ArticlePDF Available

Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development: Age- and Sex-Specific Structural Connectivity in Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) Data

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Human brain maturation is characterized by the prolonged development of structural and functional properties of large-scale networks that extends into adulthood. However, it is not clearly understood which features change and which remain stable over time. Here, we examined structural connectivity based on diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) in 121 participants between 4 and 40 years of age. DTI data were analyzed for small-world parameters, modularity, and the number of fiber tracts at the level of streamlines. First, our findings showed that the number of fiber tracts, small-world topology, and modular organization remained largely stable despite a substantial overall decrease in the number of streamlines with age. Second, this decrease mainly affected fiber tracts that had a large number of streamlines, were short, within modules and within hemispheres; such connections were affected significantly more often than would be expected given their number of occurrences in the network. Third, streamline loss occurred earlier in females than in males. In summary, our findings suggest that core properties of structural brain connectivity, such as the small-world and modular organization, remain stable during brain maturation by focusing streamline loss to specific types of fiber tracts.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development: Age- and Sex-Specic
Structural Connectivity in Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) Data
Sol Lim1,2, Cheol E. Han1,3, Peter J. Uhlhaas4,5, 6 and Marcus Kaiser1,2
1
Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 151747, South Korea,
2
School of Computing Science
and Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 7RU, UK,
3
Department of Biomedical Engineering,
Korea University, Seoul 136703, South Korea,
4
Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12
8QB, UK,
5
Department of Neurophysiology, Max-Planck Institute for Brain Research, 60438 Frankfurt a. M., Germany and
6
Ernst
Strüngmann Institute (ESI) for Neuroscience in Cooperation with Max Planck Society, Deutschordenstr. 46, Frankfurt am Main,
60528, Germany
Address correspondence to Marcus Kaiser, School of Computing Science, Claremont Tower, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 7RU,
UK. Email: m.kaiser@ncl.ac.uk
Cheol E. Han, Peter Uhlhaas and Marcus Kaiser shared senior authorship
Human brain maturation is characterized by the prolonged develop-
ment of structural and functional properties of large-scale networks
that extends into adulthood. However, it is not clearly understood
which features change and which remain stable over time. Here, we
examined structural connectivity based on diffusion tensor imaging
(DTI) in 121 participants between 4 and 40 years of age. DTI data
were analyzed for small-world parameters, modularity, and the
number of ber tracts at the level of streamlines. First, our ndings
showed that the number of ber tracts, small-world topology, and
modular organization remained largely stable despite a substantial
overall decrease in the number of streamlines with age. Second, this
decrease mainly affected ber tracts that had a large number of
streamlines, were short, within modules and within hemispheres;
such connections were affected signicantly more often than would
be expected given their number of occurrences in the network.
Third, streamline loss occurred earlier in females than in males. In
summary, our ndings suggest that core properties of structural
brain connectivity, such as the small-world and modular organiz-
ation, remain stable during brain maturation by focusing streamline
loss to specic types of ber tracts.
Keywords: brain connectivity, connectome, maturation, network analysis,
tractography
Introduction
Human brain development is characterized by a protracted tra-
jectory that extends into adulthood (Benes et al. 1994;Sowell
et al. 1999;Lebel and Beaulieu 2011). Evidence from magnetic
resonance imaging (MRI) has indicated a reduction in gray
matter (GM) volume and thickness across large areas of the
cortex and changes in subcortical structures, which may be at-
tributed to synaptic pruning and ingrowth of white matter
(WM) into the peripheral neuropil (Sowell et al. 1999,2001;
Sowell 2004;Giedd 2008;Giedd and Rapoport 2010). In con-
trast, WM-volume increases with age (Giedd et al. 1997,1999;
Paus et al. 1999;Bartzokis et al. 2001;Sowell 2004;Lenroot
et al. 2007) which could reect increased myelination of
axonal connections (Sowell et al. 2001;Sowell 2004).
In addition to volume changes, connectivity changes of
axonal ber bundles have been investigated using diffusion
tensor imaging (DTI). DTI allows the measurement of ber in-
tegrity through estimates of fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean
diffusivity (MD), which presumably relate to changes in axonal
diameter, density, and myelination (Jones 2010;Jbabdi and
Johansen-Berg 2011). Several studies reported increased FA and
decreased MD values from childhood into adulthood in several
major ber tracts and brain regions (Faria et al. 2010;Tamnes
et al. 2010;Westlye et al. 2010;Lebel and Beaulieu 2011).
Brain maturation is also accompanied by changes in the top-
ology of structural and functional networks (Fair et al. 2009;
Gong et al. 2009;Hagmann et al. 2010;Yap et al. 2011;Dennis
et al. 2013). Topological features of neural networks that are
now being linked to cognitive performance (Bullmore and
Sporns 2009) concern their small-world and modular organiz-
ation. For small-world network with brain regions or ROIs as
nodes and ber tracts as edges, there are many connections
between regions mostly located nearby. At the same time, it is
also easy to reach other brain regions far apart in the network
due to the existence of long-range connections or shortcuts
(Watts and Strogatz 1998). Therefore, small-world network
shows high efciency in facilitating information ow at both the
local and the global scales (Latora and Marchiori 2001,2003).
For example, functional connectivity with high global and local
efciency correlates with higher intelligence (Li et al. 2009;van
den Heuvel et al. 2009), while disrupted small-world topology
is associated with impaired cognition (Stam et al. 2007;Nir et al.
2012). For a modular organization, large groups of brain
regions can be considered as network modules (or clusters) if
there are relatively more connections within that group than to
the rest of the network (Hilgetag et al. 2000;Meunier et al.
2010). The higher connectivity within modules can segregate
different types of neural information processing while fewer
connections between modules allow for information inte-
gration. This community structure of the brain network incor-
porating and balancing both segregation and integration of
neural processing has been shown to be disrupted in schizo-
phrenia, autism and Alzheimers disease. (Alexander-Bloch
et al. 2010;de Haan et al. 2012;Shi et al. 2013).
Small-world and modular organization heavily rely on long-
distance connectivity: long ber tracts are more likely to
provide shortcuts for reaching other nodes in the network and
are also more likely to link different network modules (Kaiser
and Hilgetag 2006). For example, connections between hemi-
spheres or between the visual and frontolimbic network
module are long distance. By providing shortcuts, long-distance
© The Author 2013.Published by Oxford University Press.
This is an Open Access article distributedunder the terms of the CreativeCommons Attribution License(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/),which permits unrestricted
reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Page 1 of 13
Cerebral Cortex
doi:10.1093/cercor/bht333
Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published December 15, 2013
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
connections reduce transmission delays and errors, conse-
quently enabling synchronous and more precise information
processing. Conversely, a reduction in long-distance connec-
tivity is well known to impair cognitive ability by adversely af-
fecting efciency and modularity of a network (Kaiser and
Hilgetag 2004). For instance, patients with Alzheimers disease
were shown to lose long-distance projections leading to an in-
crease in functional characteristic path length (Stam et al.
2007). In addition to long-distance connections, intermodule
connections, or ber tracts linking different modules are also
important to keep the community structure of brain networks
and these also provide shortcuts for communicating with other
functional or structural modules. Reduced between-module
connectivity was strongly associated with cognitive impair-
ment in Alzheimers patients (de Haan et al. 2012).
Emerging data suggest that small-world topology and
modular organization in brain networks are already present
during early development (Fan et al. 2011;Yap et al. 2011)and
that these core features of brain networks are retained during
brain maturation despite signicant ongoing anatomical
modications. (Bassett et al. 2008;Fair et al. 2009;Gong et al.
2009;Supekar et al. 2009;Hagmann et al. 2010). Thus, we
hypothesized that certain types of ber tracts may have been
preferentially affected during development to retain important
topological features during development. These potentially
spared ber tract types are likely to include long-distance con-
nections but also ber tracts composed of fewer streamlines
and intermodule ber tracts. Fiber tracts of the latter 2 types
are often, but not necessarily, also long-distance connections
(Supplementary Material S5 and Fig. S4). Therefore, we ana-
lyzed all 3 types of ber tracts in relation to topological changes.
To test our hypothesis, we obtained DTI data from a large
cohort of subjects between 4 and 40 years and constructed
streamlines from deterministic tractography to identify ber
tracts in cortical and subcortical networks. Our results show
that the number of streamlines decreased overall with age
while small-world and modular parameters did not change.
Specically, our results showed that streamline loss occurred
mostly for ber tracts composed of more than average number
of streamlines, short and within-module/within-hemisphere
ber tracts. This focus on certain types of ber tracts goes
beyond what would be expected by a types prevalence within
a network suggesting a preferential detachment of streamlines.
In addition to modications in cortical ber tracts, pronounced
changes were observed in subcortical structures, such as basal
ganglia and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Finally, streamline
reductions occurred at an earlier age in females than in males,
suggesting sex-specic maturation of connectivity patterns
during human brain maturation.
Materials and Methods
DTI Data
We made use of a public DTI database (http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.
org/indi/pro/nki.html) provided by the Nathan Kline Institute (NKI)
(Nooner et al. 2012). DTI data were obtained with a 3 Tesla scanner
(Siemens MAGNETOM TrioTim syngo, Erlangen, Germany). T
1
-weighted
MRI data were obtained with 1 mm isovoxel, FoV 256 mm, TR = 2500 ms,
and TE = 3.5 ms. DTI data were recorded with 2 mm isovoxel, FOV = 256
mm, TR = 10 000 ms, TE = 91 ms, and 64 diffusion directions with b-factor
of 1000 s mm
2
and 12 b0 images. We included 121 participants between
4and40years.
Data Pre-Processing and Network Construction
We used Freesurfer to obtain surface meshes of the boundary between
GM and WM from T
1
anatomical brain images (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.
harvard.edu) (Fig. 1). After registering surface meshes into the DTI
space, we generated volume regions of interest (ROIs) based on GM
voxels. Freesurfer provides parcellation of 34 anatomical regions of
corticesbasedontheDeskianatlas(Fischl et al. 2004;Desikan et al.
2006) and 7 subcortical regions (Nucleus accumbens, Amygdala,
Caudate, Hippocampus, Pallidum, Putamen, and Thalamus) (Fischl
et al. 2002,2004) for each hemisphere, thus leading to 82 ROIs in
total (See Supplementary Table S5 for full and abbreviated names
of ROIs).
To obtain streamline tractography from eddy current-corrected dif-
fusion tensor images (FSL, http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/), we used
the ber assignment by continuous tracking (FACT) algorithm (Mori
and Barker 1999) with 35° of angle threshold through Diffusion toolkit
along with TrackVis (Wang et al. 2007) (Fig. 1). This program gener-
ated the tractography from the center of all voxels (seed voxels) in GM/
WM except ventricles; a single streamline started from the center of
each voxel. Thus, the number of total streamlines never exceeds the
number of seed voxels.
In addition, we also performed tractography with the following par-
ameters: a single tracking per voxel for 45° threshold and 10 random
trackings per voxel for both 35° and 45° thresholds, in total 3 more
cases. These additional analyses were performed to assure that the
results were consistent despite varied tracking parameters (Sup-
plementary Material S6 and Fig. S5).
For network reconstruction, we used the UCLA Multimodal Connec-
tivity Package (UMCP, http://ccn.ucla.edu/wiki/index.php) to obtain
connectivity matrices from the dened and registered ROIs and tracto-
graphy, counting the number of streamlines between all pairs of
dened ROIs. The resulting matrix contains the streamline count
between all pairs of ROIs as its weight. We also computed the average
connection lengths between ROIs (if there is no connection between a
pair, the length was set to zero). The connection length of a streamline
was based on its 3D trajectory.
Network Analysis
Short explanations of network measures are provided here (for
details, cf. Supplementary Material S2). Edge density represents the
proportion of existing connections out of the total number of poten-
tial connections (Kaiser 2011). Note that the weights of individual
edges (streamline count) might change but edge density will remain
the same as long as the total number of edges ( ber tracts) is un-
changed. Small-world topology can be characterized by high global
and local efciency (Latora and Marchiori 2001,2003). Global ef-
ciency represents how efciently neural activity or information is
transferred between any brain regions on average and local efciency
indicates how well neighbors of a region, or nodes that are directly
connected to that region, are interconnected. Efciency is greatly
affected by the sparsity of the network (Kaiser 2011); when there are
fewer edges and also even fewer streamlines, efciency decreases.
Thus, we normalized efciency with values obtained by 100 ran-
domly rewired networks where randomly selected edges were
exchanged while preserving both degree and strength of each node
(Rubinov and Sporns 2011). Modularity Qrepresents how modular
the network is; higher values of Qindicate that modules are more
segregated with fewer connections between modules. In contrast,
lower Qvalues indicate more connections between modules and thus
represent more distributed organization (Newman 2006). We also
compared the modular membership assignment using the normalized
mutual information (NMI) (Alexander-Bloch, Lambiotte, et al. 2012).
Within-module strength and participation coefcient show local
changes in modular organization. Within-module strength indicates
the degree to which a node is connected to others nodes in the same
module (Guimera and Amaral 2005); high within-module strength
implies that the node is more connected to nodes within the module
in which it participates than the average connectivity of the other
nodes in the module. The participation coefcient indicates how well
the node is connected to all other modules with higher values if
2Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development Lim et al.
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
many connections of the node are distributed to other modules. We
used Matlab routines from the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (Rubinov
and Sporns 2010).
Edge Group Analysis
We grouped ber tracts into categories in terms of (a) the number of
streamlines- (thin vs. thick), (b) the length of the streamline trajectory-
(short vs. long), and (c) whether they were within modules (intramo-
dule) or between modules (intermodule) and counted the streamlines
in each group. Then, we examined with general linear model (GLM) if
the number of streamlines in each category changed over age (see Stat-
istical Analysis).
As the spatial (b) and topological (c) properties often overlap but do
not always coincide (Supplementary Material S5 and Fig. S4), we investi-
gated all 3 cases (da Fontoura Costa et al. 2007;Meunier et al. 2010). In
general, short-length and intramodule edges are more numerous than
others. Therefore, largerchanges in those edges would occur for random
selection. Accordingly, we used χ
2
tests to verify any preferential detach-
ment that goes beyond the streamline loss that would be expected based
on the number of ber tracts of each type. We standardized weights and
lengths for each individual and categorized edge into 2 groups by the
mean of each participant to account for differences in brain volume and
size. For instance, an edge or a ber tract for a participant is classied as
thinwhen the weight of the ber tract is less than the average weight
of the participant. Likewise, a ber tract is considered thick when the
weight is above the average of the participant. The same procedure was
performed to differentiate short and long ber tracts. Therefore, types of
ber tracts were distinguished using a subject-specic threshold.
Individual Edge Analysis
In addition to analyzing types of ber tracts differences, we also exam-
ined changes for individual edges that included the subset of total ber
tracts that all participants had in common (128 edges, 32.3% of the
total number of edges 396 ± 20). Note that the total number of edges
was around 400, which is 12% of the total number of possible connec-
tions (n= 3321). This proportion is consistent with previous evidence
suggesting that the human brain has a sparse connectivity ranging
between 10% and 15% (Kaiser 2011). To analyze individual edges,
each edge with signicant age-related changes was mapped to the cor-
responding lobe according to Freesurfer Lobe Mapping (Table 2and
Supplementary Table S4) (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/
CorticalParcellation).
Statistical Analysis
To assess how theoretical graph measures changed during develop-
ment, we used GLM approach (see eqs. 1, 2, and 3). Linear and quadra-
tic effects of age and the interaction between age and gender were
investigated. The quadratic term of age, gender factor, and the inter-
action term between age and gender were dropped and retted when
the effects were not signicant following an F-test as all tested models
were nested. Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and Bayesian Infor-
mation Criterion (BIC) were also used for model comparison and se-
lecting variables when the F-test alone did not provide a strong
preference for a model. As AIC tends to prefer more complex models
with a larger number of variables compared to BIC (Kadane and Lazar
2004), AIC and the F-test provided consistent results in general. When
the results of the 3 tests conicted, we chose the most conservative
model with a smaller number of variables. Two-tailed tests were used for
all analyses and tests were regarded as signicant with an αlevel of 0.05.
Quadratic age effect was found to be signicant in a few ber tracts but
occurred less frequently than linear cases. We therefore chose to report
age effects of the numbers of streamlines where decrease and increase
could follow a linear or, less often, a nonlinear pattern.
y¼
b
0þ
b
1age þ
b
2sex þeð1Þ
y¼
b
0þ
b
1age þ
b
2sex þ
b
3age sex þeð2Þ
y¼
b
0þ
b
1age þ
b
2sex þ
b
3age2þeð3Þ
where yis measurement, β
0
intercept (bias), β
1
slope over age, β
2
coefcient for sex difference, β
3
coefcient for interaction effect of age
and sex or quadratic age effect, and erepresents errors (noise), which
Figure 1. Overall procedure. From T
1
-weighted images, we generated 82 regions of interests (ROIs, 34 cortical areas and 7 subcortical areas per hemisphere, on the left). From
diffusion tensor images (DTI), we reconstructed streamlines using deterministic tracking (on the right). Combining 2 preprocessing steps, we constructed weighted networks, where
the number of streamlines between any pair of ROIs formed the weight of an edge (ber tract).
Cerebral Cortex 3
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
are independent and identically distributed, having a Gaussian (i.e.,
normal) distribution with mean zero and variance σ².
Through the group analysis of edges (see Edge Group Analysis), we
identied which types of edges were undergoing developmental
changes. Using repeated-measures GLM, we tested whether 2 groups
had different slopes and χ
2
tests were used for verifying the slope
difference of GLM considering the proportion of each group with each
individual network. For individual edge analysis, χ
2
tests, and nodal
properties such as within-module strength and participationcoefcients,
false discovery rate (FDR) procedure was used with a qlevel of 0.05, ad-
justing signicance level and condence intervals (Benjamini and Hoch-
berg 1995;Benjamini et al. 2005;Jung et al. 2011). All statistical tests
were calculated in Matlab R2012b (Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA)
and R (R Development Core Team 2011) with R packages (Lemon 2006;
Bengtsson 2013;Sarkar 2008;Weisberg and Fox 2011;Suter 2011).
Results
We performed a combined analysis of ber tracts with network
parameters to examine on-going changes in ber tracts in
terms of small-world topology and modularity, which may
account for a relationship between topological changes and
modications in ber tracts.
We compared developmental changes examining the fol-
lowing features: 1) Overall connectedness: total number of
streamlines, edge density, and thin versus thick connectivity,
2) small-world organization: efciency and short- versus long-
distance connectivity, 3) modular organization: modularity and
within versus between module connectivity, and 4) local
organization: individual edge analysis.
Age Effect for Both Genders
Connectedness
Streamline count versus edge density. The total number of
streamlines decreased (β
1
=68.87, t
(118)
=5.796, P< 0.001,
Fig. 2A) with age; however, edge density remained stable
(t
(118)
= 0.757, P= 0.451, Fig. 2B).
Thick versus thin edges. Edge density or the number of ber
tracts could be maintained either through new ber tracts that
make up for lost ber tracts due to streamline reduction or
through sparing thin edges and therefore retaining existing
ber tracts while changing only weights for ber tracts. To test
the latter hypothesis, we tested whether there were differences
in developmental patterns of thick or thin edges (see Edge
Group Analysis).
Streamlines in both thick and thin edges decreased with age
[thick edges: β
1
=60.184, t
(118)
=6.195, P<0.001, Fig. 2C;
thin edges: β
1
=8.685, t
(118)
=3.27, P= 0.001]. However, the
slopes between thick and thin edges were signicantly differ-
ent (repeated-measures GLM, F
1,119
= 40.196, P<10
8
, Fig. 2C)
with the slope of thick edges showing an 8 times steeper
slope than thin edges. This preferential reduction of stream-
lines within thick edges could not be explained by the frequen-
cies of thin and thick ber tracts (χ
2
test, P<10
20
).
Small-World Topology and Long-Distance Connectivity
Efciency and small-world topology. Global and local
efciency decreased during development (global: β
1
=0.001,
t
(118)
=2.496, P= 0.014, Fig. 2D, local: β
1
=0.019, t
(118)
=
4.435, P< 0.001, Fig. 2E). Although global and local ef-
ciencies may have been slightly compromised by the loss of
streamlines, small-world features were maintained; global ef-
ciency paralleled that of the rewired network (0.88 ± 0.036,
0.9), while local efciency was much higher (4.06 ± 0.446, 4)
than that of the random networks.
Figure 2. Topological and spatial network properties. Fitted lines were drawn when there was a signicant age effect (red: female, blue: male). When multiple lines were drawn,
the lines are parallel unless otherwise noted. Black line represents signicant age affect without a sex difference. (A) Total number of streamlines, (B) edge density, (C) streamline
count in thick versus thin edges, (E) global efciency, (F) local efciency, and (G) streamline count in short versus long streamlines.
4Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development Lim et al.
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Short- versus long-distance connectivity. As topological and
spatial organizations are often linked (Kaiser and Hilgetag
2006;da Fontoura Costa et al. 2007;Meunier et al. 2010), we
tested whether the pattern of changes in short- and long-distance
connectivity corresponded to changes in efciency. From the
preservedsmall-worldtopology,wewouldexpectlongber
tracts were likely to be conserved.
Decreasing slopes of the streamline count between short and
long edges were signicantly different (F
1, 119
= 44.965, P<10
9
)
with short-distance connections showing a pronounced reduction
(short: β
1
=61.515, t
(118)
=6.773, P<10
9
,Fig.2F), which was
not solely explained by a higher proportion of short-distance
edges (χ
2
test, P<10
6
).
Modular Organization
Modularity and module membership assignment. Modularity
did not change with age (t
(118)
=1.335, P= 0.184, Fig. 3A)and
community structure remained stable during development (Sup-
plementary Table S2 and Fig. S1). Overall modular organization
basedontheNMIdidnotdifferacrossage(P= 0.355), and there
were no signicant nodal changes in membership assignment
after multiple comparison correction using FDR (detailed
information cf. Supplementary Material S3).
Within-module strength and participation coefcient. Twenty
of 82 ROIs (24.4%) showed signicant changes in within-module
strengths and participation coefcients (FDR corrected). Overall
changes were asymmetric between hemispheres, affecting
homologous ROIs either in the left or right hemisphere. Ten of
the 24 ROIs (42%) characterized by age effects were areas in
subcorticalregions,suchasthebasalganglia,thalamus,and
nucleus accumbens (Table 1). Specically, within-module
strengths decreased while participation coefcients increased,
indicating that with development connections involving basal
ganglia decreased within its module while connections to the
surrounding modules/regions decreased. In contrast, 8 ROIs
within the ACC and the paralimbic division (Mesulam 2000)were
mainly characterized by increased within-module connectivity
with age.
Within versus between module analysis. Modular membership
and modularity Qstayed relatively stable during development
although there were some ROIs that showed signicant
changes in terms of inter- versus intramodules connectivity
(see Within-Module Strength and Participation Coefcient,
Table 1). This can be realized when changes occurred mainly
within modules. The decreasing slopes of streamline count for
intra- and intermodule edges differed (repeated-measures
GLM, F
1,119
= 33.186, P<10
7
). The reduction of streamlines
occurred within modules (β
1
=61.25, t
(118)
=6.321,
P<10
8
, Fig. 3B) but not between modules (t
(118)
=1.831,
P= 0.0696, Fig. 3B). This preference was not fully explained by
the higher proportion of intramodule edges (χ
2
test, P<10
6
).
Figure 3. Modular organization. (A) Modularity Q,(B) Streamline count in within- versus between-module edges, and (C) individual edge analysis (gray: intramodule edges and light
gray: intermodule edges, both without changes over age; red: edges with a decreased streamline count, blue: edges with an increased streamline count; and yellow: edges with
sex-specic changes). When multiple lines were drawn, the lines are parallel unless otherwise noted. A list of all changes is provided in Table 2 for sex-specic changes and
Supplementary Table S4 for age effect.
Cerebral Cortex 5
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Individual Edge Analysis
To identify edge-specic age effects, we investigated 128
edges found in all participants (total number of edges:
396 ± 20), of which 64 edges showed signicant age-related
changes. The ndings were consistent across different tracto-
graphy parameters (Supplementary Material S6 and Fig. S5).
First, 57 edges (89%) showed developmental changes: 55
edges (86%) showed a reduced number of streamlines while
only 2 (3%) had an increased streamline count (Figs 3Cand
5A, Supplementary Table S4). Reduction of streamlines was
most pronounced in the frontal lobe; increased number of
streamlines only occurred for 2 connections (3%) of cingulate
cortex. These changes for both genders mainly occurred in the
frontal and parietal lobe.
Sex-Specic Age-Related Changes
Unlike developmental changes for both males and females,
only several network properties showed sex-specic develop-
mental changes. While both male and females lost short
streamlines, only female participants were characterized by a
decrease in long streamlines. However, this decrease was less
pronounced than the reduction in short streamlines (β
1
=
21.229, t
(50)
=3.372, P= 0.001, Fig. 2F). While global
modular organization (see Modularity and Module Member-
ship Assignment) did not show sex differences, 3 regions of 20
showed sex-specic developmental changes in within-module
strength and participation coefcients (Table 1). In the individ-
ual ber tract analysis, changes that only affected one gender
occurred in 7 ber tracts (11%) (Figs 46B, Table 2). There
were 4 edges with age effect only in females, and 3 edges only
in males, mostly involving occipital and parietal regions.
Sex Differences Independent of Age
Males had 800 more streamlines than females across age
(t
(118)
=3.949, P<0.001, Fig. 2A) mainly due to larger brain
size.In particular, males had larger number of streamlines
for within-module edges (Supplementary Fig. S2). Although
males showed a substantially larger number of streamlines,
male and female participants demonstrated comparable edge
density (t
(118)
=0.880, P= 0.381, Fig. 2B) as well as global ef-
ciency (Global: t
(118)
= 1.598, P= 0.113, Fig. 2D). However,
females showed higher local efciency than males (Local:
t
(118)
= 2.891, P= 0.005, Fig. 2E). Modularity (t
(118)
=0.409,
P= 0.684, Fig. 3A) and overall modular organization based on
the NMI also did not differ between genders (P= 0.177). Most
ROIs did not show gender differences in within-module
strength and participation coefcient except 4 ROIs (Table 1).
Discussion
In this study, we investigated changes in structural connectivity
(SC) between ages of 440 years from DTI data in cortical and
subcortical regions. Previous studies had shown that the
human brain undergoes vast structural changes involving al-
terations in the topology of structural and functional connec-
tivity. Yet, core properties such as small-world topology and
modular organization were retained throughout development
(Fair et al. 2009;Gong et al. 2009;Supekar et al. 2009;
Hagmann et al. 2010;Dennis et al. 2013). Therefore, we
Table 1
ROIs with age effect in within-module strength (WMS) and participation coefcient (PC)
Increased Decreased Sex-specic
WMS lh.caudalanteriorcingulate (F)
lh.entorhinal (T)
lh.parahippocampal (T)
rh.caudalanteriorcingulate (F)
rh.rostralanteriorcingulate (F)
lh.thalamus
lh.accumbens
rh.putamen ( f > m)
rh.pallidum
lh.putamen
M: decreased
rh.paracentral (F)
M:Increased
PC lh.putamen
lh.pallidum
rh.caudate
rh.putamen (m > f )
rh.pallidum (m > f )
rh.caudalanteriorcingulate (F)
rh.paracentral (F)
rh.posteriorcingulate (P)
lh.medialorbitofrontal (F)
M: increased
rh.insula (m > f )
Note: Basal ganglia showing a more distributed network and anterior cingulate cortex showed a more focused connectivity within its module (bold).
FDR corrected, with a qlevel of 0.05.
F, frontal lobe; P, parietal lobe; T, temporal lobe; O, occipital lobe; lh, left hemisphere; rh, right hemisphere; f, female; m, male.
Table 2
Edges with sex-specic age-related changes
ROI (node) Lobe ROI (node) Lobe Sex Slope FDR-adjusted P
lh.cuneus O lh.pericalcarine O Male 1.035 0.0002
lh.fusiform T lh.lateraloccipital O Female 0.9 0.041
lh.lingual O lh.pericalcarine O Female 0.535 0.041
lh.transversetemporal T lh.insula Male 0.908 0.0002
rh.postcentral P rh.insula Male 0.747 0.001
rh.medialorbitofrontal F rh.rostralanteriorcingulate P Female 0.769 0.0003
rh.precuneus P rh.superiorparietal P Female 1.351 0.023
F:1 P:4 T:2 O:5
Note: Sex-specic developmental changes were asymmetrical compared to the developmental changes for both genders.
The last row gives an overview of how often different lobes participate in these changes. Pvalues were adjusted by FDR with a qlevel of 0.05.
lh, left hemisphere; rh, right hemisphere; m, male; f, female; F, frontal lobe; P, parietal lobe; T, temporal lobe; O, occipital lobe.
6Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development Lim et al.
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
examined if specic types of ber tracts were preferentially af-
fected, which might be conducive to conserving major topolo-
gical features. Our results show that small-world features, the
number of ber tracts, and the modular organization remained
largely stable over age despite a signicant reduction of
streamlines in ber tracts. This reduction preferentially affected
ber tracts that were relatively short, consisted of more stream-
lines and were within topological modules (Fig. 7A,B). Finally,
streamline loss occurred at an earlier age in females than in
males.
Stable Small-World and Modular Organization with
Preferential Streamline Loss Within Short-Distance,
Thick, and Intramodular Fiber Tracts
We found that fewer long-distance, thin, and intermodular
ber tracts showed streamline loss than would be expected
given how often such ber tracts could have been affected by
chance. This preferential streamline loss has several impli-
cations for the stable topological features that we observed.
First, we found that small-world features were retained over
age despite the overall reduction in the number of streamlines.
A signicant decrease in many long-distance streamlines
would remove shortcuts and result in larger path lengths and
reduced global efciency while fewer connections between
neighbors would decrease local clustering and local efciency,
disrupting small-world features of a brain network. However,
global efciency stayed comparable with that of rewired net-
works, local efciency was much higher than in rewired
networks across age, conserving small-world topology (Latora
and Marchiori 2001,2003). We would therefore expect
changes mainly in short-distance connectivity. Indeed, short
streamlines were mostly affected and long-distance connec-
tivity was rather preserved. Relatively conserved streamlines in
long-distance ber tracts could be achieved by strengthening
long-range pathways while a reduction in the number of
streamlines in short ber tracts could be due to weakening of
short connections, which is consistent with previous ndings
from rs-fMRI and DTI data (Fair et al. 2009;Supekar et al.
2009;Dosenbach et al. 2010;Hagmann et al. 2010).
Second, in line with previous rs-fMRI and DTI studies (Fair
et al. 2009;Hagmann et al. 2010), modularity Qremained
stable over age. We found that the global modular organization
and module membership of ROIs were unchanged with local
changes especially in the basal ganglia. Therefore, local net-
works re-organized their relationships with other community
members while keeping the global community structure
stable. This retained modular organization (Kaiser et al. 2010;
Meunier et al. 2010) might be crucial in keeping the balance
between information integration and the segregation of separ-
ate processing streams (Sporns 2011). Too many connections
between modules would interfere with different processing
demands, for example, leading to interference between visual
and auditory processing. In addition, more intermodule con-
nections would also facilitate activity spreading potentially
leading to large-scale activation as observed during epileptic
seizures (Kaiser et al. 2007;Kaiser and Hilgetag 2010).
Figure 4. Sex-specic developmental changes in individual edge analysis for male (AC) and for female subjects (DF), where red edges represent signicant decrease, blue
edges indicate signicant increases over development, gray edges illustrate the tested edges that all subjects shared in common and the sex-specic changes were emphasized by
the thick edges. (Aand C) Sagittal views of the left hemisphere, (Band D) transverse view, and (Cand F) sagittal views of the right hemisphere, of male and female brains,
respectively. (A) Two edges showed age-related changes; one in the temporal lobe lost streamlines and the other edge in the occipital lobe gained streamlines. (C) An edge in the
parietal lobe lost streamlines. (D). Two edges in the temporal and the occipital lobes lost streamlines. (F) Two edges in the frontal and parietal lobes lost streamlines.
Cerebral Cortex 7
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
However, because of the reduction of streamlines in intramo-
dule edges, proportionally intermodule connections increased,
indicating that the brain network became more distributed
rather than modular with age as observed in previous studies,
which was associated with development of advanced cognitive
abilities by enhancing integration of neural processing (Fair
et al. 2009;Supekar et al. 2009;Hagmann et al. 2010).
In summary, we nd that long-distance and intermodular
connectivity is largely spared from the ongoing streamline
losses during development, which is potentially benecial for
the observed stability of small-world and modular connectome
features. Note that as connections between modules are not
necessarily long distance (Kaiser and Hilgetag 2006), we found
that only 47% of intermodular ber tracts also belong to the
class of long-distance connections. Retaining long-distance
and intermodular bers indicate that small-world features,
such as the number of processing steps but also the balance
between information integration and large-scale brain activity,
are kept within a critical range during development (Kaiser
and Hilgetag 2006). Preserving this balance is crucial as
changes in long-distance connectivity are a hallmark of
neurodegenerative and neurodevelopmental disorders ranging
from Alzheimers disease (Ponten et al. 2007;Stam et al. 2007)
to schizophrenia (Alexander-Bloch, Vértes, et al. 2012). There-
fore, stable topological network features might help to prevent
cognitive decits in neuropsychiatric disorders.
Another important implication of the reduced number of
streamlines is the relationship to the number of edges within a
network. Changes in streamline count can lead to a reduction
of connections within a network if an edge comprised of few
streamlines loses all its streamlines, thus reducing edge
density. However, edge density did not signicantly change
during brain maturation. Therefore, several mechanisms are
conceivable how the number of edges is maintained during de-
velopment. One option is that newly emerging edges cancel
out disappearing edges (equilibrium state), which is biologi-
cally costly by removing already established connections and
unlikely because new connections are established mostly early
in the development. Alternatively, only the weight of an edge
changes (stable state). For the latter case, a reduction of stream-
lines in thin edges, which could result in the loss of the whole
edge, needs to be prohibited. Indeed, we found that thick
Figure 5. Sex-specic developmental changes. (AG) Scatter plots of streamline count with relevant tted lines. Red: female, blue: male. Upper panel: The 4 ber tracts
demonstrating age effects only for females. Lower panel: The 3 ber tracts displaying age effects only for males. Lh, left hemisphere; rh, right hemisphere. (A) The ber tract
between lh.fusiform and lh.lateraloccipital showing a reduction of streamline counts only for females. (B). The ber tract between lh.lingual and lh.pericalcarine with a decreased
number of streamlines for females, (C). rh.medialorbitofrontalrh.rostralanteriorcingulate, (D)
rh.precuneus-rh.superiorparietal
, (E) The ber tract between lh. transversetemporal
and lh.insula with a reduced number of streamlines over age only for males, (F) rh.postcentralrh.insula, (G) lh.cuneuslh.pericalcarine. The rate of change per year and
corresponding P value is included in the gure and FDR-adjusted P values can be found in Table 2.
8Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development Lim et al.
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
edges were mostly affected from the decreased streamlines,
thus preserving the structure of the network. This is benecial,
as reducing thin bers would necessitate an increase in synap-
tic weights or number of synapses to transmit the same
amount of information. Reducing streamlines for thick bers,
on the other hand, has only a small effect on activity ow due
to the large number of remaining streamlines.
Preferential Streamline Loss for Frontal and
Subcortical Regions
Changes in individual edges were most pronounced in the
frontal lobe, a brain region that is characterized by protracted
development until the third and fourth decade of life as indi-
cated by ongoing synaptic pruning and myelination (Benes
et al. 1994;Sowell et al. 1999;Shaw et al. 2008;Petanjek et al.
2011). In addition, the ber tract between putamen and palli-
dum in the basal ganglia for the left hemisphere was character-
ized by a reduced number of streamlines. Previous studies that
examined GM volume (Sowell et al. 1999) also found changes
in GM density in putamen and pallidum in postadolescent
brain development, which are involved in learning and neuro-
development diseases (DeLong et al. 1984;Alexander and
Crutcher 1990;Hokama et al. 1995;Teicher et al. 2000;Ell
et al. 2006;DeLong Mr 2007;de Jong et al. 2008;Farid and
Mahadun 2009). Furthermore, basal ganglia were character-
ized by decreased within-module strengths and increased par-
ticipation coefcients over age. This suggests that connectivity
to within these areas decreased relative to connections to
outside of the basal ganglia, which is consistent with data from
Supekar et al. (2009) who demonstrated that subcortical func-
tional connectivity in children had higher degree and ef-
ciency than in adults.
This reorganization of corticosubcortical connectivity could
be involved in the ongoing changes of cognition and behavior
during development. The basal ganglia involve regions that are
crucially involved in neural circuits relevant for response inhi-
bition and reward modulation. Previous studies have shown
that response inhibition improves signicantly with age (Wil-
liams et al. 1999) as well as reward modulation (Gardner and
Steinberg 2005). Unlike for the basal ganglia, the ACC was
characterized by an increased connectivity within its module
with age. This observation is consistent with functional con-
nectivity of ACC that develops a more focal organization with
age (Kelly et al. 2009). ACC has also shown to mature late
through error-related ERPs (Santesso and Segalowitz 2008).
Delayed Streamline Loss for Males
Individual edge analysis revealed sex-specic age effects in the
occipital and parietal lobe but to a much lesser extent in the
frontal lobe. This is consistent with a previous WM study
where mainly the occipital lobe development varied with sex
while the growth trajectory in the frontal lobe was similar for
both genders (Baron-Cohen et al. 2005;Lenroot et al. 2007;
Giedd 2008;Perrin et al. 2009). These results can be explained
if we assume that the same mechanism of preferential stream-
line loss operates at different time-scales in males and females.
Provided that males had a similar developmental curve but
with a shifted peak (Fig. 7C,D), we can explain the sex-specic
changes. As expected from the shifted peak hypothesis
(Fig. 7C,D), the total number of streamlines for males, but not
females, remained stable at an earlier age range (428 years,
not shown) while both genders showed streamline reductions
in the age range 440 years. This delayed developmental
growth curve in streamline count can be related to later volume
growth peaks for GM and WM in males (Giedd et al. 1997;
Giedd and Rapoport 2010) and earlier myelination for females
(Benes et al. 1994).
Figure 6. Individual edge slopes representing age effect per year with FDR-adjusted condence intervals. (A) Individual edge age effect for both genders. x-axis: indices of edges,
y-axis: coefcients for age effect per year with FDR-adjusted condence intervals. The last 2 edges with positive slopes and condence interval ranges are the edges with an
increased streamline count and the others are the ber tracts characterized by a decreased number of streamlines. (B) Age-related sex effect. x-axis: indices of edges, First 4 edges
show decreasing rate of streamline count for females and the rest 3 edges displays age effect for males, y-axis: coefcients for age effect per year with FDR-adjusted condence
intervals.
Cerebral Cortex 9
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
We only observed circumscribed sex-differences indepen-
dent of age. Local efciency was higher for females than males
consistent with Gong and colleaguesnding (Gong et al.
2009) and some ROIs showing higher within-module strength
and lower participation coefcient for females can be related
to higher local efciency in females. Interestingly, absolute
difference in the number of streamlines between genders was
not uniformly distributed; males exhibited more streamlines
for intramodule edges. This is consistent with the nding that
males and females do not differ in the WM volume growth tra-
jectory in the corpus callosum (Giedd 2008). However, this
means proportionally females have more connections across
hemispheres and between modules (DeLacoste-Utamsing and
Holloway 1982;Davatzikos and Resnick 2002;Allen et al.
2003).
Structural Correlates of Streamline Loss
The observed reduction in the total number of streamlines
could be related to rs-fMRI developmental system-level
pruning(Supekar et al. 2009), considering tight coupling
between SC and functional connectivity (Honey et al. 2009,
2010). As Supekar and colleagues suggested for functional
connectivity (Supekar et al. 2009), the decreased number of
streamlines for short and intramodule connections in this
study could be due to weakening of local connections through
synaptic pruning and neuronal rewiring. These local processes
prolong until adulthood and are major factors for anatomical
developmental changes (Benes et al. 1994;Petanjek et al.
2011). The reduction of synapses and corresponding axons or
axon collaterals could potentially also lead to a decreased
number of streamlines within ber tracts. Owing to technical
limitations of DTI, pruning of dendrites and intracortical con-
nections cannot be detected. However, synaptic pruning in the
prefrontal cortex for intracortical connections (Petanjek et al.
2011) was mainly limited to children at younger ages than in
our study (Petanjek et al. 2008). In contrast, the pruning of
long-distance connection, observable in DTI, occurs in devel-
oping rhesus monkeys, both at earlier and later stages of devel-
opment (LaMantia and Rakic 1990,1994;Luo and OLeary
2005). Considering both limitations of DTI (Jones and
Leemans 2011) and previous studies (Fair et al. 2009;Supekar
et al. 2009;Dosenbach et al. 2010), changes in corticocortical
and subcorticocortical projections might underlie our results
but further investigations are needed to determine the contri-
butions of these potential biological correlates.
Studies have shown that volume for WM ber tracts in-
creased with age (Faria et al. 2010;Lebel and Beaulieu 2011)
and continued myelination also leads to an increase in WM
volume, which could explain an increase in total WM volume
while undergoing a possible reduction of ber tracts. Even
though streamlines were reduced in our study, an increased
myelination might still have taken place but might have been
overshadowed by axonal changes and vice versa. Greater
amounts of myelination would generate higher FA values
(Mädler et al. 2008;Faria et al. 2010), leading to an increase in
Figure 7. (Aand B) The schematic summary of the preferential reduction of thick, short, and within-module streamlines over age. (A) Location of change: 2 ellipses represent left
and right hemispheres and small circles inside hemispheres indicate ROIs. Lines connecting ROIs illustrate ber tracts between ROIs. Red lines are where the reduction of
streamlines occurred; thick, short or intramodule edges were mostly affected. (B) Magnitude of change: Short, thick, or intramodule edges lost more streamlines than long, thin, or
intermodule edges. x-axis: either long, thin, or intermodule streamline count (SC), y-axis: either short, thick, or intramodule SC. (Cand D) Hypothetical developmental curves for
males (blue) and females (red). (C) For the total streamline count based on the observation of our data (Fig. 2A): a longer lasting and higher peaked increase and a delayed decrease
in males. (D) For individual edges: we observed sex-specic development (Fig. 4C), which can be explained by 3 representative cases: if the 2 curves strongly overlap they show
similar decreasing patterns (case 1), if one of the curves peaks later, one curve shows a decreasing pattern while the other curve is still increasing (case 3) or simply not decreasing
yet (case 2). Therefore, depending on the time scale of the developmental trajectory, males and females may show different patterns.
10 Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development Lim et al.
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
the number of detected streamlines. For example, even if the
number of axonal projections were reduced the remaining
bers with an increased myelination could be detected easily
by tractography and compensate the lost ber tracts, leading to
no changes in the number of streamlines. Thus, the balance
between myelination and axonal pruning may have contribu-
ted to our nal results.
The reduction in streamlines with age cannot be attributable
to ongoing changes in the number of seed voxels used for trac-
tography as this number was unaffected by age. Other factors
affecting tractography include axon diameter distributions
(See detailed Discussion Jones 2010;Jones et al. 2013) and
ber curvature changes. If many ber tracts became more
curved over age, as DTI normally does not track highly curved
trajectories, the number of streamlines of the ber tract could
decrease. However, most of the ber tracts (edges) that we
tested did not change their curvature over age (83%, 106 of
128) only 22 edges (17%) showed changed curvature over de-
velopment. Of these 22, only half showed curvature increase.
For a single edge we nd streamline decrease while curvature
increased ruling out curvature as a confounding factor of our
results (See detailed Results and Discussion in Supplementary
Material S4).
Limitations
Even though the current study observes a large dataset, there
are several inherent limitations. First, the subjects were
unequally distributed across ages. Having subjects at ages
between 4 and 40 years may not be optimal for detecting major
changes as small-world and modular features were established
during the rst 2 years (Fan et al. 2011;Yap et al. 2011). Our
focus, however, was not the major structural changes but the
continuous development while keeping the network economic
(Vertes et al. 2012) and stable. Second, studies that network ap-
proaches use different denitions for weight and different nor-
malization schemes complicating the comparison between
studies. We used absolute number of streamlines as weights;
however, our results are consistent with previous studies with
slightly different weight denitions (Gong et al. 2009;
Hagmann et al. 2010). Third, our DTI approach, unlike DSI or
HARDI analysis, will not resolve crossing bers. However, the
shorter recording time of this data are an advantage when
measuring connectivity in children. Modeling through prob-
abilistic tracking with crossing bers (Behrens et al. 2007;
Jbabdi and Johansen-Berg 2011) would therefore be a future
research direction. Although streamlines do not directly corre-
spond to axonal projections (Jones 2010;Jones et al. 2013), we
found our results were consistent with previous anatomical
studies (Benes et al. 1994;Sowell et al. 1999;Gong et al. 2009;
Perrin et al. 2009).
Conclusions
The human brain undergoes vast structural changes during de-
velopment. Nonetheless, brain networks develop in a way that
preserves its topological (small-world/modular) and spatial
(long-distance connectivity) organization to secure its capability
of integration of information and individual processing of
modules. This present study showed how brain connectivity
changed during development in terms of ber tracts as well as
global network features. We showed preferential decreases
in the number of streamlines for thick, short-distance, and
within-module/within-hemisphere ber tracts. These changes
may not necessarily occur at the same time for males and
females; males seem to show a delayed start from the prolonged
development in WM and GM. However, although with different
time courses between genders, the global topological features
ensuring healthy brain development apply to both genders.
Therefore, brain networks maintain their topological stability
during brain development by preferentially modifying structural
connectivity.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material can be found at: http://www.cercor.oxford-
journals.org/.
Funding
This work was supported by National Research Foundation of
Korea funded by the Ministry of Education, Science and Tech-
nology (R32-10142 to S.L., C.E.H., and M.K.), the National Re-
search Foundation of the Korea government (MSIP NRF,
2010-0028631 to C.E.H.), the Royal Society (RG/2006/R2 to
M.K.), the CARMEN e-Science project (http://www.carmen.org.
uk) funded by EPSRC (EP/E002331/1, EP/K026992/1, EP/
G03950X/1 to M.K.), and Max-Planck Society to P.J.U. Funding
to pay the Open Access publication charges for this article was
provided by EPSRC. Conict of Interest: None declared.
References
Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. 1990. Preparation for movement: neural
representations of intended direction in three motor areas of the
monkey. J Neurophysiol. 64:133150.
Alexander-Bloch A, Lambiotte R, Roberts B, Giedd J, Gogtay N, Bull-
more E. 2012. The discovery of population differences in network
community structure: new methods and applications to brain func-
tional networks in schizophrenia. NeuroImage. 59:38893900.
Alexander-Bloch AF, Gogtay N, Meunier D, Birn R, Clasen L, Lalonde F,
Lenroot R, Giedd J, Bullmore ET. 2010. Disrupted modularity and
local connectivity of brain functional networks in childhood-onset
schizophrenia. Front Syst Neurosci. 4:147.
Alexander-Bloch AF, Vértes PE, Stidd R, Lalonde F, Clasen L, Rapoport
J, Giedd J, Bullmore ET, Gogtay N. 2012. The anatomical distance
of functional connections predicts brain network topology in
health and Schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 23:127138.
Allen JS, Damasio H, Grabowski TJ, Bruss J, Zhang W. 2003. Sexual di-
morphism and asymmetries in the gray-white composition of the
human cerebrum. Neuroimage. 18:880894.
Baron-Cohen S, Knickmeyer RC, Belmonte MK. 2005. Sex differences
in the brain: implications for explaining autism. Science.
310:819823.
Bartzokis G, Beckson M, Lu PH, Nuechterlein KH, Edwards N, Mintz J.
2001. Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in
men: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry.
58:461465.
Bassett DS, Bullmore E, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR,
Meyer-Lindenberg A. 2008. Hierarchical organization of human corti-
cal networks in health and schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 28:92399248.
Behrens TE, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MF, Woolrich MW. 2007.
Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple bre orien-
tations: what can we gain? Neuroimage. 34:144155.
Benes F, Turtle M, Khan Y, Farol P. 1994. Myelination of a key relay
zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain
during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Arch Gen Psychia-
try. 51:477484.
Cerebral Cortex 11
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Bengtsson H. 2013. R.matlab: Read and write of MAT les together
with R-to-MATLAB connectivity. R package version 2.0.5.
Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a
practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc
Series B Stat Methodol. 57:289300.
Benjamini Y, Yekutieli D, Don E, Shaffer JP, Tamhane AC, Westfall PH,
Holland B. 2005. False discovery rate: adjusted multiple condence
intervals for selected parameters [with comments, rejoinder]. J Am
Stat Assoc. 100:7193.
Bullmore E, Sporns O. 2009. Complex brain networks: graph theoreti-
cal analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci.
10:186198.
da Fontoura Costa L, Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC. 2007. Predicting the con-
nectivity of primate cortical networks from topological and spatial
node properties. BMC Syst Biol. 1:16.
Davatzikos C, Resnick SM. 2002. Degenerative age changes in white
matter connectivity visualized in vivo using magnetic resonance
imaging. Cereb Cortex. 12:767771.
de Haan W, van der Flier WM, Koene T, Smits LL, Scheltens P, Stam CJ.
2012. Disrupted modular brain dynamics reect cognitive dysfunc-
tion in Alzheimers disease. NeuroImage. 59:30853093.
de Jong LW, van der Hiele K, Veer IM, Houwing JJ, Westendorp RG,
Bollen EL, de Bruin PW, Middelkoop HA, van Buchem MA, van der
Grond J. 2008. Strongly reduced volumes of putamen and thalamus
in Alzheimers disease: an MRI study. Brain. 131:32773285.
DeLacoste-Utamsing C, Holloway RL. 1982. Sexual dimorphism in the
human corpus callosum. Science. 216:14311432.
DeLong MR, Alexander GE, Georgopoulos AP, Crutcher MD, Mitchell
SJ, Richardson RT. 1984. Role of basal ganglia in limb movements.
Hum Neurobiol. 2:235244.
DeLong Mr WT. 2007. Circuits and circuit disorders of the basal
ganglia. Arch Neurol. 64:2024.
Dennis EL, Jahanshad N, McMahon KL, de Zubicaray GI, Martin NG,
Hickie IB, Toga AW, Wright MJ, Thompson PM. 2013. Development
of brain structural connectivity between ages 12 and 30: a 4-tesla
diffusion imaging study in 439 adolescents and adults. Neuro-
Image. 64:671684.
Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D,
Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT et al. 2006. An auto-
mated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex
on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage.
31:968980.
Dosenbach NUF, Nardos B, Cohen AL, Fair DA, Power JD, Church JA,
Nelson SM, Wig GS, Vogel AC, Lessov-Schlaggar CN. 2010. Predic-
tion of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science. 329:
13581361.
Ell SW, Marchant NL, Ivry RB. 2006. Focal putamen lesions impair
learning in rule-based, but not information-integration categoriz-
ation tasks. Neuropsychologia. 44:17371751.
Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NUF, Church JA, Miezin FM,
Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2009. Functional brain networks
develop from a local to distributedorganisation. PLoS Comput
Biol. 5:e1000381.
Fan Y, Shi F, Smith JK, Lin W, Gilmore JH, Shen D. 2011. Brain anatom-
ical networks in early human brain development. NeuroImage.
54:18621871.
Faria AV, Zhang J, Oishi K, Li X, Jiang H, Akhter K, Hermoye L, Lee
S-K, Hoon A, Stashinko E et al. 2010. Atlas-based analysis of neuro-
development from infancy to adulthood using diffusion tensor
imaging and applications for automated abnormality detection.
NeuroImage. 52:415428.
Farid F, Mahadun P. 2009. Schizophrenia-like psychosis following left
putamen infarct: a case report. J Med Case Rep. 3:13.
Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van
der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S et al. 2002.
Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical
structures in the human brain. Neuron. 33:341355.
Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Segonne F, Salat
DH, Busa E, Seidman LJ, Goldstein J, Kennedy D et al. 2004. Auto-
matically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex.
14:1122.
Gardner M, Steinberg L. 2005. Peer inuence on risk taking, risk pre-
ference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood:
an experimental study. Dev Psychol. 41:625635.
Giedd JN. 2008. The teen brain: insights from neuroimaging. J Adolesc
Health. 42:335343.
Giedd JN, Castellanos FX, Rajapakse JC, Vaituzis AC, Rapoport JL.
1997. Sexual dimorphism of the developing human brain. Prog
Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 21:11851201.
Giedd JN, Jeffries NO, Blumenthal J, Castellanos F, Vaituzis AC, Fernan-
dez T, Hamburger SD, Liu H, Nelson J, Bedwell J. 1999.
Childhood-onset schizophrenia: progressive brain changes during
adolescence. Biol Psychiatry. 46:892898.
Giedd JN, Rapoport JL. 2010. Structural MRI of pediatric brain develop-
ment: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron.
67:728734.
Gong G, Rosa-Neto P, Carbonell F, Chen ZJ, He Y, Evans AC. 2009.
Age- and gender-related differences in the cortical anatomical
network. J Neurosci. 29:1568415693.
Guimera R, Amaral LA. 2005. Cartography of complex networks:
modules and universal roles. J Stat Mech. 2005:nihpa35573.
Hagmann P, Sporns O, Madan N, Cammoun L, Pienaar R, Wedeen VJ,
Meuli R, Thiran JP, Grant PE. 2010. White matter maturation re-
shapes structural connectivity in the late developing human brain.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 107:1906719072.
Hilgetag CC, Burns GAPC, ONeill MA, Scannell JW, Young MP. 2000.
Anatomical connectivity denes the organization of clusters of cor-
tical areas in the macaque monkey and the cat. Philos Trans R Soc
Lond B Biol Sci. 355:91110.
Hokama H, Shenton ME, Nestor PG, Kikinis R, Levitt JJ, Metcalf D,
Wible CG, ODonnell BF, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW. 1995. Caudate,
putamen, and globus pallidus volume in schizophrenia: a quantitat-
ive MRI study. Psychiatry Res. 61:209229.
Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran JP, Meuli R,
Hagmann P. 2009. Predicting human resting-state functional con-
nectivity from structural connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA.
106:20352040.
Honey CJ, Thivierge J-P, Sporns O. 2010. Can structure predict function
in the human brain? Neuroimage. 52:766776.
Jbabdi S, Johansen-Berg H. 2011. Tractography: where do we go from
here? Brain Connect. 1:169183.
Jones DK. 2010. Challenges and limitations of quantifying brain con-
nectivity in vivo with diffusion MRI. Imaging. 2:341355.
Jones DK, Knösche TR, Turner R. 2013. White matter integrity, ber
count, and other fallacies: the dos and donts of diffusion MRI.
NeuroImage. 73:239254.
Jones DK, Leemans A. 2011. Diffusion tensor imaging. In: Modo M,
Bulte JWM, editors. Magnetic resonance neuroimaging. New York
(NY): Humana Press. p. 127144.
Jung K, Friede T, Beißbarth T. 2011. Reporting FDR analogous con-
dence intervals for the log fold change of differentially expressed
genes. BMC Bioinformatics. 12:288.
Kadane JB, Lazar NA. 2004. Methods and criteria for model selection. J
Am Stat Assoc. 99:279290.
Kaiser M. 2011. A tutorial in connectome analysis: topological and
spatial features of brain networks. Neuroimage. 57:892907.
Kaiser M, Görner M, Hilgetag CC. 2007. Functional criticality in clus-
tered networks without inhibition. New J Phys. 9:110.
Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC. 2004. Edge vulnerability in neural and meta-
bolic networks. Biol Cybern. 90:311317.
Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC. 2006. Nonoptimal component placement, but
short processing paths, due to long-distance projections in neural
systems. PLoS Comput Biol. 2:e95.
Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC. 2010. Optimal hierarchical modular topologies
for producing limited sustained activation of neural networks.
Front Neuroinform. 4:8.
Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC, Kötter R. 2010. Hierarchy and dynamics of
neural networks. Front Neuroinform. 4:112.
Kelly AC, Di Martino A, Uddin LQ, Shehzad Z, Gee DG, Reiss PT, Mar-
gulies DS, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. 2009. Development of
anterior cingulate functional connectivity from late childhood to
early adulthood. Cereb Cortex. 19:640657.
12 Preferential Detachment During Human Brain Development Lim et al.
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
LaMantia A, Rakic P. 1990. Axon overproduction and elimination in the
corpus callosum of the developing rhesus monkey. J Neurosci.
10:21562175.
LaMantia AS, Rakic P. 1994. Axon overproduction and elimination in
the anterior commissure of the developing rhesus monkey. J Comp
Neurol. 340:328336.
Latora V, Marchiori M. 2003. Economic small-world behavior in
weighted networks. Eur Phys J B Condens Matter. 32:249263.
Latora V, Marchiori M. 2001. Efcient behavior of small-world net-
works. Phys Rev Lett. 87:198701.
Lebel C, Beaulieu C. 2011. Longitudinal development of human brain
wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. J Neurosci.
31:1093710947.
Lemon J. 2006. Plotrix: a package in the red light district of R. R-News.
6:812.
Lenroot RK, Gogtay N, Greenstein DK, Wells EM, Wallace GL, Clasen
LS, Blumenthal JD, Lerch J, Zijdenbos AP, Evans AC. 2007. Sexual
dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood
and adolescence. Neuroimage. 36:10651073.
Li Y, Liu Y, Li J, Qin W, Li K, Yu C, Jiang T. 2009. Brain anatomical
network and intelligence. PLoS Comput Biol. 5:e1000395.
Luo L, OLeary DD. 2005. Axon retraction and degeneration in develop-
ment and disease. Annu Rev Neurosci. 28:127156.
Mädler B, Drabycz SA, Kolind SH, Whittall KP, MacKay AL. 2008. Is dif-
fusion anisotropy an accurate monitor of myelination? Correlation
of multicomponent T2 relaxation and diffusion tensor anisotropy in
human brain. Magn Reson Imaging. 26:874888.
Mesulam MM. 2000. Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology.
New York (NY): Oxford University Press.
Meunier D, Lambiotte R, Bullmore ET. 2010. Modular and hierarchically
modular organization of brain networks. Front Neurosci. 4:200.
Mori S, Barker PB. 1999. Diffusion magnetic resonance imaging: its
principle and applications. Anat Rec. 257:102109.
Newman ME. 2006. Modularity and community structure in networks.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 103:85778582.
Nir T, Jahanshad N, Jack CR, Weiner MW, Toga AW, Thompson PM.
2012. Small world network measures predict white matter degener-
ation in patients with early-stage mild cognitive impairment. Proc
IEEE Int Symp Biomed Imaging. 14051408.
Nooner KB, Colcombe S, Tobe R, Mennes M, Benedict M, Moreno A,
Panek L, Brown S, Zavitz S, Li Q. 2012. The NKI-Rockland sample:
a model for accelerating the pace of discovery science in psychiatry.
Front Neurosci. 6:152.
Paus T, Zijdenbos A, Worsley K, Collins DL, Blumenthal J, Giedd JN,
Rapoport JL, Evans AC. 1999. Structural maturation of neural
\pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science.
283:19081911.
Perrin JS, Leonard G, Perron M, Pike GB, Pitiot A, Richer L, Veillette S,
Pausova Z, Paus T. 2009. Sex differences in the growth of white
matter during adolescence. Neuroimage. 45:10551066.
Petanjek Z, JudašM, Kostovi
c I, Uylings HB. 2008. Lifespan alterations
of basal dendritic trees of pyramidal neurons in the human prefron-
tal cortex: a layer-specic pattern. Cereb Cortex. 18:915929.
Petanjek Z, JudašM, Šimi
cG,Rašin MR, Uylings HBM, Rakic P, Kostovi
c
I. 2011. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human pre-
frontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 108:1328113286.
Ponten S, Bartolomei F, Stam C. 2007. Small-world networks and epi-
lepsy: graph theoretical analysis of intracerebrally recorded mesial
temporal lobe seizures. Clin Neurophysiol. 118:918927.
R Development Core Team. 2011. R: a language and environment for
statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing,
Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available
from: URL http://www.R-project.org.
Rubinov M, Sporns O. 2010. Complex network measures of brain con-
nectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 52:10591069.
Rubinov M, Sporns O. 2011. Weight-conserving characterization of
complex functional brain networks. Neuroimage. 56:20682079.
Santesso DL, Segalowitz SJ. 2008. Developmental differences in error-
related ERPs in middle- to late-adolescent males. Dev Psychol.
44:205217.
Sarkar D. 2008. Lattice: multivariate data visualization with R. New York
(NY): Springer.
Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, Green-
stein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Rapoport JL. 2008. Neurodevelopmental
trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. 28:35863594.
Shi F, Wang L, Peng Z, Wee C-Y, Shen D. 2013. Altered modular organ-
ization of structural cortical networks in children with Autism. PLoS
One. 8:e63131.
Sowell ER. 2004. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain
growth in normal children. J Neurosci. 24:82238231.
Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Jernigan TL, Toga AW. 1999. In
vivo evidence for postadolescent brain maturation in frontal and
striatal regions. Nat Neurosci. 2:859860.
Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Tessner KD, Toga AW. 2001. Mapping con-
tinued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal
frontal cortex: inverse relationships during postadolescent brain
maturation. J Neurosci. 21:88198829.
Sporns O. 2011. The non-random brain: efciency, economy, and
complex dynamics. Front Comput Neurosci. 5:5.
Stam CJ, Jones BF, Nolte G, Breakspear M, Scheltens P. 2007. Small-
world networks and functional connectivity in Alzheimers disease.
Cereb Cortex. 17:9299.
Supekar K, Musen M, Menon V. 2009. Development of large-scale func-
tional brain networks in children. PLoS Biol. 7:e1000157.
Suter H. 2011. xlsReadWrite: Natively Read and Write Excel Files (.xls).
R package version. 1.
Tamnes CK, Ostby Y, Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Due-Tonnessen P, Walhovd
KB. 2010. Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood:
regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter
volume and microstructure. Cereb Cortex. 20:534548.
Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A, Glod CA, Maas LC, Renshaw PF.
2000. Functional decits in basal ganglia of children with attention-
decit/hyperactivity disorder shown with functional magnetic res-
onance imaging relaxometry. Nat Med. 6:470473.
van den Heuvel MP, Stam CJ, Kahn RS, Hulshoff Pol HE. 2009. Ef-
ciency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance.
J Neurosci. 29:76197624.
Vertes PE, Alexander-Bloch AF, Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL, Bull-
more ET. 2012. Simple models of human brain functional networks.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 109:58685873.
Wang R, Benner T, Sorensen AG, Wedeen VJ. 2007. Diffusion toolkit: a
software package for diffusion imaging data processing and tracto-
graphy. Proc Intl Soc Mag Reson Med. 15:3720.
Watts DJ, Strogatz SH. 1998. Collective dynamics of small-worldnet-
works. Nature. 393:440442.
Weisberg S, Fox J. 2011. An R companion to applied regression. Thou-
sand Oaks (CA): Sage Publications, Inc.
Westlye LT, Walhovd KB, Dale AM, Bjørnerud A, Due-Tønnessen P,
Engvig A, Grydeland H, Tamnes CK, Østby Y, Fjell AM. 2010. Life-
span changes of the human brain white matter: diffusion tensor
imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cereb Cortex. 20:20552068.
Williams BR, Ponesse JS, Schachar RJ, Logan GD, Tannock R. 1999. Devel-
opment of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev Psychol. 35:205.
Yap PT, Fan Y, Chen Y, Gilmore JH, Lin W, Shen D. 2011. Development
trends of white matter connectivity in the rst years of life. PLoS
One. 6:e24678.
Cerebral Cortex 13
by Marcus Kaiser on December 16, 2013http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from

Supplementary resource (1)

... Aging is a complex biological process that is associated with degradation of white matter tracts (a reduction in myelin density, axonal degeneration, and decline in tract numbers) (Peters 2006;Betzel et al. 2014;Lim et al. 2015;Coelho et al. 2021), gray matter volume (Sullivan and Pfefferbaum 2006), alteration in neurotransmitter levels (Roalf et al. 2020), and changes in large scale brain networks' coordination leading to cognitive and behavioral impairments (Li et al. 2020b). It is well known that the impact of aging on brain function is nonlinear and diverse, vary person to person, leading to the deterioration of some functions (Grady et al. 2010), while few other functions, such as inductive reasoning, verbal f luency, and executive attention, may even improve with age (Salthouse 2012;Veŕıssimo et al. 2022). ...
... Age-associated loss of the brain's anatomical connectivity has been of intense interest in many recent studies (Betzel et al. 2014;Lim et al. 2015;Perry et al. 2015). Nevertheless, many of these studies have overlooked the impact on the SR and LR TLs, exceptions, and their spatial distribution with age, which we quantified here and has been one of the key contributions of this work. ...
Article
Optimal brain function is shaped by a combination of global information integration, facilitated by long-range connections, and local processing, which relies on short-range connections and underlying biological factors. With aging, anatomical connectivity undergoes significant deterioration, which affects the brain’s overall function. Despite the structural loss, previous research has shown that normative patterns of functions remain intact across the lifespan, defined as the compensatory mechanism of the aging brain. However, the crucial components in guiding the compensatory preservation of the dynamical complexity and the underlying mechanisms remain uncovered. Moreover, it remains largely unknown how the brain readjusts its biological parameters to maintain optimal brain dynamics with age; in this work, we provide a parsimonious mechanism using a whole-brain generative model to uncover the role of sub-communities comprised of short-range and long-range connectivity in driving the dynamic compensation process in the aging brain. We utilize two neuroimaging datasets to demonstrate how short- and long-range white matter tracts affect compensatory mechanisms. We unveil their modulation of intrinsic global scaling parameters, such as global coupling strength and conduction delay, via a personalized large-scale brain model. Our key finding suggests that short-range tracts predominantly amplify global coupling strength with age, potentially representing an epiphenomenon of the compensatory mechanism. This mechanistically explains the significance of short-range connections in compensating for the major loss of long-range connections during aging. This insight could help identify alternative avenues to address aging-related diseases where long-range connections are significantly deteriorated.
... Correspondence between functional and structural data intuitively suggests that, like functional networks, structural networks should become increasingly segregated during development. However, prior studies using relatively small samples report conflicting results, including declining modularity (Chen et al., 2013), increasing modularity (Chen and Deem, 2015;Huang et al., 2015), or no change with age (Hagmann et al., 2010b;Lim et al., 2015). Larger sample sizes may be necessary for resolving the variability of findings reported in previous studies. ...
... 6 Heuvel et al., 2015), and continue to develop during youth Fair et al., 2007;Fair et al., 2008;Fair et al., 2009;Dosenbach et al., 2010;Satterthwaite et al., 2013b;Supekar et al., 2009;Anderson et al., 2011). In contrast, smaller studies of structural brain networks have produced heterogeneous results regarding the development of structural network modules that have not aligned well with functional imaging data (Chen et al., 2013;Chen and Deem, 2015;Hagmann et al., 2010b;Huang et al., 2015;Lim et al., 2015). When considered in light of prior studies that have reported substantial correspondence between brain structure and function Honey et al., 2009;Mišić et al., 2016), the disparity between developmental accounts of structural and functional network modules has been difficult to reconcile. ...
Preprint
The human brain is organized into large-scale functional modules that have been shown to evolve in childhood and adolescence. However, it remains unknown whether structural brain networks are similarly refined during development, potentially allowing for improvements in executive function. In a sample of 882 participants (ages 8-22) who underwent diffusion imaging as part of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort, we demonstrate that structural network modules become more segregated with age, with weaker connections between modules and stronger connections within modules. Evolving modular topology facilitated network integration, driven by age-related strengthening of hub edges that were present both within and between modules. Critically, both modular segregation and network integration were associated with enhanced executive performance, and mediated the improvement of executive functioning with age. Together, results delineate a process of structural network maturation that supports executive function in youth.
... Besides age or pubertal development, sex explains some differences in reversal learning performance in our study, with female participants performing better than male. There may be a genuine difference between female and male RL, or the higher performance of girls and women may point to the fact that females mature earlier than males in some regards, e.g., in grey matter development 49 or structural connectivity 50 , and show earlier changes of reward-related behaviours 51 . This would further support the cross-sectional developmental differences observed in our study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Learning behavioural responses and adapting them based on feedback is crucial from a young age, continuing to develop into young adulthood. This study examines the development trajectory and contributing factors from childhood to adulthood using a reversal learning paradigm. We tested 202 participants aged 10 to 22 in an online study, where they learned and reversed stimulus-outcome associations in a new blocked design paradigm and were assessed for working memory capacity. Results showed that reversal learning performance improved with age, particularly for 10- to 14-year-olds. Flexible responses to negative feedback correlated with better reversal learning. Additionally, pubertal development and working memory were positively associated with reversal learning. These findings align with previous research, highlighting flexible feedback responses as a key factor in reversal learning. As the overall rate of flexible reactions did not change with age, it could support reversal learning independent of age, potentially changing its role during development.
... At the same time, throughout adolescence, the brain regions related to inhibitory functions gradually mature and become more activated [32]. Since adolescent girls' brains mature earlier than boys' [33], this leads to different trends in the changes in depression network intensity between adolescent boys and girls. Furthermore, "sad mood" is a core symptom in the depression networks of all three grade groups. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Adolescence is a high-risk period for depression, especially after the COVID-19 pandemic, when adolescent depression has become increasingly severe. This study employs network analysis to identify core symptoms at various stages. It explores the differences in depression symptom characteristics among Chinese adolescents of different genders during elementary, middle, and high school periods. Methods: A convenience sampling method was used to select 1553 students from various elementary, middle, and high schools in a specific city as participants. Their depression symptoms were assessed using the The Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) depression screening scale. Using graph theory-based network analysis, this study constructs a depression symptom model via a correlation network and evaluates symptom nodes and their interconnections. Results: The study found significant differences in the detection rates of depression symptoms among the three grade levels (p <0.001). However, no significant differences were found between male and female students in the detection rates and PHQ-9 scores (p >0.05). Through network analysis, this study identified the network changes in depression symptoms among Chinese adolescents of different grades and genders. The results show that “depressed mood” is the core symptom in the elementary and high school groups. At the same time, “fatigue” is the central factor affecting the depression network in the middle school group. Negative emotions and fatigue are the primary symptoms that run through the entire adolescent depression network. Conclusions: This study reveals the heterogeneity of depression symptom networks among adolescent groups of different genders and grades, providing a theoretical basis for personalized interventions for adolescent depression in the future.
... At the network level, a lower variability of some of these networks' topological characteristics and a more consistent topological robustness and stability were estimated in girls. Multiple studies have reporter developmental differences in white matter and maturation of brain circuits between girls and boys (Koolschijn & Crone, 2013 Q20 ; Lenroot & Giedd, 2010;Lim et al., 2015) and sex-related differences in the topological organization of distinct circuits (Ingalhalikar et al., 2014). Our results are not only aligned with these findings but also indicate that there may be inherent sex differences in brain dynamics that are independent of pubertal stage. ...
Article
Full-text available
Intrinsic brain dynamics play a fundamental role in cognitive function, but their development is incompletely understood. We investigated pubertal changes in temporal fluctuations of intrinsic network topologies (focusing on the strongest connections and coordination patterns) and signals, in an early longitudinal sample from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study, with resting-state fMRI (n = 4,099 at baseline; n = 3,376 at follow-up [median age = 10.0 (1.1) and 12.0 (1.1) years; n = 2,116 with both assessments]). Reproducible, inverse associations between low-frequency signal and topological fluctuations were estimated (p < 0.05, β = −0.20 to −0.02, 95% confidence interval (CI) = [−0.23, −0.001]). Signal (but not topological) fluctuations increased in somatomotor and prefrontal areas with pubertal stage (p < 0.03, β = 0.06–0.07, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.11]), but decreased in orbitofrontal, insular, and cingulate cortices, as well as cerebellum, hippocampus, amygdala, and thalamus (p < 0.05, β = −0.09 to −0.03, 95% CI = [−0.15, −0.001]). Higher temporal signal and topological variability in spatially distributed regions were estimated in girls. In racial/ethnic minorities, several associations between signal and topological fluctuations were in the opposite direction of those in the entire sample, suggesting potential racial differences. Our findings indicate that during puberty, intrinsic signal dynamics change significantly in developed and developing brain regions, but their strongest coordination patterns may already be sufficiently developed and remain temporally consistent.
... Furthermore, disorders of white matter integrity in the angular and supramarginal gyrus were adjusted during treatment. FA value can measure the integrity of white matter microstructure, reflecting potential changes in axon diameter, density, and myelination (Lim et al., 2015). The DLPFC plays an important role in cognitive The FA values from ROIs of the patient (t0, t1, t2 and t3). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Long-term post-stroke cognitive impairment (PSCI) exhibits an accelerated rate of long-term cognitive decline, which can impair communication, limit social engagement, and increase rate of institutional dependence. The aim of this case report is to provide evidence for the potential of home-based transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation (taVNS) for home-bound patients with severe, long-term PSCI. Methods A 71-year-old male suffered a stroke two and a half years ago, which imaging reported foci of cerebral infarction visible in the left temporal and parietal lobes. The patient was performed taVNS twice a day for 30 min, 5 times a week for 8 weeks. The patient was evaluated the changes of cognitive function and brain white matter at 4 time points: baseline (t0), 4 weeks without taVNS after baseline (t1), 4 weeks of intervention (t2), and 8 weeks of intervention (t3). The effect of taVNS on white matter changes was visualized by DTI. Results After 8 weeks of taVNS treatment, the scores of Montreal cognitive assessment improved and the time to complete the shape trails test decreased. The DTI results showed that white matter in bilateral dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex remodeled after taVNS. Conclusion Eight-week home-based taVNS may be beneficial to long-term PSCI. Further studies of home-based taVNS treating patients with long-term PSCI are needed.
... Between ages 7 and 11, female subcortical forebrain nuclei reach adult volume, while males' volume is greater but likely reduces later in adulthood [24]. Nerve fiber tract streamline reduction occurs earlier in females [25], while occipital area thinning is faster in males [26]. Many species are characterized by different maturation rates between sexes [27][28][29][30][31]. Direct temporal comparison of females and males is challenged by sex-specific phenotypic timelines, evident in quantifiable gene expression patterns. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many species, including fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster), are sexually dimorphic. Phenotypic variation in morphology, physiology, and behavior can affect development, reproduction, health, and aging. Therefore, designating sex as a variable and sex-blocking should be considered when designing experiments. The brain regulates phenotypes throughout the lifespan by balancing survival and reproduction, and sex-specific development at each life stage is likely. Changes in morphology and physiology are governed by differential gene expression, a quantifiable molecular marker for age- and sex-specific variations. We assessed the fruit fly brain transcriptome at three adult ages for gene expression signatures of sex, age, and sex-by-age: 6698 genes were differentially expressed between sexes, with the most divergence at 3 days. Between ages, 31.1% of 6084 differentially expressed genes (1890 genes) share similar expression patterns from 3 to 7 days in females, and from 7 to 14 days in males. Most of these genes (90.5%, 1712) were upregulated and enriched for chemical stimulus detection and/or cilium regulation. Our data highlight an important delay in male brain gene regulation compared to females. Because significant delays in expression could confound comparisons between sexes, studies of sexual dimorphism at phenotypically comparable life stages rather than chronological age should be more biologically relevant.
Article
Understanding music listening behaviour benefits those seeking to market music to the general population and those choosing music to feature in retail environments or advertising. In particular, understanding how consumers consume different genres is important. There are, however, two conflicting bodies of knowledge. One finds segments of music listeners who differ on music genre preferences, while the other finds (media and brand) user profiles are more similar than different. Our research provides additional evidence by surveying over 1,000 representative respondents in the United States regarding their listening behaviour of 13 music genres. The proportion of listeners of each genre is compared with the average profile for all genres using those segmentation variables previously found to cause differing genre preferences (e.g. age and income). While some minor differences exist, notably that younger listeners prefer electronica/dance, the overall results show genre user profiles are more similar than different. The theoretical implication is that using approaches designed to find minor differences will do precisely that and magnify them. At the same time, for those marketing music in the industry, achieving a broader market coverage may be more effective in expanding their listener bases than focussing on narrower segments.
Article
Our understanding of brain iron regulation and its disruption in disease is limited. Excess iron affects motor circuitry, contributing to Parkinson’s disease (PD) risk. The molecular mechanisms regulating central iron levels, beyond a few well-known genes controlling peripheral iron, remain unclear. We generated scores based on the archetypal brain iron accumulation observed in magnetic resonance imaging scans of individuals with excessive dietary iron absorption and hemochromatosis risk. Genome-wide analysis revealed that this score is highly heritable, identifying loci associated with iron homeostasis, and driven by peripheral iron levels. Our score predicted gait abnormalities and showed a U-shaped relationship with PD risk, identifying individuals with threefold increased risk. These results establish a hormetic relationship between brain iron and PD risk, where central iron levels are strongly determined by genetics via peripheral iron. This framework combining forward and reverse genetics is a powerful study design to understand genomic drivers underlying high dimensional phenotypes.
Article
Full-text available
The stop-signal procedure was used to examine the development of inhibitory control. A group of 275 participants, 6 to 81 years of age, performed a visual choice reaction time (go) task and attempted to inhibit their responses to the go task when they heard a stop signal. Reaction times to the stop and go signals were used to assess performance in inhibition and response execution, respectively. Results indicated the speed of stopping becomes faster with increasing age throughout childhood, with limited evidence of slowing across adulthood. By contrast, strong evidence was obtained for age-related speeding of go-signal reaction time throughout childhood, followed by marked slowing throughout adulthood. Hierarchical regression confirmed that the age-related change in inhibitory control could not be explained by general speeding or slowing of responses. Findings are discussed in regard to the contrast between the development of inhibition and response execution and the utility of the stop-signal procedure.
Article
The common approach to the multiplicity problem calls for controlling the familywise error rate (FWER). This approach, though, has faults, and we point out a few. A different approach to problems of multiple significance testing is presented. It calls for controlling the expected proportion of falsely rejected hypotheses — the false discovery rate. This error rate is equivalent to the FWER when all hypotheses are true but is smaller otherwise. Therefore, in problems where the control of the false discovery rate rather than that of the FWER is desired, there is potential for a gain in power. A simple sequential Bonferronitype procedure is proved to control the false discovery rate for independent test statistics, and a simulation study shows that the gain in power is substantial. The use of the new procedure and the appropriateness of the criterion are illustrated with examples.
Article
This thoroughly revised new edition of a classic book provides a clinically inspired but scientifically guided approach to the biological foundations of human mental function in health and disease. It includes authoritative coverage of all the major areas related to behavioral neurology, neuropsychology, and neuropsychiatry. Each chapter, written by a world-renowned expert in the relevant area, provides an introductory background as well as an up-to-date review of the most recent developments. Clinical relevance is emphasized but is placed in the context of cognitive neuroscience, basic neuroscience, and functional imaging. Major cognitive domains such as frontal lobe function, attention and neglect, memory, language, prosody, complex visual processing, and object identification are reviewed in detail. A comprehensive chapter on behavioural neuroanatomy provides a background for brain-behaviour interactions in the cerebral cortex, limbic system, basal ganglia, thalamus, and cerebullum. Chapters on temperolimbic epilepsy, major psychiatric syndromes, and dementia provide in-depth analyses of these neurobehavioral entities and their neurobiological coordinates. Changes for this second edition include the reflection throughout the book of the new and flourishing alliance of behavioral neurology, neuropsychology, and neuropsychiatry with cognitive science;major revision of all chapters; new authorship of those on language and memory; and the inclusion of entirely new chapters on psychiatric syndromes and the dementias. Both as a textbook and a reference work, the second edition of Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology represents an invaluable resource for behavioural neurologists, neuropsychologists, neuropsychiatrists, cognitive and basic neuroscientists, geriatricians, physiatrists, and their students and trainees.
Article
We present a technique for automatically assigning a neuroanatomical label to each location on a cortical surface model based on probabilistic information estimated from a manually labeled training set. This procedure incorporates both geometric information derived from the cortical model, and neuroanatomical convention, as found in the training set. The result is a complete labeling of cortical sulci and gyri. Examples are given from two different training sets generated using different neuroanatomical conventions, illustrating the flexibility of the algorithm. The technique is shown to be comparable in accuracy to manual labeling.
Article
Often in applied research, confidence intervals (CIs) are constructed or reported only for parameters selected after viewing the data. We show that such selected intervals fail to provide the assumed coverage probability. By generalizing the false discovery rate (FDR) approach from multiple testing to selected multiple CIs, we suggest the false coverage-statement rate (FCR) as a measure of interval coverage following selection. A general procedure is then introduced, offering FCR control at level q under any selection rule. The procedure constructs a marginal CI for each selected parameter, but instead of the confidence level 1 — q being used marginally, q is divided by the number of parameters considered and multiplied by the number selected. If we further use the FDR controlling testing procedure of Benjamini and Hochberg for selecting the parameters, the newly suggested procedure offers CIs that are dual to the testing procedure and are shown to be optimal in the independent case. Under the positive regression dependency condition of Benjamini and Yekutieli, the FCR is controlled for one-sided tests and CIs, as well as for a modification for two-sided testing. Results for general dependency are also given. Finally, using the equivalence of the CIs to testing, we prove that the procedure of Benjamini and Hochberg offers directional FDR control as conjectured.