ArticlePDF Available

Helmholtz Illusion Makes you Look Fit Only When you are Already Fit, But not for Everyone

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

A square filled with horizontal stripes is perceived as thinner than one with vertical stripes (Helmholtz illusion). This is not consistent with a common belief that horizontally striped clothing makes a person look fatter, and studies on this problem have shown inconsistent results. Here, we demonstrate three factors that could have complicated the issue. First, the Helmholtz effect is stronger for a thin figure than for a fat one, with possible reversal for the latter. Second, we found large variability across participants, suggesting dependence on features to attend. Third, there was strong hysteresis as to the order of testing fat and thin figures, suggesting the effect of surrounding people in daily life. There can be yet other factors, but we should note that this apparently simple case of application of a geometrical illusion in daily perception should be taken as a rather complex phenomenon.
Content may be subject to copyright.
a Pion publication
i-Perception (2013) volume 4, pages 347–351
dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0595rep perceptionweb.com/i-perceptionISSN 2041-6695
Hiroshi Ashida
Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Kyoto 6068501, Japan; e-mail: ashida@psy.bun.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Kana Kuraguchi
Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Kyoto 6068501, Japan; e-mail: kuraguchi.kana.23c@st.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Kiyofumi Miyoshi
Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Kyoto 6068501, Japan; e-mail: miyoshi80@gmail.com
Received 5 March 2013, in revised form 1 June 2013; published 10 July 2013
Abstract. A square lled with horizontal stripes is perceived as thinner than one with vertical stripes
(Helmholtz illusion). This is not consistent with a common belief that horizontally striped clothing
makes a person look fatter, and studies on this problem have shown inconsistent results. Here,
we demonstrate three factors that could have complicated the issue. First, the Helmholtz effect is
stronger for a thin gure than for a fat one, with possible reversal for the latter. Second, we found
large variability across participants, suggesting dependence on features to attend. Third, there was
strong hysteresis as to the order of testing fat and thin gures, suggesting the effect of surrounding
people in daily life. There can be yet other factors, but we should note that this apparently simple
case of application of a geometrical illusion in daily perception should be taken as a rather complex
phenomenon.
Keywords: Helmholtz illusion, fashion.
1 Results and discussion
It is commonly believed that horizontal stripes on clothing make them look fatter. This, however,
conicts with a known geometrical illusion found by Helmholtz (1867); namely, a square lled with
horizontal stripes looks taller and a square lled with vertical stripes looks wider. He mentioned the
same effect on ladies’ frocks. Thompson and Mikellidou (2011) examined the effects of stripes on
clothing in a series of experiments and concluded that Helmholtz was correct. However, subsequently
in 2012, an amateur scientist Val Watham in the United Kingdom won the prize in a BBC radio pro-
gram, demonstrating that the common belief is true in an experiment of watching videos of people
walking with horizontally or vertically striped clothes (http://www.bbc.co.uk/radio4/features/sywtbas/
nalists/stripes/), with the supervision of Thompson himself. We may conclude that the Helmholtz
illusion could be overridden by other factors in a realistic scene. There could be, however, a number
of other factors that may also affect the results. Here, we demonstrate some of such factors, lying on
both the stimulus and the observer sides.
One can notice that the main female gure used in Thompson and Mikellidou (2011) is relatively
thin and tall (Figure 1a). This contrasts with the fat men in the gure of Imai (1982), as shown in
Figure 1(b), with which he supported the common belief; the width of the man with vertical stripes
was judged thinner than the one with horizontal stripes by 15.37% (137 participants, SD 5 9.24%).
This large difference has led us to the possibility that the effect of stripes might depend on the shape
of the person, which is also suggested by Experiment 3 in Thompson and Mikellidou (2011), where
they found fading of the effect of stripes on simple cylinders as they became fatter. To test this idea,
we conducted an experiment in which we compared the points of subjective equality (PSEs) in terms
of the body width (i.e. when gures with horizontal and vertical stripes look equally fat) for relatively
thin and fat gures (Figure 2).
The results are summarized in Figure 3, where a positive PSE supports Helmholtz (a person
with horizontal stripes appeared thinner than one with vertical stripes), whereas a negative PSE sup-
ports the common belief (a person with horizontal stripes appeared fatter). This gure highlights three
Short report
Helmholtz illusion makes you look t only when
you are already t, but not for everyone
348 Ashida H, Kuraguchi K, Miyoshi K
major aspects of the results that demonstrate complexity underlying this phenomenon. First, the over-
all effect is positive and in accordance with the Helmholtz illusion. Second, the thin body yielded
signicantly larger Helmholtz illusion than the fat body (F(1, 26) 5 6.573, p 5 0.0165, by a three-way
mixed-design analysis of variance for block order gender body type). This result is consistent
with Experiment 3 of Thompson and Mikellidou (2011) using cylinders, and supports our hypothesis.
Third, the block order had a signicant effect (fat-rst vs. thin-rst; F(1, 26) 5 4.262, p 5 0.0491).
The effect of participants’ gender was not signicant (F(1, 26) 5 1.439, p 5 0.241), but note that
gender difference was not our primary interest and the numbers of participant were not matched. Also,
there was no signicant two-way or three-way interaction (p > 0.2 for all).
There seems to be a strong hysteresis effect, because the results in the second block are pulled
towards those in the rst block. The reason for this effect is unknown, but it may reect response
biases learned through the rst block. We therefore analysed the results of the rst block (block 1)
separately that should reect more naïve responses of each participant (between-participant compari-
sons). In block 1, the difference between the thin and the fat gures is more pronounced (t(28) 5 4.526,
Figure 1. (a) The typical visual stimuli used in Thompson and Mikellidou (2011).1 (b) A demonstration of
fattening by horizontal stripes in Imai (1982).
Figure 2. Stimuli used in our experiment: (a) a pair of thin gures and (b) a pair of fat gures.
1 The gure was not reproduced correctly in the published paper, and this was copied from the original
(P. Thompson, personal communication).
Helmholtz illusion on clothing 349
p 5 0.00047). Averaged PSE was positive for the thin gure (t(14) 5 3.976, p 5 0.00056), but it was
negative for the fat gure, although it was not signicantly below zero (t(14) 5 0.917, p 5 0.258).
There is another factor that has not been much attended to in the literature, namely, very large vari-
ability across participants. As seen in Figure 4, PSEs of individual participants in block 1 are broadly
distributed, spreading from negative to positive for both thin and fat gures. This could be partly
because we did not instruct participants to judge on the basis of a particular criterion. Giving a more
precise instruction could have reduced the variability, but we preferred to see more natural judgements
as people would do in daily life.
In summary, we have identied three factors that may underlie the inconsistency over the effects
of stripes on clothing. First, we provided striking evidence that the effect of stripes depends on the
body shape. Outts with horizontal stripes can make a slender person even tter, but the effect is not
ensured or could be reversed for others (to be consistent with what is commonly conceived). This sug-
gests that one practical way to make the Helmholtz illusion more effective would be to wear a long
dress as in Figure 1(a), which might be less useful for men. The reason is yet to be investigated, but it
might be a general sensory or perceptual effect as the result parallels the nding that the Oppenl-Kundt
illusion (overestimation of the interval lled with vertical lines) became weaker when the lling lines
were longer than a certain maximum (Wackerman & Kastner, 2010). Second, the effect can be widely
variable across people, making it of even less practical use. Third, the strong effect of order may imply
that the effect of striped outts could depend on the clothing and/or tness of surrounding people.
We still have not solved the entire problem, as there are other potential factors that could have
characterized the results in the Watham experiment. First, the effect of 3D cues with the vertical stripes
(Taya & Miura, 2007) may have been pronounced in videos (kinetic depth effect). Second, a direct
comparison between two gures, also adopted by Thompson and Mikellidou (2011), may emphasize
Figure 3. Mean PSEs for the two body conditions: all averaged, block 1, and block 2. Bars show 1 SEM across
participants.
Figure 4. A histogram that shows the distributions of individual PSEs in block 1.
350 Ashida H, Kuraguchi K, Miyoshi K
geometrical measures compared with the case of rating one person at one time, although our pre-
liminary study did not support this idea. Finally, we could speculate on an intriguing possibility that
motion should be rather crucial; horizontal stripes would yield weaker motion signals than vertical
stripes when people walk around upright. In such a case, we would expect more blurring with horizon-
tal stripes while more effective motion deblurring could operate with vertical stripes that would lead
to faster (stronger) motion signals (e.g. Castet, Lorenceau, Shiffrar, & Bonnet, 1993). This could result
in relative fattening of a person with horizontally striped clothing. These possibilities remain specula-
tive, but we should consider multiple factors and individual differences, which allows us with further
interesting questions for future studies.
2 Methods
Thirty-one undergraduate students participated for a course credit. One of them was omitted from the
analysis because the overall psychometric function lay below 0.4 and the estimation of PSEs was unre-
liable. The results from remaining 30 (19 females) were analysed. The participants observed a com-
puter screen (Mitsubishi 23-inch or Apple 24-inch Cinema Screen LCDs) from a distance of 48 cm
by using a chin rest. They were naïve to the exact purpose of the experiment, and were given detailed
explanations later during the class.
Static computer-graphics images of thin and fat gures were generated by using a female template
model in the Poser 8 software (SmithMicro Inc.). Each gure was dressed with a shirt with either
vertical or horizontal stripes with the white–black ratio of 3:1 (Figure 2). The width of the black lines
was constant (about 0.24 deg) for both body shapes. In each set, the gure with vertical stripes was
used as a standard stimulus, and the body width of the gure with horizontal stripes was varied as a
test stimulus by modifying the 3D parameters between the shoulder and the waist under Poser, from
210% to 110% of the standard stimulus in 5% steps. The shape of the horizontally striped gure was
manipulated because it yielded less local distortion that could provide a cue than the vertically striped
one. Each standard gure subtended approximately 17.8 deg vertically and 7.2 deg (thin) or 13.1 deg
(fat) horizontally. The stimulus step was relatively coarse, because our preliminary observation sug-
gested that the judgement was difcult with smaller differences. Given the PSEs that turned out to be
less than 5%, this could have reduced the precision of the PSE estimates but should not have affected
their accuracy on average.
A method of constant stimuli was used to estimate the points of subjective equality (PSE) in per-
ceived width of the gures between horizontal and vertical stripes. Superlab 4.5 on Windows (Cedrus
Inc.) was used to control the experiment. Each trial started with a xation mark. On participants’ press-
ing a key, the xation mark was replaced with a pair of gures that were presented for 1.8 s, shown side
by side with a centre-to-centre distance of 13.1 deg. The participants were asked to judge which looked
fatter and respond by pressing a key (2AFC). There was no time pressure for the response. The side
of the standard stimulus was counterbalanced across trials. Each of the ve test gures was presented
20 times on each side with a randomized order, resulting in 40 judgements per test size. Fat and thin
gures were tested in separate blocks; each participant completed one block for each body type with a
30-s break between them, with the order counterbalanced across participants. The whole session took
less than 25 min for each participant. Three or four participants were tested in a laboratory room at the
same time, using separate computers and monitors.
PSE was calculated for individual participant as the 50% point of the psychometric function that
was estimated by the Probit analysis (Finney, 1971), using the glm() function of the R language.
Acknowledgments. HA was supported by JSPS grant-in-aid for scientic research (A22243044).
References
Castet, E., Lorenceau, J., Shiffrar, M., & Bonnet, C. (1993). Perceived speed of moving lines depends on
orientation, length, speed and luminance. Vision Research, 33(14), 1921–1936. doi:10.1016/0042-
6989(93)90019-S
Finney, D. J. (1971). Probit analysis (3rd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Helmholtz, H. v. (1867). Handbuch der physiologischen Optik III. Leipzig: Voss. (English translation by J. P.
C Southall for the Optical Society of America, Treatise on Physiological Optics, volume 3, New York:
Dover, 1925).
Imai, S. (1982). Tateshima yokoshima no nazo (in Japanese). Psychology, 29, 12. Tokyo: Saiensu-sha.
Copyright 2013 H Ashida, K Kuraguchi, K Miyoshi
Published under a Creative Commons Licence a Pion publication
351 Ashida H, Kuraguchi K, Miyoshi K
Taya, S., & Miura, K. (2007). Shrinkage in the apparent size of cylindrical objects. Perception, 36(1), 3–16.
doi:10.1068/p5597
Thompson, P., & Mikellidou, K. (2011). Applying the Helmholtz illusion to fashion: Horizontal stripes won’t
make you look fatter. i-Perception, 2(1), 69–76. doi:10.1068/i0405
Wackerman, J., & Kastner, K. (2010). Determinants of lled/empty optical illusion: Search for the locus of
maximal effect. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 70, 423–434.
Hiroshi Ashida received BA, MA, and PhD at Kyoto University (psychology).
After working as a postdoctoral researcher at the ATR Human Information
Processing Research Laboratories, Research Associate at Kyoto University,
and Associate Professor at Ritsumeikan University, he is now Associate
Professor at Kyoto University. His main research interests are visual
processing of motion and visual illusion in general. He also studies fMRI after
learning the basics at Royal Holloway, UK, in 2004–2005. He prefers vertical
stripes on his shirts anyway.
Kana Kuraguchi received BA and MA in psychology from Kyoto University
and is studying for PhD with Hiroshi Ashida at Kyoto University. Her main
research interest is in visual attractiveness of human faces. She is also
a licensed instructor of Ikebana, the Japanese traditional art of ower
arrangement.
Kiyofumi Miyoshi received BA and MA in psychology from Kyoto University
and is studying for PhD with Hiroshi Ashida at Kyoto University. His main
research interest is in re-evaluation of implicit and explicit memory.
... In a more recent study, Ashida et al. (2013) examined whether the thinning effect of horizontal stripes depends on body size. In their study, participants saw the image of a female model wearing a shirt with either horizontal or vertical stripes and asked participants to match the size of the vertically striped stimulus with thinner and wider versions of the horizontally striped stimulus. ...
... However, not all research findings on Helmholtz illusion are unequivocal. For example, Imai (1982; as reported by Ashida et al., 2013; original paper in Japanese) using the drawing of arather overweight-male figure wearing either a horizontally or vertically striped shirt, found that the vertically striped stimulus was judged 15.37% thinner than that with horizontal stripes. These findings suggest that for larger body sizes horizontal stripes have a widening effect, opposite to the Helmholtz's illusion. ...
... As compared to previous experiments, the present study differentiates in two ways. First, the stimuli used in previous studies were either line drawings of human figures (Imai, 1982;Thompson & Mikellidou, 2011) or computerized images (Ashida et al., 2013; with the exception of the stereoscopically seen mannequins in Thompson & Mikellidou, 2011, Experiment 4), not necessarily close to what a real human figure looks like. To improve stimuli's realism, we used the photo of a real female model wearing a dress with horizontal stripes (green-black) scraped from the internet. ...
Article
Full-text available
In Helmholtz’s illusion, a square with horizontal stripes appears taller than an identical square with vertical stripes. This effect has also been observed in experiments with human stimuli, where a human figure wearing a dress with horizontal stripes appears thinner than a drawing clad in vertical stripes. These findings do not agree with the common belief that clothes with horizontal stripes make someone appear wider, neither do they disentangle whether the horizontal or vertical stripes account for the thinning effect. In the present study, we focused on the effect of horizontal stripes in clothes comparing horizontal stripes against no-stripes (not against vertical; Experiments 1 and 2), using photos of a real-life female model, and controlling for the average luminance of the stripes (Experiment 2). Results showed that horizontal stripes and lower luminance have—independently—a small-to-moderate thinning effect on the perceived size of the body, and the effect is larger when the two variables are combined. In Experiment 3, we further show that the thinning effect due to the luminance of the dress is enhanced when the general background gets darker.
... In our study, the obstacles were rectangular rather than square, indicating that the effect of the Helmholtz illusion on the foot clearance may not have been observed. Previous studies have also suggested that the effect of the Helmholtz illusion depends on the shape of the object (Taya & Miura, 2007;Thompson & Mikellidou, 2011), with wider objects diminishing this effect (Ashida et al., 2013). Finally, another possibility is that the Helmholtz illusion alters perception but not movement. ...
Article
Full-text available
An adequate foot clearance height while stepping over an obstacle is important for safety in daily life. In the present study, we examined whether visual illusions affect foot clearance during a stepping-over action, and whether this is further influenced by gaze behavior. Twelve participants stepped over an obstacle placed four meters away under conditions of three different obstacle characteristics: white, horizontal, or vertical lines. We measured the participants’ foot clearances during the step-over action and their gaze behavior during the approaching phase. Participants stepped significantly higher over the obstacles in the vertical lines (illusion) condition. The duration of gaze fixation on the obstacle positively correlated with increased foot clearance in the vertical condition, suggesting that the effect of the visual illusion on foot clearance was enhanced by prolonged gaze fixation. Conversely, prolonged fixation negatively correlated with foot clearance in the white (control) condition, implying that a cautious perception of an obstacle may contribute to efficient stepping-over action.
... vertical or no) stripes (Koutsoumpis et al., 2021;Thompson & Mikellidou, 2011). However, the effect of striped clothing depends on the body size of the wearer and the contexts (e.g., effects of presentation order of horizontal and vertical stripes) and varies widely across observers (Ashida et al., 2013). Counterexamples supporting the common belief have also been reported (Imai, 1982, see Ashida et al., 2013Swami & Harris, 2012;Watham, 2012;Taya & Miura, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Clothes with horizontal (or vertical) stripes are perceived as wider and shorter (slimmer and taller)." This belief is common yet inconsistent with the Helmholtz illusion. It has often attracted attention from researchers of perception. Despite the controversy among empirical studies, it is persistently supported by the general public. This article explores the early appearance of this common belief in Japan in historical records. Consequently, we discovered the descriptions of the common belief in a Japanese beauty handbook titled "Miyako Fuzoku Kewai Den [Cosmetic manners and customs in Edo]," published in 1813. In Japan, this belief was not born in modern times. Instead, it was established over 200 years ago, when vertical striped patterns on clothes were popularized. People wearing clothes with horizontal or vertical stripes are believed to be perceived as wider and shorter, or slimmer and taller, respectively. This common belief has attracted attention from researchers of perception because it is inconsistent with the Helmholtz illusion, where a square of the same area appears to be wider (or slimmer) in the case of a vertical (or horizontal) striped
... Some designers and brands have been designing to make the body form look slimmer through optical illusion effects. Colour contrasts and vertical contrast panels have helped to generate fashion illusion effects [25]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Possessing a potbelly has been identified as a major problem that affects the self-esteem and social appeal of affected men. Efforts to deal with the condition include exercise and diet which present a major challenge to most people on the grounds of health and fitness. The study sought to explore the principle of fashion illusion as an alternative means of dealing with potbelly in affected working-class men. The study aims at integrating fashion illusion in garment design and construction to flatter pot bellies in men as means of enhancing their social appeal and self-esteem. The study employed a qualitative research method using a case study design. Observations and interviews were used as instruments to collect relevant information about the respondents in the Asokwa, Subin and Oforikrom Metropolis all in Kumasi, the Ashanti Region of Ghana. The selected Sub-metros were characterized by significant commercial activities involving the targeted working-class men with pot bellies. The study found that potbelly resulted in negative self-esteem, reduced happiness, and poor appearance among affected men. The respondents embraced fashion illusion as a positive means of enhancing images and making people feel better about themselves. It was recommended that the fashion illusion technique should be popularized to enhance the shape of potbellied working-class men, making them feel self-esteem and productivity.
... In other words, why do dark clothes make individuals appear to weigh slightly less? Although mathematical models have been proposed [20,22], empirical evidence sometimes contradicts the models [2]. Asking crowd workers why they think a person in an image appears to weigh less could shed new light on the question of causal pathways. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In many societies, appearing slim is considered attractive. The fashion industry has been attempting to cater to this trend by designing outfits that can enhance the appearance of slimness. Two anecdotal rules, widespread in the world of fashion, are (1) choose dark clothes and (2) avoid horizontal stripes, in order to appear slim. Thus far, empirical evidence has been unable to conclusively determine the validity of these rules, and there is consequently much controversy regarding the impact of both color and patterns on the visual perception of weight. In this paper, we aim to close this gap by presenting the results from a series of large-scale crowdsourcing studies that investigate the above two claims. We gathered a dataset of around 1,000 images of people from the Web together with their ground-truth weight and height, as well as clothing attributes about colors and patterns. To elicit the effects of colors and patterns, we asked crowd workers to estimate the weight in each image. For the analysis, we controlled potential confounds by matching images in pairs where the two images differ with respect to color or pattern, but are similar with respect to other relevant aspects. We created image pairs in two ways: first, observationally, i.e., from two real images; and second, experimentally, by manipulating the color or pattern of clothing in a real image via photo editing. Based on our analysis, we conclude that (1) dark clothes indeed decrease perceived weight slightly but statistically significantly, and (2) horizontal stripes have no discernible effect compared to solid light-colored clothes. These results contribute to advancing the debate around the effect of specific clothing colors and patterns and thus provide empirical grounds for everyday fashion decisions. Moreover, our work gives an outlook on the vast opportunities of using crowd sourcing in the modern fashion industry.
... In other words, why do dark clothes make individuals appear to weigh slightly less? Although mathematical models have been proposed [11,14] , empirical evidence sometimes contradicts the models [27] . Asking crowd workers why they think a person in an image appears to weigh less could shed new light on the question of causal pathways. ...
Article
In many societies, appearing slim (corresponding to a small body-mass index) is considered attractive. The fashion industry has been attempting to cater to this trend by designing outfits that can enhance the appearance of slimness. Two anecdotal rules, widespread in the world of fashion, are to choose dark clothes and avoid horizontal stripes, in order to appear slim. Thus far, empirical evidence has been unable to conclusively determine the validity of these rules, and there is consequently much controversy regarding the impact of both color and patterns on the visual perception of weight. In this paper, we aim to close this gap by presenting the results from a series of large-scale crowdsourcing studies that investigate the above two claims. We gathered a dataset of around 1000 images of people from the Web together with their ground-truth weight and height as well as clothing attributes about colors and patterns. To elicit the effects of colors and patterns, we asked crowd workers to estimate the weight in each image. For the analysis, we controlled potential confounds by matching images in pairs where the two images differ with respect to color or pattern, but are similar with respect to other relevant aspects. We created image pairs in two ways: firstly, observationally, i.e., from two real images; and secondly, experimentally, by manipulating the color or pattern of clothing in a real image via photo editing. Based on our analysis, we conclude that dark clothes indeed decrease perceived weight slightly but statistically significantly, and horizontal stripes have no discernible effect compared to solid light-colored clothes. These results contribute to advancing the debate around the effect of specific clothing colors and patterns and thus provide empirical grounds for everyday fashion decisions. Moreover, our work gives an outlook on the vast opportunities of using crowd sourcing in the modern fashion industry.
... The fundamental ability of stripes to highlight and distinguish an object from its surroundings has been used throughout history in the form of clothing and products. (Piaget,1953), the number of stimuli and its placement (Robinson,1972), the surface dimensions of objects (Taya Maura,2007) (Thompson & Mikellidou,2011) and the human body shape (Ashida et al, 2013). Hermann Von Helmholtz (1925) reported that an identical square made of vertical lines seem wider and shorter than a square comprised of horizontal lines. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The desire for perfect body figures is ubiquitous across the world. The beauty standards are intangible characteristics, which is very subjective. This research looks into types of design patterns-acting as optical illusions-that can affect viewer perceptions of body size. Some theories are stating that certain patterns can influence how broad or thin the wearer looks. The study proposes to develop different types of design to assist the manipulation of visual perceptions of the wearers' actual size through a mediation of printed textile design pattern. Our overall approach is to create patterns by manipulating horizontal and vertical lines in creating design patterns to aid viewer perceptions of body size. The methods used were of quantitative and involved mind-measuring investigation. The data is taken from psychometric scaling-paired comparison and magnitude scaling method. Paired comparison is a simple way to approach respondents from all age groups and by using magnitude scaling, participants can save time by viewing four designs simultaneously for assessing. In this paper we have discovered possibilities of patterns, with different positioning and placement of patterns visually manipulates sizes of female and male silhouettes through the mediation of horizontal and vertical patterns.
... With regard to women's clothing, he suggested horizontally striped skirts in order to make women look taller and thinner. Several studies reported results in line with von Helmholtz's assumptions (e.g., Ashida, Kuraguchi, & Miyoshi, 2013;Maehara, Saida, & Wake, 2015;Thompson & Mikellidou, 2011). However, one study (Swami & Harris, 2012) reported the opposite effect: a woman appeared thinner when wearing a vertically striped (or plain) skirt as compared to a horizontally striped skirt. ...
Article
The texture of an object's surface influences its perceived spatial extent. For example, Hermann von Helmholtz reported that a square patch with black and white stripes appears elongated perpendicular to the stripes' orientation. This time-honoured finding stands in contrast with more recent recommendations by interior-design experts who suggest that stripe wall patterns make rooms appear elongated in the direction parallel to the stripes' orientation. In a series of four experiments, we presented stripe wall patterns and varied the orientation of the stripes (horizontal vs. vertical) and their density (number of stripes per degree of visual angle). Subjects estimated the width and height of stereoscopically presented interior spaces. Stripe patterns with higher densities made rooms appear both wider and higher than did stripe patterns with lower densities or plain walls. In contrast to both the predictions from the Helmholtz-square and the design guidelines, this effect was only weakly modulated by pattern orientation, in the sense that rooms appeared elongated in the direction parallel to the stripes' orientation. We conclude that object-based texture effects cannot be generalised to interior space perception. For a room's perceived spatial extent, pattern density is more important than pattern orientation.
Article
Full-text available
A square filled with parallel horizontal or vertical lines appears perceptually extended in the direction orthogonal to the lines. Here, we suggest that this Helmholtz illusion arises due to changes in spatial attention that entail changes at very early stages of perceptual processing. Three experiments are reported which tested this assumption. In Experiment1 and Experiment2, transient attentional cues were flashed in such a way that they either promoted (congruent condition) or hindered (incongruent condition) the attentional state presumably induced by the target objects. We predicted a decline of the illusion in the incongruent condition compared with the congruent condition. This prediction was confirmed in both experiments. However, the influence of (in)congruent attention cuing on the Helmholtz illusion depended on more sustained distributions of attention as well. An influence of sustained attention on the illusion was confirmed in Experiment 3, in which changes of attentional focus were induced by a secondary task. Overall, the results were consistent with our claim that the origin of the Helmholtz illusion is closely linked to the distribution of spatial attention.
Article
Full-text available
A square composed of horizontal lines appears taller and narrower than an identical square made up of vertical lines. Reporting this illusion, Hermann von Helmholtz noted that such illusions, in which filled space seems to be larger than unfilled space, were common in everyday life, adding the observation that ladies' frocks with horizontal stripes make the figure look taller. As this assertion runs counter to modern popular belief, we have investigated whether vertical or horizontal stripes on clothing should make the wearer appear taller or fatter. We find that a rectangle of vertical stripes needs to be extended by 7.1% vertically to match the height of a square of horizontal stripes and that a rectangle of horizontal stripes must be made 4.5% wider than a square of vertical stripes to match its perceived width. This illusion holds when the horizontal or vertical lines are on the dress of a line drawing of a woman. We have examined the claim that these effects apply only for 2-dimensional figures in an experiment with 3-D cylinders and find no support for the notion that horizontal lines would be 'fattening' on clothes. Significantly, the illusion persists when the horizontal or vertical lines are on pictures of a real half-body mannequin viewed stereoscopically. All the evidence supports Helmholtz's original assertion.
Article
Full-text available
In this study, the perceived speed of a tilted line translating horizontally (for a duration of 167 msec) is evaluated with respect to a vertical line undergoing the same translation. Perceived speed of the oblique line is shown to be underestimated when compared to the vertical line. This bias increases: (1) when the line is further tilted, (2) with greater line lengths, (3) with lower contrasts, and finally (4) with a speed of 2.1 deg/sec as compared to a higher speed of 4.2 deg/sec. These results may be accounted for by considering that two velocity signals are used by the visual system to estimate the speed of the line: the translation of this line (this signal does not depend on the line's orientation) and the motion component normal to the line (this signal depends on orientation). We suggest that these two signals are encoded by different types of units and that the translation signal is specifically extracted at the line endings. We further suggest that these signals are integrated by a weighted average process according to their perceptual salience. Other interpretations are considered at the Ught of current models dealing with the two-dimensional integration of different velocity signals.
Article
Full-text available
A subdivided path in the visual field usually appears longer than an empty path of the same length. This phenomenon, known as the filled/empty or Oppel-Kundt illusion, depends on multiple properties of the visual stimulus, but the functional dependences have not been yet precisely characterized. We studied the illusory effect as a function of its two main determinants, the height of vertical strokes subdividing a spatial interval of a fixed length (visual angle 2.8 degree) and the number of the filling strokes, using the standard-variable distance matching paradigm. Non-monotonic dependence of the effect (over-reproduction of the spatial extension) on the varied parameters was observed in two experimental series. In the first series, the maximum effect was obtained for the fillers height roughly equal to the delimiters height (visual angle 0.25 degree); in the second series, the maximum effect was obtained for 11-13 equispaced fillers, and more accurately estimated to 15-16 as a result of a functional fit. Both data series were successfully modeled by curves generated by a single two-parametric system of form functions. Problems of determination of the maximum effect are discussed, and arguments for a genuinely multivariate approach are presented.
Article
Full-text available
A novel illusion in apparent size is reported. We asked observers to estimate the width and depth of vertically oriented elliptic cylinders depicted with texture or luminance gradients (experiment 1), or the height of horizontally oriented elliptic cylinders depicted with binocular disparity (experiment 2). The estimated width or height of cylinders showed systematic shrinkage in the direction of the gradual depth change. The dissimilarity of 2-D appearance amongst our stimuli implies a large variation in spatial-frequency components and brightness contrasts, eliminating the possibility that these parameters contributed to the illusion. Also, the mechanism inappropriately triggered by pictorial depth cues (eg size scaling) may be irrelevant, because the illusion was obtained even when binocular disparity alone specified the shape of the cylinders. The illusion demonstrated here suggests that our visual system may determine the size of 3-D objects by accounting for their depth structures.
Tateshima yokoshima no nazo
  • S Imai
Imai, S. (1982). Tateshima yokoshima no nazo (in Japanese). Psychology, 29, 12. Tokyo: Saiensu-sha.
Handbuch der physiologischen Optik III. Leipzig: Voss. (English translation by J. P. C Southall for the
  • H V Helmholtz
Helmholtz, H. v. (1867). Handbuch der physiologischen Optik III. Leipzig: Voss. (English translation by J. P. C Southall for the Optical Society of America, Treatise on Physiological Optics, volume 3, New York: Dover, 1925).