ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

People often shop when feeling sad, but whether and why shopping reduces residual (lingering) sadness remains an open question. Sadness is strongly associated with a sense that situational forces control the outcomes in one’s life, and thus we theorized that the choices inherent in shopping may restore personal control over one’s environment and reduce residual sadness. Three experiments provided support for our hypothesis. Making shopping choices helped to alleviate sadness whether they were hypothetical (Experiment 1) or real (Experiment 2). In addition, all experiments found support for the underlying mechanism of personal control restoration. Notably, the benefits of restored personal control over one’s environment do not generalize to anger (Experiments 2 and 3), because anger is associated with a sense that other people (rather than situational forces) are likely to cause negative outcomes, and these appraisals are not ameliorated by restoring personal control over one’s environment.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Research Report
The benets of retail therapy: Making purchase decisions
reduces residual sadness
Scott I. Rick , Beatriz Pereira, Katherine A. Burson
Ross School of Business, University of Michigan, 701 Tappan Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Received 14 August 2012; received in revised form 10 December 2013; accepted 22 December 2013
Available online 27 December 2013
Abstract
People often shop when feeling sad, but whether and why shopping reduces residual (lingering) sadness remains an open question. Sadness is
strongly associated with a sense that situational forces control the outcomes in one's life, and thus we theorized that the choices inherent in shopping
may restore personal control over one's environment and reduce residual sadness. Three experiments provided support for our hypothesis. Making
shopping choices helped to alleviate sadness whether they were hypothetical (Experiment 1) or real (Experiment 2). In addition, all experiments found
support for the underlying mechanism of personal control restoration. Notably, the benets of restored personal control over one's environment do not
generalize to anger (Experiments 2 and 3), because anger is associated with a sense that other people (rather than situational forces) are likely to cause
negative outcomes, and these appraisals are not ameliorated by restoring personal control over one's environment.
© 2013 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Retail therapy; Shopping; Appraisal tendency theory; Sadness; Decision-making
Introduction
How do people regulate distress? Several common responses
to distress have been documented, such as rumination, overeat-
ing, and alcohol consumption. Distress can also encourage
unplanned purchases (e.g., Atalay & Meloy, 2011, Study 1).
Shopping that is motivated by distress—“retail therapy”—is
often lamented as ineffective, wasteful, and a dark sideof
consumer behavior (Kasser & Sheldon, 2000). Popular press
accounts of retail therapy typically paint an equally dismal picture
(Tuttle, 2010).
We propose that retail therapy has been viewed too negatively.
Shopping may be an effective way to minimize sadness that
lingers (residual sadness) following a sadness-inducing event.
We focus on shopping's potential to reduce residual sadness in
particular, as previous research has demonstrated that sadness
increases comfort-seeking (Raghunathan, Pham, & Corfman,
2006) and willingness-to-pay (Cryder, Lerner, Gross, & Dahl,
2008; Lerner, Small, & Loewenstein, 2004).
Prior research has provided some suggestive evidence that
shopping can convey psychological benefits (Gardner & Rook,
1988). In a diary study, Atalay and Meloy (2011, Study 3)
found that most participants reported positive feelings when
reflecting on their most recent purchase that was motivated by a
desire to repair mood. Faber and Christenson (1996, Table 3)
found that people recalled that they were less likely to
experience sadness while shopping than immediately before
going shopping.
However, causal conclusions remain elusive, as no prior
research investigating the influence of shopping on emotion
or mood has utilized experimental designs. Without random
assignment to shopping or equally engaging control
activities, it is unclear whether shopping conveys benefits
beyond those produced merely by distraction or the passage
of time.
In addition, research in this area has only loosely conceptu-
alized both affect and shopping. Atalay and Meloy (2011) utilized
broad measures of mood (p. 642) and positive emotion and
negative emotion indices (p. 653), rather than investigating the
experience of specific emotions. Faber and Christenson (1996,
p. 809) asked participants to report how they generally feel
while shopping,without referencing any specific shopping
Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: srick@umich.edu (S.I. Rick), bpereira@umich.edu
(B. Pereira), kburson@umich.edu (K.A. Burson).
1057-7408/$ -see front matter © 2013 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2013.12.004
Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
ScienceDirect
Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373 380
episode. Because shoppingcan have many components, in-
cluding browsing, interacting with salespeople, choosing, paying,
acquiring, and consuming, retrospective reports about shopping
cannot shed light on which component(s) are necessary for
healing to occur.
This last point is not merely a descriptive shortcoming.
Differences in the effectiveness of specific components could
shed light on why shopping reduces residual sadness. To develop
hypotheses about why some components will be particularly
influential, we consider sadness from an appraisal tendency theory
perspective (Han, Lerner, & Keltner, 2007). Appraisal theory
suggests that the way people cognitively appraise their environ-
ment is both a cause and consequence of different emotions.
Smith and Ellsworth (1985) identified six appraisals that
differentiate emotions: the extent to which the current situation
is pleasant, predictable, demanding of attention, demanding of
effort, under human (versus situational) control, and under one's
own or other people's control. Thus, similarly valenced emotions
can differ on other important dimensions (e.g., anger and fear are
both aversive, but anger is associated with greater certainty;
Lerner & Keltner, 2001).
Sadness, more than any other emotion, is associated with a
perceived deficiency in personal control over one's environment
(Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). People who are sad are especially
likely to view outcomes as governed by situational forces and
chance, rather than their own actions. To the extent that these
appraisals create or maintain the experience of sadness (Han et al.,
2007), aspects of shopping that restore a sense of personal control
over one's environment may subsequently reduce residual
sadness. Indeed, Garg and Lerner (2013, p. 112) proposed that
researchers should investigate whether feeling less helpless
correspond[s] with feeling less sad.
Prior research suggests that the ability to choose tends to
enhance one's sense of personal control (Inesi, Botti, Dubois,
Rucker, & Galinsky, 2011; Langer, 1975). Because choices are
inherent to shopping (e.g., choosing whether to buy), shopping
may restore a sense of control and thus minimize residual
sadness.
Of course, aside from choice, other aspects of the shopping
experience could influence sadness. For example, shopping
may provide distraction (cf. Kim & Rucker, 2012) or social
interaction (O'Guinn & Faber, 1989). In what follows, we
experimentally isolate the influence of choice on the experience
of sadness by utilizing simplified paradigms that necessarily
strip away extraneous factors that can accompany naturalistic
shopping. For example, there is no consumption or social
interaction in our experiments. We control for the benefits of
distraction in Experiment 1 by including a browsingcontrol
condition, in which participants must interact with products but
cannot buy any.
We focus on choice for two reasons. First, choice is the
component of shopping that is most theoretically linked to
personal control. Given that sadness is characterized by a lack
of personal control over one's environment, the control imbued
by making shopping choices may help reduce residual sadness.
Second, we focus on choice because it is arguably the most
fundamental component of shopping. While shopping may or
may not involve factors not present in our experiments (e.g.,
social interaction), shopping always involves choice.
We propose thatmaking shopping choices can help to restore a
sense of personal control over one's environment, but many
people may have difficulty quantifying and articulating the extent
to which they feel control over their environment. (In their classic
demonstration, SmithandEllsworth(1985,p.820)utilized a
group of participants pre-screened to be highly emotionally
expressive, and asked them to recall their experiences of control
during a specific emotional event, rather than their current,
ambient feelings of control over their environment.) Thus, inwhat
follows we shed light on our proposed process by experimentally
manipulating personal control (cf. Spencer, Zanna, & Fong,
2005). We do so by manipulating whether participants can freely
choose among a broad product assortment (Experiment 1),
whether participants believe they can ensure that they obtain
their preferred product (Experiment 2), and whether participants
recall an instance of high or low control over their environment
(Experiment 3).
Experiment 1
Experiment 1 tested the hypothesis that making shopping
choices helps to restore personal control over one's environ-
ment, which can in turn help to alleviate residual sadness. We
randomly assigned participants to choose which of several
products they would hypothetically buy, or to judge which of
those products would be most useful when traveling. Concep-
tually, our intention was to manipulate the extent to which
participants could exercise personal autonomy during the task
(since only a handful of the products are appropriate for travel,
but any could be selected by hypothetical buyers), while
holding constant distraction and (lack of) product acquisition
across conditions.
Method
One hundred adults (52% female, mean age: 36) participated
in an online study for a small payment. We recruited participants
via Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), a platform validated by
Paolacci, Chandler, and Ipeirotis (2010). We initially collected a
baseline measure of emotions. Specifically, participants indicated
the extent to which they were currently experiencing seven
different emotions (amused,sad,indifferent,angry,depressed,
happy,rage), by moving a slider along a 12 mm line anchored by
the labels not at alland very much.Responses were scored on
a0100 scale based on where participants rested the slider.
Participants then viewed a three-minute clip from The Champ
portraying the death of a boy's father, which reliably induces
sadness (Rottenberg,Ray,&Gross,2007). We then took a
second measure of emotions, identical to our baseline measure.
We then randomly assigned participants to a Choosing or
Browsing condition, adapting a design by Mazar and Zhong
(2010). Choosers were told to imagine buying $100 worth of
products, by placing them in a shopping cart.Choosers then
viewed 12 products (e.g., slippers, headphones; see Fig. 1),
each priced at $25. Choosers were asked to select four products
374 S.I. Rick et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373380
they would hypothetically like to buy, by clicking on four
products and dragging them into a box labeled Your Shopping
Cart.Choosers were informed that the shopping was
hypothetical, and they had no expectation of obtaining these
items.
Browsers viewed the same 12 products and were asked to
judge which four products would be most useful when traveling,
by clicking on four products and dragging them into a box labeled
Travel Items.There was no significant difference in the amount
of time spent on the Choosing and Browsing tasks (60.7 s vs.
58.2 s; t(98) b1).
We then administered a third (and final) measure of
emotions, identical to our baseline measure.
We conducted a pre-test on MTurk (N = 42, 43% female,
mean age: 33) to confirm that the Choosing task was more likely
to generate feelings of control than the Browsing task.
Participants completed both the Choosing and Browsing tasks
(which were labeled the Shopping Cart task and the Travel Items
task, respectively, for participants). The order of tasks was
counterbalanced across participants. We then asked, In which
task did you feel you had more control over the items you
selected?Participants selected one of five options: Definitely the
Travel Items task, Probably the Travel Items task, No difference
between the Shopping Cart and Travel Items tasks, Probably the
Shopping Cart task, and Definitely the Shopping Cart task.
Seventy-nine percent of participants reported that they (probably
or definitely) felt more control while Choosing, whereas only 2%
reported that they (probably or definitely) felt more control while
Browsing (χ
2
(1) = 50.6, pb.0001). Thus, while both tasks
likely generate, to some extent, a sense of personal control over
one's environment, the Choosing task is a more effective way to
increase a sense of personal control.
Results
To verify that our manipulation worked as intended, we
examined whether the selection of products reflected greater
autonomy among Choosers. Free choice among options imbues
a sense of control and autonomy by allowing people to
implement or reveal their individual preferences. Browsers
Fig. 1. Products utilized in Experiment 1.
375S.I. Rick et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373380
were asked to select the products most appropriate for travel,
but only a handful of the products were appropriate for travel.
By contrast, Choosers were free to select any product.
Choosers' greater ability to control which products they select
should be reflected by greater variance in their selections.
Fig. 2 displays the proportion of participants selecting each
product by condition. There was significantly greater variance
in product selection among Choosers than Browsers (F(1,22) =
4.47, pb.05, Levene's test). If Browsers could freely choose
to the same extent as Choosers, there would be a similar degree
of variance in selected products across conditions. The unequal
variance in product selections across conditions is consistent
with the notion that Choosers experienced more control during the
task.
We next tested our central hypothesis that choosing would be
more likely than browsing to alleviate residual sadness. Sad and
depressed ratings correlated highly (r(98) N.65, pb.0001 at
each measurement), and were averaged to form a sadness index at
each measurement. We created a residual sadness score by
subtracting participants' baseline index scores from their final
index scores. This commonly-used method controls for broad
individual differences in the tendency to experience and express
emotions (cf. Kermer, Driver-Linn, Wilson, & Gilbert, 2006;
Oveis et al., 2009; Rogosa & Willett, 1983; Wilson, Wheatley,
Meyers, Gilbert, & Axsom, 2000). Three outliers, with residual
sadness scores more than three standard deviations from the
mean, were excluded.
As expected, residual sadness scores were significantly lower
among Choosers than Browsers (M=2.9,SD = 8.6 vs. M=8.1,
SD =14.5; t(95) = 2.13, pb.05). In other words, making
hypothetical buying choices was more likely to return partici-
pants to their baseline level of sadness than was browsing.
Thus, Experiment 1 provides initial support for the hypothesis
that making shopping choices helps to alleviate residual sadness.
The results of the pre-test and the greater variance in product
selections among Choosers suggest that the mechanism under-
lying this effect is a restoration of personal control.
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 extends our investigation in three ways. First,
all participants made real (consequential) shopping decisions.
Second, to isolate the role of restored control in retail therapy, all
participants actually obtained their preferred option, but we
manipulated the extent to which they had apparent control over
the process of obtaining that option. Third, to shed additional
light on the process of personal control restoration, we
examined whether the benefits of making shopping choices
are specific to sadness or generalize to other negative emotions.
In particular, we examined whether making shopping choices
also helps to alleviate anger. Anger is generally as aversive as
sadness, but is associated with a sense that other people (rather
than situational forces) cause negative outcomes (Smith &
Ellsworth, 1985). Whereas making shopping choices may help
restore a sense of personal control over one's environment,
those choices are unlikely to reduce the extent to which other
people are viewed as unfairly or unduly influential. Thus,
enhancing control over one's environment should have more
impact on sadness than on anger.
Method
One hundred forty-seven undergraduates from a Midwestern
university participated for course credit. Six participants who
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
Head-
phones
Gel
Mask
Slippers Keyboard Speakers Massager Puzzle Apples
Game
Pet
Lounge
Salt &
Pepper
Decanter Cheese
Board
Proportion of
participants
who selected
the product
Browsers
Choosers
Fig. 2. Proportion of participants selecting each product by condition (Experiment 1).
376 S.I. Rick et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373380
failed to follow instructions (e.g., texting during the experi-
ment) were excluded. We initially collected a baseline measure
of eight emotions (sad,indifferent,angry,happy,depressed,
enraged,amused,neutral)on0100 scales.
We then induced either sadness or anger. In the Sadness
condition, participants viewed the same clip from The Champ
used in Experiment 1. In the Anger condition, participants viewed
a2.5-minuteclipfromCry Freedom that portrays young,
unarmed protesters being gunned down by opposition forces.
This clip reliably induces anger (Rottenberg et al., 2007).
Following the emotion induction, participants were told that
they would be given a $5 spending budget as part of a real
shopping experience. They were told that they could buy one of
three products (a set of post-it notes, a set of highlighters, or a set
of ball point pens), all offered in the lab for $5 (approximately
equal to their actual retail prices). They were also told that they
could choose to buy none of these products, and trade in their
spending budget for $1 in real cash. We disincentivized not
buying because walking away with the full $5 budget would
have been an easy choice for most participants (as they did not
come to the lab intending to stock up on office supplies). This
ensured that there was engagement in the shopping task and that
choices were actively considered. Our sadness versus anger
induction did not significantly influence the proportion of
participants choosing to buy one of the products (62% vs. 74%,
χ
2
(1) = 2.46, pN.10). We pooled across the buyer versus
non-buyer distinction in our analyses, since both actively made
a choice.
We also embedded a manipulation of personal agency in the
shopping task by adapting a procedure validated by Berman and
Small (2012). Specifically, we told participants that, after they
made their choice, the computer would randomly draw a number
from 1 to 10. If the randomly generated number was even, they
would simply obtain whatever they chose. However, if the
randomly generated number was odd, the computer would ignore
their choice and randomly make a selection on their behalf. Note
that there were four possible choices in our paradigm buying
the post-it notes, buying the highlighters, buying the pens, or
trading in the spending budget for $1 in real cash. Regardless of
participants' own choice, if the randomly generated number was
odd, participants believed that the computer could randomly
select any one of these four options on their behalf.
In the Personal Control condition, the randomly selected
number was even, and participants were told that they would
obtain what they chose. In the Situational Control condition,
the randomly selected number was odd, and then the computer
ostensibly made a random selection on their behalf (in fact,
always selecting the option participants had selected for
themselves). Thus, at the end of this task and before collecting
our final measure of emotions, all participants knew that they
would obtain the option they preferred. All that varied across
the Personal Control versus Situational Control conditions was
the amount of control participants believed they had over the
process.
We then administered a second (and final) measure of
emotions, identical to our baseline measure. Finally, partici-
pants either obtained their selected product or $1 in cash.
Results
Sad and depressed ratings correlated highly (r(139) N.42,
pb.0001 at each measurement), and were averaged to form a
sadness index at each measurement. Angry and enraged ratings
correlated highly (r(139) N.56, pb.0001 at each measure-
ment), and were averaged to form an anger index at each
measurement. We created residual sadness and anger scores by
subtracting participants' baseline index scores from their final
index scores.
Fig. 3 displays the focal residual emotion in each condition (i.e.,
residual sadness in the Sadness conditions and residual anger in the
Anger conditions). As predicted, residual sadness scores in the
Sadness conditions were significantly higher in the Situational
Control condition than in the Personal Control condition (M=3.73,
SD = 10.78 vs. M=2.28, SD = 11.79; t(69) = 2.24, pb.05).
However, residual anger scores in the Anger conditions
did not differ significantly between the Situational Control
and Personal Control conditions (M= 14.75, SD = 25.69 vs.
M=14.17, SD = 30.91; t(68) = .09, p=.93).
1
Experiment 2 suggests that real shopping can help to
alleviate residual sadness, unless that shopping experience
further reduces personal control over situational forces. By
contrast, restoring personal control is as ineffective as further
jeopardizing personal control at alleviating residual anger.
Anger is naturally associated with a sense that other people are
likely to cause negative outcomes, and thus restoring control
over ambient environmental forces does not appear to address
the key control deficit associated with anger.
Experiment 3
Experiment 3 extends our investigation by focusing more
closely on the underlying mechanism of personal control
restoration. Specifically, we examine whether a direct manipu-
lation of control over one's environment (outside of a shopping
context) is more likely to influence sadness than anger.
Method
Three hundred and one undergraduates from a Southern
university (51% female) participated for course credit. We
initially collected a baseline measure of nine emotions (happy,
angry,sad,indifferent,enraged,depressed,amused,anxious,
and neutral)on0100 scales.
1
It is worth noting that the Situational Control versus Personal Control
manipulation did not operate by inuencing happiness, despite the fact that
residual happiness (nal happy rating minus baseline happy rating) was
signicantly correlated with both residual sadness (r(139) = .48, pb.0001)
and residual anger (r(139) = .28, pb.001), pooling across conditions.
Residual happiness in the Sadness conditions did not differ signicantly
between the Situational Control and Personal Control conditions (M= 7.02,
SD = 28.22 vs. M= 4.61, SD = 25.49; t(69) = .38, p= .71). Likewise,
residual happiness in the Anger conditions did not differ signicantly between
the Situational Control and Personal Control conditions (M= 7.83, SD = 33.38
vs. M= 17.15, SD = 29.96; t(68) = 1.23, p= .22).
377S.I. Rick et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373380
We then induced sadness, anger, or neutral emotion. We
included a Neutral condition to examine whether our subse-
quent control manipulation (discussed below) generated any
emotional costs or benefits even in the absence of a negative
emotion induction. In the Sadness condition, participants
viewed the clip from The Champ used in previous experiments.
In the Anger condition, participants viewed a four-minute clip
from My Bodyguard that portrays a bullying incident. This clip
reliably induces anger (Rottenberg et al., 2007). In the Neutral
condition, participants viewed a 2.5-minute clip from National
Geographic about coral reefs (cf. Lerner et al., 2004).
Following the emotion induction, we asked participants to
either recall an instance in which they experienced control over
an important situation (Personal Control condition) or experi-
enced no control over an important situation (Situational
Control condition). We carefully worded our recall prompts to
prevent participants from considering instances in which they
had control over other people, or were controlled by other
people (see Appendix).
We then administered a second (and final) measure of
emotions, identical to our baseline measure.
Results
Sad and depressed ratings correlated highly (r(299) N.67,
pb.0001 at each measurement), and were averaged to form a
sadness index at each measurement. Angry and enraged ratings
correlated highly (r(299) N.73, pb.0001 at each measure-
ment), and were averaged to form an anger index at each
measurement. We created residual sadness and anger scores by
subtracting participants' baseline index scores from their final
index scores.
Fig. 4 displays the focal residual emotion in the Sadness and
Anger conditions. As predicted, residual sadness scores in the
Sadness conditions were significantly higher in the Situational
Control condition than in the Personal Control condition (M=
12.1, SD =14.8vs.M=2.89,SD = 16.3; t(99) = 2.97, pb.01).
However, residual anger scores in the Anger conditions did not
differ significantly between the Situational Control and Personal
Control conditions (M=4.6,SD =14.8vs.M=4.7,SD =17.7;
t(97) = .03, p=.97).
In the Neutral conditions, both residual sadness and residual
anger were slightly higher in the Situational Control condition
than in the Personal Control condition, though neither difference
was statistically significant (residual sadness: M=3.7,SD =15.5
vs. M=0.3, SD =7.3; t(99) = 1.74, p= .085; residual
anger: M=3.6,SD =11.0vs.M=0.2,SD =6.1;t(99) = 1.97,
p= .052). Residual scores for all the other individual emotions
measured (happy,indifferent,amused,anxious,neutral)alsodid
not differ significantly across the Situational Control and Personal
Control conditions (all psN.05). Thus, the Neutral condition
offers marginal evidence suggesting that personal control can help
to mitigate mild levels of naturally occurring sadness and anger.
However, Fig. 4 suggests that when experiences of sadness and
anger are more focal and acute, personal control uniquely helps to
alleviate sadness.
Thus, consistent with our theoretical framework (and
Experiment 2), Experiment 3 suggests that the acute experience
of sadness (which is naturally associated with a sense that
situational forces control outcomes) is smaller when personal
Note: Error bars represent ± 1 standard error.
Fig. 4. Focal residual emotion by condition (Experiment 3).
Note: Error bars represent ± 1 standard error.
20
Situational Control
Personal Control
15
10
Intensity of
Residual Emotion
5
0
-5
Residual Sadness
in Sadness Conditions
Residual Anger
in Anger Conditions
Fig. 3. Focal residual emotion by condition (Experiment 2).
378 S.I. Rick et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373380
control over one's environment is restored than when it is further
jeopardized. By contrast, the acute experience of anger (which is
naturally associated with a sense that other people cause negative
outcomes) is unaffected by differences in control over one's
environment. To the extent that making shopping choices
enhances feelings of personal control over one's environment,
these results suggest that shopping is likely to alleviate sadness
but not necessarily anger.
General discussion
Previous research suggests that distress can increase willingness
to spend (e.g., Atalay & Meloy, 2011; Lerner et al., 2004), but the
question of whether retail therapyactually helps to reduce
distress has only been addressed in correlational designs, utilizing
surveys and interviews of people who chose to shop when feeling
bad. We addressed this gap by experimentally examining whether
making shopping choices could help to reduce residual sadness.
Three experiments provided support for the notion that
making shopping choices helps to restore a sense of personal
control over one's environment, and thus helps to alleviate
residual sadness. We observed these benefits regardless of
whether the shopping was hypothetical or real. We also
documented support for the underlying mechanism of personal
control restoration. We found that the effects of manipulating
personal control over one's environment did not generalize to
anger. Anger is associatedwith a sense that other people are likely
to cause negative outcomes, and changes in personal control over
situational forces cannot necessarily reduce the extent to which
other people are viewed as unfairly or unduly influential.
Our work contributes to research on emotion and decision-
making. Most work in this area has focused on how specific
emotions influence decision-making and consumption (e.g.,
Cryder et al., 2008; Garg & Lerner, 2013; Garg, Wansink, &
Inman, 2007; Lerner et al., 2004). By contrast, our paper joins a
growing stream of research examining how decision-making
influences the experience of specific emotions (Berman &
Small, 2012; Gal & Liu, 2011).
Our work also contributes to appraisal theories of emotion.
Prior work had demonstrated that the cognitive appraisals that
accompany an emotion can be deactivated by addressing the
source of emotion (e.g., the cognitive effects of anger are extin-
guished when the perpetrator that caused the anger is punished;
Goldberg, Lerner, & Tetlock, 1999). Our work suggests that
counteracting a particular cognitive appraisal (here, restoring
personal control after it has been lost) may help to extinguish the
emotion that elicited the appraisal, consistent with the recursive
nature of emotions and their associated appraisals (Han et al.,
2007).
Limitations and future directions
An ideal test of the impact of restored personal control on the
experience of sadness and anger would require inductions that
initially produced similarly intense experiences of sadness and
anger (before the focal control manipulation). Otherwise,
differences in initial emotional intensity could potentially explain
differences in responsiveness to the focal control manipulation.
Unfortunately, pooling across the Personal and Situational
Control conditions in Experiment 2, residual anger in the Anger
conditions was significantly greater than residual sadness in the
Sadness conditions (14.45 vs. 0.68, pb.01). By contrast, residual
anger in the Anger conditions was slightly lower than residual
sadness in the Sadness conditions in Experiment 3 (4.67 vs. 7.53,
p= .21). Despite these differences (within and across experi-
ments), we always found that residual sadness was sensitive to the
Personal versus Situational control manipulations, but residual
anger never was. Thus, our results do not appear to be an artifact
of intensity differences across induced emotions. (An intensity
account would suggest that only the most intense or least intense
emotion would be influenced by our control manipulations.)
Future research on retail therapy could examine shopping
aspects other than choice that might address the key symptom
(sadness) but not its cause (loss of personal control). While we
carefully controlled for many features associated with shopping
so that we could isolate restoration of control as a key feature of
retail therapy, we believe that residual sadness may also be
reduced in ways that do not directly address the control deficit.
For example, social interaction may increase positive emotions
and perhaps mitigate sadness (O'Guinn & Faber, 1989). The
distraction provided by shopping is another possibility. Neither
of these shopping aspects replace the lost control intrinsic to
sadness, but both may impact a consumer's overall emotional
state.
Future research could also further explore the foundational
link between control and sadness. We found that restoring
personal control following exposure to sad stimuli helped to
reduce residual sadness. Future work could examine whether
imbuing people with high personal control helps immunize
them from the effects of sad stimuli encountered later.
Conclusion
Retail therapy is often considered wasteful and ineffective. For
example, when we asked 100 adults (52% female, mean age: 35)
for the first word that came to mind when hearing retail therapy,
they were more than twice as likely to provide a clear negative
response (e.g., nonsense,debt) than to provide a clear positive
response (e.g., fun,enjoyment) (19% vs. 8%; χ
2
(1) = 5.18,
pb.05; other responses were neutral, such as shopping). But no
prior research had experimentally examined whether and why
shopping when sad can actually help to reduce residual sadness.
Our work suggests that making shopping choices can help to
restore a sense of personal control over one's environment and
reduce residual sadness. Whether the increased control afforded
by shopping results in a loss of control later (due to increased
debt), and thus counteracts the temporary benefits of retail
therapy, remains an important open question.
Authors' note
We thank the Editor, the Associate Editor, three anonymous
reviewers, Simona Botti, Cynthia Cryder, and Gabriele Paolacci
for valuable feedback.
379S.I. Rick et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373380
Appendix. Control inductions used in Experiment 3
Situational control
Please recall a particular incident in which you did not have
any control over a situation important to you. By control, we
mean a circumstance in which you could not control your
environment in a way that allowed you to achieve something
you wanted. For instance, this could be a time when you failed
to exert the control necessary to overcome an obstacle, or when
your actions could not change an important situation to meet
your needs. Note that this does not involve lack of controlor
lack of powerover other people, just lack of control over
your environment. Please describe this situation in which you
did not have any controlwhat happened, how you felt, etc.
Personal control
Please recall a particular incident in which you had complete
control over a situation important to you. By control, we mean a
circumstance in which you controlled your environment in a way
that allowed you to achieve something you wanted. For instance,
this could be a time when you succeeded in exerting control to
overcome an obstacle, or when your actions effectively changed
an important situation to meet your needs. Note that this does not
involve controlor powerover other people, just control over
your environment. Please describe this situation in which you had
complete, effective controlwhat happened, how you felt, etc.
References
Atalay, A. S., & Meloy, M. G. (2011). Retail therapy: A strategic effort to
improve mood. Psychology and Marketing,28(6), 638659.
Berman, J. Z., & Small, D. A. (2012). Self-Interest without selfishness: The
hedonic benefit of imposed self-interest. Psychological Science,23(10),
11931199.
Cryder, C. E., Lerner, J. S., Gross, J. J., & Dahl, R. E. (2008). Misery is not
miserly: Sad and self-focused individuals spend more. Psychological
Science,19(6), 525530.
Faber, R. J., & Christenson, G. A. (1996). In the mood to buy: Differences in
the mood states experienced by compulsive buyers and other consumers.
Psychology and Marketing,13(8), 803819.
Gal, D., & Liu, W. (2011). Grapes of wrath: The angry effects of self-control.
Journal of Consumer Research,38(3), 445458.
Gardner, M. P., & Rook, D. W. (1988). Effects of impulse purchases on
consumers' affective states. In M. J. Houston (Ed.), Advances in Consumer
Research. (pp. 127130) Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.
Garg, N., & Lerner, J. S. (2013). Sadness and consumption. Journal of
Consumer Psychology,23(1), 106113.
Garg, N., Wansink, B., & Inman, J. J. (2007). The influence of incidental affect
on consumers' food intake. Journal of Marketing,71(1), 194206.
Goldberg, J. H., Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Rage and reason: The
psychology of the intuitive prosecutor. European Journal of Social
Psychology,29, 781795.
Han, S., Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2007). Feelings and consumer decision
making: The appraisal-tendency framework. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
17(3), 158168.
Inesi, M. E., Botti, S., Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2011).
Power and choice: Their dynamic interplay in quenching the thirst for
personal control. Psychological Science,22(8), 10421048.
Kasser, T., & Sheldon, K. M. (2000). Of wealth and death: Materialism, mortality
salience, and consumption behavior. Psychological Science,11(4), 348351.
Kermer, D. A., Driver-Linn, E., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2006). Loss
aversion is an affective forecasting error. Psychological Science,17(8),
649653.
Kim, S., & Rucker, D. D. (2012). Bracing for the psychological storm:
Proactive versus reactive compensatory consumption. Journal of Consumer
Research,39(4), 815830.
Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,32(2), 311328.
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,81(1), 146159.
Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and purse
strings: Carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological
Science,15(5), 337341.
Mazar, N., & Zhong, C. -B. (2010). Do green products make us better people?
Psychological Science,21(4), 494498.
O'Guinn, T. C., & Faber, R. J. (1989). Compulsive buying: A phenomenolog-
ical exploration. Journal of Consumer Research,16, 147157.
Oveis, C., Cohen, A. B., Gruber, J., Shiota, M. N., Haidt, J., & Keltner, D.
(2009). Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with tonic
positive emotionality. Emotion,9(2), 265270.
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running experiments on
Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making,5(5), 411419.
Raghunathan, R., Pham, M. T., & Corfman, K. P. (2006). Informational
properties of anxiety and sadness, and displaced coping. Journal of
Consumer Research,32(4), 596601.
Rogosa, D. R., & Willett, J. B. (1983). Demonstrating the reliability of the
difference score in the measurement of change. Journal of Educational
Management,20(4), 335343.
Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. D., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using
films. In J. A. Coan, & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion
elicitation and assessment (pp. 928). New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in
emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,48(4), 813838.
Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain:
Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in
examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,89(6), 845851.
Tuttle, B. (2010, July 19). Seeking retail therapywhen what you need is real
therapy. Time Magazine, July 19, 2010 (Retrieved from http://moneyland.
time.com/2010/07/19/too-much-retail-therapy-and-youll-need-real-therapy)
Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000).
Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,78(5), 821836.
380 S.I. Rick et al. / Journal of Consumer Psychology 24, 3 (2014) 373380
... Consumers often shop to escape from the reality and daydream about an imaginary world where they do not need to worry about an imminent problematic situation (Atalay & Meloy, 2011;Kang & Johnson, 2011). Shopping could be a therapy and an alternate way to deal with (Atalay & Meloy, 2011;Irwin, 2018;Kang & Johnson, 2011) and alleviate (Atalay & Meloy, 2011;Rick et al., 2014) negative psychological states. Kang and Johnson (2011) operationalized retail therapy in terms of consumers' therapeutic shopping motivation, positive mood reinforcement and negative mood reduction due to shopping and therapeutic shopping outcomes (e.g., elevated positive mood). ...
... Therefore, it demands alternative ways to cope with the stress that people are experiencing during the pandemic. Previously, researchers found shopping as a means to cope with negative psychological states (Atalay & Meloy, 2011;Irwin 2018;Kang & Johnson, 2011;Rick et al., 2014). Therefore, we explored how COVID-stress influences different shopping behaviors, opening a new avenue for research suggesting shopping as a tool to cope with COVID-stress. ...
... Researchers indicated that people shop to deal with negative psychological and emotional states (Atalay & Meloy, 2011;Irwin, 2018;Kang & Johnson, 2011;Rick et al., 2014). We contribute to the recent literature which indicated an upsurge in hedonic (Di Crosta et al., 2021), impulsive (Ahmed et al., 2020), and materialistic (Song et al., 2020) consumption amid the pandemic by adding that COVID-stress encourages therapeutic (hedonic) shopping. ...
Article
Full-text available
Based on the cognitive dissonance theory and compensatory consumer behavior model, we tested a conceptual model on compensatory consumption during the Covid pandemic by conducting an online survey (n = 490) with the U.S. national population. Analyzing the data with structural equation modeling, we found that COVID-stress positively influenced panic buying and therapeutic shopping but did not influence need-based buying. Need-based buying negatively influenced panic buying and therapeutic shopping. MANCOVA test revealed that when both COVID-stress and perceived financial insecurity are high, panic buying and therapeutic shopping are higher as compared to when COVID-stress and perceived financial insecurity are low. Based on the findings of our study, we suggest that therapeutic shopping and panic buying could be strategies for coping with COVID-stress.
... Importantly, shopping behavior in women has been linked to increases in positive mood and decreases in negative mood suggesting that it may be a useful strategy for regulating affect (Rick et al., 2014;Son & Lee, 2021). If women develop a habit of shopping to regulate their mood, it is plausible that this strategy might be imprudently extended to products which have adverse effects on mood. ...
... Instead, there was a general reduction in negative mood across all the online shopping conditions. This reduction in negative mood is consistent with prior shopping research and further supports arguments that online shopping might be an effective mood regulation strategy for women (Rick et al., 2014;Son & Lee, 2021). While we supported several of our hypotheses, many of the correlational effects were modest and the experimental effects require replication. ...
... Secondly, our results and others', suggests that online shopping is a potentially effective strategy for regulating mood amongst women (Rick et al., 2014;Son & Lee, 2021). This has important clinical implications given the role of mood across many psychological disorders (Beauchaine & Cicchetti, 2019;Robinaugh et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Online apparel shopping is popular amongst women and offers salient visual information for making body image and self-worth judgements. Apparel segments which emphasize the value of women's bodies are particularly effective for eliciting low body image and self-worth. Across two studies, we investigated the association between self-reported and experimental online activewear exposure on women's self-worth, body image, appearance attitudes, mood and gaze behavior. In Study 1, participants (N = 399) completed a survey collecting their online apparel shopping habits, body appreciation, self-esteem, appearance comparison tendencies and self-objectification attitudes. Activewear was the second-most popular apparel segment amongst women (after casualwear) and weekly activewear browse time was positively correlated with appearance comparison tendencies, desires to be muscular/athletic and body shame. In Study 2, participants (N = 126) were randomly allocated to browse an activewear, casualwear or homewares website and completed pre and post measures of mood, body image, implicit self-esteem and body gaze behavior. In the activewear condition, there was a significant reduction in positive body image and implicit self-esteem scores. There were no experimental effects for body gaze behavior. These findings illustrate that apparel choices have value for understanding the aetiology of maladaptive body image attitudes and low self-esteem in women.
... This may, in turn, reduce the experience of insomnia and psychological distress and prevent the impairment of psychological well-being among those tending to believe conspiracy theories. Control restoration has been demonstrated to reduce aggression (Poon et al., 2020b), improve cognitive flexibility (Bukowski et al., 2019), and lower sad or depressed mood (Rick et al., 2014), lending credence to the clinical use of control restoration interventions in coping with and addressing the consequences of having a conspiracy mentality. Thus, clinical psychologists may consider using control restoration interventions to assist with people's insomnia, psychological distress, and impaired psychological well-being related to conspiracy mentality. ...
... Some forms of consumption such as gift-giving are obvious candidates for authentic manifestations of care for others: Being kind to others and showing appreciation through gifts are likely to be strongly associated with many consumers' vision of the good life (Chan and Mogilner, 2016;Park, 1998;Wolfinbarger and Yale, 1993). By contrast, retail therapywhich is often conceptualised as a form of self-care for managing negative mood and anxietyis less clear cut: while research demonstrates that retail therapy does have positive effects on the individual consumer, the therapeutic value decreases quickly (Atalay and Meloy, 2011;Kang and Johnson, 2010;Mouakhar-Klouz et al., 2016;Rick et al., 2014). There is, thus, a clear need for future research to explore what forms of consumption are authentic expressions of care and, by inference, when caring for others and self through consumption is meaningful. ...
Article
Full-text available
It is controversial whether consumption can constitute genuine, existential meaning for the individual. Building on philosophical explorations of subjective meaning, this study suggests a dynamic relationship between existential and teleological consumption. On the one hand, consumers demonstrate deep-level engagement with entities in the marketing eco-system (such as brand narratives and certain service encounters) to explore their own potentiality and develop an authentic vision of the good life. This is existential consumption. On the other, consumers adopt teleological modes of consumption where products and services are used more instrumentally to enact their vision of the good life. It is proposed that consumer choice is existentially meaningful insofar as it is conducive to the development or realisation of the individual vision of the good life. The theory and its implications are discussed in the context of recent deterministic and pessimistic/nihilistic challenges to marketing theory.
... Our design in Study 2 allowed for this possibility: participants could make the decision to keep money for themselves or to give it to a charity, but whether their decision had the intended outcome was chosen randomly. Such random manipulations of decisions' outcomes have already been used in the study of emotions generated by decisions (see Study 2 in Rick et al., 2014) and allow to separate the effect of the decision itself from the impact of its outcome. ...
Article
Full-text available
Giving money to others feels good. It is now standard to use the label ‘warm glow feelings’ to refer to the pleasure people take from giving. But what exactly are warm glow feelings? And why do people experience them? To answer these questions, we ran two studies: a recall task in which participants were asked to remember a donation they made, and a donation task in which participants were given the opportunity to make a donation before reporting their affective states. Correlational and experimental results converge towards the conclusion that, if the nature of the warm glow is straightforward, its source is multifaceted. Regarding the nature of the ‘warm glow’, the pleasure people took in giving was mainly predicted by one particular type of positive emotion and was indeed described by participants as ‘warm’. Regarding the underlying psychological mechanisms, warm glow feelings were elicited by positive appraisals regarding the donor’s moral character, positive appraisals regarding the actual impact of the donor’s donation on the welfare of others and a feeling of communion with others. We discuss the theoretical implications of our findings.
... Drawing from retail therapy, shopping can provide consumers with therapeutic utilities; when consumers perceive that shopping is beneficial for the regulation of negative emotions, they are more likely to make purchase decisions. The perception of shopping as a therapeutic process for psychological deficits and the belief in the utilities of therapeutic shopping have been empirically shown to influence consumers' purchase behavior (Rick et al., 2014). With regard to revenge buying, consumers have been troubled by the biological need dissatisfaction, lack of self-esteem and self-control, as well as negative emotions (i.e., anxiety, boredom) during the health crisis. ...
Article
Full-text available
With the stabilizing of COVID-19 and the lessening of isolation measures, the consumption market is gradually recovering. Consumers exhibit a tendency to compensate for the previous pent-up demand, triggering the phenomenon of revenge spending. Nevertheless, research on revenge buying during the pandemic has been limited thus far. Moreover, the role of negative emotions and retail therapy has not been well explored. Therefore, drawing from retail therapy theory and the stimulus-organism-response framework, this study aims to investigate how negative emotions, physiological motivations, and self-seeking stimulate consumers to form positive perceptions that revenge buying can provide therapeutic utilities, and consequently choose revenge buying. Empirical data were collected from China. The structural equation modelling results reveal that anxiety, boredom, self-seeking, and psychological motivation have a positive influence on therapeutic shopping motivation , therapeutic shopping value and therapeutic shopping outcome, which subsequently influence revenge buying behavior. The theoretical model provides a novel perspective to research revenge buying. Additionally, the results provide managerial implications for consumer, retailers, and policymakers to make preparedness for revenge buying in future health crises.
... Накопленный опыт мировых исследований показывает, что продукты питания могут потребляться под влиянием различных эмоциональных состояний, в частности для снижения эмоционального напряжения, получения желаемых эмоций. Таким образом, покупка и последующее потребление продуктов питания могут быть ориентированы на создание положительных впечатлений и выступать в качестве способа совладания с возникающими трудностями (Rick et al., 2014;Lee et al., 2000;Horváth, Adıgüzel, 2018;van Strien et al., 2019). ...
Article
The article considers the relationship between the consumer’s emotional state and impulse purchases. The authors analyze the effects of the consumer’s impulse behavior on buying food products. According to numerous studies of consumer behavior conducted in Russia, the share of purchases defined as impulse purchases has been steadily increasing. Insufficient understanding of the specifics and nature of impulse purchases may negatively affect both consumers’ well-being, including their psychological and financial state and companies’ financial sustainability in the long term. The article aims to improve understanding of consumer behavior in Russia and identify the relationship between the consumer’s emotional state and impulse purchases of food products. Previous research into the topic has allowed the authors to form understanding of impulse purchases and systematize factors influencing them. The effect of consumer’s emotional state on impulse purchases of food products has also been described.An empirical study including a series of in-depth interviews and a quantitative survey forms the basis for the present research. To identify the relationship between the consumer’s emotional state and impulse purchases of food products in Russia the structural equation modeling method is applied. The result of testing are reveal several alternative models, differences in the patterns of impulse buying behavior of female and male respondents. The research confirmsthat negative emotions significantly affect sensitivity to visual characteristics of products, impulsivity, and unplanned purchasing with both female and male consumers. Positive emotions, in contrast, demonstrate statistical significance with men solely. These findings can be explained by the consumer’s desire to reduce emotional tension and improve emotional state.The article provides a basis for further research into emotionally driven consumer behavior and improve predictability of purchasing behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Sense of control refers to a person’s belief that he or she is capable of obtaining desired outcomes, avoiding undesired outcomes, and achieving goals. This concept has been raised and studied by sociologists, but in recent years, there has been a gradual increase in research on the impact of consumer sense of control. However, a comprehensive review in this field is still lacking. This paper provides an overview of previous research on four aspects of consumer sense of control: (1) sense of control and compensatory behavior, (2) sense of control and emotional coping, (3) sense of control and charitable behavior, and (4) desire for control. Furthermore, the paper highlights three directions as key areas of future research: (a) exploring antecedents of sense of control, (b) investigating underlying psychological mechanisms of compensatory behavior resulting from a reduced sense of control, and (c) examining moderating effects on desire for control.
Article
Emotions are the focal factor influencing consumer evaluation and behaviour. However, the present literature remains fragmented owing to conflicting findings on effects of emotions. Identifying moderator(s) of emotion's effect resolves these inconsistencies and helps comprehend when emotions are effective marketing tools. This study jointly reviews the literature on eight discrete emotions spanning over two decades (2000–2021), synthesizing and analysing the research on moderators attenuating, intensifying, or even reversing the effect of discrete emotions, documenting them at an individual and situational level. Based on that, an integrated contextually bounded emotion effect framework is advanced that resolves inconsistencies in literature and enables us to (1) draw instructive insights around discrete emotion dynamic effects owing to contextual variability, (2) identify similarities/differences within/across emotions and contextual factors (3) provide research‐informed managerial implication and directions for future research.
Article
Full-text available
This research introduces the distinction between compensatory consumption that is engaged in after, as opposed to before, one experiences a self-threat (termed reactive vs. proactive compensatory consumption). Five experiments document the phenomenon of proactive compensatory consumption as well as corresponding boundary conditions for its effect. Furthermore, whereas both reactive and proactive compensatory consumption are associated with seeking products that symbolically relate to an experienced or potential threat, we demonstrate that reactive compensatory consumption is more likely to be associated with the use of products for the purpose of distraction. We examine how and when these different forms of compensatory consumption affect consumers’ preferences versus actual consumption behavior. Implications for delineating reactive versus proactive compensatory consumption in the literature, as well as the use of consumption for the purpose of symbolic self-completion versus distraction, are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Sadness influences consumption, leading individuals to pay more to acquire new goods and to eat more unhealthy food than they would otherwise. These undesirable consumption effects of sadness can occur without awareness, thus representing more than just conscious attempts at “retail therapy.” In an experiment with real food consumption, the present paper examines the hypothesis that sadness' impact on consumption could be attenuated if the choice context counteracted appraisals of helplessness and enhanced a sense of individual control. Results revealed that: (1) sadness elevates self-reports of helplessness in response to the emotion-inducing situation, (2) helplessness mediates the sadness–consumption effect, and (3) inducing a sense of control (via choice) attenuates sadness' effect.
Article
This article presents the Appraisal‐Tendency Framework (ATF; Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Lerner & Tiedens, 2006) as a basis for predicting the influence of specific emotions on consumer decision making. In particular, the ATF addresses how and why specific emotions carry over from past situations to color future judgments and choices. After reviewing the main assumptions and the 5 main principles of the framework, 2 streams of research are presented. One stream addresses emotional carryover effects on the assessment of risk; the other addresses carryover effects on the assessment of monetary value. Because risk assessment and value assessment are fundamental psychological processes, understanding them has the potential to yield manifold implications for consumer judgment and decision making. The concluding sections highlight limitations and future directions of the framework.
Article
This study explores the conditions under which experimentally primed anger influences both attributions of responsibility and the processes by which people make such attributions. Drawing on social functional theory, it was hypothesized that people are best thought of as ‘intuitive prosecutors’ who lower their thresholds for making attributions of harmful intent and recommending harsh punishment when they both witness a serious transgression of societal norms and believe that the transgressor escaped punishment. The data support the hypotheses. Anger primed by a serious crime ‘carried over’ to influence judgments of unrelated acts of harm only when the perpetrator of the crime went unpunished, notwithstanding the arousal of equally intense anger in conditions in which the perpetrator was appropriately punished or his fate was unknown. Participants in the perpetrator-unpunished condition also relied on simpler and more punitive attributional heuristics for inferring responsibility for harm. Copyright © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
This study explores the conditions under which experimentally primed anger influences both attributions of responsibility and the processes by which people make such attributions. Drawing on social functional theory, it was hypothesized that people are best thought of as 'intuitive prosecutors' who lower their thresholds for making attributions of harmful intent and recommending harsh punishment when they both witness a serious transgression of societal norms and believe that the transgressor escaped punishment. The data support the hypotheses. Anger primed by a serious crime 'carried over' to influence judgments of unrelated acts of harm only wizen the perpetrator of the crime went unpunished, notwithstanding the arousal of equally intense anger in conditions in which the perpetrator appropriately punished or his fate was unknown. Participants in the perpetrator-unpunished condition also relied on simpler and more punitive attributional heuristics for inferring responsibility for harm. (C) 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Chapter
Research on emotion has undergone explosive growth during the past few decades, marked by new theories (e.g., evolutionary analyses; Tooby & Cosmides, 1990), methods (e.g., anatomically-based systems for coding facial expressive behavior, see Cohn this volume), and findings (see Cacioppo & Gardner, 1999). Some of the research in this area has been correlational, focusing on factors that naturally co-vary with emotional processes, such as chronological age, physical health, or social status. However, experimental research also has flourished, focusing on emotional processes in the context of relatively well-controlled laboratory environments. Our chapter on the use of emotion-eliciting films, like many of the contributions to the Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment, lies squarely within this second, experimental tradition.
Article
This study explores the conditions under which experimentally primed anger influences both attributions of responsibility and the processes by which people make such attributions. Drawing on social functional theory, it was hypothesized that people are best thought of as 'intuitive prosecutors' who lower their thresholds for making attribu- tions of harmful intent and recommending harsh punishment when they both witness a serious transgression of societal norms and believe that the transgressor escaped punishment. The data support the hypotheses. Anger primed by a serious crime 'carried over' to influence judgments of unrelated acts of harm only when the perpetrator of the crime went unpunished, notwithstanding the arousal of equally intense anger in conditions in which the perpetrator was appropriately punished or his fate was unknown. Participants in the perpetrator-unpunished condition also relied on simpler and more punitive attributional heuristics for inferring responsibility for harm. Copyright # 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.