ArticlePDF Available

The Role of Agonistic Sounds in Male Nest Defence in the Painted Goby Pomatoschistus pictus

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Animals often vocalize during territorial challenges as acoustic signals may indicate motivation and fighting ability and contribute to reduce aggressive escalation. Here, we tested the function of agonistic sounds in territorial defence in the painted goby. Pomatoschistus pictus, a small vocal marine fish that defends nests during the breeding season. We first measured the number of times a male approached, avoided, explored, entered and exited two unattended nests associated with either conspecific agonistic sounds or a control: silence or white noise. Acoustic stimuli were played back when the male approached a nest. In a second experimental set, we added visual stimuli, consisting of a conspecific male in a small confinement aquarium near each nest. Even though we found no effect of the visual stimuli, the sound playbacks induced similar effects in both experimental conditions. In the sound vs. silence treatment, we found that when males approached a nest, the playback of conspecific sounds usually triggered avoidance. However, this behaviour did not last as in longer periods males visited nests associated with agonistic sounds more often than silent ones. When the control was white noise, we found no significant effect of the playback treatment in male behaviour. Although we cannot exclude the possibility that other sounds may dissuade nest occupation, our results suggest that agonistic sounds act as territorial intrusion deterrents but are insufficient to prevent nest intrusion on their own. Further studies are needed to test the significance of sound production rate, spectral content and temporal patterns to deter territorial intrusion in fish.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH PAPER
The Role of Agonistic Sounds in Male Nest Defence in the
Painted Goby Pomatoschistus pictus
Ricardo Pereira*, Stefania Rismondo*, Manuel Caiano*, Silvia S. Pedroso, Paulo J. Fonseca* &
Maria Clara P. Amorim
* Departamento de Biologia Animal e Centro de Biologia Ambiental, Faculdade de Ci^
encias da Universidade de Lisboa, Lisboa, Portugal
Unidade de Investigac
ß
~
ao em Eco-Etologia, Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada Instituto Universit
ario, Lisboa, Portugal
Correspondence
Maria Clara P. Amorim, Unidade de
Investigac
ß
~
ao em Eco-Etologia, Instituto
Superior de Psicologia Aplicada Instituto
Universit
ario, Rua Jardim do Tabaco 34,
1149-041 Lisboa, Portugal.
E-mail: amorim@ispa.pt
Received: April 1, 2013
Initial acceptance: May 19, 2013
Final acceptance: September 25, 2013
(M. Manser)
doi: 10.1111/eth.12180
Abstract
Animals often vocalize during territorial challenges as acoustic signals may
indicate motivation and fighting ability and contribute to reduce aggres-
sive escalation. Here, we tested the function of agonistic sounds in territo-
rial defence in the painted goby. Pomatoschistus pictus, a small vocal marine
fish that defends nests during the breeding season. We first measured the
number of times a male approached, avoided, explored, entered and
exited two unattended nests associated with either conspecific agonistic
sounds or a control: silence or white noise. Acoustic stimuli were played
back when the male approached a nest. In a second experimental set, we
added visual stimuli, consisting of a conspecific male in a small confine-
ment aquarium near each nest. Even though we found no effect of the
visual stimuli, the sound playbacks induced similar effects in both experi-
mental conditions. In the sound vs. silence treatment, we found that
when males approached a nest, the playback of conspecific sounds usually
triggered avoidance. However, this behaviour did not last as in longer peri-
ods males visited nests associated with agonistic sounds more often than
silent ones. When the control was white noise, we found no significant
effect of the playback treatment in male behaviour. Although we cannot
exclude the possibility that other sounds may dissuade nest occupation,
our results suggest that agonistic sounds act as territorial intrusion deter-
rents but are insufficient to prevent nest intrusion on their own. Further
studies are needed to test the significance of sound production rate,
spectral content and temporal patterns to deter territorial intrusion in fish.
Introduction
In nature, resources such as those needed for breeding
(e.g. food, water accessibility, vegetation, shelter
availability and nesting sites) are often limited and of
variable quality. Hence, in order to have a chance to
reproduce, individuals (typically males) may compete
intensely over these limited resources (Huntingford &
Turner 1987; Andersson 1994). This competition is
often solved through confrontations that frequently
start with a mutual assessment phase, characterized
by low-level agonistic displays and are only expected
to escalate to overt aggression if asymmetries between
individuals’ fighting ability and resource ownership
are small (Huntingford & Turner 1987; Briffa & Sned-
don 2007). Low-level aggressive behaviour involved
in such contests is often accompanied by sound
production in a variety of different taxa (insects -
Gerhardt & Huber 2002; fish - Ladich & Myrberg
2006; anurans - Davies & Halliday 1978; birds de
Kort et al. 2009; and mammals - Clutton-Brock & Al-
bon 1979). In most vertebrates, such as frogs, birds
and mammals, the sounds are the result of the passage
of air through specialized vocal organs (larynx, syr-
inx) driving the vibration of membranes. These
sounds typically exhibit strong frequency and ampli-
tude modulation (Ladich 2004). By contrast, teleost
fishes have evolved a diversity of sound-producing
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 1
Ethology
mechanisms that often generate low-frequency
pulsed sounds lacking complex patterns of frequency
and amplitude modulation (Amorim 2006; Ladich &
Fine 2006; Rice & Bass 2009). This makes fish particu-
lar suitable models to study the function of acoustic
signals with conceptually simple playback experi-
ments. Nevertheless, carrying out sound playback
experiments with fish can be technically challenging
due to the lack of available commercial underwater
loudspeakers that can reproduce fish sounds accu-
rately (Fonseca & Maia Alves 2012).
Sounds produced by fishes during territorial
defence usually function as a complement to visual
behaviour such as colour alterations or aggressive
visual displays, including fin erection or quivering
(Ladich & Myrberg 2006). Though sound seems to
play an important role in fish communication there is
little experimental evidence demonstrating the func-
tion of acoustic signals in fish territorial defence
(Ladich & Myrberg 2006; Vasconcelos et al. 2010).
Experiments with muted specimens and with sound
playback suggest that in an agonistic context acoustic
signals can reveal valuable information about the sen-
der and may help avoid overt confrontation and thus
energy depletion or even injury and death (Valinsky
& Rigley 1981; Riggio 1981; Ladich et al. 1992; Ladich
1998; Raffinger & Ladich 2009; Bertucci et al. 2010;
for a review see Ladich & Myrberg 2006). For exam-
ple, body size can be advertised by the production of
longer sounds with lower dominant frequencies and
higher amplitudes that result from larger body struc-
tures (Myrberg et al. 1993; Lobel & Mann 1995; Con-
naughton et al. 2000; Amorim & Neves 2008; Colleye
et al. 2009; Amorim et al. 2013). Also, fish sounds
may potentially inform the receiver of the sender’s
motivation during a confrontation (Ladich 2004).
Indeed, steroid circulating levels that affect territorial
behaviour and aggressiveness may also influence the
patterns of sonic muscles contraction or of fin stridu-
lation (Fine & Pennyacker 1986; Connaughton et al.
1997; Remage-Healey & Bass 2006).
In this study, we aimed at examining the role of fish
agonistic sounds in territorial defence using playback
experiments in a small marine benthic species, the
painted goby (Pomatoschistus pictus). Painted goby
males produce drums and thumps to attract and court
females and only drums during agonistic interactions
(Amorim & Neves 2007, 2008). During the breeding
season (JanuaryMay), males compete aggressively
over nesting sites and have few breeding opportuni-
ties, as they only live up to 2 years (Miller 1986).
Males provide exclusive paternal care and use visual
and acoustic behaviour to defend nests that are used
both in mate attraction and to hold the eggs laid by
females (Bouchereau et al. 2003; Amorim & Neves
2007, 2008). As agonistic drums duration and drum
bout duration increase with male size, it is likely that
acoustic signals are involved in male-male assessment
(Amorim & Neves 2008).
We experimentally examined whether males
approached and intruded vacant nests (silent or asso-
ciated with white noise) more readily than ‘occupied’
nests. We mimicked nest occupation by playing back
a sequence of conspecific agonistic sounds whenever
a male approached a nest within a body length. We
checked for several reactions of the subject male to
vacant and ‘occupied’ nests, including approaching,
avoiding, exploring, entering and exiting the nests.
We further checked for nest intrusion behaviour in a
similar-design experiment that used additional visual
stimuli, consisting of a male in a confinement aquar-
ium placed next to the nest. We predicted that males
would be more reluctant to intrude nests associated
with agonistic sound, especially in the presence of a
conspecific male. Specifically, we expected that males
avoided more often and intruded less frequently nests
associated with agonistic sounds.
Materials and Methods
Fish Collection and Maintenance
We captured approx. 120 adult fish from January to
April 2012 at Parede (38°41N, 9°21W), during low
spring tides at night with the help of hand nets and
flashlights. We separated fish by gender which we
assessed by examining the external papilla (rounder
in females and longer and pointed in males) and by
the presence of nuptial colours in males and swollen
bellies in ripe females (Bouchereau et al. 2003). We
kept a maximum of eight fish per stock 18 l aquarium
(24 924 932 cm), provided with sand, shelters and
a closed-circuit flow of artificial filtered sea water kept
at 16°C. Fish were maintained under a 12L/12D
photoperiod and fed daily ad libitum with chopped
shrimps. At the end of experiments (May/June 2012),
the fishes were returned to their collection location in
a healthy condition.
Experimental Procedure
Thirty-five-litre glass experimental aquaria (26 951
931 cm) also provided with a sand substrate were
divided into three compartments by transparent or
opaque partitions. Water, conditioned by dedicated
filter, pumps and cooler, was similarly kept at 16°C.
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH2
Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim
The aquaria were placed above two 3-cm-thick stone
slabs separated by two levels of rubber foam shock
absorbers. This helped to insulate the aquaria from
floor born vibrations and increased acoustic signal-
to-noise ratio, thus reducing possible interference
with our sound playbacks. Twenty-four hours before
an experiment, one male was placed in the central
compartment of the aquarium to allow it to become
resident. Only males that showed nuptial colours
and territorial behaviour in the stock tanks were
used. Each lateral compartment was equipped with
one artificial PVC shelter (5.5 cm length and 3 cm
inner diameter) partially covered with sand and one
custom made speaker (see below). The speaker was
protected by a plastic net to exclude fish from the
speaker area. We also covered both lateral walls of
the experimental aquarium with a fine net to
reduce areas where a fish might interact with its
own reflection.
Playback Treatment
Prior to all trials, we presented a 2 min sound pre-
stimulus during which subject fish were exposed to
conspecific agonistic sounds and a control, white
noise (WN) or silence. The 2 min sound pre-stimulus
intended to inform the subject fish of territorial own-
ership in the lateral compartments before the test per-
iod. Both the playback treatment (silence vs. sound or
WN vs. sound) and the stimuli assignment to the
lateral sides of the aquarium were randomly selected.
When sound was emitted at both sides of the
aquarium, that is, during WN vs. sound treatment, we
insured that the stimuli did not overlap to avoid
acoustic interference and masking. Different males
were used for each playback treatment.
Agonistic sounds used in the playback treatments
consisted of drums from three different males (stan-
dard length: 3.3, 3.3 and 3.8 cm) from our sound
archive (2010). We made three pre-stimulus drum
playback sequences. Each drum pre-stimulus
sequence comprised three different sounds from the
same male (Fig. 1a). Only sounds with a good sig-
nal-to-noise ratio were used. We also selected
sounds that would represent the natural variability
in number of pulses, sound duration and sound fre-
quency. All files had the same duration and acoustic
energy, and the spacing between sounds followed a
typical pattern observed in sound production during
territorial defence in this species (Fig. 1b). Each pre-
stimulus sequence presented a total of 34 sounds,
that is, a sound rate of 17 per min, a typical rate
for a motivated male (Amorim & Neves 2008). An
identical number of WN files were created and pre-
sented similar sound intervals as the drum
sequences and were equalized to the same sound
amplitude. Also, WN did not contain the pulse pat-
tern of the agonistic drums but had similar acoustic
energy and hence a shorter duration.
During the test period, every time a male
approached a nest and reached within one body
length from the entrance of the nest we immediately
played three sounds of the correspondent playback
treatment (sound or WN). This stimulus consisted of
the first three sounds of the sound pre-stimulus
sequence used in a particular trial. Playback experi-
ments were performed using custom made devices
composed by an underwater speaker and a driver
(Fonseca & Maia Alves 2012), which are able to
reproduce low-frequency pulsed fish sounds with
great accuracy, such as the ones emitted by the
painted goby (cf. Fig. 1 in Fonseca & Maia Alves
2012). These were fed through a D/A converter (Edi-
rol UA-25, Roland, Japan; 16 bit, 8 kHz) controlled by
Adobe Audition 3.0 (Adobe Systems Inc., Mountain
View, CA, USA), which allowed independent play-
backs on two channels. The amplitude of the
sound playback (drum or WN) was measured with a
(a)
(b)
(c)
Fig. 1: Oscillogram of three different drums used in a playback
sequence (a). A similar sequence of only three drums was played back
during trials whenever a subject male approached a nest within a body
length. A longer sequence of 2 min containing the same three drums
was used in the sound pre-stimulus in which the interval between
sounds followed a natural pattern. In (b), approximately 1 min of the
2 min sound pre-stimulus is schematized depicting the rate of 17
sounds per min used in the playback sequence. Note that an agonistic
sound sequence comprised three different sounds from the same male.
(c) Illustrates a played back drum recorded in the middle of the central
compartment. Note that this drum is from a different playback
sequence than the one depicted in (a).
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 3
R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions
hydrophone (Br
uel & Kjær 8104, Br
uel & Kjær, Nae-
rum, Denmark, sensitivity 205 dB re1V/lPa; fre-
quency response within 1 dB from 0.1 Hz to
180 kHz), connected to a Br
uel & Kjær 2238 Mediator
Sound Level Meter, Naerum, Denmark) and adjusted
to mimic that of a painted goby male at 12 cm dis-
tance measured in previous experiments (approx.
130 dB SPL, re. 1 lPa; Amorim et al. 2013). An addi-
tional hydrophone (High Tech 94 SSQ, High Tech
Inc., Gulfport, MS, USA; sensitivity 165 dB re1V/
lPa; frequency response within 1 dB from 30 Hz to
6 kHz) was kept on the central compartment of the
experimental aquarium, to register any sound pro-
duced during trials and to monitor sound playback.
Played back sounds reached the central compartment
with a good signal-to-noise ratio and quality (Fig. 1c).
Playback Experiments
We carried out two experiments that differed in
the presence of visual stimuli during the test period:
‘playback only’ and ‘playback+visual’.
The ‘playback only’ experiment started 15 min after
we turned off all the equipment in the room to reduce
noise background in the experimental aquarium. We
started video/sound recordings, removed the opaque
partitions and exposed the subject fish to a 5 min
visual pre-stimulus. This consisted of a male inside a
small confinement aquarium (8 97922 cm), pro-
vided with sand substrate and high enough to prevent
chemical communication, placed in each lateral com-
partment next to the transparent partition (Fig. 2a).
Stimulus males were matched in size, that is, differ-
ence in standard length ratio was <10%. This visual
pre-stimulus intended to arouse territorial behaviour
in the subject fish and thus to increase responsiveness
during the trial (see Lugli 1997). We finished the
visual pre-stimulus period by replacing the opaque
partitions. We then removed the lateral stimulus
males and allowed a silence period of 8 min which
was followed by the 2-min sound pre-stimulus. The
PVC and acrylic plastic partitions allowed good
transmission of the sound between compartments.
Afterwards, all partitions were removed and the fish
behaviour was observed and recorded for another
5 min (test period).
In the ‘playback+visual’ experiment, there was no
visual pre-stimulus and trials started with the 2-min
sound pre-stimulus, prior to the partitions’ removal,
followed by the 5-min-test period. The stimulus males
stayed between the nest and the side wall of the
experimental aquarium throughout the test period
(Fig. 2b).
We used different males in the ‘playback only’ and
‘playback+visual’ experiments. Fish were measured
for standard length (SL) and weighted (W) after each
test. A total of 12 subject males with SL ranging from
3.4 to 3.9 cm (mean standard deviation [SD] =3.7
0.18 cm) and W from 0.4 to 0.88 g (0.67 0.12 g)
were tested in the ‘playback only’ experiment. From
these, eight and four males were tested in the silence
vs. sound and in the WN vs. sound treatments, respec-
tively. A total of 14 subject males were tested in the
‘playback+visual’ experiment with SL ranging from
3.4 to 3.9 cm (3.7 0.16 cm) and W from 0.48 to
0.88 g (0.7 0.13 g), half of which were tested in
the silence vs. sound and the other half in the WN vs.
sound treatments. All males interacted with the nests
except from two males tested in the ‘playback+visual’
experiment. These were 3.6 cm SL, 0.58 g W (silence
vs. sound treatment) and 3.9 cm SL, 0.62 g W (WN
vs. sound treatment).
Behavioural Recording and Analysis
Fish behaviour was recorded with a video camera
(Sony DCR-HC39, Sony, Tokyo, Japan) positioned
(a)
(b)
Fig. 2: Top view of experimental setup during the visual pre-stimulus
on the ‘playback only’ (a) and during the whole ‘playback+visual’ (b)
experiments. Males in glass confinement aquaria (8 97922 cm) were
placed near the transparent partitions (dashed lines) in ‘playback only’
experiments to provide a visual pre-stimulus before playback proce-
dures. In ‘playback+visual’ experiments visual stimulus males were
placed instead between the nest and the side wall of the experimental
tank (26 951 931 cm) throughout the experiment.
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH4
Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim
50 cm in front of the experimental tank and subse-
quently analysed with Etholog (v.2.2, Ottoni 2000).
During the pre-stimuli period video recording
captured the entire aquarium, while during the test
period, we focused on the behaviour of the test male.
We tallied the frequency of occurrence of the fol-
lowing nest-related behavioural parameters: approach
(move within a body length of the nest), avoidance
(abruptly swims away from the nest), explore (swim
around and on top of the nest), enter and exit the
nest. Avoidance, explore and ‘initial enter’ were con-
sidered when they occurred within 5 s of the male’s
approach to the nest and hence approximately within
5 s of the playback stimulus. Enter and exit refer to
the number of times a male entered, following an
approach and exited a nest with no such time restric-
tion. Notice that in the case a male left the vicinity of
the nest and later approached it again and entered,
we considered that the first approach did not result in
nest intrusion (enter). The total time spent on each
side of the aquarium and the first side explored by the
fish were also registered. Trials were considered valid
when the tested male approached at least one of the
nests.
Statistical Analyses
Playback effects were analysed based on the different
behaviours exhibited by the subject males. We car-
ried out repeated measures ANOVAs to compare each
male behaviour (approach, avoidance, explore, enter
and exit) in relation to control nests and ‘sound-
associated’ nests, that is, using playback treatment
(control vs. sound) as the dependent variable (i.e.
the repeated measures variable), as males could
interact with both nest types. We also used experi-
ment type: ‘playback only’ and ‘playback+visual’ as a
factor (independent variable). We analysed sepa-
rately the data for the playback treatments silence vs.
sound and WN vs. sound. The two males that did not
interact with the nests were excluded from this
analysis, and hence, 24 males were considered in
total.
We tested male preference for the sound or control
sides of the experimental aquarium with Wilcoxon
non-parametric tests, where we considered the total
time spent on each side during the test period. We
carried out Binomial tests to examine whether males
first swam to the sound (or the control, i.e. silence or
WN) side of the aquarium more often than expected
by chance alone. The intention was to determine
whether there was a first preference based on the pre-
stimuli playback treatment (silence vs. sound or WN
vs. sound). We performed further Binomial tests to
examine whether the first nest occupation was made
randomly relative to nest type (test or control). In
these analyses, we pooled the ‘playback only’ and
‘playback+visual’ data because the presence of the
visual stimuli had no effect on the male’s behaviour
towards the nest (see results). We also used the full
data set, that is, the 26 tested males in these analyses
(except for the first nest occupation), as the two males
that did not interact with nests still showed active
exploration of the full aquarium.
Statistical analyses were conducted with Statistica
(10, Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, USA), and all data were trans-
formed when necessary to meet assumptions of the
used parametric tests. When there was no normality
of the transformed data, non-parametric tests were
used.
Results
During the acoustic pre-stimulus, when the partitions
were in place, subject males did not exhibit any mea-
surable reaction to the sound playback. In contrast,
during the visual pre-stimulus (in the ‘playback only’
experiment), tested males repeatedly swam up and
down the partition in front of the visual stimulus
males. Upon the removal of the partitions, the subject
male readily moved towards one side of the experi-
mental aquarium, this behaviour being more obvious
when an additional visual stimulus was present. In
the absence of conspecifics, males showed a general
explorative behaviour swimming around the aquar-
ium. However, when confined conspecific males were
present (‘playback+visual’ experiment), subject males
often interacted with the opponent by performing
quivering lateral and frontal displays while darkening
the chin.
In the treatment, silence vs. sound fish showed a
tendency (p =0.07) to approach more often nests
associated with sound than silent nests (Table 1,
Fig. 3a). Figure 4 depicts the number of times males
approached nests associated with different playback
treatments during both experiment types (‘playback
only’ and ‘playback+visual’) and hence the number of
received playback stimuli. Interestingly, males
avoided nests associated with agonistic drums more
frequently than silent nests (Table 1, Fig. 3b). In spite
of this, the frequency with which males entered both
nest categories within 5 s of an approach showed no
significant differences (Table 1), but in longer periods,
males entered and exited nests associated with sound
more frequently than silent ones (Table 1, Fig. 3c, d).
Hence, the first reaction of an approaching male
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 5
R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions
towards an ‘occupied’ nest (i.e. associated with
sounds) was often avoidance, but this behaviour
would not last and could be followed by a nest visit.
We found no significant differences between the
exploring behaviour towards silent or ‘occupied’
nests (Table 1). In addition, we found no effect of
experiment type for any variable (Table 1).
Regarding the treatment WN vs. sound, there were
no significant effects of the variables playback treat-
ment and experiment type for the nest-related behav-
iours approach, explore, enter (any time frame) and
exit performed by males towards the nest (Table 2).
Nevertheless, we found a significant effect of experi-
ment type on avoidance behaviour as males avoided
nests more often when there was no visual stimulus
nearby (Table 2, Fig. 5).
After the pre-stimuli, males swam to one side of the
aquaria (sound vs. control) at random, that is, swim-
ming towards the sound side was not different than
expected by chance (Binomial test, p >0.05). Fish
swam to the sound side 10 times out of 15 in the
silence vs. sound treatment and 6 of 11 in the WN vs.
sound treatment. However, when considering only
the males that entered a nest during trials (N =19), it
was significantly more likely that the first nest that
males entered was a nest associated with agonistic
sound rather than a control nest (Binomial test,
p=0.002). The first nest to be intruded was a nest
associated with agonistic sound 16 of 19 times: 6 of 8
times when the control was WN and 10 of 11 times
Table 1: Effects of playback treatment (PBK: silence vs. sound) and
experiment type (EXP: ‘playback’ only or ‘playback+visual’) on nest-
related behaviours: approach, avoidance, explore, initial enter (upon 5 s
of approach), enter (no time restraints) and exit
Dependent variable Factor Fp
Approach PBK F
1,12
=4.64 0.07
Exp F
1,12
=0.00 0.23
PBK 9Exp F
1,12
=1.16 0.35
Avoidance PBK F
1,12
=5.37 0.04
Exp F
1,12
=2.15 0.17
PBK 9Exp F
1,12
=2.15 0.17
Explore PBK F
1,12
=0.02 0.89
Exp F
1,12
=2.44 0.14
PBK 9Exp F
1,12
=0.94 0.35
Log-initial enter PBK F
1,12
=0.24 0.64
Exp F
1,12
=0.07 0.80
PBK 9Exp F
1,12
=1.64 0.22
Log-enter PBK F
1,12
=4.84 0.048
Exp F
1,12
=0.30 0.59
PBK 9Exp F
1,12
=1.91 0.19
Log-exit PBK F
1,16
=4.47 0.05
Exp F
1,16
=0.11 0.74
PBK 9Exp F
1,16
=2.01 0.18
Data were transformed when necessary to meet the ANOVA assumptions.
(a) (b)
(c) (d)
Fig. 3: Comparison of the frequency of the subject male behaviour towards the nest between playback treatments (silence and sound): (a) approach,
(b) avoidance, (c) enter and (d) exit. Avoidance was measured in a time window of 5 s after approach, whereas enter and exit were measured with
no time restraints. Dots and error bars are means and 95% confidence intervals, respectively. Data were transformed when necessary to meet the
ANOVA assumptions. (*)p <0.10; *p<0.05.
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH6
Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim
when the control was silence. The time until first nest
occupation averaged 91.2 s for nests associated with
agonistic sound (N =16) but only 50.3 s for control
nests (N =3). Males also spent more time in the
sound than in the control side of the aquarium
(Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test: T=83.0,
N=26, p =0.02; Fig. 6).
Discussion
In fish, sounds play an important role in establishing
and defending territories allowing to assess the condi-
tion of competitors and to decide whether they should
possibly risk injury by engaging in a fight (Ladich
2004). In this study, we used a small marine nest-
reproducing fish as a model and observed the effect of
playing back conspecific aggressive sounds, which
mimicked the presence of a vocalizing nest-holder, on
nest occupation.
In the silence vs. sound playback treatment, we
found that although subject males first headed ran-
domly to a side of the aquarium (sound or control
side), they tended to approach nests associated with
conspecific sounds more frequently than silent ones.
As expected, a frequent first reaction of males
approaching nests associated with agonistic sounds
Fig. 4: Mean standard deviation of the number of times a male
approached a nest associated with different stimuli during the ‘playback
only’ and the ‘playback+visual’ experiments. Note that the number of
approaches to a nest also corresponds to the number of received stim-
uli. Sound1 and sound2 correspond to the sound playbacks performed
during the silence vs. sound and WN vs. sound treatments, respectively.
Table 2: Effects of playback treatment (PBK: WN vs. sound) and experi-
ment type (Exp: ‘playback’ only or ‘playback+visual’) on nest-related
behaviours: approach, avoidance, explore, initial enter (upon 5 s of
approach), enter (no time restraints) and exit
Dependent variable Factor Fp
Log-approach PBK F
1,8
=0.04 0.84
Exp F
1,8
=0.10 0.76
PBK 9Exp F
1,8
=0.24 0.64
Log-avoidance PBK F
1,8
=0.01 0.92
Exp F
1,8
=33.41 0.00
PBK 9Exp F
1,8
=0.01 0.92
Explore PBK F
1,8
=0.02 0.88
Exp F
1,8
=1.82 0.21
PBK 9Exp F
1,8
=0.60 0.46
Initial enter PBK F
1,8
=0.08 0.78
Exp F
1,8
=0.08 0.78
PBK 9Exp F
1,8
=2.11 0.18
Enter PBK F
1,8
=0.73 0.42
Exp F
1,8
=0.49 0.42
PBK 9Exp F
1,8
=0.00 0.97
Exit PBK F
1,8
=1.07 0.33
Exp F
1,8
=0.36 0.56
PBK 9Exp F
1,8
=0.02 0.89
Data were log-transformed when necessary to meet the ANOVA
assumptions.
Fig. 5: Avoidance behaviour exhibited by subject males towards the
nest according to different experiment types: ‘playback only’ and ‘play-
back+visual’. Dots and error bars are means and 95% confidence inter-
vals, respectively. *** p<0.001.
Fig. 6: Mean standard deviation of the amount of time spent by
tested males on each side of the aquarium according to playback treat-
ment, that is, control vs. sound. *p<0.05.
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 7
R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions
was avoidance, a behaviour that was significantly less
likely to occur on approaching silent nests. However,
contrary to our predictions, after the first avoidance
reaction, males entered ‘occupied’ nests more fre-
quently than silent ones, probably because they could
not associate the sound to a male nest-holder. Alto-
gether, the results suggest that agonistic sounds have
a significant role in territorial defence in the painted
goby and may act as a deterrent to nest intrusion until
the absence of a nest-holder physically defending the
nest is confirmed. Agonistic sounds have been shown
to be important in territorial defence in a few other
fish species. Schuster (1986) observed that in the
dwarf gourami Colisa lalia territorial defence was
approx. sixfold more effective when attacks chasing
intruders were accompanied by sounds than when
they were silent. Valinsky & Rigley (1981) muted
juveniles of skunk loaches Yasuhikotakia horae and
showed that muted individuals had fewer chances to
chase intruders from their shelters. Consistent with
our results, Ladich & Myrberg (2006) and Vasconcelos
et al. (2010) suggest that fish agonistic sounds can act
as a ‘keep-out’ signal towards intruders. For example,
nest-holding Lusitanian toadfish (Halobatrachus
didactylus) males defend their territories with loud
sounds (boatwhistles), which often elicits fleeing
behaviour from the intruder, thus decreasing the
chances of escalated fights and nest takeovers (Va-
sconcelos et al. 2010). Myrberg (1997) also showed
with a series of elegant field experiments that play-
backs of the chirp sound of bicolour damselfish males
(Stegastes partitus) made in unguarded territories inhi-
bit territorial intrusion by neighbouring conspecific
males for a significantly longer time than when there
is no sound playback. Consistent with our study, Myr-
berg also found that the deterrent effect of the sound
was short-term when transmitted in the absence of
the resident (Myrberg 1997). Similarly, in birds acous-
tic signals can slow down, but not prevent, territory
intrusion (reviewed in Nowicki et al. 1998). For
example, great tit males (Parus major) take longer to
intrude recently unoccupied territories if they are
associated with conspecific song playback than silent
territories (Krebs 1976).
The fact that males visited more often and were
more likely to first intrude nests associated with con-
specific sounds, and spent more time in the sound side
of the aquarium, deserves further attention. For
example, males could be selecting for good-quality
nests already chosen by other males thus saving the
time and energy costs of searching for a suitable nest-
ing site (Lindstr
om & Pampoulie 2005). Also, as
females of the congeneric P. minutus prefer males that
already have eggs in the nest (Forsgren et al. 1996),
the search for such nests could be an added value if
mate selection in the studied species follows the same
pattern.
In the trials using white noise as a control, we regis-
tered no significant effect of the playback treatment in
any male nest behaviour suggesting that males may
not distinguish conspecific sound from white noise.
However, the lack of significant differences in the
reaction towards nests associated with agonistic sound
or white noise should be seen with caution as it is
likely due to the low sample size. In the bicolour dam-
selfish, Myrberg (1997) found that white noise had
no effect as a territorial deterrent. Consistently,
Schwarz (1974) found that agonistic sounds reduced
aggression in the cichlid, Archocentrus centrarchus,in
contrast with control noise that rendered no effect in
malemale agonistic interactions. These and other
studies (reviewed in Ladich & Myrberg 2006) support
our suggestion that low sample sizes hampered
the ability to find a treatment effect in this set of
experiments.
Fish are believed to mainly use the temporal
patterns of calls for communication (Ladich 2004;
Amorim 2006). Therefore, we removed the specific
temporal pattern from the white noise stimuli used in
our experiments to avoid relaying to the receiver
information likely contained in the pulse pattern. As a
consequence, and to keep the same acoustic energy
among the sound stimuli, a parameter that might
affect the behaviour by changing the overall stimula-
tion of the hearing pathway, the white noise stimuli
were shorter than the agonistic drums. However, by
doing this, we were modifying another possible rele-
vant parameter, the duration of the stimuli, which
thus did not corresponded to the duration of the aver-
age drum and might have influenced the fish
response. For example, McKibben & Bass (1998) car-
ried out phonotactic experiments with plainfin mid-
shipman females (Porichthys notatus) and concluded
that duty cycle (sound on time, i.e. acoustic energy)
and also sound duration affected female preference.
The decrease in duty cycle and sound duration of mat-
ing-like signals decreased the likelihood of phonotaxis
possibly because playback sounds decreased their
resemblance to the long tonal mate attraction hums
made by nesting males.
Although test and visual stimuli painted goby
males often engaged in mutual visual displays, the
presence of confined males had in general no effect
on the male’s nest-related behaviour. Most available
studies have shown that agonistic sounds usually
need to be associated with signals of other modali-
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH8
Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim
ties to elicit appropriate behavioural responses (Lad-
ich 2004). For example, Bertucci et al. (2010)
experimentally separated or coupled visual and
acoustic signals to test the role of sounds produced
during malemale aggressive interactions in a cichlid
fish, Metriaclima zebra, and observed that acoustic
signals alone never triggered aggression. However,
when associated with visual signals agonistic sounds
significantly lowered the levels of aggressive behav-
iour observed when visual signals were presented
alone. The general lack of effect of additional visual
stimuli further points to a prevalent and indepen-
dent role of acoustic signals in territorial defence in
our study species, a role scarcely documented in
fish. However, we cannot rule out that the lack of
effects induced by the visual stimulus male might
be due to test fish perceiving that the male in
the confinement aquarium was not a threat and
could not defend the available nest. Nevertheless,
Myrberg (1997) found in the aforementioned field
experiments with the bicolour damselfish that
the visual presence of a confined resident near the
shelter (a male in a confinement bottle) was a more
effective deterrent to intrusion than sound playbacks
alone.
The function of sounds emitted during agonistic
behaviour is not fully understood in fishes and the
relative paucity of studies have shown that agonistic
sounds can have different roles depending on species
or individual status (reviewed in Raffinger & Ladich
2009). For example, in the satinfin shiner Cyprinella
analostana, a small cyprinid where males aggressively
defend territories while producing single knocks or
series of knocks (Stout 1963), playbacks of rapid series
of knocks can increase or inhibit males’ aggression
depending on the receiver’s social status. When domi-
nant males interacting with a mirror received play-
backs of agonistic sounds, they increased aggression,
in contrast to submissive males where the playback
induced the opposite effect (Stout 1975).
This study has revealed that acoustic communica-
tion is important in territorial defence in the painted
goby and likely other fishes as agonistic sounds slo-
wed down territorial intrusions. Additional experi-
ments are required to test the significance of sound
rates, spectral and temporal patterns to deter territo-
rial intrusion in fish and to ascertain the specificity of
the acoustic features that can prime territorial behav-
iour. Other studies not involving direct assessment of
the function of particular sound features on territorial
defence have, however, shed light on signal parame-
ters that might be used by fish to encode-decode rele-
vant information. For example, sound pressure level,
sound frequency or sound emission rate were demon-
strated to give information on fish size and condition
and therefore resource holding potential (Ladich
1998; McKibben & Bass 1998; Amorim et al. 2010).
However, the best approach to assess the function of
signals is the use of playback experiments. This has
been common practice to infer the territorial function
of acoustic signals and has shown in other taxa the
importance of particular acoustic traits in male-male
assessment (Davies & Halliday 1978; de Kort et al.
2009). Associating nests with acoustic stimuli differing
in the above-mentioned features could shed light to
whether agonistic sounds only inform the intruder of
the presence of a nest-holder or if they are also used
in male-male assessment.
Acknowledgements
We are thankful to Susana Varela for comments on
the manuscript. This research was funded by the
Science and Technology Foundation, Portugal (project
PTDC/MAR/68868/2006, pluriannual program UI&D
331/94 and UI&D 329). The methods for animal
collection, housing, handling and experimental proto-
cols comply with Portuguese animal welfare laws,
guidelines and policies. The authors have no conflict
of interest.
Literature Cited
Amorim, M. C. P. 2006: Diversity of sound production in
fish. In: Communication in Fishes, Vol. 1 (Ladich, F.,
Collin, S. P., Moller, P. & Kapoor, B. G., eds). Science
Publishers, Enfield, N.H., pp. 71104.
Amorim, M. C. P. & Neves, A. S. M. 2007: Acoustic signal-
ling during courtship in the painted goby, Pomatoschistus
pictus. J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. UK 87, 10171023.
Amorim, M. C. P. & Neves, A. S. M. 2008: Male painted
gobies (Pomatoschistus pictus) vocalise to defend
territories. Behaviour 145, 10651083.
Amorim, M. C. P., Sim~
oes, J. M., Mendonc
ßa, N., Bandarra,
N. M., Almada, V. C. & Fonseca, P. J. 2010: Lusitanian
toadfish song reflects male quality. J. Exp. Biol. 213,
29973004.
Amorim, M. C. P., Pedroso, S. S., Bolgan, M., Jord~
ao, J.
M., Caiano, M. & Fonseca, P. J. 2013: Painted gobies
sing their quality out loud: acoustic rather than visual
signals advertise male quality and contribute to mating
success. Funct. Ecol. 27, 289298.
Andersson, M. 1994: Sexual Selection. Princeton Univ.
Press, Princeton, New Jersey.
Bertucci, F., Beauchaud, M., Attia, J. & Mathevon, N.
2010: Sounds modulate males’ aggressiveness in a cich-
lid fish. Ethology 116, 11791188.
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 9
R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions
Bouchereau, J. L., Houder, V., Marques, A. & Rebelo, J. E.
2003: A new distribution record and the reproductive
strategy of Pomatoschistus pictus adriaticus (Pisces: Gobii-
dae) in the Mediterranean Sea. J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. UK
83, 11571161.
Briffa, M. & Sneddon, L. U. 2007: Physiological constraints
on contest behaviour. Funct. Ecol. 21, 627637.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. & Albon, S. D. 1979: The roaring of
red deer and the evolution of honest advertisement.
Behaviour 69, 145169.
Colleye, O., Fr
ed
erich, B., Vandewalle, P., Casadevall, M.
& Parmentier, E. 2009: Agonistic sounds in the skunk
clownfish Amphiprion akallopisos: size-related variation
in acoustic features. J. Fish Biol. 75, 908916.
Connaughton, M. A., Fine, M. L. & Taylor, M. H. 1997:
The effects of seasonal hypertrophy and atrophy on
fiber morphology, metabolic substrate concentration
and sound characteristics of the weakfish sonic muscle.
J. Exp. Biol. 200, 24492457.
Connaughton, M. A., Taylor, M. H. & Fine, M. L. 2000:
Effects of fish size and temperature on weakfish distur-
bance calls: implications for the mechanism of sound
generation. J. Exp. Biol. 203, 15031512.
Davies, N. B. & Halliday, T. R. 1978: Deep croaks and fight
assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature 274, 683685.
Fine, M. L. & Pennyacker, K. R. 1986: Hormonal basis for
sexual dimorphism of the sound-producing apparatus of
the oyster toadfish. Exp. Neurol. 92, 289298.
Fonseca, P. J. & Maia Alves, J. 2012: A new concept in
underwater high fidelity low frequency sound genera-
tion. Rev. Sci. Instrum. 83, 055007.
Forsgren, E., Karlsson, A. & Kvarnemo, C. 1996: Female
sand gobies gain direct benefits by choosing males with
eggs in their nests. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 39,9196.
Gerhardt, H. C. & Huber, F. 2002: Acoustic Communica-
tion in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and
Diverse Solutions. The Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago
and London.
Huntingford, F. & Turner, A. K. 1987: Animal Conflict.
Chapman & Hall, London.
de Kort, S. R., Eldermire, E. R. B., Cramer, E. R. A. & Vehr-
encamp, S. L. 2009: The deterrent effect of bird song in
territory defense. Behav. Ecol. 20, 200206.
Krebs, J. R. 1976: Bird song and territorial defence. New
Scientist 70, 534536.
Ladich, F. 1998: Sound characteristics and outcome of
contests in male croaking gouramis (Teleostei).
Ethology 104, 517529.
Ladich, F. 2004. Sound production and acoustic communica-
tion. In: The Senses of Fishes. Adaptations for the Recep-
tion of Natural Stimuli (van der Emde, G., Mogdans, J. &
Kapoor, B. G., eds). Narosa, New Delhi, pp. 210230.
Ladich, F. & Fine, M. L. 2006. Sound-generating mecha-
nisms in fishes: a unique diversity in vertebrates. In:
Communication in Fishes, Vol. 1 (Ladich, F., Collin, S.
P., Moller, P. & Kapoor, B. G., eds). Science Publishers,
Enfield, N.H., pp. 343.
Ladich, F. & Myrberg, A. A. 2006. Agonistic behavior and
acoustic communication. In Communication in fishes,
Vol. 1 (Ladich, F., Collin, S. P., Moller, P. & Kapoor, B.
G., eds). Science Publishers, Enfield, N.H., pp.
122148.
Ladich, F., Bischof, C., Schleinzer, G. & Fuchs, A. 1992:
Intra- and interspecific differences in agonistic vocaliza-
tion in croaking gouramis (Genus: Trichopsis, Anaban-
toidei, Teleostei). Bioacoustics 4, 131141.
Lindstr
om, K. & Pampoulie, C. 2005: Effects of resource
holding potential and resource value on tenure at nest
sites in sand gobies. Behav. Ecol. 16,7074.
Lobel, P. S. & Mann, D. 1995: Spawning sounds of the
damselfish, Dascyllus albisella (Pomacentridae), and rela-
tionship to male size. Bioacoustics 6, 187198.
Lugli, M. 1997: Response of male goby, Padogobius mar-
tensii, to aggressive sound playback following pre-
experimental visual stimulation. Behaviour 134,
11751188.
McKibben, J. R. & Bass, A. H. 1998: Behavioral assessment
of acoustic parameters relevant to signal recognition
and preference in a vocal fish. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104,
35203533.
Miller, P. J. 1986: Gobiidae. In: Fish of the North-eastern
Atlantic and Mediterranean, Vol. III (Whitehead, P. J. P,
Bauchot, M. L., Hureau, J. C., Nielsen, J. & Tortonese,
E., eds). UNESCO, Paris, pp. 10191985.
Myrberg, A. A. Jr 1997: Sound production by a coral reef
fish (Pomacentrus partitus): evidence for a vocal, territo-
rial “keep-out” signal. Bull. Mar. Sci. 60, 10171025.
Myrberg, A. A. Jr, Ha, S. J. & Shamblott, H. 1993: The
sounds of bicolor damselfish (Pomacentrus partitus): pre-
dictors of body size and a spectral basis for individual
recognition and assessment. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 94,
30673070.
Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A. & Hughes, M. 1998: The terri-
tory defense function of song in song sparrows: a test
with the speaker occupation design. Behaviour 135,
615628.
Ottoni, E. B. 2000: EthoLog 2.2: a tool for the transcription
and timing of behavior observation sessions. Behav.
Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 32, 446449.
Raffinger, E. & Ladich, F. 2009: Acoustic threat displays
and agonistic behaviour in the red-finned loach
Yasuhikotakia modesta. J. Ethol. 27, 239247.
Remage-Healey, L. & Bass, A. H. 2006: A rapid
neuromodulatory role for steroid hormones in the
control of reproductive behavior. Brain Res. 1126,
2735.
Rice, A. N. & Bass, A. H. 2009: Novel vocal repertoire and
paired swimbladders of the three-spined toadfish,
Batrachomoeus trispinosus: insights into the diversity of
the Batrachoididae. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 13771391.
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH10
Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim
Riggio, R. 1981: Acoustical correlates of aggression in the
bicolor damselfish, Pomacentrus partitus. M.Sc. Thesis,
University of Miami, Miami, USA.
Schuster, S. 1986: Untersuchungen zur Bioakustik des
Zwergfisches Colisa lalia (Perciformes, Belontiidae).
Zool. Beitr. 29, 295306.
Schwarz, A. 1974: The inhibition of aggressive behavior by
sound in cichlid fish, Cichlasoma centrarchus. Z. Tierpsy-
chol. 35, 508517.
Stout, J. F. 1963: The significance of sound production
during the reproductive behaviour of Notropis analost-
anus (Family Cyprinidae). Anim. Behav. 11,8392.
Stout, J. F. 1975: Sound communication during the
reproductive behavior of Notropis analostanus (Pisces:
Cyprinidae). Am. Midl. Nat. 94, 296325.
Valinsky, W. & Rigley, L. 1981: Function of sound produc-
tion by the skunk loach Botia horae (Pisces, Cobitidae).
Z. Tierpsychol. 55, 161172.
Vasconcelos, R. O., Sim~
oes, J. M., Almada, V. C., Fonseca,
P. J. & Amorim, M. C. P. 2010: Vocal behaviour during
territorial intrusions in the Lusitanian toadfish: boat-
whistles also function as territorial ‘keep-out’ signals.
Ethology 116, 155165.
Ethology 119 (2013) 1–11 ©2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 11
R. Pereira, S. Rismondo, M. Caiano, S. S. Pedroso, P. J. Fonseca & M. C. P. Amorim Fish Sounds as Deterrents in Nest Intrusions
... While birdsong is one of the most well studied phenomena in animal communication, fewer studies have attempted to experimentally demonstrate the territorial function of vocalizations in other taxa. Speaker occupation experiments have been used in bicolor damselfish (Pomacentrus partitus; Myrberg, 1997) and painted gobies (Pomatoschistus pictus; Pereira et al., 2013), and muting experiments more recently in Lusitanian toadfish (Halobatrachus didactylus; Conti et al., 2015) to show that vocalizations serve as a "keepout" signal to other conspecifics. Due to the limitations of finding species amenable to such experimental designs, our understanding of the territorial function of vocalizations in mammals has been limited to observational field studies or playback experiments that induce an aggressive response in the territory holder (Smith, 1978;Grinnell et al., 1995;Barlow & Jones, 1997;Reby et al., 1999;Grinnell & McComb, 2001;Sharpe & Goldingay, 2009). ...
... Our findings support the conclusions of previous speaker occupation studies in both songbirds and territorial fishes that have demonstrated the importance of acoustic signalling for territory defence and retention (Krebs, 1977;Krebs et al., 1978;Falls, 1988;Myrberg, 1997;Nowicki et al., 1998;Pereira et al., 2013). Experimental tests of the role of vocalizations in actively deterring conspecific rivals in mammals have been less commonly performed (but see Waser, 1975;Harrington & Mech, 1979). ...
... Painted gobies approached 'occupied' nests more frequently than silent nests but then demonstrated avoidance after approaching the agonistic calls. However, when the gobies were unable to associate the sound with the physical presence of a territory holder they proceeded to intrude on the 'occupied' territory (Pereira et al., 2013). Anecdotally, we observed similar patterns of behaviour in red squirrels. ...
Article
Full-text available
In many species, territory advertisement is thought to be one of the primary functions of acoustic communication. North American red squirrels are a territorial species in which 'rattles' have long been thought to be the principal signal communicating territory ownership. These vocalizations have been assumed to deter intruders, thus reducing energetic costs and the risk of injury associated with direct aggressive interactions. However, this hypothesis has not been directly tested. Here we used a speaker occupation experiment to test whether red squirrel rattles function to deter conspecific rivals. We studied 29 male squirrels and removed each individual from his territory twice in a paired design. During the experimental treatment, we simulated the owner's presence after its removal by broadcasting the owner's rattle from a loudspeaker at the centre of the territory once every 7 min. During the control treatment, the territory was left in silence following the temporary removal of the owner. We found that the presence of a speaker replacement reduced the probability of intrusion by 34% and increased the latency to first intrusion by 7%, providing support for the hypothesis that rattles play an active role in reducing intrusion risk. However, intrusions were not completely averted by the speaker replacement, indicating that for some individuals vocalizations alone are not a sufficient deterrent without other cues of the territory owner.
... Our results, obtained using a speaker occupation design, are the first to experimentally have demonstrated the importance of acoustic signalling for territory defence and retention (Krebs,295 1977; Krebs et al., 1978;Falls, 1988;Myrberg, 1997;Nowicki et al., 1998;Pereira et al., 2013). 296 However, while in red squirrels the number of intrusions was clearly reduced by the rattle playback, 297 the presence of the speaker replacement alone was not 100% effective in deterring intrusions. ...
... Painted gobies approached 'occupied' nests 319 more frequently than silent nests but then demonstrated avoidance after approaching the agonistic 320 calls. However, when the gobies were unable to associate the sound with the physical presence of a 321 territory holder they proceeded to intrude on the 'occupied' territory (Pereira et al., 2013). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In many species, territory defense is thought to be one of the primary functions of acoustic communication. North American red squirrels are a territorial species in which 'rattles' have long been thought to be the principal signal communicating territory ownership. These vocalizations have been assumed to deter intruders, thus reducing energy costs and the risk of injury associated with direct aggressive interactions. However, this hypothesis has not been directly tested. Here we used a speaker occupation experiment to test whether red squirrel rattles function to deter conspecific rivals. We studied 29 male squirrels and removed each individual from his territory twice in a paired design. During the experimental treatment we simulated the owner's presence after its removal by broadcasting the owner's rattle from a loudspeaker at the center of the territory once every seven minutes. During the control treatment the territory was left in silence after the temporary removal of the owner. We found that the presence of a speaker replacement reduced the probability of intrusion by 34% and increased the latency to first intrusion by 7%, providing support for the hypothesis that rattles play an active role in reducing intrusion risk. However, intrusions were not completely averted by the speaker replacement, indicating that vocalizations alone are not sufficient without other cues of the territory owner.
... Tests of playback quality were only reported for the prototype system at low amplitude levels and short distances of up to 12 cm; it remains to be seen if larger models of this system will perform well for playback of higher amplitude sounds at farther distances. The electromagnetic playback system was not tested in this study, but it has been used very successfully for fish playback experiments specifically using low frequency, pulsed, fish sounds at close distance (Pereira et al., 2014;Amorim et al., 2013;Wackermannova et al., 2017;Chabrolles et al., 2017;Amorim et al., 2019). This speaker system has the best potential if it becomes widely available. ...
Article
Acoustic playback is a key method used to determine the behavioral significance of animal sounds, including fishes. This study presents the first comparison of the acoustic quality of underwater speakers for the playback of fish sounds. Seven underwater acoustic playback systems were tested for their ability to accurately reproduce the low frequency, pulsed, courtship sounds of a small fish, Tramitichromis intermedius (Cichlidae). Results indicated that in an aquarium with low ambient noise and at low amplitude playback levels (<120 dB re 1 μPa), the Clark Synthesis speakers were the best choice for playback at moderate distances (>20 cm), and that the Electro-Voice UW30 was the best speaker for short distance (<20 cm) playback of low frequency fish sounds. However, in aquaria with higher levels of ambient noise and at higher amplitude playback levels, the Clark Synthesis speakers performed best. However, none of these speaker systems reproduced a high-fidelity quality fish sound. It is important when using underwater speakers for behavioral studies that there is a careful assessment of the played back sound and comparison to the original sound.
... Fish use sounds (production and/or detection) in multiple ways (Bass & Ladich 2008;Ladich 2013;Popper & Hawkins 2019): specific sounds can be used during migrations and as directional orientation clues for larvae to settle (Tolimieri et al. 2004) but mostly sounds are received and produced to acquire and transmit information. Many fish species actively produce sounds to communicate the presence of predators or food to their conspecifics (Bessey & Heithaus 2013), during aggressive behaviours (Millot et al. 2011), to defend their territories (Myrberg 1997;Pereira et al. 2014) and to attract mates: indeed, a very important context during which teleosts vocalise is courtship (e.g., Amorim & Neves 2007;Parmentier et al. 2010;Amorim et al. 2012;Maruska et al. 2012;Casaretto et al. 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
The deleterious effects of anthropogenic noise on animal communication are nowadays recognised, not only in urban environments but also in terrestrial habitats and along coasts and in open waters. Yet, the assessment of short- and long-term exposure consequences of anthropogenic noise in marine organisms remains challenging, especially in fish and invertebrates. Males of the Mediterranean damselfish Chromis chromis vocalise and perform visual displays (multimodal communication) to attract mates. The frequency-range of courtship vocalisations overlaps with low-frequency noise generated by maritime activities, resulting in a reduced detection distance among conspecifics. We quantified the number of courtship-related visual displays performed by males living in areas with different levels of maritime traffic. We also tried to manipulate ambient noise in the field to test male short-term response to increased noise levels. Males living in busier areas (near to a harbour) performed significantly more visual displays than those living in less congested areas. When exposed to artificially-increased ambient noise level (playback of boat noise), males did not adjust the number of visual displays accordingly. Yet, we note how assessing the actual effect of maritime traffic in marine populations in their natural environments is particularly difficult, as the effects of boat noise cannot be easily disentangled from a variety of other intrinsic or environmental factors, discussed in the paper. We thus present suggestions to obtain more robust analyses of variations of courtship behaviours in territorial fishes. We hope this will facilitate a further understanding of the potential long-term effects of anthropogenic noise, whose analyses should be prioritised in the context of environmental impact assessment, resource management and biodiversity conservation.
... For example, the male Lusitanian toadfish (Halobatrachus didactylus), may use boatwhistle vocalisations as a warning signal against other males [16]. Also, playback experiments of conspecific sounds in the absence of a resident male have been shown to play a territory guarding role against intrusions from potential competitors in the bicolour damselfish, Stegastes paritus [17], and in the painted goby, Pomatoschistus pictus [18]. However, studies on the function of sounds in territorial or resource defence are far and few between for heterospecific species, in particular where the vocalizations of known predators induces flight in potential prey. ...
Article
Full-text available
Studies on the behavioural function of sounds are very rare within heterospecific interactions. John Dory (Zeus faber) is a solitary, predatory fish that produces sound when captured, but has not been documented to vocalize under natural conditions (i.e. in the wild). The present study provides the first in-situ recordings of John Dory vocalisations and correlates them to behavioural response of snapper (Pagrus auratus) a common species found through New Zealand. Vocalisations or ‘barks’, ranged between 200–600 Hz, with a peak frequency of 312 ± 10 Hz and averaged 139 ± 4 milliseconds in length. Baited underwater video (BUV) equipped with hydrophones determined that under natural conditions a John Dory vocalization induced an escape response in snapper present, causing them to exit the area opposite to the position of the John Dory. We speculate that the John Dory vocalisation may be used for territorial display towards both conspecifics and heterospecifics, asserting dominance in the area or heightening predatory status.
Article
The meeting of congeners to form aggregations, groups or shoals and the meeting of male and female spawners during the breeding season are strategies too serious to be left to chance alone. Rather, they result from the exchange of precise signals that are sufficiently informative to encourage appropriate encounters that allow the formation of complementary and well‐matched couples that condition true sexual selection. Visual signals are the most convenient and the fastest, and they are therefore the most commonly used in light environments. Fish use chemical messages from their physical and social environment through a very acute sense of smell based on olfactory receptors located in the epithelium of their nasal cavities. Parasitic infestations disrupt fish's motor and sensory systems and reduce their ability to form groups. Social relationships are particularly developed during fish reproduction, both at the beginning of the process and then during the monitoring and protection of eggs and larvae.
Article
Full-text available
Papers on sexual selection often highlight the incredible diversity of sexually selected traits across animals. Yet, few studies have tried to explain why this diversity evolved. Animals use many different types of traits to attract mates and outcompete rivals, including colours, songs, and horns, but it remains unclear why, for example, some taxa have songs, others have colours, and others horns. Here, we first conduct a systematic survey of the basic diversity and distribution of different types of sexually selected signals and weapons across the animal Tree of Life. Based on this survey, we describe seven major patterns in trait diversity and distributions. We then discuss 10 unanswered questions raised by these patterns, and how they might be addressed. One major pattern is that most types of sexually selected signals and weapons are apparently absent from most animal phyla (88%), in contrast to the conventional wisdom that a diversity of sexually selected traits is present across animals. Furthermore, most trait diversity is clustered in Arthropoda and Chordata, but only within certain clades. Within these clades, many different types of traits have evolved, and many types appear to have evolved repeatedly. By contrast, other major arthropod and chordate clades appear to lack all or most trait types, and similar patterns are repeated at smaller phylogenetic scales (e.g. within insects). Although most research on sexual selection focuses on female choice, we find similar numbers of traits (among sampled species) are involved in male contests (44%) and female choice (55%). Overall, these patterns are largely unexplained and unexplored, as are many other fundamental questions about the evolution of these traits. We suggest that understanding the diversity of sexually selected traits may require a shift towards macroevolutionary studies at relatively deep timescales (e.g. tens to hundreds of millions of years ago).
Thesis
Full-text available
As the first part of my thesis, I provide a computer program which allows users to determine under what circumstances they should measure particle motion; I explain how these measurements can be made and provide a program for analysing this type of data. The main part of my thesis comprises experiments investigating the impact of repeated exposure to traffic noise, the most common anthropogenic source of noise in the marine environment, on fishes and invertebrates during development. In all three chapters involving experiments on fish in tanks and in the field, I found that a variety of behaviours were impacted by traffic noise playback. I also found that predictable noise can lead to different impacts on development compared to unpredictable noise, but that some species of fish may be able to habituate to traffic noise, while others suffer lower survival. Further, I found that the development and survival of seahares can be negatively impacted by traffic-noise playback. Fishes and invertebrates provide a vital food source to millions of people and form crucial links in many food webs; studying their behaviour, development and fitness can give us an insight into population and community level impacts of noise that are relevant to species survival and evolution. Developing some of the novel ideas and techniques discussed in this thesis will enable us to advance this vital area of research.
Article
The aquatic environment is increasingly bombarded by a wide variety of noise pollutants, whose range and intensity are increasing with each passing decade. Yet little is known about how aquatic noise affects marine communities. To determine the implications that changes to the soundscape may have on fishes, a meta-analysis was conducted focusing on the ramifications of noise on fish behavior and physiology. Our meta-analysis identified 42 studies that produced 2,354 data points, which in turn indicated that anthropogenic noise negatively affects fish behavior and physiology. The most predominate responses occurred within foraging ability, predation risk, and reproductive success. Additionally, anthropogenic noise was shown to increase the hearing thresholds and cortisol levels of numerous species while tones, biological, and environmental noise were most likely to affect complex movements and swimming abilities. These findings suggest that the majority of fish species are sensitive to changes in the aquatic soundscape, and depending on the noise source, species responses may have extreme and negative fitness consequences. As such this global synthesis should serve as a warning of the potentially dire consequences facing marine ecosystems if alterations to aquatic soundscapes continue on their current trajectory. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
Aquatic noise has the potential to travel extreme distances and as such many marine species rely on the soundscape for auditory information regarding habitat selection, predator or prey locations, and communication. These species not only take advantage of the prevailing sounds but also contribute to the soundscape through their own vocalizations. Certain sounds have the potential to negatively effect marine species resulting in unbalanced predator-prey interactions and disrupted communication. In an attempt to determine the implications that changes to the soundscape may have on fishes, we conducted a meta-analysis focusing on how anthropogenic and biological noises may alter fish behavior and physiology. We reviewed 3,174 potentially relevant papers of which 44 met our criteria and were used in the analysis. Results indicated that anthropogenic noise has an adverse effect on marine and freshwater fish behavior and physiology. Alternatively biological and environmental noises did not significantly alter fish behavior and physiology. These findings suggest that although certain species may be more susceptible to anthropogenic noise than others, the vast majority of fish have the potential to be negatively affected by noise pollution, while biological noises may not have the same negative consequences for fish behavior and physiology.
Article
Full-text available
Fishes have evolved the largest diversity of sonic organs among vertebrates. The main group of sound producing mechanisms is based on the swimbladder. These can be vibrated by intrinsic drumming muscles located in the wall of the swimbladder (toadfishes, searobins), or by extrinsic drumming muscles, which originate on structures such as the skull, vertebral processes or body wall musculature. Extrinsic drumming muscles insert either directly on the swimbladder (e.g. pimelodid catfish, tiger perches) or vibrate the swimbladder indirectly either via broad tendons (piranhas, drums) or via bony plates (elastic springs in doradid, mochokids and ariid catfishes). Pectoral sound-producing mechanisms include vibration of the pectoral girdle (sculpins), rubbing of the enhanced pectoral spine in a groove of the shoulder girdle (catfishes), and plucking of enhanced fin tendons (croaking gouramis). In addition, sounds can be produced by other morphological structures such as dorsal fin spines, neck vertebrae and pharyngeal teeth grating. In a few taxa, such as catfishes, two different sound-producing mechanisms (swimbladder and pectoral) are present simultaneously. In several other well-known vocalizing taxa (damsel fishes, gobies, loaches) the mechanisms remain unidentified. Sound-generating mechanisms may be similarly developed in males and females (croaking gourami) or sexually dimorphic, in which case they are always better developed in males. In toadfishes males possess a relatively higher sonic muscle mass than females, whereas in some drum species muscles are totally absent in females. In the midshipman Porichthys notatus, territorial males possess larger sonic muscles than parasitic sneaker males, which steal fertilizations. In drums sonic musculature hypertrophies seasonally, a process apparently controlled by the hormone testosterone.
Article
Full-text available
Fish sound characteristics are associated with different sound-generating mechanisms. Sounds produced by swimbladder-related mechanisms usually comprise low-frequency pulses produced at different rates. Fishes emit one to five sound types that do not show such outstanding variability as found in other taxa. However, closely related species show consistent differences in their sounds and in some species even individuality is found. Of particular interest are differences in courtship sounds made by closely related sympatric species that may promote reproductive isolation. Differences between individuals of the same species may in turn play a role in sexual selection through male-male competition and female mate choice. Other known sources of variability are related to context, including motivation and recent social status, season, time of day, ontogenetic changes and sexual dimorphism. Fish sound variability is mainly based on temporal patterning of sounds or pulses within a sound and on frequency variation (sometimes modulation). Such variability has been found to play a role in the social life of fishes.
Article
Field and laboratory observations were made on the reproductive behavior, ecology and sounds produced by males of Notropis analostanus during reproductive behavior. Based on these observations, experiments were performed on sound communication during the reproductive behavior of this species. Reproductive activities began in middle to late May when the males set up territories around a suitable egg site. The males defended these territories by chasing and fighting behavior which included the lateral threat display. During this aggressive behavior, "single knocks" and "rapid series of knocks" were produced. Courtship behavior began when a female approached the territory and the male swam out to her. If the female did not swim away, the male swam in circles around her and then swam to the egg site where it would perform "solo-spawning" motions. Spawning was accomplished when the female followed the male to the egg site and assumed a spawning position with the male in a dorsolateral position to her. During the courtship behavior the male produced "single knocks" and "purrs." The functions of sounds produced by male Notropis analostanus during reproductive behavior were studied by playing back these sounds under experimental conditions designed to clarify their functions as communicative signals. The following conclusions were reached: 1. The "single knocks" were not effective in influencing aggressive behavior of males nor did they promote courtship. 2. The "rapid series of knocks" stimulated aggressive behavior by a dominant male and inhibited the entry of a submissive male into the territory. 3. The "rapid series of knocks" inhibited the courtship behavior of the male and reduced cooperation by the female during courtship. 4. The "purrs" increased the courtship behavior of the male and promoted the cooperation of the female during courtship.
Article
Frühere Arbeiten über die Lautproduktion von Cichlasoma centrarchus deuteten darauf hin, daß Laute aggressives Verhalten der Fische untereinander verringern (Schwarz 1974). Diese Hypothese wurde experimentell geprüft. Test-♂♂ bekamen gleichzeitig den Anblick eines Artgenossen und entweder Schweigen (S) oder vom Band gespielte Kontrollaute (CN) oder Fischlaute (FN) geboten; dann wurde die Zahl der darauf folgenden hoch aggressiven (HAE) und schwach aggressiven (LAE) Begegnungen bestimmt. Vorspielen der CN änderte die Anzahl der HAE nicht, Vorspielen der FN verringerte sie erheblich. FN hemmte HAE bei ♂♂ mehr als bei ♀♀. Da die ♀♀ Laute produzieren, die denen der ♂♂ in Art und Wirkung ähneln (Schwarz in prep.), kann man dieses Ergebnis durch gelegentliches nichtaggressives, optisch auffallendes Verhalten der ♀♀ zum Teil erklären.
Article
Three playback experiments with a freshwater goby, Padogobius martensii, examined the effects of recent aggressive experience on the response of the territorial fish to the aggressive sound alone. Experiments 1 and 2 were conducted on a first group of fifteen resident males (i.e. males individually housed within laboratory tanks for at least 5 days). Experiment 1 simply consisted of playing back the sound to the resident male not exposed to social stimulation for at least 48 h. In experiment 2 the same male was exposed to its mirror image before the start of playback. Results showed that in experiment 1 males ignored or even tended to avoid the site of sound stimulation, whereas in experiment 2 they attended the sound site more frequently during the sound playback. In both experiments the mirror and the loudspeaker were placed on the same side of the tank. The third playback experiment was therefore developed to include mirror and loudspeaker on opposite sides of the tank. A different group of sixteen males was used for these experiments. Results showed that the sound playback may be more important than the mirror site in directing the approach response of the male. Furthermore, the magnitude of sound interference was positively correlated with the level of mirror aggressiveness shown by the male before playback. Functional implications of the responses to the aggressive sound alone for the territorial fish are discussed.
Article
1.1. A brief description of the reproductive behaviour of Notropis analostanus was given,2.2. The sounds produced by fighting males increased the occurrence and average duration of the aggressive behaviour of reproductively mature males.3.3. The sounds produced when males court females increased the occurrence of aggressive behaviour between males, but did not increase the average duration of aggressive behaviour. “Solo-spawning” increased when the courtship sounds were played back, even though no females were present.4.4. The courtship sounds when played back to 1 male and 2 females increased the occurrence and average duration, of courtship behaviour.5.5. The fighting sounds when played back to 1 male and 2 females did not increase the occurrence and average duration of courtship behaviour.6.6. Males are able to discriminate between the 2 sounds.7.7. No orientation to the sound source was encountered.