Berkeley, CA 94720-1650. E-mail: ekross@umich.edu or ayduk@
berkeley.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 106, No. 2, 304–324 0022-3514/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0035173
304
which we construed broadly as the capacity to transcend one’s
egocentric viewpoint of a stimulus, facilitates self-regulation. Re-
search in children and adults indicates, for example, that psycho-
logical distancing strategies enhance people’s capacity to exert
self-control when faced with tempting options in the short term
(e.g., Fujita, Trope, Liberman, & Levin-Sagi, 2006;Kober et al.,
2010;Mischel & Rodriguez, 1993;Sigel & McGillicuddy-De Lisi,
2003). In a similar vein, research on self-reflection indicates that
cueing people to reflect on painful past experiences from a self-
distanced or “fly-on-the-wall” visual perspective helps them re-
flect on their experiences without ruminating (Gruber, Harvey, &
Johnson, 2009;Kross & Ayduk, 2011; also see Ray, Wilhelm, &
Gross, 2008;Wisco & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011).
The concept of self-distancing also factors prominently into
several cognitive and behavioral treatment paradigms. Beck (1970)
described distancing as a process that allows clients to think
objectively about irrational thoughts and emphasized the impor-
tance of this process for effective cognitive therapy (pp. 189
190)—a view that many scholars have since echoed (e.g., Fresco,
Segal, Buis, & Kennedy, 2007;Ingram & Hollon, 1986). The
concept of distancing, although sometimes referred to as “decen-
tering” or “self as context,” is also central to many newer forms of
cognitive therapy, which emphasize the importance of enhancing
psychological distance from the self for allowing people to observe
and accept their feelings (e.g., Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, &
Lillis, 2006;Teasdale et al., 2002).
These findings are noteworthy in the current context because
researchers have speculated that the language people use to refer to
the self may influence self-distancing. For example, research on
expressive writing conceptualizes the degree of first-person pro-
nouns that people use when writing about emotional experiences
as a marker of self-distancing—the fewer first-person pronouns
people use, the more people attempt to distance themselves from
their experiences (e.g., Cohn, Mehl, & Pennebaker, 2004; also see
Pennebaker & King, 1999). Supporting this inference, cueing
people to reflect on emotional (Grossmann & Kross, 2010;Kross
& Ayduk, 2008) and nonemotional (Mcisaac & Eich, 2002) expe-
riences from the visual perspective of a psychologically distanced
observer (compared with a first-person visual perspective) leads
them to describe these experiences using fewer first-person pro-
nouns.
Why might certain patterns of language use, in particular peo-
ple’s use of non-first-person pronouns and their own name to refer
to the self, promote self-distancing? In general, people use these
parts of speech when thinking about, referencing, or speaking to other
people. Thus, if people use these parts of speech to refer to the self,
this may enhance self-distancing by leading people to think about
themselves as though they were someone else—albeit another self
whose inner thoughts and feelings they have privileged access to.
In contrast, people virtually exclusively use first person pronouns
when thinking about or referring to the self from their egocentric
point of view. Thus, people who use these parts of speech during
introspection should think about the self as they normally do—
from a self-immersed, first-person perspective.
In sum, our analysis suggests that the language people use to
refer to the self during introspection may influence self-distancing,
and thus have consequential implications for their ability to regu-
late their thoughts, feelings, and behavior under stress. Our first
goal was to explore this question.
Exploring Generalizability to Vulnerable Populations:
Social Anxiety
Our second goal was to explore whether social anxiety moder-
ates the self-regulatory benefits of non-first-person language use
during introspection. We focused on social anxiety because it can
be studied in the laboratory with a high degree of ecological
validity. That is, the situations that socially anxious people fear
most—being evaluated by others in a social context—can be
simulated in the laboratory relatively easily, in ways that lend
themselves well to testing predictions about mechanisms underly-
ing self-regulation. Moreover, social anxiety is one of the most
common forms of anxiety (e.g., Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, &
Walters, 2005). From the apprehensive partygoer to the anxious
public speaker to the socially phobic patient, countless people
experience social anxiety and the concomitant negative psycho-
logical and physical consequences that it generates.
Cognitive models of social anxiety suggest that socially anxious
individuals experience high levels of anxiety in response to the
threat of future social interactions (D. M. Clark & Wells, 1995;
Rapee & Heimberg, 1997). When these situations are possible,
they worry excessively about the likelihood of performing poorly,
which interferes with their performance. The perception and actu-
ality of their poor performance reinforce their negative beliefs
about the self, giving rise to maladaptive postevent processing or
rumination (Brozovich & Heimberg, 2008).
In current cognitive behavioral therapies, social anxiety is
treated by challenging negative expectations about upcoming
events (e.g., D. M. Clark et al., 2003). However, to the extent that
the language people use to refer to the self during introspection
influences self-regulation, it might provide an additional (poten-
tially easily implemented) tool for helping vulnerable populations
cope with social threat.
Research Overview
Seven studies examined whether language use during introspec-
tion influences people’s ability to self-regulate under social stress.
Studies 1a and 1b were proof-of-principle studies. They examined
whether using non-first-person pronouns and one’s own name
(non-first-person group from hereon) rather than first-person pro-
nouns (first-person group from hereon) during introspection en-
hances self-distancing. Studies 2 and 3 then examined the impli-
cations of these different forms of self-talk for regulating the
cognitive, emotional, and behavioral sequalae of social stress.
Studies 4 and 5 examined how these different forms of self-talk
influence the way people appraise future stressors. Finally, Study
6 consisted of a meta-analysis that combined data from Studies 2
through 5 to examine whether trait social anxiety moderates the
self-regulatory effects of non-first-person language use.
Studies 1a and 1b
Our starting point is that using non-first-person pronouns and
one’s own name to refer to the self during introspection promotes
self-distancing. One way to test this prediction is to examine
whether language use during self-reflection influences the vantage
point that people adopt when visualizing emotional experiences. If
non-first-person language use (i.e., “linguistic” self-distancing)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
305
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
enhances self-distancing, then people who use this type of lan-
guage during introspection should be more likely to report seeing
themselves in their past experience from an observer’s visual
perspective (i.e., “visual” self-distancing) than people who use
first-person pronouns during introspection. This logic is broadly
consistent with construal level theory, which suggests that poten-
tiating psychological distance in one domain enhances psycholog-
ical distance in other domains (Trope & Liberman, 2003,2010).
We tested this prediction by asking participants to analyze their
feelings surrounding an anger-provoking (Study 1a) and anxiety-
provoking (Study 1b) negative autobiographical experience using
first-person pronouns or non-first-person pronouns and their own
name. We focused on two types of negative emotional experiences
in these studies to examine the reliability and generalizability of
our findings. After participants analyzed their feelings, we asked
them to indicate the degree to which they adopted the visual
perspective of an observer as they reflected on their feelings.
Method
Participants. In Study 1a, participants were 56 undergradu-
ates (M
age
!18.95 years, SD
age
!4.74; 38 females; 52% White,
23% Asian American, 11% African American, and 14% other)
who received course credit for their participation. In Study 1b, 93
participants were recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk
(MTurk; M
age
!32.23 years, SD
age
!12.98; 50 females; 79.6%
White, 12.9% Asian American, 2.2% African American, and 5.5%
other).
Procedure and materials.
Baseline affect. After providing informed consent, partici-
pants in both studies rated how they felt “right now” (1 !very
negative,9!very positive; Study 1a: M!6.35, SD !1.17; Study
1b: M!6.74, SD !1.46).
Negative experience recall task. Next, we asked participants
to recall for Study 1a an anger-related autobiographical experi-
ence:
No matter how well two people get along there are times when they
experience conflict . . . [T]ake a few moments right now to recall a
time when you experienced such conflict with another person—a time
when you became truly enraged at this person.
For Study 1b, we asked participants to recall an anxiety-related
autobiographical experience:
No matter how satisfied people are with their lives, there are times
when they worry and experience anxiety . . . [T]ake a few moments
right now to think about a time from your past when you worried
about something happening to you.
They were given as much time as they needed to recall their
experience (Study 1a: M
sec
!37.18, SD
sec
!19.36; Study 1b:
M
sec
!45.16, SD
sec
!24.72). Aside from cueing people to recall
different types of experiences, the only other difference between
the two sets of recall instructions was that in Study 1b, we
instructed participants to “close their eyes” before the memory
recall instructions, whereas participants in Study 1a did not receive
this instruction.
Experimental manipulation. Participants were then randomly
assigned to reflect on their feelings surrounding their recalled
experience using first-person pronouns (Study 1a: n!28; Study
1b: n!49) or non-first-person pronouns and their own name
(Study 1a: n!28; Study 1b: n!44). The instructions used to
manipulate linguistic self-distancing were identical in both studies.
Participants in the first person group were told:
One of the things we’re interested in in this study is the language
people use to understand their feelings. Some people try to understand
their feelings by thinking about themselves using first-person pro-
nouns, so this is what we would like you to do. Please try to
understand why you felt the way you did in the experience you just
recalled using the pronouns “I” and “my” as much as possible. In
other words, ask yourself, “Why did I feel this way? What were the
underlying causes and reasons for my feelings?”
Participants in the non-first-person group were told:
One of the things we’re interested in in this study is the language
people use to understand their feelings. Some people try to understand
their feelings by thinking about themselves using their own name and
other non-first-person pronouns, so this is what we would like you to
do. Please try to understand why you felt the way you did in the
experience you just recalled using the pronoun “you” and “[your own
name]” as much as possible. In other words, if your name was Jane,
you would ask yourself, “Why did Jane feel this way? What were the
underlying causes and reasons for Jane’s feelings?”
2
Participants had 60 s to reflect on their feelings following these
instructions.
Visual self-distance. Following prior research (Mischowski,
Kross, & Bushman, 2012;Park et al., 2013), participants com-
pleted two items to measure visual self-distancing after the reflec-
tion period was over. First, participants rated the extent to which
they saw the event replay through their own eyes versus watched
the event unfold as an observer as they analyzed their feelings (1 !
predominantly immersed participant,7!predominantly dis-
tanced observer). Second, they rated how far away from the scene
they were as they analyzed their feelings (1 !very close, saw it
through my own eyes;7!very far, saw it as if an observer).
Ratings on these items were averaged to create a visual self-
distancing index, Study 1a: "!.74, M!2.72, SD !1.40; Study
1b: "!.85, M!3.13, SD !1.66.
Instructional manipulation check. Because we had less con-
trol over the online environment in which Study 1b was performed,
we administered an instructional manipulation check at the end of
the study to ensure that participants paid attention to our instruc-
tions. Specifically, participants read, “We are interested in whether you
actually take the time to read the directions . . . [I]n order to
demonstrate that you have read these instructions, please select
‘online shopping’ and ‘check e-mail only’ below.” Participants
were then presented with a series of Internet activity choices with
“online shopping” and “check e-mail only” embedded among
them.
Results and Discussion
Preliminary analyses. Twenty-one participants failed the in-
structional manipulation check: 14 in the first person group and seven in
the non-first-person group. They were excluded from subsequent
analyses on a priori grounds, leaving 72 participants in Study 1b,
35 in the first person group, and 37 in the non-first-person group.
2
Jane was replaced with Joe for male participants.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
306 KROSS ET AL.
This exclusion rate (23%) is consistent with prior studies that have
used instructional manipulation checks (Oppenheimer, Meyvis, &
Davidenko, 2009). Exclusions did not differ by condition, Study
1a: #
2
(1) !0.06, p!.80; Study 1b: #
2
(1) !2.13, p!.15.
All analyses controlled for baseline affect, which the groups did
not differ on, Fs $1. Gender was not related to the dependent
variable in either study, Fs$1, and controlling for it did not
substantively alter any of the results. Three participants (one in
Study 1a; two in Study 1b) had missing values because of omitted
responses.
Visual self-distancing. As expected, participants in the non-
first-person groups displayed significantly higher levels of visual
self-distancing than participants in the first-person groups in both
Study 1a, F(1, 52) !5.96, p!.018, %p
2!.103, and Study 1b, F(1,
67) !4.45, p!.039, %p
2!.062 (see Figure 1). These findings
demonstrate that using non-first-person pronouns and one’s own
name to refer to the self enhances self-distancing.
Studies 2 and 3: Overview
Having established that using non-first-person language to refer
to the self during introspection increases self-distancing, we ex-
amined in Studies 2 and 3 whether language use during introspec-
tion influences self-regulation under social stress. Both Studies 2
and 3 focused on these same core issues but were designed by
different laboratories and implemented independently. As a result,
they had similar procedures and focused on a partially overlapping
set of dependent variables. Thus, they provide convergent evi-
dence for the role that language use plays in self-regulation across
different situations, laboratories, and measures.
3
Study 2
We recruited participants for a study on the psychology of first
impressions. They were told that they would be asked to make a
positive first impression on another person. Prior to engaging in
this task, we asked them to reflect on their feelings concerning
their upcoming social interaction using either first-person pro-
nouns or non-first-person pronouns and their own name. We then
examined the effect of this manipulation on participants’ anxiety
and performance during the social interaction. We predicted that
participants in the non-first-person group would experience less
anxiety and perform better during the impression formation task.
Method
Participants. Participants were 97 undergraduate women
(M
age
!20.05 years, SD
age
!1.86; 26.8% White, 49.5% Asian
American, 5.1% African American, and 18.6% other) who re-
ceived course credit or payment for participating. We focused on
women only in this study because they are disproportionately
affected by anxiety-related problems, including social anxiety
(Kessler et al., 1994;Spurr & Stopa, 2002;Weinstock, 1999).
Procedure and materials.
Phase 1: Trait social anxiety. Participants completed the 12-
item Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (BFNE; Leary, 1983)
online approximately 5 days (M!5.19 days, SD !3.41) before
the study. Sample items include, “I am afraid that others will not
approve of me” and “Sometimes I think I am too concerned with
what other people think of me” (1 !not at all characteristic of me,
5!extremely characteristic of me;"!.80; M!37.03, SD !
9.23).
Phase 2: Cover story and premanipulation anxiety. Informed
consent was obtained at the beginning of the experiment. As part
of this process, we informed participants that they would be having
a videotaped conversation with another participant. Next, partici-
pants rated how anxious they felt (1 !not at all,5!extremely;
M!2.30, SD !0.96).
Phase 3: Explanation of study. An experimenter told partic-
ipants that the study focused on impression formation. Specifi-
cally, they were told that their goal would be to make a good first
impression during a short conversation with an opposite sex par-
ticipant after which they would be evaluating each other’s perfor-
mance. In addition, they were informed that their conversation
would be videotaped and assessed by trained psychologists who
would be rating their social skills. Such “getting to know you”
tasks are commonly used to elicit social anxiety in the laboratory
(e.g., J. V. Clark & Arkowitz, 1975;Glass, Merluzzi, Biever, &
Larsen, 1982;Turner, Beidel, & Larkin, 1986).
Next, the experimenter added that an additional goal of the study
was to examine how people can effectively prepare themselves to
make good first impressions. Thus, they were told that they would
be receiving instructions about how to prepare themselves for their
social interaction in a few moments. After these instructions were
conveyed, the experimenter left the participant alone in a room
without any distractions for 2 min. During this time, the experi-
menter retrieved the participant’s condition assignment from an-
other room.
Phase 4: Experimental manipulation. After the 2-min wait-
ing period, the experimenter told participants, “We are interested
in the different ways people go about preparing themselves psy-
chologically for meeting new people and what effect each type of
self-preparation has on performance.” They were then randomly
3
In Studies 2 and 3, a number of additional measures were included for
exploratory purposes. They are available upon request from the corre-
sponding authors.
Figure 1. The effect of condition on visual self-distancing in Study 1a
and Study 1b measured on scales ranging from 1 (predominantly immersed
participant)to7(predominantly distanced observer). Error bars indicate
standard errors.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
307
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
assigned to a first-person (n!48) or non-first-person (n!49)
condition using instructions that were virtually identical to those
used in Study 1. The main difference was that participants were
asked to think about their feelings concerning an upcoming
anxiety-provoking event (rather than a past experience). Following
these instructions, participants reflected over their thoughts and
feelings alone for 3 min.
Phase 5: Manipulation check and preinteraction anxiety.
Following the experimental manipulation, participants answered
the following questions, “How well do you feel you followed the
speech preparation instructions” (1 !not at all well,5!perfectly
well) and “To what extent did you use the first-person pronouns I
and me (or non-first-person pronouns you and your own name) to
refer to yourself when you were working through your thoughts
and feelings about the upcoming conversation (1 !not at all,5!
exclusively). Scores on these questions were averaged to create a
manipulation check index ("!.68; M!3.79, SD !0.70). Next,
participants rated their anxiety again, using the same question that
was administered prior to the manipulation (M!2.84, SD !
0.97).
Phase 6: Social evaluation task. Subsequently, the experi-
menter introduced the participant to a new experimenter and their
interaction partner, who was in fact a confederate. Prior research
indicates that opposite sex interactions are more anxiety provoking
than same sex interactions, especially for socially anxious people
(e.g., Turner et al., 1986). Therefore, following prior research (e.g.,
Burgio, Merluzzi, & Pryor, 1986;J. Clark & Arkowitz, 1975;
Glass et al., 1982;Kashdan & Roberts, 2006), the confederate in
this study was always male. Both the new experimenter and
confederate were blind to condition.
Unstructured interaction. The interaction began when the new
experimenter and confederate entered the room and sat across from
the participant. The experimenter told the participant that he or she
had to leave the room to calibrate the cameras and would return
shortly. The participant was given no instructions about how to
interact with the confederate during this period. We included this
unstructured interaction period to explore whether the manipula-
tion influenced participants’ tendencies to spontaneously initiate
conversation with their partner. Confederates were instructed not
to initiate conversation for the first 30 s. After an initial silence of
30 s, the confederate initiated contact by saying, “It wasn’t easy to
find the room here today.” Three condition-blind raters coded
whether participants initiated conversation during the first 30 s
(intraclass correlation [ICC] !1).
Structured interaction. When the experimenter returned, he or
she presented the participant and the confederate with instructions
for the social interaction. Following the procedure used by Me-
leshko and Alden (1993), participants were given a list of topics to
talk about (e.g., “What have you always wanted to try but
haven’t?”; “What is your biggest pet peeve?”). The experimenter
instructed them to take turns selecting and answering questions
from this list until they had both answered four questions. The
confederate always began the exchange and disclosed on the same
four questions with all participants. Confederates’ responses were
scripted so that the content and delivery of their disclosures was
consistent. The experimenter left the room prior to the initiation of
the conversation and returned after the conversation was over.
Three judges rated participants’ performance during this phase
on two dimensions. First, they rated how nervous the participant
was (1 !below average level of nervousness,5!above average
level of nervousness; ICC !.73; M!2.87, SD !0.78). Second,
they rated participants’ overall performance using a modified
version of the Social Performance Rating Scale (Fydrich, Chamb-
less, Perry, Buergener, & Beazley, 1998)—a behavioral assess-
ment of social performance designed for social phobia. Specifi-
cally, judges coded participants’ behavior along the following four
dimensions using a 5-point scale (1 !poor,5!excellent): gaze,
vocal quality, speech length, and discomfort (ICC !.73).
4
Gaze
refers to whether the participant made appropriate eye contact with
the confederate. Vocal quality refers to whether the participant
spoke clearly and varied her vocal tone in an engaging manner.
Length refers to whether the participant spoke for an appropriate
amount of time when answering the questions (i.e., neither being
overly brief nor long-winded) and lacked awkward pauses. Dis-
comfort refers to whether the participant showed verbal (e.g.,
speech dysfluencies) and nonverbal (e.g., self-manipulative behav-
iors like hair twirling, facial touching, and so on) indicators of
anxiety during the interaction. Following Fydrich et al. (1998), we
created a measure of overall performance by summing partici-
pants’ scores on these dimensions ("!.80; M!13.10, SD !
2.05).
Preliminary analyses indicated that scores on the nervousness
and overall performance indexes were highly correlated. There-
fore, we collapsed them to create a composite behavioral index of
social interaction performance after reverse scoring the nervous-
ness scale and then standardizing scores on each measure ("!
.84).
Phase 7: Postinteraction anxiety. Participants rated their anx-
iety at the end of the interaction using the same question they
completed earlier (M!1.81, SD !0.92).
Phase 8: Debriefing. Finally, participants were debriefed for
suspicion and compensated.
Results
Preliminary analyses. Four participants in each condition
were excluded—four because they suspected that their partner was
a confederate and/or inferred the study aims during the funneled
debriefing, and four because of protocol errors (e.g., the confed-
erate did not follow protocol)—leaving 89 participants, 44 in the
first-person group and 45 in the non-first-person group. Exclusions
did not differ by condition, #
2
(1) !0.00, p!.98.
The groups did not differ on premanipulation anxiety, t(86) !
0.54, p!.590, or trait social anxiety, t(87) !&0.61, p!.541.
Both of these variables were, as expected, related to several of the
dependent variables (see Table 1). Therefore, they were included
as covariates. Degrees of freedom vary slightly across analyses due
to missing data.
Manipulation check. There was no effect of condition on the
manipulation check, F(1, 84) !0.00, p!.973, %p
2!.000
(first-person: M!3.80, SE !0.10; non-first-person: M!3.80,
SE !0.10), indicating that both groups followed the instructions
equally well. It should be noted that one-sample ttests indicated
that the mean manipulation check score for each group was sig-
nificantly greater than the midpoint of the manipulation check
4
We did not include the conversational flow category because the
structured nature of the interaction did not allow us to code this dimension.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
308 KROSS ET AL.
scale: first-person: t(43) !13.95, p$.001, d!4.25; and
non-first-person: t(44) !12.14, p$.001, d!3.66, indicating that
both groups also implemented the instructions successfully.
Anxiety. We examined the effect of condition on state anxiety
by performing a repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).
Time (t) of state anxiety assessment was the within-participant
variable (three: premanipulation [t1] vs. preinteraction [t2] vs.
postinteraction [t3]), condition was the between-participants vari-
able (two: first-person vs. non-first-person), and trait social anxiety
(continuous) was the covariate. This analysis revealed two effects.
First, condition interacted marginally with time, F(2, 84) !
2.93, p!.059, %p
2!.065. To understand the meaning of this
interaction, we performed univariate ANCOVAs on the change in
anxiety from the (a) first to the second anxiety assessment (prein-
teraction anxiety [t2] MINUS premanipulation anxiety [t1]), and
(b) the second to third anxiety assessment (postinteraction anxiety
[t3] MINUS preinteraction anxiety [t2]) with condition as the
between-subjects predictor and trait social anxiety as the covariate.
These tests demonstrated that condition did not influence change
in anxiety from the first to the second anxiety assessment, F(1,
85) !0.12, p!.726, %p
2!.001, but did influence change in
anxiety from the second to the third anxiety assessment, F(1,
86) !4.12, p!.045, %p
2!.046. As Figure 2, Panel A illustrates,
the latter result indicated that participants in the non-first-person
group displayed a sharper decrease in anxiety after the social
interaction than participants in the first-person group. This effect
remained significant when performance during the structured
phase of the interaction was controlled for, F(1, 83) !4.14, p!
.045, %p
2!.048, suggesting that it was not the case that people felt
better simply because they performed better.
Second, trait social anxiety interacted with time to predict state
anxiety, F(2, 84) !3.48, p!.035, %p
2!.077. To understand the
meaning of this interaction, we conducted parallel analyses as
described previously. These analyses revealed no relationship be-
tween trait social anxiety and change in state anxiety from the first
to the second anxiety assessment, F(1, 85) !0.18, p!.670, %p
2!
.002, but a significant association between trait social anxiety and
change in state anxiety from the second to the third anxiety
assessment, F(1, 86) !5.65, p!.020, %p
2!.062. The latter result
demonstrated that the more apprehensive participants were about
social evaluation, the more their anxiety levels declined following
the interaction, pr !.25, p!.020, possibly reflecting the relief
socially anxious individuals experienced after the interaction was
over.
Behavior. We performed logistic regression on initiating con-
tact (yes !0 vs. no !1) with the confederate during the unstruc-
tured phase of the social interaction task with condition as the
between-subjects predictor and trait social anxiety and premanipu-
lation anxiety as covariates. None of these variables significantly
predicted contact initiation, ps'.13. However, this likely reflects
a ceiling effect as 84% of participants initiated conversation with-
out prompting.
Table 1
Study 2 Zero-Order Correlations Between Measured Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Trait social anxiety .18
.23
!
&.04 .09 &.05
2. Premanipulation anxiety (T1) .33
!!!
.43
!!!
&.16 .08
3. Preinteraction anxiety (T2) .29
!!
.04 .04
4. Postinteraction anxiety (T3) .01 .04
5. Conversation initiation &.04
6. Performance
Note.T!Time.
p!.10.
!
p!.05.
!!
p!.01.
!!!
p!.005.
Figure 2. The effect of condition on anxiety over time (Panel A) and adaptive social interaction performance
(Panel B) in Study 2. Error bars indicate standard errors. Time 1 !t1.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
309
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
Next, we performed an ANCOVA on judges’ ratings of perfor-
mance during the structured phase of the interaction with condi-
tion as the between-subjects predictor and trait social anxiety and
premanipulation anxiety as covariates. Judges’ ratings indicated
that the non-first-person group performed better on the social
interaction task than the first-person group, F(1, 82) !7.18, p!
.009, %p
2!.081 (see Figure 2, Panel B). Neither premanipulation
anxiety, F(1, 82) !0.51 p!.478, %p
2!.006, nor trait social
anxiety, F(1, 82) !0.08, p!.774, %p
2!.001, predicted this
variable.
Summary and Discussion
Study 2 examined whether the language people use to refer to
the self during introspection influences how they feel and behave
under social stress. It generated three key findings.
First, both groups were equally capable of implementing the
self-talk manipulations. This suggests that using non-first-person
language during introspection is a feasible alternative to using
first-person language.
Second, contrary to our expectations, condition did not influ-
ence the increase in anxiety participants reported from the first to
the second assessment. However, condition did predict changes in
anxiety from the second to the third anxiety assessment, indicating
that participants in the non-first-person group (compared with the
first-person group) displayed a sharper decrease in anxiety from
before to after the social interaction. This finding is particularly
noteworthy because it suggests that using non-first-person lan-
guage to refer to the self during introspection may lead people to
recover more quickly from social stressors.
Finally, the experimental manipulation influenced participants’
behavior during the social interaction. Judges indicated that par-
ticipants in the non-first-person group were less nervous during the
interaction and performed better than their first-person counter-
parts. These findings highlight the adaptive behavioral implica-
tions that using non-first-person pronouns and one’s own name
during introspection have for people when they engage in tasks
that elicit social stress.
Study 3
Study 3 examined the implications of the same two types of
self-talk that Study 2 focused on for allowing people to regulate
their thoughts, feelings, and behavior under social evaluative
stress. However, a different social stress induction was used, the
focus was on different dependent variables, and men as well as
women were included. In so doing, we aimed in Study 3 to extend
the Study 2 findings in four ways.
First, in Study 3, we examined whether the Study 2 results
would generalize to an arguably more powerful social stress in-
duction—a public speech task rather than an impression formation
task (Beazley, Glass, Chambless, & Arnkoff, 2001;Turner et al.,
1986). Building on prior research indicating that visual self-
distancing manipulations generalize to powerful negative affect
inductions (Mischowski et al., 2012), we predicted that the benefits
associated with using non-first-person pronouns and one’s own
name during introspection would generalize to this novel context.
Second, although Study 2 included a premanipulation measure
of affect that the two groups did not differ on, this measure was
administered after participants were informed about the nature of
the study. Thus, one could argue that the premanipulation anxiety
measure used in Study 2 did not constitute a true baseline measure
because anxiety was likely induced to a certain degree among all
participants. Study 3 included a true baseline affect measure that
was administered before participants learned about the nature of
the study.
Third, whereas Study 2 focused specifically on anxiety, Study 3
included measures of both global affect and shame. Shame is the
key discrete emotion elicited by public speech tasks (Dickerson &
Kemeny, 2004).
Finally, prior research indicates that people often perseverate
over their performance in social-anxiety-provoking tasks, which
fuels social anxiety—a process that is often referred to as postevent
processing (Brozovich & Heimberg, 2008; also see Nolen-
Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008). Therefore, Study 3
included multiple measures of postevent processing to examine
whether the experimental manipulations influenced this variable.
To the extent that reflecting on the self using non-first-person
pronouns and one’s own name enhances people’s ability to regu-
late their social anxiety, we predicted that participants who en-
gaged in this process should also display less postevent processing.
Method
Participants. Participants were 89 undergraduates (60 fe-
males; M
age
!19.01 years, SD
age
!1.04; 73.0% White, 12.4%
Asian American, 6.7% African American, and 7.9% other) who
received course credit or $20 for their participation.
Procedure and materials.
Phase 1: Trait social anxiety. We assessed trait social anxiety
using two measures to enhance reliability: the BFNE ("!.88;
M!36.02, SD !8.13) and the 17-item Social Phobia Inventory
(SPIN; Connor et al., 2000; scale: 0 !not at all,4!extremely;
"!.88; M!21.19, SD !9.91).
5
Sample items for the SPIN
include, “I am bothered by blushing in front of other people,” and
“Parties and social events scare me.” Both measures were admin-
istered approximately 4 days before the experiment (M
days
!3.66,
SD
days
!1.76). Scores on the BFNE and SPIN were standardized
and collapsed to form a single trait social anxiety index ("!.69).
Phase 2: Baseline affect. After providing informed consent,
participants rated how they felt “right now” (1 !very negative,
7!very positive;M!4.93, SD !1.07).
Phase 3: Stress induction. We induced social stress using a
modified version of the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST; Kirsch-
baum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993). This task involves having
participants deliver a public speech in front of an evaluative
audience without receiving sufficient time to prepare; it is one of
the most powerful ways of inducing stress in the laboratory among
humans (Dickerson, Gruenewald, & Kemeny, 2004;Dickerson &
Kemeny, 2004). Following established procedures (Kirschbaum et
al., 1993), the experimenter told participants that they would have
to give a speech on why they are qualified for their “dream” job to
a panel of interviewers trained to evaluate speech performance.
They were also told that their performance would be videotaped.
5
Due to a protocol error, SPIN Item 17 was not administered. We
replaced this missing value with each participant’s mean rating of the first
16 items.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
310 KROSS ET AL.
They were then taken to a small room, which contained a desk and
chair, and given 5 min to prepare. They were not permitted to take
notes during this time.
Phase 4: Experimental manipulation. After the 5-min prep-
aration period, participants were told:
Besides preparing the content of a speech, people also need to prepare
themselves psychologically before giving a speech, so we are inter-
ested in learning about the different ways people go about preparing
themselves to give a speech, and what effect each type of self-
preparation has on performance.
They were then randomly assigned to the first-person (n!45)
or the non-first-person (n!44) condition using instructions that
were virtually identical to those used in Study 2. The key differ-
ences were that (a) in this study, participants were directed to
analyze their emotions before giving a speech (rather than before
engaging in a social interaction task) and (b) “he or she” was given
as an example of the type of non-first-person pronoun participants
in the non-first-person condition could use in addition to “you.’
Phase 5: Speech task. Next, the experimenter returned and led
the participant to another room where they delivered their 5-min
speech to two confederates posing as evaluators. A video camera
positioned in their field of vision recorded their performance.
Phase 6: Speech performance. Two condition-blind coders
watched videotapes of participants’ speeches and rated them on
three dimensions: confidence, nervousness, and overall perfor-
mance (1 !below average,5!above average). After reverse
scoring the nervousness scores, coders’ ratings were found to be
consistent across these dimensions (Rater 1: ICC !.75; Rater 2:
ICC !.78). Therefore, we collapsed across all three dimensions to
create a single speech performance index for each coder. Judge’s
ratings on these indexes were collapsed to form a single speech
performance index ("!.86; M!3.39, SD !0.83).
Phase 7: Postspeech global affect and shame. After partici-
pants delivered their speeches, they again rated how they felt “right
now” to allow measurement of global affect (1 !very negative,
7!very positive;M!4.72, SD !1.16). Next, they completed 10
items that compose the Shame and Pride subscales (e.g., “I want to
sink into the floor and disappear,” “I feel proud”; 1 !not feeling
this way at all,5!feeling this way very strongly) of the State
Shame and Guilt Scale (Marschall, Sanftner, & Tangney, 1994).
We focused on how the manipulation influenced emotions that fall
on the shame–pride dimension because they are particularly rele-
vant to public speech challenges (Dickerson et al., 2004). Re-
sponses to these items were averaged after reverse scoring the
pride ratings, so that higher scores reflected greater shame ("!
.90; M!20.25, SD !7.16).
Phase 8: Postevent processing. Next, the experimenters in-
formed participants that they had to set up the next phase of the
study in another room and asked them to sit quietly until they
returned in 5 min. This provided participants with an opportunity
to ruminate over their speech performance (for a similar approach
to assessing rumination, see Ayduk & Kross, 2008;Gerin, David-
son, Christenfeld, Goyal, & Schwartz, 2006;Zoccola, Dickerson,
& Zaldivar, 2008). At the end of this 5-min period, postevent
processing was assessed in two ways.
First, we asked participants to describe in writing the stream of
thoughts that flowed through their mind as they waited for the
experimenter to return. Usinga0(not at all)to4(completely)
scale, two condition-blind raters coded these essays for recounting
and reconstruing. Prior research has linked the tendency to rumi-
nate over negative experiences with higher levels of recounting
and lower levels of reconstruing (Ayduk & Kross, 2010b;Gross-
mann & Kross, 2010;Kross, Ayduk, & Mischel, 2005). Recount-
ing was operationalized as statements in which participants re-
hearsed the specific chain of events and emotions that they
experienced during the speech task (e.g., “I was feeling nervous
and fidgeted a lot while I was speaking”). Reconstruing was
operationalized as statements in which participants described re-
alizations about their experience during the task (e.g., “I was only
given 5 min to prepare my speech and was thus almost set up to not
do well”). Interrater reliability was high for both recounting
(ICC !.79; M!1.13, SD !1.34) and reconstruing (ICC !.82;
M!0.49, SD !0.81). Following prior research (e.g., Ayduk &
Kross, 2010b;Kross & Ayduk, 2008;Kross, Duckworth, Ayduk,
Tsukayama, & Mischel, 2011), we collapsed across both measures
by subtracting reconstruing scores from recounting scores to create
a single thought content index. Higher scores reflected a greater
tendency to recount versus reconstrue (M!0.63, SD !1.21).
Second, after participants wrote their essay, they completed the
five-item Rumination Questionnaire (RQ; Mellings & Alden,
2000), which we modified to apply to a speech task. Specifically,
participants rated (1 !not at all,7!a lot) five items that assessed
the degree to which they brooded over their speech task perfor-
mance (e.g., “To what extent did you criticize yourself about not
handling the speech task well?”; "!.70; M!3.65, SD !1.11).
As expected, these two postevent processing measures were
closely related ("!.71). Therefore, we created a single index of
maladaptive postevent processing by collapsing the data after
standardizing scores on each measure.
Phase 9: Debriefing. Participants were debriefed at the end of
the study for suspicion and then compensated. No participant
voiced suspicion about the study.
Results
Preliminary analyses. The groups did not differ on baseline
affect, t(85) !0.26, p!.798, trait social anxiety, t(87) !0.70, p!
.485, or gender, #
2
(1) !0.09, p!.764. As in Study 2, baseline affect
and trait social anxiety were included as covariates. Gender was not
related to any of the dependent variables, and controlling for it did not
substantively alter any of the results (for zero-order correlations, see
Table 2). Therefore, it is not discussed further. Degrees of freedom
vary across analyses due to missing data.
Affect: Global affect and shame. To facilitate comparisons
between global affect and shame, we first reverse scored partici-
pants’ baseline and postmanipulation global affect scores so that
higher numbers on these scales reflected more negative affect,
consistent with scores on the Shame scale. We then examined the
effect of condition on global affect by performing a repeated-
measures ANCOVA with time of global affect measurement as the
within-participants variable (two: baseline vs. postspeech task),
condition as the between-participants variable (two: first-person
vs. non-first-person) and trait social anxiety as the covariate. This
analysis revealed a significant condition by time interaction, F(1,
83) !8.39, p!.005, %p
2!.092, indicating that participants in the
first-person group displayed a significant increase in negative
affect over time, F(1, 42) !9.80, p!.003, %p
2!.189. As Figure
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
311
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
3, Panel A, illustrates, however, participants in the non-first-person
group were buffered against this increase, F(1, 40) !1.20, p!
.280, %p
2!.029. The interaction between trait social anxiety and
time was not significant, F(1, 84) !0.13, p!.719, %p
2!.002.
We also performed an ANCOVA on shame with condition as
the between-subjects predictor, controlling for trait social anxiety
and baseline affect. This analysis revealed an effect of condition,
F(1, 82) !7.70, p!.007, %p
2!.086. As Figure 3, Panel B,
illustrates, non-first-person participants felt less shame after the
speech task. Although the effect of baseline affect was not signif-
icant, F(1, 82) !0.60, p!.441, %p
2!.007, there was a significant
effect of trait social anxiety in the predicted direction, F(1, 82) !
5.20, p!.025, %p
2!.060—trait social anxiety and shame corre-
lated positively, pr !.24, p!.025.
Finally, conceptually replicating the Study 2 results, the effect
of condition on global affect, F(1, 81) !9.47, p!.003, %p
2!.105,
and shame, F(1, 81) !4.91, p!.030, %p
2!.057, remained
significant when controlling for performance.
Behavior. The effect of condition on speech performance was
significant, F(1, 83) !5.43, p!.022, %p
2!.061. As Figure 3,
Panel C, illustrates, non-first-person participants performed better
during the speech task. The effects of trait social anxiety, F(1,
83) !0.21, p!.652, %p
2!.002, and baseline affect, F(1, 83) !
1.19, p!.278, %p
2!.014, were not significant.
Postevent processing. The effect of condition on postevent
processing was significant, F(1, 83) !5.47, p!.022, %p
2!.062,
indicating that non-first-person participants engaged in less poste-
vent processing than first-person participants (see Figure 3, Panel
D). The effects of trait social anxiety, F(1, 83) !2.21, p!.141,
%p
2!.026, and baseline affect, F(1, 83) !0.01, p!.941, %p
2!
.000, were not significant. The effect of condition on postevent
Table 2
Study 3 Zero-Order Correlations Between Measured Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Trait social anxiety .12 .01 &.07 .09 .26
!
.21
!
2. Gender &.01 .00 &.02 .06 .06
3. Baseline affect &.11 &.01 &.09 .00
4. Performance .35
!!!
&.30
!!!
&.07
5. Global affect .69
!!!
.26
!
6. Shame .35
!!!
7. Postevent processing
Note. To facilitate comparisons between global affect and shame, we reverse scored participants’ baseline and
postmanipulation global affect scores so that higher numbers on the scales reflected more negative affect.
!
p!.05.
!!!
p!.005.
Figure 3. The effect of condition on global affect (Panel A), shame (Panel B), adaptive speech task
performance (Panel C), and postevent processing (Panel D) in Study 3. Error bars indicate standard errors.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
312 KROSS ET AL.
processing, F(1, 82) !5.12, p!.026, %p
2!.059, remained
significant when controlling for performance.
Summary and Discussion
In sum, participants who used non-first-person pronouns and
their own name during introspection performed better on the
speech task, experienced less global negative affect and shame
after delivering their speech, and engaged in less postevent pro-
cessing. These findings extend the Study 2 results in three ways.
First, they demonstrate that non-first-person language use dur-
ing introspection promotes self-regulation under conditions that
are well known to arouse both psychological and physiological
stress—an anxiety-provoking public-speaking challenge. Second,
they demonstrate that language use during self-reflection has im-
plications not simply for how people think and feel in the moment
but also for their tendency to engage in postevent processing. This
finding is particularly noteworthy given the role that postevent
processing plays as a risk factor for poor psychological (e.g.,
Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008) and physical (Brosschot, Gerin, &
Thayer, 2006) health, and its prevalence in social phobia (e.g.,
Brozovich & Heimberg, 2008). Finally, controlling for gender did
not alter any of the results we observed. This suggests that the
benefits associated with language use as a tool for promoting
self-regulation may extend to both genders. Future research that
includes larger samples of men is needed, however, to confirm this
finding.
Studies 4 and 5 Overview
The aforementioned studies demonstrate that non-first-person
language use during introspection is a form of self-distancing that
promotes self-regulation under social stress. However, they do not
address how this process influences people’s appraisals of future
anxiety-provoking events. Studies 4 and 5 addressed this issue.
Study 4
Study 4 had three goals. Its first goal was to examine how
language use during introspection influences people’s appraisals of
future stressors. In particular, it sought to examine the effect of
language use on challenge-threat appraisals.
People naturally appraise future stressors along a challenge–
threat continuum (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996; also see Lazarus
& Folkman, 1984). Challenge appraisals occur when people per-
ceive their ability to cope (i.e., their resources) with a situation as
exceeding the demands of the situation; threat appraisals occur
when people perceive the demands of the situation as exceeding
their ability to cope (e.g., Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996;Lazarus &
Folkman, 1984).
In the current study, we hypothesized that non-first-person lan-
guage use would lead people to appraise future stressors in more
challenging and less threatening terms. This prediction was moti-
vated by research indicating that visual self-distancing leads peo-
ple to focus relatively less on the concrete, emotionally arousing
aspects of negative past experiences and relatively more on recon-
struing their experiences in ways that provide insight and closure
(for review, see Ayduk & Kross, 2010a;Kross, 2009;Kross &
Ayduk, 2011; cf. Fujita et al., 2006;Trope & Liberman, 2003,
2010). In the current context, we reasoned that focusing on the
concrete, emotionally arousing aspects of a future stressful expe-
rience are precisely those elements of the situation that should lead
a person to conclude that the demands of the situation outweigh his
or her ability to cope—i.e., less challenge and more threat. The
primary goal of Study 5 was to test this hypothesis.
A secondary goal was to rule out two alternative predictions
concerning how language use influences the way people cogni-
tively represent future stressors. First, we examined the relation-
ship between language use during introspection and cognitive
avoidance—that is, avoiding cognitions that give rise to anxiety.
We focused on this relationship because a number of researchers
have speculated that self-distancing blunts emotional reactions via
a cognitive avoidance mechanism (e.g., Kenny & Bryant, 2007;
Kenny et al., 2009;Kuyken & Moulds, 2009).
Another possibility is that non-first-person language use en-
hances people’s tendency to imagine how they appear from the
perspective of the audience evaluating them. This prediction is
motivated by research indicating that psychological-distancing
manipulations can enhance perspective taking (e.g., Eyal &
Epley, 2010;Schultz & Heimberg, 2008). Although there is
conflicting evidence on whether such perspective taking is
helpful (e.g., Galinsky, Maddux, Gilin, & White, 2008)or
harmful (e.g., Schultz & Heimberg, 2008), we nevertheless
examined whether non-first-person language use enhances au-
dience perspective taking to examine the role that this process
plays in the phenomenon at hand.
The final goal of this study was to examine the reliability of our
failure to observe an effect of language use on anticipatory anxiety
in Study 2. As noted previously, this null effect was unexpected.
Thus, we measured anticipatory anxiety again in this study to
examine whether it would replicate.
We examined these issues using a modified version of the
speech task paradigm used in Study 3. Specifically, we replicated
the Study 3 procedure with the following exceptions. After par-
ticipants reflected on their feelings, we asked them to rate the
extent to which they appraised the upcoming speech task as a
challenge or threat, and their current level of anticipatory anxiety.
We then asked participants to describe in writing the stream of
thoughts that flowed through their mind during the reflection
period and coded these essays for challenge–threat appraisals,
avoidance, and audience perspective taking.
After participants completed the writing task, they were in-
formed that the study was over and debriefed. We did not have
participants deliver their speech because we felt that asking par-
ticipants to answer each of the aforementioned questions and then
describe in writing how they thought about their experience would
compromise the downstream effects of the manipulations, muddy-
ing the conclusions we could draw about outcomes that were
measured subsequently (for similar argument, see Ellsworth &
Gonzalez, 2003;Kassam & Mendes, 2013;Spencer, Zanna, &
Fong, 2005).
Method
Participants. Participants were 97 undergraduates (66 fe-
males; M
age
!20.39 years, SD
age
!5.16; 51% White, 35% Asian
American, 8% African American, and 6% other) who received
course credit or $20 for participating in this two-session study.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
313
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
Eleven participants did not return for the second session, and five
participants declined to participate after learning that they would
have to give a speech (but before the manipulations were admin-
istered). Thus, data were not available from these participants for
analyses.
Procedure and materials.
Phase 1: Trait social anxiety. During a pretest session held
approximately 9 days before the experiment (M!8.56 days,
SD !5.73), participants completed the BFNE ("!.88; M!
36.64, SD !8.61) and SPIN ("!.90; M!19.46, SD !11.14).
Scores on these measures were standardized and collapsed to form
a trait social anxiety index ("!.74).
Phase 2: Baseline affect. During a laboratory session follow-
ing the pretest, participants provided informed consent and then
rated how they felt “right now” (1 !very negative,7!very
positive;M!5.20, SD !1.15).
Phase 3: Stress induction. See Study 3.
Phase 4: Experimental manipulation. See Study 3.
Phase 5: Self-report appraisals and anticipatory anxiety.
After the reflection period, participants completed a brief survey
that contained three questions. We measured threat by asking
participants, “How demanding do you expect the upcoming speech
task will be?” (1 !not very demanding,to7!extremely de-
manding;M!3.27, SD !0.92). We measured challenge by
asking participants, “How well do you think you will be able to
cope with the speech task?” (1 !not very well,5!extremely
well;M!3.35, SD !0.84). Finally, we measured anticipatory
anxiety by asking participants, “How stressed/anxious do you feel
about the upcoming speech task?” (1 !not very stressed/anxious,
7!extremely stressed/anxious;M!3.40, SD !1.00). Following
prior research (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996;Epel, Daubenmier,
Moskowitz, Folkman, & Blackburn, 2009), we computed a
challenge-to-threat ratio by dividing challenge scores by threat
scores (M!1.19, SD !0.77) to index appraisals. Higher scores
on this measure indicated that participants appraised the speech
task more as a challenge (vs. threat).
Phase 6: Stream of thought essays. Next, participants de-
scribed in writing the stream of thoughts that flowed through their
mind during the reflection period. Usinga0(not at all) to 3
(completely) scale, two raters blind to condition coded these essays
on the following dimensions.
Challenge and threat appraisals. We operationalized chal-
lenge as statements in which participants indicated that they could
cope with the upcoming stressor and/or statements in which par-
ticipants provided advice or encouragement to themselves to fa-
cilitate their performance (e.g., “I feel well experienced for my
dream job, and I know that if I can talk about it correctly . . .”). We
operationalized threat as statements in which participants indicated
feeling unprepared or overwhelmed by the demands of task (e.g.,
“I thought about my stress level and how well I would do in this
task. I fear I will be too nervous and not speak well”).
Cognitive avoidance. We operationalized avoidance as state-
ments indicating that participants thought about something other
than their upcoming speech task (e.g., “I started to think about
other things”).
Audience perspective taking. We operationalized audience
perspective taking as statements indicating that participants put
themselves in the shoes of the other people involved in the event
they were thinking about to surmise how they would be viewed by
them (e.g., “I was focused on saying what I think others would
want to hear”).
Interrater reliability was good for threat (ICC !.89; M!1.21,
SD !1.12), challenge (ICC !.77; M!1.78, SD !1.10),
cognitive avoidance (ICC !1.00; M!0.02, SD !0.13), and
audience perspective taking (ICC !.69; M!0.10, SD !0.33).
We divided coded challenge appraisals by coded threat appraisals
after adding a constant (“1”) to judges’ ratings of each essay so
that the scores could be divided. Higher scores reflected more
challenge compared with threat appraisals (M!1.81, SD !1.29).
The stream-of-thought essay measure was added to the protocol
shortly after the study began. Therefore, nine participants were
missing data on this measure. In addition, five participants dis-
closed their condition in their essays. Therefore, data from these
participants were not included in the analyses involving essay-
derived variables; there was no way of doing so without revealing
participants’ condition information to the coders.
Phase 7: Debriefing. Participants completed a funneled de-
briefing at the end of the study to assess whether they were aware
of the study aims and followed instructions. They were then
debriefed and compensated.
Results
Preliminary analyses. Two participants in the first-person
group and four participants in the non-first-person group were
excluded on a priori grounds—five indicated that they did not
believe they would have to deliver a speech and one indicated that
he did not follow the manipulation instructions—leaving 75 par-
ticipants, 37 in the first person group and 38 in the non-first-person
group. Exclusions did not differ by condition, #
2
(1) !0.65, p!
.42.
The groups did not differ on baseline affect, t(72) !0.82, p!
.425, trait social anxiety, t(73) !&0.11, p!.914, or gender,
#
2
(1) !0.01, p!.941. As in Studies 2 and 3, baseline affect and
trait social anxiety were included as covariates. Gender was related
to self-report challenge–threat appraisals such that women dis-
played more challenge–threat appraisals than men (see Table 3).
However, controlling for this variable did not influence the results.
Therefore, it is not discussed further. Degrees of freedom vary
slightly across analyses due to missing data.
Challenge versus threat. Condition significantly predicted
self-report challenge–threat appraisals, F(1, 70) !7.24, p!.009,
%p
2!.094. Non-first-person participants appraised the speech task
in more challenging and less threatening terms than first-person
participants (see Figure 4, Panel A). Baseline affect, F(1, 70) !
10.24, p!.002, %p
2!.128, and trait social anxiety, F(1, 70) !
7.69, p$.007, %p
2!.099, also predicted this variable—baseline
affect was positively related to this variable, pr !.36, p!.007,
whereas trait social anxiety negatively related to it, pr !–.32, p!
.007.
Condition also predicted coded challenge–threat appraisals, F(1,
54) !6.17, p!.016, %p
2!.103—non-first-person participants
appraised the task in more challenging and less threatening terms
than first-person participants (for examples, see Table 4; also see
Figure 4, Panel B). Neither baseline affect, F(1, 54) !0.78, p!
.382, %p
2!.014, nor trait social anxiety, F(1, 54) !1.47, p!.231,
%p
2!.026, predicted this variable.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
314 KROSS ET AL.
Cognitive avoidance and audience perspective taking. We
observed a floor effect on avoidance—1% of the essays contained
avoidance content. Thus, we did not examine this variable further.
Neither condition, F(1, 54) !0.07, p!.795, %p
2!.001, nor trait
social anxiety, F(1, 54) !0.07, p!.799, %p
2!.000, nor baseline
anxiety, F(1, 54) !0.03, p!.874, %p
2!.000, predicted audience
perspective taking.
Anticipatory anxiety. The effect of condition on anticipatory
anxiety was significant, F(1, 70) !4.78, p!.032, %p
2!.064.
Non-first-person participants (M!3.19, SE !0.15) reported
feeling less anxious about their upcoming speech than first-person
participants (M!3.67, SE !0.15). We also observed marginally
significant effects of trait social anxiety, F(1, 70) !3.43, p!
.068, %p
2!.047, and baseline affect, F(1, 70) !3.32, p!.073,
%p
2!.045. Higher scores on trait social anxiety positively pre-
dicted anticipatory anxiety, pr !.22, p!.068, whereas baseline
affect negatively predicted it, pr !–.21, p!.073.
Summary and Discussion
In Study 4, we examined whether the language people use to
reflect on their anxious feelings when preparing to give a public
speech alters the way they appraise that event. It generated four
key findings.
First, as predicted, participants who reflected on their feelings
using non-first-person language appraised their upcoming speech
in more challenging and less threatening terms than participants
who reflected on their feelings using first-person language. This
effect was observed on two types of appraisal measures, highlight-
ing the robustness of these results. These findings are noteworthy
because prior research indicates that challenge–threat appraisals
influence how people think, feel, and behave under social stress.
Specifically, challenge (compared with threat) appraisals predict
adaptive subjective, physiological, and behavioral responses (e.g.,
Blascovich, Seery, Mugridge, Norris, & Weisbuch, 2004;Dienst-
bier, 1989;Jamieson, Nock, & Mendes, 2012).
Second, in contrast to the Study 2 results but consistent with our
initial predictions, non-first-person participants displayed less an-
ticipatory anxiety than first-person participants. Thus, it is possible
that our failure to observe an effect on anticipatory anxiety in
Study 2 was due to chance. Given that we expected and found an
effect of condition on this variable in Study 4, we caution against
overinterpreting the null results in Study 2.
Figure 4. The effect of condition on the ratio of self-reported (Panel A) and coded (Panel B) challenge–threat
appraisals in Study 4. Error bars indicate standard errors.
Table 3
Study 4 Zero-Order Correlations Between Measured Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Trait social anxiety .14 .07 .20 &.26
!
&.18 .06 &.09
2. Gender &.12 .08 &.29
!
&.07 &.13 .08
3. Baseline affect &.17 .29
!
.00 .01 &.03
4. Anticipatory anxiety &.60
!!
&.42
!!
.12 &.05
5. Challenge–threat (self-report) .43
!!
&.15 &.04
6. Challenge–threat (essay) &.27 &.05
7. Audience perspective taking &.05
8. Avoidance
Note. Higher scores on baseline affect and global affect reflect lower levels of negative affect. Higher scores on challenge–threat reflect more challenge
relative to threat appraisals.
!
p!.05.
!!
p!.01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
315
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
Finally, virtually no one avoided focusing on their feelings. This
is consistent with research indicating that adopting a visual self-
distanced perspective to analyze one’s feelings does not promote
cognitive avoidance (Ayduk & Kross, 2010b;Kross & Ayduk,
2008,2009;Kross, Gard, Deldin, Clifton, & Ayduk, 2012). We
also failed to find a significant effect of language use on audience
perspective taking, which suggests that using non first person
language to reflect on one’s feelings does not differentially prime
people to think about how they appear in the eyes of others
compared to first person language use. These findings, in conjunc-
tion with the aforementioned challenge–threat appraisal results,
begin to demonstrate how non-first-person language use does and
does not influence the way people think about future anxiety-
provoking stressors.
Study 5
The primary goal in Study 5 was to conceptually replicate the
effect of language use on challenge–threat appraisals using a larger
sample from a different participant pool. We accomplished this
goal by recruiting participants using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk.
The second goal was to examine the generalizability of the
effect of language use on challenge–threat appraisals. Toward this
end, we asked participants to reflect on their feelings surrounding
any future anxiety-provoking event (not just a speech task).
Our final goal was to refine our understanding on how language
use influences challenge–threat appraisals by more directly assess-
ing the relationship between these variables. In Study 4,
challenge–threat appraisals were measured by asking participants
to self-report how they appraised their upcoming speech task and
then retrospectively report on the stream of thoughts that flowed
through their mind when they analyzed their feelings. Although
both of these are valid measures that are regularly used to interpret
how people appraise events, they are both one step removed from
the appraisal process itself. That is, they do not directly assess the
thoughts and feelings that become activated when people reflect on
events. To directly assess such thoughts, we implemented an
alternative approach in Study 5 that involved asking participants to
work through their current thoughts and feelings surrounding an
upcoming anxiety event in writing using first-person or non-first-
person language. By having people write about their thoughts and
feelings as they experienced them, we hoped to obtain a direct
window into how the language manipulations influence the way
people appraise upcoming social stressors.
6
Method
Participants. Participants were 153 individuals (72 female;
M
age
!34.64 years, SD
age
!12.38; 82.4% White, 7.8% Asian
American, 3.3% African American, and 6.5% other) who were
recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (Mturk). They were
compensated $0.50 for their participation.
Procedure and materials.
Cover story. We obtained informed consent at the beginning
of the experiment. Participants were recruited for a study on
“writing about emotions from a certain perspective.”
Baseline emotion. Participants rated how they felt “right now”
using a sliding scale (0 !very positive, 100 !very negative;M!
33.92, SD !20.50).
Trait social anxiety. Next, participants completed the BFNE
("!.94; M!32.65, SD !9.31).
Social anxiety reflection task. We then asked participants to
think about a current source of social anxiety. They read the
following instructions:
No matter how satisfied people are with their lives, there are times that
they worry and experience anxiety about things that may go wrong
when they interact with other people. Take a few moments right now
to think about a specific experience with another person or people that
you worry about happening to you from time to time. This could be
as minor as worrying about a friend not calling you back or more
serious like giving a speech in front of lots of people. As you do this,
try to identify a specific experience that makes you feel especially
anxious whenever you think about it. Although it may be difficult,
most people can usually come up with at least one potential social
event that they worry about. Take your time as you try to do this.
6
Additional measures were included for exploratory purposes in this
study. They are available upon request from the corresponding authors.
Table 4
Sample Threat and Challenge Appraisals as a Function of Condition in Study 4
Stream-of-thought essay samples
First-person condition:
I thought that I was so nervous because when I give a speech, I need to feel prepared; however, I don’t think I am prepared enough to give a
speech such as this one.
I can’t prepare an oral speech in 3 min. It takes days for me to examine my strengths, weaknesses, etc. I need to have my oral speech written down
and perfected, and therefore, this is not going to work out.
Nervousness. Shock. Not much time to prepare. What did I get myself into? Oh, my goodness. My palms are sweating. What are my weaknesses?
Think of really good strengths.
Non-first-person condition:
First, I asked myself what was I nervous about? It’s not like this will be the first interview or speech I’ve ever had to give. And even if it doesn’t
go perfectly, it won’t be the end of the world. I mostly think reassuring and comforting thoughts to motivate and encourage myself.
The topic of my speech, specific wording, the times that I have given a speech like this before. The fact that it’s not a “speech” and that word is
often associated with a scare tactic and panic inducer.
I told myself that I’m not under a lot of pressure for this. I’m qualified and have worked hard; I have confidence in my abilities.
Note. 73% of participants who received the highest possible score on the challenge-to-threat ratio variable were in the non-first-person group; 67% of
participants who received the lowest possible score on the challenge-to-threat ratio variable were in the first-person group.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
316 KROSS ET AL.
We categorized the type of socially anxious experiences partic-
ipants reflected on into the following categories: work-related
(12%; e.g., job interview; anxiety-provoking colleague interac-
tion), money-related (3%; e.g., borrowing money; being home-
less), health-related (4%; worrying about family member’s health),
performance-related (19%; e.g., class presentation; exam perfor-
mance), interpersonal-relationship-related (40%; e.g., interacting
with ex-partner; being excluded from a group), or other (22%; e.g.,
impending move).
Experimental manipulations. The experimental manipulation
was virtually identical to those in previous studies. The only
differences were that participants were asked to write (rather than
think) using first-person or non-first-person language to refer to
the self.
Appraisals. Two condition-blind judges rated each essay for
challenge (ICC !.78, M!0.46, SD !0.91), threat (ICC !.81,
M!1.78, SD !1.02), avoidance (ICC !.72, M!0.07, SD !
0.30), and audience perspective taking (ICC !.84, M!0.65,
SD !0.87) using the same criteria as in Study 4. To preserve
judges’ blindness to condition, all non-first-person participant es-
says were converted into first-person essays—that is, names and
non-first-person pronouns were replaced with Ior my, and the
appropriate verb tenses and articles were modified to ensure clarity
by a third experimenter who did not code the essays.
Following the procedure used in Study 4, we computed chal-
lenge (vs. threat) scores by dividing challenge by threat after
adding a constant (“1”) to each index (M!0.73, SD !0.85).
Higher scores reflected more challenge compared with threat ap-
praisals.
Debriefing. Participants were asked to “describe any other
activities that [they] were engaging in while [they] filled out the
survey” and then thanked.
Results
Preliminary analyses. In the current study, participants’ es-
says constituted the manipulation check—that is, by reading par-
ticipants essays, we were able to determine if they (a) used the type
of language they were instructed to use when writing about their
anxiety-provoking experience and (b) wrote about a current or
future anxiety-provoking experience. Twenty-nine participants
failed this check and were thus excluded on a priori grounds—17
wrote about a past (rather than current or future) anxiety-
provoking experience, and 12 did not write about an anxiety-
provoking experience using the type of language they were asked
to use. In addition, seven participants indicated that they were
doing something else while they participated (e.g., watched TV,
text messaging) and were thus also excluded on a priori grounds.
This left 117 participants, 62 in the first-person group and 55 in the
non-first-person group. Exclusions did not differ by condition (20
in the first-person and 16 in the non-first-person, #
2
(1) !0.07, p!
.79. The overall exclusion rate (24%) is consistent with the rate
observed in Study 1b, which also used Mturk.
The groups did not differ on baseline affect, t(115) !1.05, p!
.295; trait social anxiety, t(115) !0.21, p!.983; gender, #
2
(1) !
0.63, p!.426; or type of social anxiety provoking experience,
#
2
(6) !5.39, p!.495.
7
Controlling for gender and type of social
anxiety provoking experience did not substantively alter any of the
results we report. Therefore, these variables are not discussed
further. As in Studies 2–4, baseline affect and trait social anxiety
were included as covariates (for zero order correlations, see Table
5). Degrees of freedom vary across analyses due to missing data.
Challenge versus threat. Conceptually replicating the Study
5 findings, condition predicted challenge (vs. threat) appraisals,
F(1, 113) !14.27, p$.001, %p
2!.112—the writing samples of
non-first-person participants (M!1.01, SE !0.11) contained
more challenge (vs. threat) appraisals than the writing samples of
first-person participants (M!0.44, SE !0.10). Table 6 provides
examples of the appraisals participants wrote in each condition.
They demonstrate that asking people to use their own name and
other non-first-person pronouns to refer to the self transformed the
way participants thought about their experiences.
8
Alternative mechanisms: Avoidance and audience perspec-
tive taking. Similar to Study 4, we observed a floor effect on
avoidance—6% of the essays contained avoidance content. Thus,
we did not examine this variable further.
Neither condition, F(1, 113) !0.29, p!.592, %p
2!.003, nor
baseline affect, F(1, 113) !0.40, p!.531, %p
2!.003, predicted
audience perspective taking. Trait social anxiety did, however,
predict this variable, F(1, 113) !5.64, p!.019, %p
2!.048
higher scores on trait social anxiety predicted more audience
perspective taking, pr !.22, p!.019.
Summary and Discussion
These findings conceptually replicate the Study 4 results. They
also demonstrate that the effect of language use on challenge–
threat appraisals generalizes to thinking about a range of social-
anxiety-provoking experiences (not only a speech task) and to a
nonundergraduate sample.
Study 6
Although we included trait social anxiety as a covariate in
Studies 2–5, we did not examine whether this variable interacted
with condition in any of these studies because our statistical power
to observe significant interactions in each study was low, consid-
erably lower than our power to detect significant main effects
(Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004;Smith, 2000). To overcome this
limitation, we performed a meta-analysis in Study 6 on the data
from Studies 2–5 (for a similar approach, see Selcuk, Zayas,
Gunaydin, Hazan, & Kross, 2012). Specifically, we examined
whether trait social anxiety interacted with condition to predict the
following dependent measures, which were assessed in more than
one study: anticipatory anxiety (Studies 2–4), postmanipulation
affect (Studies 2–3), challenge–threat (Studies 4–5), behavior
(Studies 2 and 3), and audience perspective taking (Studies 4 and
7
Because there was overlap between the work- and performance-related
categories, we also collapsed across them when performing covariate
analyses. Doing so did not influence any results.
8
After the manipulation, participants were asked to rate how difficult it
was for them to implement the instructions. Non-first-person participants
reported experiencing marginally more difficulty than first-person partic-
ipants, F(1, 113) !3.48, p!.07, %p
2!.030. The effect of condition on
challenge–threat appraisals remained highly significant when we con-
trolled for this variable, F(1, 112) !12.13, p!.001, %p
2!.098. Thus, the
subjective difficulty of the manipulation (or dysfluency) does not account
for the effect of language use on appraisals.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
317
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
5). We did not examine the moderating role of trait social anxiety
on avoidance because we observed a floor effect on this variable in
both of the studies that assessed it.
Prior research indicates that vulnerable individuals (i.e., those
who score high on individual difference measures of depression or
bipolar disorder) benefit as much, or more, from reflecting on
emotional experiences from a visual self-distanced perspective as
nonvulnerable individuals (Gruber et al., 2009;Kross & Ayduk,
2009;Kross et al., 2012;Wisco & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011). Ex-
trapolating from these findings, we expected people who scored
high on trait social anxiety to benefit as much or more from
engaging in non-first-person self-talk as people who scored low on
trait social anxiety on each of the dependent measures we exam-
ined.
Method
Data analytic strategy. To obtain effect sizes for the interac-
tion between condition and trait social anxiety, we first computed
the condition by trait social anxiety interaction term in each study.
Next, we computed the correlation between this interaction term
and the dependent variable of interest in each study, controlling for
baseline affect, condition, and trait social anxiety. We then trans-
formed effect sizes to zscores using Fisher’s r-to-ztransformation.
Finally, the transformed effect sizes (i.e., zscores) were weighted
by the sample size and averaged across studies (see Table 7). The
significance value for this average weighted effect size was com-
puted by dividing it by the standard error. This yielded a Zobt test
statistic with a corresponding pvalue (for similar approach, see
Zayas & Shoda, 2005).
Results and Discussion
Descriptive statistics. The translational relevance of the re-
sults of this study depends largely on whether participants in our
samples represent the full spectrum of social anxiety scores. Ide-
ally, one would want to have a sufficient number of individuals
who scored low and high on trait social anxiety to draw inferences
about the generalizability of this work. Therefore, we first exam-
ined whether trait social anxiety scores in each study were nor-
mally distributed and covered the range of possible scores on the
trait social anxiety scales we administered. As Table 8 demon-
strates, both of these criteria were met.
To further address this issue, we examined whether any partic-
ipants in our samples displayed trait social anxiety scores that
resembled those scores displayed by people diagnosed with social
phobia. Carleton, Collimore, McCabe, and Antony (2011) sug-
gested that a score of 25 or above on the BFNES (a shortened
version of the full BFNE scale that we administered in our studies)
reliably distinguishes individuals diagnosed with social anxiety
disorder from individuals diagnosed with other Axis 1 disorders.
Therefore, we used 25 as a cutoff for determining whether the
scores of participants in our studies on this measure resemble those
Table 5
Study 5 Zero-Order Correlations Between Measured Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Trait social anxiety .04 .28
!!!
&.14 .21
!
&.03
2. Gender .16
&.11 .05 &.03
3. Baseline affect &.03 .00 &.02
4. Challenge–threat &.04 &.11
5. Audience perspective taking .12
6. Avoidance
Note. Higher scores on baseline affect reflect lower levels of negative
affect. Higher scores on challenge-threat reflect more challenge relative to
threat appraisals.
p!.10.
!
p!.05.
!!!
p!.005.
Table 6
Sample Threat and Challenge Appraisals as a Function of Condition in Study 5
Stream-of-thought essay samples
First-person condition:
I worry about giving a presentation to a customer at work. I am afraid that I will come across as unprofessional or not knowledgeable. I am nervous
that they will ask questions that I will not know the answers for. I think I have these feelings because I have had similar experiences in the past
where I got nervous because I didn’t have an answer.
I am afraid that I won’t get a job if I mess up during an interview. And I always mess up in some way. I never know what to say, and I am always
incredibly nervous. I end up in a feedback loop of nervousness causing bad interviews causing nervousness. Even if I got a job, I think I would
still be afraid of interviews.
A large worry that I have fairly often is regarding public speaking. More specifically, I am in college, so I have many classes that include a number
of students. Often times upwards of 300 students. A worry that I get anxious over is that one day the professor will call on me to answer a
question or further explain a class concept, and I will freeze up and be unable to respond. I have always had a fear of public speaking; it most
likely branches off from my introverted personality. My personality has always been about keeping to myself, and usually staying quiet.
Non-first-person condition:
[Participant’s name], you need to slow down. It’s a date; everyone gets nervous. Oh jeez, why did you say that? You need to pull it back. Come on
man, pull it together. You can do this.
You worry too much about what other people think. You need to focus on what needs to be done, and what you can do to execute it. The simple
fact that other people will be around does [not] change what you need to do. Focus on you, and you will be fine.
You can do this, [participant’s name]! You can arrange this retirement party for [XXX], and get a good turnout even though he is not well liked.
You can come up with a speech. You can get someone else to speak well of him. You can keep the cost of this party within budget.
[Participant’s name], you can do this! You can get people to pay for the meal and drinks, and you can get them to contribute to a gift. You will
make this retirement party a good memory for [XXX].
Note. 75% of participants who received the highest possible score on the challenge-threat variable were in the non-first-person group; 67% of participants
who received the lowest possible score on the challenge-threat variable were in the first-person group. [Participant’s name] and [XXX] are place holders
used to protect confidentiality.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
318 KROSS ET AL.
scores displayed by people with clinically diagnosed social anxi-
ety. For the SPIN, Connor et al. (2000) noted that individuals
diagnosed with social phobia who were judged by a clinician to be
moderately ill to very severely ill scored between 33 and 52.
Table 9 presents the percentage of participants whose trait social
anxiety scores were in the range displayed by people who receive
clinical diagnoses of social phobia. Overall, 10% of our sample
scored in this range. This percentage is remarkably consistent with
the 12% lifetime prevalence of social phobia in the United States
(Kessler et al., 2005), which further suggests that our sample was
representative of the larger population in the prevalence of social
anxiety and included participants characterized by the full spec-
trum of social anxiety symptoms.
9
Meta-analysis. Trait social anxiety did not moderate the ef-
fect of language use on anticipatory anxiety, Zobt !&1.21, p!
.226; challenge–threat, Zobt !&1.23, p!.218; audience per-
spective taking, Zobt !0.22, p!.828; performance under stress,
Zobt !&0.32, p!.751; or postperformance affect, Zobt !0.51,
p!.610. Thus, individuals who scored high versus low on trait
social anxiety benefited similarly from introspecting using non-
first-person language. These findings begin to speak to the poten-
tial generalizability of language use as a means of helping vulner-
able individuals cope with social anxiety.
General Discussion
Immediately after James’ infamous interview, Internet message
boards were abuzz with people questioning his sanity. Although
such reactions are understandable—we are not accustomed to
people referring to themselves using their own name—the current
findings suggest that doing so promotes self-distancing (Studies 1a
and 1b); enhances people’s ability to regulate their thoughts,
feelings, and behavior under social stress (Studies 2 and 3); and
leads them to appraise social-anxiety-provoking events in more
challenging and less threatening terms (Studies 4 and 5). They also
demonstrate that the self-regulatory effects of this process extend
to people regardless of their dispositional vulnerability to social
anxiety (Study 6).
Basic Science Implications
In addition to highlighting the causal implications of language
use during introspection for self-regulation, these findings extend
research on self-distancing in three ways. First, no previous re-
search has examined how people can self-distance to regulate
anxiety surrounding impending social stressors or examined
whether individual differences in social anxiety moderate this
process. The current findings address these issues directly. They
suggest that self-distancing manipulations may be useful in help-
ing people cope not only with depression and anger related to
ruminating over the past but also social anxiety surrounding the
future.
Second, the current research demonstrates that self-distancing
influences people’s tendency to appraise upcoming social stressors
in more challenging and less threatening terms. In so doing, this
work integrates research on self-distancing with a large literature
on coping and appraisals.
9
In Study 4, 16 participants did not return for the speech task following
the baseline session. The trait social anxiety scores for these participants
were marginally higher than the scores for participants who did return, t!
1.77, p!.08. However, the trait social anxiety scores of only three of these
participants met the clinical cutoff for social anxiety.
Table 8
Trait Social Anxiety Score Distribution Statistics
Study
BFNE scale range
(12–60) SPIN scale range (068)
Skew Score range Skew Score range
2&.08 17–56
3 .25 19–56 .47 3–45
4 .58 20–59 .83 2–47
5 .51 16–56
Note. Due to a protocol error, 15 people did not complete the Social
Phobia Inventory (SPIN) in Study 2. Thus, we did not include it in our
analyses. The SPIN was not administered in Study 5. BFNE !Brief Fear
of Negative Evaluation Scale (shortened version).
Table 7
Effect Sizes (R) Representing the Associations Between Condition (Trait Social Anxiety and the
Key Dependent Variables Assessed In Studies 2–5
Study
Condition (Trait Social Anxiety
Challenge–threat Perspective
taking Anticipatory
anxiety Postmanipulation
affect Behavior
2—&.09 .01 &.09
3 — — .07 .04
4&.05 &.11 &.11 — —
5&.12 .08 —
Mean weighted z&.09 .02 &.10 .04 &.03
Note. Dashes indicate that the measure was not assessed. In Study 3, we collapsed shame and global affect (r!
.69, p$.001) to create a single postmanipulation affect variable. In Study 4, we collapsed self-reported and
content-analyzed challenge–threat scores (r!.43, p$.001) to create a single variable. In cases where
participants had scores on one challenge–threat measure but not the other, we used the score they had as their
value.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
319
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
Third, prior research has relied exclusively on visual imagery
techniques to examine the role self-distancing plays in self-control.
The current findings extend this work by demonstrating that lan-
guage can be harnessed to promote self-distancing as well. Al-
though these different self-distancing tactics are clearly related
(see Studies 1a and 1b), it would be a mistake (in our view) to
conceptualize them as equivalent for two reasons. First, although
linguistic self-distancing significantly influenced visual self-
distancing in Studies 1a and 1b, it explained only 8% (on average)
of the variance in participants’ visual self-distancing scores. If
these tactics were identical, then one would have expected the
percentage of variance explained to be much higher. Second, it is
possible that one self-distancing tactic might work better than
another in certain contexts. For example, in circumstances where
people have the time and luxury to close their eyes and reflect on
a painful past experience, the visual self-distancing tactic might be
most useful. However, this tactic may be difficult to implement
“online” when people are in the midst of performing a task or
interacting with other people. In such situations, linguistic self-talk
techniques may be easier to implement. A key challenge for future
research is to examine when and why different self-distancing
tactics are maximally effective.
Another important issue to address in future work are the causal
pathways that underlie the effects of non-first-person self-talk on
the different outcomes we assessed in these studies—anticipatory
anxiety, challenge–threat appraisals, performance under stress,
postperformance affect, and postevent processing. On the one
hand, our findings regarding challenge-threat appraisals provide
initial clues about the processes that may explain some of the
benefits of non-first-person self-talk on self-regulation. Specifi-
cally, prior research indicates that experimentally enhancing
challenge-threat appraisals enhances performance under stress and
reduces post performance distress and perseveration (e.g., Blasco-
vich et al., 2004;Dienstbier, 1989;Jamieson et al., 2012). For
example, Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, and Schmader (2010)
found that cueing people to appraise their anxiety surrounding
having to take the Graduate Record Examination (GRE) in chal-
lenging terms led them to perform better on the exam, worry less
about how anxious they were after the exam, and display healthier
patterns of sympathetic nervous system activation. Thus, it is
possible that non-first-person self-talk impacted performance un-
der stress and postmanipulation affect and perseveration via its
effects on appraisals. Future research is needed, however, to con-
firm these inferences and rule out alternative causal pathways.
On the other hand, the causal relationships between some of the
other variables we assessed in these studies are less clear. The
design we employed did not allow us to examine whether antici-
patory affect mediates the effect of condition on appraisals, or vice
versa, or whether the relationship between these variables is bidi-
rectional. Addressing this question in the future is important for
refining our understanding of how self-talk impacts self-regulation
and will require designs that continuously or semicontinuously
measure affect and appraisals over time to allow for reciprocal and
recursive relationships to emerge.
It is also possible—and we suspect likely—that non-first-person
language use enhances self-control through additional mechanisms
that we did not assess (for a discussion of the importance of
recognizing the potential complexity of identifying causal path-
ways in psychological research, see Bullock, Green, & Ha, 2010).
For example, it may influence people’s autonomic reactions to
stress and how they strategically deploy their attention before,
during, and after stressful tasks. Future research is needed to
examine whether these and other theoretically relevant variables
play an additional role in explaining how non-first-person lan-
guage use influences self-regulation.
Another question raised by these findings concerns how they
speak to William James’s (1890) suggestion that referring to the
self using the word me leads people to think about the self as an
object of attention (also see Libby & Eibach, 2011). From our
perspective, language use should promote self-distancing when
people use parts of speech to refer to the self that they typically use
to refer to other people—i.e., second- and third-person pronouns
and one’s own name. Because me is virtually always used to refer
to the self in daily discourse—it is in fact a first-person pro-
noun—we do not expect people’s use of this pronoun to promote
self-distancing. In part, this is due to the fact that me and Iare often
used in conjunction with each other to refer to the self. To
demonstrate this point, we analyzed participants’ essays in Study
5 for the presence of me using the Linguistic Inquiry Word Count.
Note that participants were not explicitly asked to think about
themselves using the pronoun me (they were told to use first-
person pronouns and given Ias an example). Nevertheless, the
effect of condition on me was significant, F(1, 134) !55.07, p$
.001, %p
2!.29, indicating that first-person pronoun participants
used me more frequently than non-first-person participants. The
use of Iand me was also highly correlated, r!.57, p$.001.
These findings highlight how tightly interconnected the use of
first-person pronouns are in natural discourse, and how likely they
are to lead to similar types of mental representations of the self.
Links to Construal Level Theory
Psychological distance has been the focus of research in numer-
ous areas of psychology over the past half-century. However,
within the past decade, construal-level theory (CLT; Trope &
Liberman, 2003,2010) has emerged as the dominant approach to
studying this construct, which raises the following question: How
does the concept of self-distance, as discussed throughout this
article, compare with the concept of psychological distance as
discussed in CLT?
In our view, there is strong conceptual overlap between these
concepts. Both are operationalized in terms of allowing people to
transcend egocentric viewpoints (e.g., Kross et al., 2012;Trope &
Liberman, 2010), both lead people to generate less concrete and
more abstract mental representations (for reviews, see Ayduk &
Kross, 2010a;Kross & Ayduk, 2011;Trope & Liberman, 2010),
Table 9
Trait Social Anxiety Score Distributions Within Clinical Range
Study BFNE scores "25 SPIN scores "33
2 9%
3 4% 12%
4 7% 17%
5 10%
Note. Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (BFNES) refers to a
shortened version of the measure. SPIN !Social Phobia Inventory.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
320 KROSS ET AL.
and both have been shown to promote self-regulation (e.g., Fujita
et al., 2006;Kross & Ayduk, 2011).
These similarities notwithstanding, whereas people can adopt a
psychologically distanced perspective when focusing on virtually
any type of stimulus (e.g., the self, another person, another object),
the concept of self-distance refers to instances in which people
specifically focus on the self from a distanced perspective. Thus,
one difference surrounding these concepts and the literatures that
use them concerns specificity: whereas research on self-distancing
focuses exclusively on self-relevant phenomena, research on psy-
chological distance has a broader focus.
Another point to consider when comparing these constructs
concerns the assumption in CLT that four types of psychological
distance exist: temporal, spatial, social, and hypothetical (Trope &
Liberman, 2010). Because CLT research has not, to our knowl-
edge, examined self-distance in the way that we manipulate this
process, it is not clear whether it represents an additional distanc-
ing dimension, or whether it can be captured by one of the
aforementioned categories. On the one hand, one could argue that
self-distance is a form of “social distance,” which maps onto a
“self-versus-other” distinction (see Liberman, Trope, & Stephan,
2007;Trope & Liberman, 2003). When considering this view,
however, it is important to recognize that asking people to think
about the self from a distanced perspective is very likely to be
psychologically different from reflecting on someone else entirely.
In the former situation, a person has privileged access to his or her
inner thoughts and feelings. In the latter situation, they do not.
Therefore, it is possible that self-distance and social distance
represent different dimensions of psychological distance.
10
Future
research is needed to address this issue and is important for
integrating research on CLT and self-distance.
Implications for Socially Anxious Individuals
These findings have multiple implications for research and
theory on social anxiety, as well as coping more generally. With
respect to social anxiety, they highlight the effect that shifting the
language people use to refer to the self under stress has on the
cascade of events during and after a social interaction. In this vein,
the fact that we observed beneficial effects of non-first-person
language use on postevent processing—a cognitive process that
has been linked with maladaptive psychological and physical
health outcomes in both healthy and clinical populations (e.g.,
Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008)—is particularly noteworthy. It sug-
gests that the current findings may have broad implications for
self-control research and practice in multiple populations.
An interesting question for future research concerns whether
stable individual differences characterize the spontaneous use of
non-first-person language to refer to the self during introspection.
The Lebron James interview provides anecdotal evidence suggest-
ing that some people spontaneously think about the self using
non-first-person pronouns and their own name under stressful
circumstances. Consistent with this anecdote, a recent study by
Zell, Warriner, and Albarracin (2012) demonstrated that people are
more likely to use second-person pronouns (compared with first-
person pronouns) when thinking about situations that involve
self-control than those that do not. Whether the spontaneous use of
non-first-person pronouns during self-talk translates into enhanced
self-regulation, however, remains unclear, as does whether certain
populations of individuals (i.e., healthy vs. clinical) differentially
engage in this process.
When considering the broader implications of these findings, it
is noteworthy that our manipulation was quite simple—partici-
pants were told to use their own name and other non-first-person
pronouns when reflecting over their feelings. That such a small
shift in the way people reflected on the self led to such consistent
results suggests that taking the next steps to examine how these
strategies operate in daily life and generalize to clinical popula-
tions may be worthwhile.
Self-Distancing and Its Motivational Context
Finally, it is important to recognize that we studied self-
distancing in a particular context in this study—one in which
participants were asked to self-distance and then analyze their
feelings. We focused on the joint operation of self-distancing and
“asking why” because previous research has consistently shown
that self-distancing in the service of fulfilling such epistemic needs
leads to beneficial outcomes (Kross & Ayduk, 2011; also see
Wilson & Gilbert, 2008). However, it is also possible for people to
self-distance to achieve other goals. For example, a person could
self-distance to observe his or her feelings, an approach that
mindfulness and acceptance-based therapies advocate (Bishop et
al., 2004;Fresco et al., 2007;Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002).
They could also self-distance to avoid their emotions, a process
that many researchers would characterize as harmful (Kenny &
Bryant, 2007;Kenny et al., 2009). These examples suggest that
whether self-distancing is helpful or harmful may depend critically
on how people focus on the self and their emotions once they
engage in this process.
This is especially true for social anxiety because some research
indicates that when socially anxious individuals imagine future
stressful social interactions, they spontaneously put themselves in
the shoes of an evaluative audience, suggesting that self-distancing
is harmful for people with social anxiety (e.g., Schultz & Heim-
berg, 2008). However, the current research clearly demonstrates
that self-distancing, when used to understand one’s emotions, is
quite distinct from focusing on the self from the perspective of an
evaluative audience (i.e., what socially anxious people spontane-
ously do) and has beneficial downstream consequences for cogni-
tion, affect, and behavior. Nevertheless, more research is needed to
replicate these effects in both undergraduate and nonundergraduate
samples characterized by clinical social anxiety.
Concluding Comment
Self-talk is a ubiquitous human phenomenon. We all have an
internal monologue that we engage in from time to time. The
current research demonstrates that small shifts in the language
people use to refer to the self as they engage in this process
consequentially influences their ability to regulate their thoughts,
10
This is not to say that thinking about the self from a distance bears no
similarity to thinking about other people (i.e., social distance). Prior re-
search indicates, for example, that psychological distance diminishes peo-
ple’s reliance on introspective information (e.g., Pronin & Ross, 2006).
That said, individuals still have privileged access to introspective informa-
tion when they reflect on the self from a distance in a way that they do not
when they reflect on another person.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
321
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
feelings, and behavior under social stress, even for people who are
dispositionally vulnerable to social anxiety.
References
Ayduk, Ö., & Kross, E. (2008). Enhancing the pace of recovery: Self-
distanced analysis of negative experiences reduces blood pressure reac-
tivity. Psychological Science, 19, 229–231. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280
.2008.02073.x
Ayduk, Ö., & Kross, E. (2010a). Analyzing negative experiences without
ruminating: The role of self-distancing in enabling adaptive self-
reflection. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 841–854.
doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00301.x
Ayduk, Ö., & Kross, E. (2010b). From a distance: Implications of spon-
taneous self-distancing for adaptive self-reflection. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 98, 809829. doi:10.1037/a0019205
Beazley, M. B., Glass, C. R., Chambless, D. L., & Arnkoff, D. B. (2001).
Cognitive self-statements in social phobia: A comparison across three
types of social situations. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 781–
799. doi:10.1023/A:1012927608525
Beck, A. T. (1970). Cognitive therapy: Nature and relation to behavior
therapy. Behavior Therapy, 1, 184–200. doi:10.1016/S0005-
7894(70)80030-2
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Car-
mody, J.,...Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational
definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230–241.
doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph077
Blascovich, J., Seery, M. D., Mugridge, C. A., Norris, R. K., & Weisbuch,
M. (2004). Predicting athletic performance from cardiovascular indexes
of challenge and threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40,
683–688. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.007
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal
regulation. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-
ogy (Vol. 28, pp. 1–51). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative
cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related phys-
iological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60,
113–124. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074
Brozovich, F., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). An analysis of post-event
processing in social anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28,
891–903. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.01.002
Bullock, J. G., Green, D. P., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Yes, but what’s the
mechanism? (Don’t expect an easy answer). Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 98, 550–558. doi:10.1037/a0018933
Burgio, K. L., Merluzzi, T. V., & Pryor, J. B. (1986). Effects of perfor-
mance expectancy and self-focused attention on social interaction. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1216–1221. doi:10.1037/
0022-3514.50.6.1216
Carleton, R. N., Collimore, K. C., McCabe, R. E., & Antony, M. M.
(2011). Addressing revisions to the brief fear of negative evaluation
scale: Measuring fear of negative evaluation across anxiety and mood
disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 822–828. doi:10.1016/j
.janxdis.2011.04.002
Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., McManus, F., Hackman, A., Fennell, M., Camp-
bell, H.,...Louis, B. (2003). Cognitive therapy versus fluoxetine in
generalized social phobia: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Jour-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 1058–1067. doi:10.1037/
0022-006X.71.6.1058
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In
R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.),
Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 69–93). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Clark, J. V., & Arkowitz, H. (1975). Social anxiety and the self-evaluation
of interpersonal performance. Psychological Reports, 36, 211–221. doi:
10.2466/pr0.1975.36.1.211
Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic markers
of psychological change surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychologi-
cal Science, 15, 687–693. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00741.x
Connor, K. M., Davidson, J. R. T., Churchill, L. E., Sherwood, A., Foa, E.,
& Weisler, R. H. (2000). Psychometric properties of the Social Phobia
Inventory (SPIN): New self-rating scale. British Journal of Psychiatry,
176, 379–386. doi:10.1192/bjp.176.4.379
Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). When the
social self is threatened: Shame, physiology, and health. Journal of
Personality, 72, 1191–1216. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00295.x
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol
responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research.
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355–391. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.3
.355
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implica-
tions for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84–100.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.1.84
Ellsworth, P. C., & Gonzalez, R. (2003). Questions and comparisons:
Methods of research in social psychology. In M. A. Hogg & J. Cooper
(Eds.), The Sage handbook of social psychology (pp. 2442). London,
England: Sage.
Epel, E., Daubenmier, J., Moskowitz, J. T., Folkman, S., & Blackburn, E.
(2009). Can meditation slow rate of cellular aging? Cognitive stress,
mindfulness, and telomeres. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences,
1172, 34–53. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04414.x
Eyal, T., & Epley, N. (2010). How to seem telepathic: Enabling mind
reading by matching self-construal. Psychological Science, 21, 700
705. doi:10.1177/0956797610367754
Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator and
mediator effects in counseling psychology research. Journal of Coun-
seling Psychology, 51, 115–134. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.51.1.115
Fresco, D. M., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., & Kennedy, S. (2007). Relationship
of posttreatment decentering and cognitive reactivity to relapse in major
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 447–
455. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.75.3.447
Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal
levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
90, 351–367. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351
Fydrich, T., Chambless, D. L., Perry, K. J., Buergener, F., & Beazley,
M. B. (1998). Behavioral assessment of social performance: A rating
system for social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 995–
1010. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00069-2
Galinsky, A. D., Maddux, W. W., Gilin, D., & White, J. B. (2008). Why
it pays to get inside the head of your opponent: The differential effects
of perspective taking and empathy in negotiations. Psychological Sci-
ence, 19, 378–384.
Gerin, W., Davidson, K. W., Christenfeld, N. J. S., Goyal, T., & Schwartz,
J. E. (2006). The role of angry rumination and distraction in blood
pressure recovery from emotional arousal. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68,
64–72. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000195747.12404.aa
Glass, C. R., Merluzzi, T. V., Biever, J. I., & Larsen, K. H. (1982).
Cognitive assessment of social anxiety: Development and validation of
a self-statement questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6,
37–55. doi:10.1007/BF01185725
Greenberg, J. (2010). Exiting via the low road. Retrieved from http://sports
.espn.go.com/chicago/nba/columns/story?id!5365985
Grossmann, I., & Kross, E. (2010). The impact of culture on adaptive
versus maladaptive self-reflection. Psychological Science, 21, 1150
1157. doi:10.1177/0956797610376655
Gruber, J. Harvey, A. G., & Johnson, S. L. (2009). Reflective and rumi-
native processing of positive emotional memories in bipolar disorder and
healthy controls. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 697–704. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.005
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
322 KROSS ET AL.
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006).
Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.06
.006
Ingram, R. E., & Hollon, S. D. (1986). Cognitive therapy for depression
from an information processing perspective. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.),
Information processing approaches to clinical psychology (pp. 259
281). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Holt.
doi:10.1037/11059-000
Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Mind over matter:
Reappraising arousal improves cardiovascular and cognitive responses
to stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 417–422.
doi:10.1037/a0025719
Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, T. (2010).
Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal
improves performance on the GRE. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 46, 208–212. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.015
Kashdan, T. B., & Roberts, J. E. (2006). Affective outcomes and cognitive
processes in superficial and intimate interactions: Roles of social anxiety
and curiosity. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 140–167. doi:
10.1016/j.jrp.2004.10.005
Kassam, K. S., & Mendes, W. B. (2013). The effects of measuring
emotion: Physiological reactions to emotion situations depend on
whether someone is asking. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e64959. doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0064959
Kenny, L. M., & Bryant, R. A. (2007). Keeping memories at an arm’s
length: Vantage point of trauma memories. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 45, 1915–1920. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.004
Kenny, L. M., Bryant, R. A., Silove, D., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., &
McFarlane, A. C. (2009). Distant memories: A prospective study of
vantage point of trauma memories. Psychological Science, 20, 1049
1052. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02393.x
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., & Walters, E. E. (2005).
Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM–IV disorders
in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 62, 593–602. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M.,
Eshleman, S.,...Kendler, K. S. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month
prevalence of DSM-III–R psychiatric disorders in the United States:
Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 51, 8–19. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier
Social Stress Test”: A tool for investigating psychobiological stress
responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 7681. doi:
10.1159/000119004
Kober, H., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Kross, E., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., &
Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive
regulation of craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
of the USA, 107, 14811–14816. doi:10.1073/pnas.1007779107
Kross, E. (2009). When the self becomes other. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 1167, 35–40. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009
.04545.x
Kross, E., & Ayduk, Ö. (2008). Facilitating adaptive emotional analysis:
Distinguishing distanced-analysis of depressive experiences from
immersed-analysis and distraction. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 34, 924–938. doi:10.1177/0146167208315938
Kross, E., & Ayduk, Ö. (2009). Boundary conditions and buffering effects:
Does depressive symptomology moderate the effectiveness of distanced-
analysis for facilitating adaptive self-reflection? Journal of Research in
Personality, 43, 923–927. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.004
Kross, E., & Ayduk, Ö. (2011). Making meaning out of negative experi-
ences by self-distancing. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
20, 187–191. doi:10.1177/0963721411408883
Kross, E., Ayduk, Ö., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking “why” does not
hurt: Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative
emotions. Psychological Science, 16, 709–715. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2005.01600.x
Kross, E., Duckworth, A., Ayduk, Ö., Tsukayama, E., & Mischel, W.
(2011). The effect of self-distancing on adaptive versus maladaptive
self-reflection in children. Emotion, 11, 1032–1039. doi:10.1037/
a0021787
Kross, E., Gard, D., Deldin, P., Clifton, J., & Ayduk, Ö. (2012). “Asking
why” from a distance: Its cognitive and emotional consequences for
people with major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
ogy, 121, 559–569. doi:10.1037/a0028808
Kuyken, W., & Moulds, M. L. (2009). Remembering as an observer: How
is autobiographical memory retrieval vantage perspective linked to de-
pression? Memory, 17, 624634. doi:10.1080/09658210902984526
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New
York, NY: Springer.
Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation
Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371–375. doi:
10.1177/0146167283093007
Libby, L. K., & Eibach, R. P. (2011). Visual perspective in mental imager:
A representational tool that functions in judgment, emotion, and self-
insight. In M. P. Zanna & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 185–245). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Stephan, E. (2007). Psychological distance. In
E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: A hand-
book of basic principles (pp. 353–381). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Marschall, D., Sanftner, J., & Tangney, J. P. (1994). The State Shame and
Guilt Scale. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University.
Mcisaac, H. K., & Eich, E. (2002). Vantage point in episodic memory.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 146–150. doi:10.3758/BF03196271
Meleshko, K. G., & Alden, L. E. (1993). Anxiety and self-disclosure:
Toward a motivational model. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 64, 1000–1009. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.1000
Mellings, T. M. B., & Alden, L. E. (2000). Cognitive processes in social
anxiety: The effects of self-focus, rumination and anticipatory process-
ing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 243–257. doi:10.1016/
S0005-7967(99)00040-6
Mischel, W., & Rodriguez, M. (1993). Psychological distance in self-
imposed delay of gratification. In R. R. Cocking & K. A. Renninger
(Eds.), The development and meaning of psychological distance (pp.
109–121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mischowski, D., Kross, E., & Bushman, B. (2012). Flies on the wall are
less aggressive: The effect of self-distancing on aggressive affect, cog-
nition, and behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48,
1187–1191. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.012
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking
rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400424. doi:
10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
Oppenheimer, D. M., Meyvis, T., & Davidenko, N. (2009). Instructional
manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 867–872. doi:10.1016/
j.jesp.2009.03.009
Park, J., Ayduk, O., O’Donnell, L. O., Chun, J., Gruber, J., Kamali, M.,...
Kross, E. (2013). Cognitive and neural processes underlying positive
emotion regulation in bipolar I disorder. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic
Inquiry and Word Count [Computer software]. Austin, TX: LIWC Inc.
Pennebaker, J. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Linguistic styles: Language use
as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 77, 1296–1312. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1296
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
323
SELF-TALK, REGULATION, AND SOCIAL ANXIETY
Pronin, E., & Ross, L. (2006). Temporal differences in trait self-ascription:
When the self is seen as an other. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 90, 197–209. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.197
Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive–behavioral model of
anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 741–756.
doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00022-3
Ray, R. D., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the mind’s eye?
Anger rumination and reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 94, 133–145. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.133
Schultz, L. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). Attentional focus in social
anxiety disorder: Potential for interactive processes. Clinical Psychology
Review, 28, 1206–1221. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.04.003
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-
based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing
relapse. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Selcuk, E., Zayas, V., Gunaydin, G., Hazan, C., & Kross, E. (2012). Mental
representations of attachment figures facilitate emotional recovery fol-
lowing upsetting autobiographical memory recall. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 103, 362–378. doi:10.1037/a0028125
Sigel, I., & McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V. (2003). Rod Cocking’s legacy:
The development of psychological distancing. Journal of Applied De-
velopmental Psychology, 24, 697–711. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2003.09
.004
Smith, E. R. (2000). Research design. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.),
Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp.
17–39). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal
chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational
analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 89, 845–851. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845
Spurr, J. M., & Stopa, L. (2002). Self-focused attention in social phobia
and social anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 947–975. doi:
10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00107-1
Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., &
Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse
in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 70, 275–287. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.2.275
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological
Review, 110, 403–421. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological
distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440463. doi:10.1037/a0018963
Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Larkin, K. T. (1986). Situational determi-
nants of social anxiety in clinic and nonclinic samples: Physiological and
cognitive correlates. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
523–527. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.54.4.523
Weinstock, L. S. (1999). Gender differences in the presentation and man-
agement of social anxiety disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60,
9–13.
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of
affective adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 370
386. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00085.x
Wisco, B. E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Effect of visual perspective
on memory and interpretation in dysphoria. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 49, 406412. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.03.012
Zayas, V., & Shoda, Y. (2005). Do automatic reactions elicited by thoughts
of romantic partner, mother, and self related to adult romantic attach-
ment? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1011–1025.
doi:10.1177/0146167204274100
Zell, E., Warriner, A. B., & Albarracin, D. (2012). Splitting of the mind:
When the you I talk to is me and needs command. Social Psychological
and Personality Science, 3, 549–555. doi:10.1177/1948550611430164
Zoccola, P. M., Dickerson, S. S., & Zaldivar, F. P. (2008). Rumination and
cortisol responses to laboratory stressors. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70,
661–667. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817bbc77
Received November 17, 2012
Revision received October 1, 2013
Accepted October 21, 2013 !
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
324 KROSS ET AL.
... Electronic copy available at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=5007851 P r e p r i n t n o t p e e r r e v i e w e d compassion and Present Orientation -also wanted to keep in mind a selfdecentering/decentering (Bernstein et al., 2015;Kross et al., 2014) and self-compassion (Neff et al., 2021) as potentially relevant dimensions. ...
... Decentering, or self-decentering, involves viewing one's experiences from a detached perspective, which can reduce the intensity of negative emotions and enhance emotional regulation. Studies by Kross et al. (2014) and Bernstein et al. (2015) have demonstrated that self-decentering techniques can decrease emotional reactivity and improve problem-solving abilities. In educational settings, the ability to distance oneself from stressful academic situations can help students manage their stress and maintain focus on their studies (Ayduk & Kross, 2010). ...
... Both can uniquely predict emotional regulation and personal growth when one intends to be or can be mindful after an academic setback (Miyagawa et al., 2018;Powers & LaBar, 2019). Decentering involves a psychological distance from the emotional experiences, allowing the analytic and experiencing self to separate from each other (Kross et al., 2014). On the other hand, self-compassion encourages embracing a kind and accepting attitude toward oneself in moments of suffering or failure (see Neff et al., 2021). ...
... The dialogic aspects of human cognition are probably most explicit in the phenomenon of inner speech or the "little voice [s] in the head" (Gregory & Langland-Hassan, 2023) that manifest themselves in many people's conscious experience (Fernyhough & Borghi, 2023). The dialogic perspective on inner speech has been adopted, among other things, to shed light on the features of auditory verbal hallucinations (1996,2004) and the role of self-talk in motivation (Kross et al., 2014). However, it is up for debate whether a) inner speech is indeed at least sometimes dialogic and b) whether the dialogical interchange in one's mind can be meaningfully conceptualized outside of the phenomenon of inner speech. ...
... Adopting dialogism about the mind has substantial explanatory pay-offs when it comes to explaining many mental phenomena, including self-knowledge (Wilkinson, 2020) and the role of self-talk in motivation (Kross et al., 2014). Dialogism is also arguably implicit in particular accounts of the emergence of norm-governed behavior. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The idea of dialogic interchange between mutually influencing “voices” within an individual has long played a part in psychology and the philosophy of mind. Although the dialogic aspects of human cognition are the most explicit in inner speech, some authors have argued that other types of mental activity can be (or always are) dialogic. In this paper, we introduce two dimensions of dialogism—strength and depth—and present a particularly deep form of dialogism of the mind. We provide one possible interpretation of deep dialogism from the developmental perspective. Further, we show how this form of dialogism has important payoffs for explaining the mechanisms and the phenomenology of human psychological experiences in general and mental health conditions in particular.
... This thinking pattern is often closely associated with social anxiety and may be a significant feature of social anxiety disorder. Rumination is considered a crucial influencing factor for symptoms of depression and social anxiety (Kross et al., 2014). It can not only intensify depressive emotions but also impair an individual's problem-solving abilities, making it difficult to effectively address challenges. ...
... Emotion-focused coping strategies, on the other hand, are a range of unhealthy, ineffective, or avoidant methods used to deal with stress, challenges, or negative emotions. It is easy to understand that individuals engaged in rumination may generate negative emotions (Kross et al., 2014), which can lead them to adopt more emotion-focused coping behaviors, such as avoiding difficulties, denying problems, or indulging in negative emotions. Conversely, emotion-focused coping strategies may also exacerbate Frontiers in Psychology 05 frontiersin.org ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study evaluates the potential mechanisms through which stress affects sleep quality and examines the mediating roles of rumination, social anxiety, emotion-focused coping strategies, and smartphone dependence in the relationship between stress and sleep quality. Methods From October 2023 to April 2024, we conducted three surveys with 426 university students and utilized structural equation modeling to explore the mechanisms by which stress impacts sleep quality. Results Stress not only significantly predicts sleep quality but also significantly affects sleep quality through rumination, emotion-focused coping strategies, and smartphone dependence as independent mediators. Additionally, stress influences sleep quality through both dual-mediation and triple-mediation paths. Conclusion Stress has a significant direct and indirect impact on sleep quality. This study reveals the complex mechanisms through which stress affects sleep quality. Improving individuals’ sleep quality requires not only considering the influence of real-life stressors but also examining the intersecting effects between stress and factors such as ruminative thinking, social anxiety, smartphone dependency, and emotion-focused coping strategies. The intense social competition in Chinese society exacerbates the decline in sleep quality, highlighting the need for the government to implement more policies aimed at maintaining the physical and mental health of the population to alleviate the increasingly severe sleep issues and mental health crisis.
... Например, некоторые исследования посвящены изучению внешнего общения с самим собой -автокоммуникации (Лотман, 1992). В других исследованиях изучался феномен разговора с собой, выявлялись разные причины и преимущества его применения: упрощение выполнения задач (Kirkham, Breeze, & Marί-Be a, 2012), ускорение поиска предметов (Lupyan & Swingley, 2012), самодистанцирование с целью лучшего управления собственными эмоциями (Kross et al., 2014), улучшение спортивных показателей (Boroujeni & Shahbazi, 2011). В литературных источниках есть обоснование того, как работа с внутренними диалогами с помощью различных психотехник может помочь разрешить трудности человека, повлиять на элементы его Я-концепции. ...
Article
The article is devoted to the study of the psychotechnical potential of ego-directed speech, that is, addressing oneself as another person. The idea is based on the cultural-historical theory of L. S. Vygotsky about the stages of a child’s mastery of cultural means of influence in social interaction. It is proposed to develop this idea — one more stage, influencing oneself from the position of someone else, which allows one to theoretically substantiate the presence of a transformative/psychotechnical effect. The authors designed and tested a procedure for self-referral in the form of an audio recording: goal setting, sound recording of self-referral, repeated listening to the recording. The results showed that during goal setting, criticality to expected changes decreases, the first listenings cause voice confrontation (the vector of the psychologist’s work with the client is to emphasize the task of accepting one’s voice and oneself), and continuous repetition of the appeal to oneself actually makes positive changes in the self-attitude of the study participants. ƒThe proposed technique has the effect of flexible adjustment depending on the presence or absence of voice confrontation in a person. For people with vocal confrontation, repeated listening to the message will be more effective in increasing self-acceptance and reducing internal contradictions. People without voice confrontation will be able to make neutral attitudes more positive and soften negative attitudes.
... To summarise, in the present study the processing of negative situational and negative implicational information about an upcoming stressor did not serve to differentially in uence the degree to which participants exhibited anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration in response to this stressor. Nonetheless, these ndings build on previous work by Watkins (2008) and Kross (2005Kross ( , 2014, as this is the rst study to examine the in uence of exposure to information which is either abstract and decontextualised in nature (implicational) or distinct and situation speci c in nature (situational), on the two distinct facets of trait anxiety. From this foundational study, we hope further research will be conducted to illuminate the information processing biases implicated in the experience of anxiety reactivity and perseveration, furthering our theoretical, and applied understanding of these dimensions of trait anxiety. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Purpose While long considered a unitary dimension, research has moved towards a multidimensional understanding of trait anxiety. Most relevant to this study, is the conceptualisation of two dissociable dimensions of trait anxiety; anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration. Despite the consolidation of this understanding over the past decade, the mechanisms underlying these two dimensions are not well established. The present study examined the novel role of information processing in the experience of anxiety reactivity and perseveration, specifically, how enhanced processing of situational information influences reactivity, and how enhanced processing of implicational information influences perseveration. Method Undergraduate university students (N = 142) completed an experimental session, where they encountered an interview-based stressor. As participants approached this stressor, they were provided with information reflecting one of the two content categories, intended to encourage a situational or implicational processing mode. To facilitate measurement of anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration, state anxiety was assessed at crucial junctures across the experimental session. Results Our findings did not provide evidence for the role of enhanced processing of situational information in the experience of anxiety reactivity, nor the role of enhanced processing of implicational information in the experience of anxiety perseveration. An experimental manipulation involving valence revealed that receiving positive information reduced the intensity of state anxiety elevations, in the aftermath of the information processing task. Conclusions Findings are discussed with reference to limitations and future extensions that could investigate the potential role of cognitive biases in this proposed relationship between enhanced information processing and anxiety reactivity and perseveration.
... Specifically, instructing participants to stop using first-person singular pronouns and present-tense verbs downregulates negative emotions, ostensibly by increasing psychological distance from the self and the current moment (Nook, 2023;Orvell et al., 2019). For example, preparing for a stressful speech using "you" or one's name rather than "I" reduces distress and improves performance (Kross et al., 2014), and writing without using first-person singular pronouns reduces affective reactions to aversive stimuli Moser et al., 2017;Nook et al., 2017;Orvell et al., 2021). Thus, increasing linguistic distance can downregulate emotions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emerging research shows that distancing one’s language by reducing the use of “I” and present-tense verbs is associated with more successful emotion regulation. However, construal level theory posits that increasing psychological distance should also shift mental representations to be more abstract. Here, we test whether successful emotion regulation is related to linguistic measures of both abstractness and psychological distance in two emotion regulation experiments reported previously ( N 1 = 217, N 2 = 464). As hypothesized, we found that (i) linguistic measures of psychological distancing and abstractness were highly correlated, (ii) linguistic abstractness increased when people used cognitive reappraisal strategies to regulate their responses to negative images, (iii) instructing participants to distance their language by not using the word “I” increased linguistic abstractness, and (iv) the extent to which people made their language more abstract while regulating correlated with regulation success. These results extend theoretical arguments that psychological distance and abstract construals are strongly connected in the mind. Furthermore, they provide novel evidence that successful cognitive reappraisal is associated with more abstract representations of stimuli and that this cognitive shift can be achieved merely by shifting one’s language.
Chapter
Based on the warranted need for effective strategies to navigate the current dynamic business environment, self-talk and grit can be useful tools. The author examined the differences in self-talk frequency and grit levels in individuals across genders, age groups, education levels, employment status, current roles, and additional learning efforts. Data was collected from 576 individuals using surveys and hypotheses were analyzed using Spearman's Rho, Mann-Whitney U-test, and Kruskal Wallis. Findings revealed that working adults who engaged in learning efforts had higher self-talk frequency and perseverance (grit dimension), of which the latter variables showed a strong positive correlation. Results are useful to learning and development specialists to utilize self-talk as a tool to garner better performance, training, and well-being outcomes. It identifies the unexplored relationship between self-talk and grit in working adults in the Indian population.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitively reappraising a stressful experience—reinterpreting the situation to blunt its emotional impact—is effective for regulating negative emotions. English speakers have been shown to engage in linguistic distancing when reappraising, spontaneously using words that are more abstract or impersonal. Across two preregistered studies (N = 299), we investigated whether such shifts in language use generalize to Spanish, a language proposed to offer unique tools for expressing psychological distance. Bilingual speakers of Spanish and English and a comparison group of English monolinguals transcribed their thoughts in each of their languages while responding naturally to negative images or reappraising them. Reappraisal shifted markers of psychological distance common to both languages (e.g., reduced use of “I”/“yo”), as well as Spanish-specific markers (e.g., greater use of “estar”: “to be” for temporary states). Whether these linguistic shifts reflected successful emotion regulation depended on language experience: in exploratory analyses, the common markers were more strongly linked to reduced negative affect for late than early Spanish learners, and one Spanish-specific marker (“estar”) also predicted reduced negative affect for early learners. Our findings suggest that people distance their language in both cross-linguistically shared and language-specific ways when regulating their emotions.
Article
Full-text available
Predictive linguistics is a growing field at the interface of language sciences, cognitive sciences, and artificial intelligence, focusing on how humans and machines use predictive processes to process and produce language. This discipline is distinguished by its proactive approach, emphasizing the internal mechanisms that allow us to anticipate future linguistic structures. It explores the underlying processes at all levels of language-morphological, syntactic, semantic, pragmatic, etc.-and aims to understand how these predictions influence the production and comprehension of discourse. The main goal of predictive linguistics is to model cognitive mechanisms to better understand how the brain processes language, but also to develop models capable of generating, predicting, and understanding language in a manner similar to human cognition. By focusing on inner speech and the cognitive mechanisms that allow us to predict linguistic structures at different levels, predictive linguistics aims to create language models capable of reproducing these predictive capabilities. In doing so, it could revolutionize natural language processing and pave the way for artificial general intelligence, capable of linguistic awareness similar to that of humans.
Article
Full-text available
Although the focus of research for decades, there is a surprising lack of consensus on what is (and what is not) self-control. We review some of the most prominent theoretical models of self-control, including those that highlight conflicts between smaller-sooner versus larger-later rewards, “hot” emotions versus “cool” cognitions, and efficient automatic versus resource-intensive controlled processes. After discussing some of their shortcomings, we propose an alternative approach based on tenets of construal level theory (Trope et al., 2021) that integrates these disparate models while also providing novel insights. Specifically, we model self-control as a problem of regulatory scope—the range of considerations one accounts for in any decision or behavior. Self-control conflicts occur when the pursuit of specific local opportunities threatens the ability to address motivational priorities that span a broader array of time, places, individuals, and possibilities. Whereas a more contractive consideration of relevant concerns may prompt indulgence in temptation, a more expansive consideration of concerns should not only help people identify the self-control conflict but also successfully resolve it. We review empirical evidence that supports this new framework and discuss implications and new directions. This regulatory framework not only clarifies what is and what is not self-control but also provides new insights that can be leveraged to enhance self-control in all its various forms.
Article
Full-text available
Can language use reflect personality style? Studies examined the reliability, factor structure, and validity of written language using a word-based, computerized text analysis program. Daily diaries from 15 substance abuse inpatients, daily writing assignments from 35 students, and journal abstracts from 40 social psychologists demonstrated good internal consistency for over 36 language dimensions. Analyses of the best 15 language dimensions from essays by 838 students yielded 4 factors that replicated across written samples from another 381 students. Finally, linguistic profiles from writing samples were compared with Thematic Apperception Test coding, self-reports, and behavioral measures from 79 students and with self-reports of a 5-factor measure and health markers from more than 1,200 students. Despite modest effect sizes, the data suggest that linguistic style is an independent and meaningful way of exploring personality.
Article
Full-text available
From W. B. Cannon’s identification of adrenaline with “fight or flight” to modern views of stress, negative views of peripheral physiological arousal predominate. Sympathetic nervous system (SNS) arousal is associated with anxiety, neuroticism, the Type A personality, cardiovascular disease, and immune system suppression; illness susceptibility is associated with life events requiring adjustments. “Stress control” has become almost synonymous with arousal reduction. A contrary positive view of peripheral arousal follows from studies of subjects exposed to intermittent stressors. Such exposure leads to low SNS arousal base rates, but to strong and responsive challenge- or stress-induced SNS-adrenal-medullary arousal, with resistance to brain catecholamine depletion and with suppression of pituitary adrenal-cortical responses. That pattern of arousal defines physiological toughness and, in interaction with psychological coping, corresponds with positive performance in even complex tasks, with emotional stability, and with immune system enhancement. The toughness concept suggests an opposition between effective short- and long-term coping, with implications for effective therapies and stress-inoculating life-styles.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated the impact of performance expectancies and self-focused attention on social performance in a mixed-sex dyad. A group of 48 undergraduate men with self-reported moderate social anxiety were divided into two groups on the basis of their high or low performance expectancies. Subjects were asked to respond to several self-report questionnaires before and after making a 4–5-min phone call to a female confederate for the purpose of getting acquainted. Half of the subjects in each expectancy group performed in the presence of a self-focusing stimulus (video camera). When anxiety level was controlled for, focus of attention alone had a very limited effect on performance. Expectancy had a significant influence on social performance, but only if subjects were self-focused. Thus confident subjects were rated by judges as more socially skilled than were doubtful subjects, but only when the camera was present. The interaction between these variables parallels previous research and supports the self-regulation model of Carver and Scheier.
Article
Full-text available
Nonclinic socially anxious individuals, clinic socially anxious patients, and nonsocially anxious subjects were assessed for changes in patterns of physiological reactivity and cognitions across three interpersonal tasks. The clinic and nonclinic samples of socially anxious subjects differed significantly from the nonsocially anxious subjects in level of physiological reactivity and type of cognition. There were no significant differences between the two anxious groups. The results indicate that both thoughts and physiological reactivity were influenced by situational parameters. The results are discussed in terms of the importance of situational impact in assessments of behavior in socially anxious individuals and the comparison of subjects selected on the basis of performance on various social anxiety inventories to patients who seek treatment in an anxiety disorders clinic.