Content uploaded by Marc Clarà
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Marc Clarà on Feb 16, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Learning online: MOOCs, connectivism, and cultural psychology
Marc Clarà and Elena Barberà
Clarà, M. & Barberà, E. (2013): Learning online: massive open online courses (MOOCs),
connectivism, and cultural psychology. Distance Education, 34(1), 129-136.
In this reflection, we discuss the connectivist conception of learning in Web 2.0
environments, which underpins the pedagogy of what are known as cMOOCs
(connectivist massive open online courses) (Siemens, 2012). We argue that this
conception of learning is inadequate and problematic, and we propose that
cultural psychology is best suited to address the explanatory challenges that Web
2.0 poses on learning, and therefore, it is also best suited to provide MOOCs with
more adequate and less problematic pedagogy. We suggest two initial and
general pedagogical principles based on cultural psychology upon which to begin
building this new pedagogy for MOOCs.
Keywords: Web 2.0; e-learning; distance education
Introduction
Massive open online courses (MOOCs) are receiving increasing attention and interest
from several communities involved in online distance education. Although MOOCs
were initiated and guided by very specific pedagogical assumptions, the phenomenon
has spread out without necessarily following their initial associated pedagogy. This fact
led Siemens (2012) to differentiate between cMOOCs and xMOOCs. This second type
of MOOC is mainly developed by world-leading campus-based universities (such as the
Massachusetts Institute of Technology or Stanford University) as an evolution of an
institutional strategy regarding digital technology and on-campus teaching (Armstrong,
2012). Thus, xMOOCs are not pedagogically driven, and the consequence is that they
assume pedagogies mainly based on behaviorist psychology (Bates, 2012). cMOOCs,
on the other hand, were developed with the explicit aim of exploring a pedagogy that
takes advantage of Web 2.0 for learning. Pioneers of cMOOCs (mainly Siemens and
2
Downes) argued that none of behaviorism, cognitivism, or constructivism could
adequately explain learning as it happens in Web 2.0. Consequently, they articulated
some psychological assumptions that they argued to be a new learning theory, called
connectivism. These psychological assumptions led them to propose the pedagogy that
in 2008 gave birth to the first MOOC (Mackness, Mak, & Williams, 2010).
Disregarding the reasons why some superficial characteristics of cMOOCs were
assumed by world-leading campus-based universities when they created their xMOOCs,
and although the pedagogy of xMOOCs is not currently based on connectivism (but on
behaviorism), connectivism is likely to play an important role in the future evolution of
MOOCs, because when xMOOCs are forced to modernize their pedagogy—and this
will occur soon (Daniel, 2012)—the first model to look at will be cMOOCs. For this
reason it is important, at the current stage of this story, to carefully and critically
consider the psychological assumptions that connectivism proposes as a way of
understanding and explaining learning in Web 2.0.
In this reflection, we argue that connectivism is not able to adequately explain
how learning occurs (neither in Web 2.0 nor in any other environment) because it does
not address many of the central attributes of learning. We then suggest that a promising
psychological tradition able to explain learning in Web 2.0, and therefore, able to drive
the future pedagogies of MOOCs, is the Vygotskian tradition of cultural psychology.
Theoretical assumptions of learning in the Web 2.0 environment
When Siemens (2005, 2006) and Downes (2005, 2006, 2012) first proposed the core
psychological principles of connectivism, they proceeded from the idea that the existing
learning theories were unable to explain some challenging characteristics posed by Web
2.0. These characteristics are mainly the following: (a) the rapid growth of knowledge,
which makes knowledge itself a dynamic phenomenon; (b) the new kinds of production
3
and externalization of knowledge, which multiply the perspectives embedded in
knowledge.
Siemens (2005, 2006) and Downes (2005, 2006, 2012) summarized the existing
learning theories in three theoretical positions: behaviorism, cognitivism and
constructivism. They assumed that these three positions share two key attributes: (a)
knowledge resides in the individual; and (b) knowledge is a thing—a representation—
that people create or appropriate. Siemens and Downes argued that these two attributes
are not compatible with the characteristics of knowledge in Web 2.0. In their view, the
dynamism of knowledge in Web 2.0 contradicts the thingness of knowledge assumed by
the existing learning theories, and the multiplicity of perspectives embedded in
knowledge in Web 2.0 contradicts the individual location of knowledge assumed by the
existing learning theories.
As a consequence of this rationale, Siemens (2005, 2006) and Downes (2005,
2006, 2012) proposed a new learning theory, called connectivism, which can be
basically summarized by two fundamental ideas that respond to what they identify as
explanatory challenges of learning in Web 2.0.
First, in reaction to the thingness of knowledge assumed by the existing learning
theories, and as explanation of the dynamic nature of knowledge, connectivism states
that knowledge is subsymbolic, and that representations are just epiphenomena of
knowledge, but not its matter. The following quote by Downes (2006) is illustrative of
this idea:
It [knowledge] is, rather (and carefully stated), a recognition of a pattern in a set of
neural events (if we are introspecting) or behavioural events (if we are observing).
We infer to mental contents the same way we watch Donald Duck on TV – we
think we see something, but that something is not actually there – it’s just an
organization of pixels.
4
Therefore, to know means to form a pattern of neuronal associations, which at the
experiential level gives the impression of a representation. The patterns of association
can be highly changeable, and therefore representations, which are their epiphenomena,
are dynamic.
Second, these neuronal associative patterns are caused by the learner’s
recognition of associative patterns between informational entities (named nodes) located
outside the learner and organized in a network. In the Web 2.0 environment, the nodes
would be people, materials, and tools that the learner connects to. This idea of external
network as the starting point of the learning and knowing process is proposed as a
reaction to the idea of knowledge as residing in the individual, and as explanation of the
multiplicity of perspectives embedded in knowledge.
Although connectivism apparently addresses the challenges posed by Web 2.0
for learning, we argue that it does not provide adequate explanation to learning
phenomena because it neglects other crucial aspects of learning. We develop this
critique much more extensively elsewhere, so in this reflection, we will only point at
three important explanatory problems of the connectivist theoretical assumptions.
The first problem is that connectivism does not address at all what is known as
the “learning paradox.” This paradox, first posed by Socrates (Plato, 380 B.C./2002),
can be applied to connectivism as follows: How do you recognize a pattern if you do
not already know that a specific configuration of connections is a pattern? When a
pattern is connected for the first time, why are the nodes connected in that specific way,
and why is that configuration seen as a pattern? Connectivism leaves this question
unaddressed, and therefore unresolved. This theoretical problem causes an important
learning problem in cMOOCs: Many learners, especially those who do not have high
5
self-regulation skills, feel lost and without any direction and support in cMOOCs (Kop,
2011; Mackness et al., 2010).
The second problem is that connectivism underconceptualizes interaction and
dialogue, by understanding it as a learner’s connection to a human node in the network.
In the connectivist understanding, this connection to a human node is binary (on-off)
and static. Besides, the human node is seen as part of the (external) connective pattern
constitutive of knowledge (Downes, 2012). These ideas are in clear contradiction with
the evidence on interaction that the scientific community has been observing for
decades, also in online learning environments. It is well known that the other can be
crucial in a learning process without being part of what is learnt, but by being the
assistant of this learning. It is also well known that interaction is a process (not a state)
which evolves, and that the evolution and dynamics of interaction are related with the
learning process—think, for example, of the well-documented interactional process of
transfer of responsibility (e.g., Coll, Onrubia, & Mauri, 2008; Lave & Wenger, 1991;
Van de Pol, Volman, & Beishuizen, 2010). Just because of its dynamic nature,
interaction can be hardly characterized as a binomy like interaction-no interaction (on-
off). This serious underconceptualization causes another important learning problem in
cMOOCs: Many learners experience problems in finding proper ways to establish and
maintain a fruitful learning dialogue with others (Kop, 2011; Mackness et al., 2010).
The third problem is that connectivism is unable to explain concept
development. There is no need to mention that there is an extremely large amount of
evidence documenting the fact that a concept develops over time—in other words, one
specific concept is not of the same nature when used by a 4-year-old child as when this
child is 12 years old. The important explanatory problem experienced currently by
connectivism is the same problem that all associationisms in the history of psychology
6
have also faced before, and it is one of the key reasons that led to abandonment of
associationism in the 20th century (Vygotsky, 1986). The problem, in connectivist
terms, can be posed as follows: If a concept consists of a specific pattern of
associations, how can it be explained that the concept develops but the pattern of
associations remains the same? From our point of view, this problem inevitably leads to
abandonment of the connectivist idea of knowledge as associative patterns, which
actually means abandoning the whole theoretical assumption of connectivism.
The consequence of all of this is that, although at first glance the connectivist
proposals may seem appealing, connectivism does not provide an adequate explanation
of learning phenomena in Web 2.0, and therefore it is not able to provide an adequate
pedagogy for MOOCs. In the next section, we propose that cultural psychology is a
promising starting point to provide a conceptual explanation of learning in Web 2.0,
which is able to underlie and support an adequate pedagogy for MOOCs.
The Vygotskian tradition of cultural psychology
The genealogy of the Vygotskian psychological tradition is quite complex to trace. In
this reflection, we consider that this tradition is articulated around the axis formed by
the works of Vygotsky-Leontiev-Ga’lperin-Davydov-Cole-Engeström. This tradition is
commonly known as cultural-historical activity theory (CHAT) or cultural psychology
(Blunden, 2010; Cole, 1996; Engeström, 2001). Summarizing the theoretical universe
developed by this tradition is, of course, beyond the scope of this reflection. Instead, we
will briefly focus on three ideas that together can adequately address the two challenges
of learning in Web 2.0 identified by connectivists.
The first idea is that representations (knowledge) are tools (psychological tools
i
)
that mediate between the subject and the object. Representations are not originally
located within the subject; their origin is external. Moreover, representations (as tools)
7
only exist in use, as mediating between subject and object. Therefore, a representation is
not something that the subject has but something that the subject uses in relation to an
object.
The second idea is that representations are distributed (present and available) in
communities and systems of activity. A community (Wenger, 1998) can be understood
as a group of people who have a mutual engagement (or in other words, a common
object) and a shared repertoire (or in other words, a shared set of tools to relate to the
object). A system of activity can be understood as a tradition within which a community
can be located in the long-term history (Engeström, 2001). The tools distributed in a
community and in a system of activity are transformed and redistributed by different
mechanisms (Engeström, 1987). The most powerful mechanism is the interaction of two
or more systems of activity as a consequence of a partially shared object.
The third idea is that learning consists of the learner’s internalization of the use
of a representation in relation to an object. This internalization is possible only if the
learner has the opportunity of using the representation jointly with others within a zone
of proximal development. As a consequence of their participation in this joint activity
there is internalization by the learners, who can finally use the representation on their
own (Vygotsky, 1978, 1986). Internalization always implies certain transformations in
the use of the representation: When using the representation autonomously, the learner
always uses it in slightly different ways than the people who were involved in the joint
activity that made internalization possible. These transformations in use can be at the
same time externalized by the learner and therefore distributed in the community and
the system of activity; in this way these new uses can be internalized by other users who
will also transform and externalize them (Engeström, 1999).
8
In our view, these ideas adequately meet the two explanatory challenges that,
according to Siemens (2005, 2006) and Downes (2005, 2006, 2012), Web 2.0 poses for
learning, namely the dynamism of knowledge and the multiplication of perspectives
embedded in knowledge. From the viewpoint of cultural psychology, representations
are things; however, they are not fixed objects but mediational means. Thus, the object
can remain the same, but the representations used to relate to this object may be highly
changeable. This dynamism and changeability of representations is a consequence of
the two main mechanisms of transformation and re-distribution that we mentioned
earlier: the interaction between systems of activity and the process of internalization-
externalization. Both mechanisms necessarily require a dialectic interaction among a
multiplicity of perspectives (Engeström, 1999, 2001).
Web 2.0 amplifies and accelerates these two mechanisms (Engeström &
Sannino, 2010). The consequence is that the dynamism and the multiplicity of
perspectives inherent in any representation are currently much more evident and larger
than in earlier decades. However, from the viewpoint of cultural psychology, this
phenomenon is not new, and it is fully explainable: it just responds to a (technologically
accelerated) transformation and re-distribution of representations in order to relate to an
object.
From this conceptualization, we suggest two initial and general pedagogical
principles regarding MOOCs. The first principle is to enable, both didactically and
technologically, the visualization of the objects that learners choose to relate to, in order
to make possible and evident the use of representations as mediating tools between the
learner and the object. Too often, pedagogies have confused the representations with the
object, so that it has been assumed that the object of the learner’s activity is the
representation itself; in the Web 2.0 era, with the acceleration of transformation and re-
9
distribution of representations, this erroneous assimilation of representation-object may
lead both learners and teachers to a crisis about what is worthy to know and learn.
Pedagogy in Web 2.0 environments needs to make more evident than ever this
differentiation between representation-object, and needs to provide learners with
visualized objects that give direction to their learning, since objects remain relatively
stable even when the representations and (other) tools used to relate with them change
and transform rapidly.
The second principle is to enable, both didactically and technologically,
opportunities for joint activity; that is, opportunities for people to use together a
representation in order to relate to a common object. Only if these opportunities are
provided will the uses of representations be internalized (i.e., learned) by participants.
This requires the possibility of dialogicity in interaction (Wells & Mejía Arauz, 2006)
and dialogue sustained over time, in order to make possible processes of transfer or
responsibility.
Conclusion
Since its formulation in 2005, connectivism has received strong critiques from several
authors from different points of view. Kop and Hill (2008), for example, have argued
that connectivism cannot be considered a learning theory (yet) because its postulates do
not meet the criteria to be so considered. Lange (2012) has been more severe, and has
argued that connectivism is not a learning theory, not only because it does not meet the
criteria, but because it does not offer anything new, just a mixture of ideas already
present in other well-established learning theories. In the same vein, Verhagen (2006)
has suggested that connectivism is a pedagogical view, but not a learning theory;
however, even as a pedagogical view, both Verhagen and Lange have argued that
connectivism does not offer any relevant contribution. Bell (2011) has argued that
10
connectivism must be seen as just a phenomenon, and not as a learning theory or as a
pedagogical view. From this view, Bell has suggested that connectivism can be
understood and practiced from different conceptualizations and pedagogies.
As can be seen, these critiques mainly address the ontological and
epistemological status of connectivism (what kind of thing connectivism is—learning
theory, pedagogy, phenomenon—and in which sense connectivism is better than other
similar things of the same status—better than other learning theories, or better than
other pedagogies). In this reflection, we aimed to examine connectivism from an
additional point of view: we tried to avoid discussing the status of connectivism in order
to focus the discussion on the psychological contents of its postulates.
The conclusion of this discussion is that, taken from a psychological point of
view, connectivism, as currently formulated, should be abandoned as a learning theory
and as a theoretical guide for pedagogy in MOOCs and in Web 2.0 environments in
general. As such, we have reached the same conclusion as those who have discussed the
ontological and epistemological status of connectivism.
However, the need to abandon connectivism does not mean the abandonment of
MOOCs, which seem to be expanding rapidly among universities all over the world
(Bates, 2012). Therefore, if connectivism is not a valid learning theory, and considering
that xMOOCs are adopting old behavioral pedagogies, there is the urgent need to build
an adequate pedagogy for MOOCs, based on a valid learning theory. We have argued
that this learning theory could be the Vygotskian cultural psychology, and we have
proposed two initial and general principles for a future pedagogy of MOOCs: the
visualization of objects and the enabling of dialogic and sustained joint activity. Other
authors, such as Ravenscroft (2011) or Kop, Fournier, and Mak (2011), have also seen
in the Vygotskian tradition an inspiration for trying new pedagogies in Web 2.0. The
11
main concern of these authors has been enabling dialogue, which is related to the
second pedagogical principle outlined in this reflection.
As a final thought, it is perhaps worthwhile to note that the psychological
discussion conducted in this reflection against connectivism recalls some arguments—
especially the discussion on concept development—from old discussions against
associationism in the early and mid-20th century (e.g., Vygotsky, 1986, 1997; Fodor,
1983).
It seems that in its periodic reappearance in the psychological arena,
associationism is attempting to come back in the Web 2.0 era. Although connectivism
deserves to be fully considered and discussed, we think that it is also important in this
discussion not to forget why associationism was discarded in the past as a central
explanation in psychology. Some of those reasons may still be important nowadays.
Notes on contributors
Dr. Marc Clarà (PhD in psychology) is associate professor at the University of Barcelona and
research assistant at the eLearn Center of the Universitat Oberta de Catalunya. His research
interests include the relationship between knowledge and action, the nature of educational joint
activity, and the mediation of digital media in learning processes.
Dr. Elena Barberà (PhD in psychology) is the research director of the eLearn center at the
Universitat Oberta de Catalunya and adjunct teacher at the South-eastern University of Florida.
Her research interests include e-assessment, learning strategies and teaching and learning
processes in online innovative contexts.
References
Armstrong, L. (2012, August 6). Coursera and MITx: Sustaining or disruptive? [Web
log message]. Retrieved from
http://www.changinghighereducation.com/2012/08/coursera-.html
12
Bates, T. (2012, August 5). What’s right and what’s wrong about Coursera-style
MOOCs? [Web log message]. Retrieved from
http://www.tonybates.ca/2012/08/05/whats-right-and-whats-wrong-about-
coursera-style-moocs/
Bell, F. (2011). Connectivism: Its place in theory-informed research and innovation in
technology-enabled learning. International Review of Research in Open and
Distance Learning, 12(3), 98–118. Retrieved from
http://www.irrodl.org/index.php/irrodl/index
Blunden, A. (2010). An interdisciplinary theory of activity. Leiden: Brill.
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA:
Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Coll, C., Onrubia, J., & Mauri, T. (2008). Ayudar a aprender en contextos educativos:
El ejercicio de la influencia educativa y el análisis de la enseñanza [Supporting
learning in educational contexts: The exercise of educational influence and the
analysis of teaching]. Revista de Educación, 346, 33–70. Retrieved from
http://www.revistaeducacion.mec.es/inicio.html
Daniel, J. (2012). Making sense of MOOCs: Musings in a maze of myth, paradox and
possibility. Seoul: Korean National Open University. Retrieved from
http://www.tonybates.ca/wp-content/uploads/Making-Sense-of-MOOCs.pdf
Downes, S. (2005). An Introduction to Connective Knowledge. Stephen’s Web (22
December). Retrieved from http://www.downes.ca/cgi-bin/page.cgi?post=33034
Downes, S. (2006, October 16). Learning networks and connective knowledge. Paper
presented at the Instructional Technology Forum. Retrieved from
http://itforum.coe.uga.edu/paper92/paper92.html
13
Downes, S. (2012). Connectivism and connective knowledge: Essays on meaning and
learning networks. Retrieved from
http://www.downes.ca/files/books/Connective_Knowledge-19May2012.pdf
Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding. Helsinki: Orienta-Konsulfit Oy.
Engeström, Y. (1999). Activity theory and individual and social transformation. In Y.
Engeström, R. Miettinen, & R. Punamäki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory
(pp. 19–38). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Engeström, Y. (2001). Expansive learning at work: Toward an activity theoretical
reconceptualization. Journal of Education and Work, 14, 133–156. Retrieved
from http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cjew20
Engeström, Y., & Sannino, A. (2010). Studies of expansive learning: Foundations,
findings and future challenges. Educational Research Review, 5, 1–24.
doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2009.12.002
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kop, R. (2011). The challenges to connectivist learning on open online networks:
Learning experiences during a massive open online course. International Review
of Research in Open and Distance Learning, 12(3), 19–37. Retrieved from
http://www.irrodl.org/index.php/irrodl/index
Kop, R., Fournier, H., & Mak, J. S. F. (2011). A pedagogy of abundance or a pedagogy
to support human beings? Participant support on massive open online courses.
International Review of Research in Open and Distance Learning, 12(7), 74–93.
Retrieved from http://www.irrodl.org/index.php/irrodl/index
14
Kop, R., & Hill, A. (2008). Connectivism: Learning theory of the future or vestige of
the past? International Review of Research in Open and Distance Learning, 9(3),
1–13. Retrieved from http://www.irrodl.org/index.php/irrodl/index
Lange, M. (2012). Talk:Connectivism. Retrieved from
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Talk:Connectivism
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation.
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Mackness, J., Mak, S. F. J., & Williams, R. (2010). The ideals and reality of
participating in a MOOC. In L. Dirckinck-Holmfeld, V. Hodgson, C. Jones, M. de
Laat, D. McConnell, & T. Ryberg (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International
Conference on Networked Learning 2010 (pp. 266–274). Lancaster: Lancaster
University. Retrieved from
http://www.lancs.ac.uk/fss/organisations/netlc/past/nlc2010/abstracts/Mackness.ht
ml
Plato. (2002). Meno. (J. Holbo & B. Waring, Trans.). Retrieved from
http://www.ma.utexas.edu/users/rgrizzard/M316L_SP12/meno.pdf (Original work
published 380 B.C.)
Ravenscroft, A. (2011). Dialogue and connectivism: A new approach to understanding
and promoting dialogue-rich networked learning. International Review of Research
in Open and Distance Learning, 12(3), 139–160. Retrieved from
http://www.irrodl.org/index.php/irrodl/index
Siemens, G. (2005). Connectivism: a learning theory for the digital age. International
Journal of Instructional Technology and Distance Learning 2(1), January.
Retrieved from http://www.itdl.org/journal/jan_05/article01.htm
15
Siemens, G. (2006, November 12). Connectivism: Learning theory or pastime for the
self-amused? [Web log message]. Retrieved from
http://www.elearnspace.org/Articles/connectivism_self-amused.htm
Siemens, G. (2012, July 25). MOOCs are really a platform. [Web log message].
Retrieved from http://www.elearnspace.org/blog/2012/07/25/moocs-are-really-a-
platform/
Van de Pol, J., Volman, M., & Beishuizen, J. (2010). Scaffolding in teacher-student
interaction: A decade of research. Educational Psychology Review, 22(3), 271–296.
doi:10.1007/s10648-010-9127-6
Verhagen, P. (2006). Connectivism: A new learning theory? Retrieved from
http://www.scribd.com/doc/88324962/Connectivism-a-New-Learning-Theory
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. In M. Cole, V.
J. Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman (Eds.), Mind in society (pp. 79–91).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). The historical meaning of the crisis in psychology: A
methodological investigation. In R. Rieber & J. Wollock (Eds.), The collected
works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 3. Problems of the theory and history of psychology
(pp. 233–345). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Wartofsky, M. W. (1979). Models: Representation and the scientific understanding.
Dordrecht: Reidel.
Wells, G., & Mejía Arauz, R. (2006). Dialogue in the classroom. The Journal of the
Learning Sciences, 15, 379–428. Retrieved from
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hlns20
16
Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and identity.
Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
i
The Vygotskian tradition makes a distinction between technical tools (or primary artifacts) and
psychological tools (or secondary artifacts) (e.g.,Vygotsky, 1986, or Wartofsky, 1979). The
details of this distinction are outside the scope of this reflection; the important idea here is
that both are instrumental, both are conceptualized as (external) tools which mediate the
relationship subject-object.