ArticlePDF Available

Associations Between Contrast Sensitivity and Aging

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Objective: The aim of this study was to assess age-related visual functions (visual acuity and contrast sensitivity) and compare the results by different age groups. Material and methods: A total of 231 patients were examined. The patients were divided into 5 age groups: 10 patients in group 1, 30-39 years; 40 patients in the group 2, 40-49 years; 77 patients in the group 3, 50-59 years; 71 patients in the group 4, 60-70 years; and 33 patients in the group 5, 71-85 years. A typical Snellen's chart (the direction of the gap in Landolt C) was used for noncorrected and best-corrected visual acuity testing. Contrast sensitivity was evaluated by employing a Ginsburg Box, VSCR-CST-6500. Results: Noncorrected visual acuity was significantly better in the group 2 than the group 3 (0.86 [0.28] vs. 0.69 [0.33], P=0.018). Moreover, noncorrected and best-corrected visual acuity was significantly better in the group 4 than the group 5 (0.52 [0.35] vs. 0.35 [0.28], P<0.001; and 0.9 [0.21] vs. 0.69 [0.27], P<0.005, respectively). Contrast sensitivity at the nighttime without glare was significantly worse in the group 2 than the group 1 at the spatial frequencies of 3, 12, and 18 cycles per degree (P=0.001, P=0.05, and P=0.01, respectively). The patients in the group 2 had significantly worse contrast sensitivity at the nighttime and daytime with glare at the spatial frequencies of 1.5, 12, and 18 cycles per degree (P=0.054, P=0.04, and P=0.01 and P=0.011, P=0.031, and P=0.011, respectively). The greatest differences in contrast sensitivity were observed between the groups 4 and 5, and it was 2 to 4 times better in the group 4. Comparing these groups, all the differences at the nighttime and daytime with and without glare were significant. Conclusions: Contrast sensitivity was worst among the oldest persons (71-85 years), and it began to worsen already in the persons aged 40-49 years. Contrast sensitivity was very similar in the age groups of 40-49 and 50-59 years.
Content may be subject to copyright.
273
Medicina (Kaunas) 2013;49(6)
Medicina (Kaunas) 2013;49(6):273-7
Associations Between Contrast Sensitivity and Aging
Rasa Liutkevičienė1, 2, Džastina Čebatorienė1, Giedrė Liutkevičienė3,
Vytautas Jašinskas1, Dalia Žaliūnienė1
1Department of Ophthalmology, Medical Academy, Lithuanian University of Health Sciences,
2Neuroscience Institute, Medical Academy, Lithuanian University of Health Sciences,
3Maxpharma Baltija UAB, Lithuania
Key Words: contrast sensitivity; visual acuity; aging; functional acuity contrast test.
Summary. Objective. The aim of this study was to assess age-related visual functions (visual
acuity and contrast sensitivity) and compare the results by different age groups.
Material and Methods. A total of 231 patients were examined. The patients were divided into 5
age groups: 10 patients in group 1, 30–39 years; 40 patients in the group 2, 40–49 years; 77 pa-
tients in the group 3, 50–59 years; 71 patients in the group 4, 60–70 years; and 33 patients in the
group 5, 71–85 years. A typical Snellen’s chart (the direction of the gap in Landolt C) was used for
noncorrected and best-corrected visual acuity testing. Contrast sensitivity was evaluated by employ-
ing a Ginsburg Box, VSCR-CST-6500.
Results. Noncorrected visual acuity was significantly better in the group 2 than the group 3
(0.86 [0.28] vs. 0.69 [0.33], P=0.018). Moreover, noncorrected and best-corrected visual acuity
was significantly better in the group 4 than the group 5 (0.52 [0.35] vs. 0.35 [0.28], P<0.001; and
0.9 [0.21] vs. 0.69 [0.27], P<0.005, respectively). Contrast sensitivity at the nighttime without
glare was significantly worse in the group 2 than the group 1 at the spatial frequencies of 3, 12, and
18 cycles per degree (P=0.001, P=0.05, and P=0.01, respectively). The patients in the group 2
had significantly worse contrast sensitivity at the nighttime and daytime with glare at the spatial
frequencies of 1.5, 12, and 18 cycles per degree (P=0.054, P=0.04, and P=0.01 and P=0.011,
P=0.031, and P=0.011, respectively). The greatest differences in contrast sensitivity were observed
between the groups 4 and 5, and it was 2 to 4 times better in the group 4. Comparing these groups,
all the differences at the nighttime and daytime with and without glare were significant.
Conclusions. Contrast sensitivity was worst among the oldest persons (71–85 years), and it be-
gan to worsen already in the persons aged 40–49 years. Contrast sensitivity was very similar in the
age groups of 40–49 and 50–59 years.
Correspondence to R. Liutkevičienė, Department of Ophthal-
mology, Medical Academy, Lithuanian University of Health
Sciences, Eivenių 2, 50028 Kaunas, Lithuania
E-mail: rliutkeviciene@gmail.com
Introduction
Optical and neuron degenerative changes of the
visual system that inuence a steady decrease in
visual acuity are observed from the age of approxi-
mately 40 years (1). With aging, people’s vision be-
comes less clear; big objects can be seen clearly, but
problems occur when people try to discern minor
things and minor details. Additionally, senile mio-
sis develops, the eye lenses become less clear and
stiffer, and the accommodation and convergence re-
serves start to decrease (1). These changes reduce
the access of light to the retina. Some authors sug-
gest that contrast sensitivity starts decreasing from
the age of 20 years (2). These processes are believed
to progress due to the atrophy of the retinal gan-
glion cell layer (1). Ganglion cells help determine
contrast and quickly evaluate the differences in light
intensity (3).
The visual acuity test is the simplest method,
which is most commonly used by ophthalmologists,
to examine the function of vision. An optotype chart,
which contains 12 rows of signs (letters, numbers,
rings with a gap, and drawings of various objects), is
used for the examination of visual acuity. According
to Snellen, visual acuity can be expressed in spatial
frequencies; however, the highest spatial frequencies
are evaluated by the standard visual acuity test only
under conditions of maximum contrast (V=1.0 match
30 cycles per degree when contrast is 100%); hence,
contrast sensitivity is partially evaluated. Therefore,
optotypes can be seen even with decreased contrast
sensitivity. The Snellen’s chart provides limited infor-
mation about functional vision (4).
Functional or “practical” vision is described as
our day-to-day vision, i.e., what people see and how
they process this information (5). A regular Snel-
len’s chart allows evaluating the patients’ ability to
determine black letters on a white background from
the distance, but it does not show visual quality (5),
whereas the functional acuity contrast test (FACT)
is considered to be more informative and accurate in
examining and evaluating visual functions.
For a detailed visual examination, various func-
274
Medicina (Kaunas) 2013;49(6)
tions, such as cognitive perception, health of the
visual system, and the central processing function,
are tested. Studies have shown that the assessment
of visual acuity with the typical Snellen’s chart using
Landolt’s rings (C optotypes) alone is insufcient
for the visual function testing because it provides
limited information about the central vision; thus, it
is necessary to determine not only visual acuity, but
also contrast sensitivity (6).
The aim of this study was to assess age-related
visual functions (visual acuity and contrast sensitiv-
ity) and compare the results by different age groups.
Material and Methods
The study was conducted in the Department of
Ophthalmology, Hospital of Lithuanian University
of Health Sciences, after the permission (No. BE-
2-14) from Kaunas Regional Ethics Committee for
Biomedical Research was obtained.
In total, 231 patients were examined. The patients
were divided into the following 5 age groups: group
1, 10 patients (19 eyes) aged 30–39 years; group 2,
40 patients (80 eyes) aged 40–49 years; group 3,
77 patients (153 eyes) aged 50–59 years; group 4,
71 patients (142 eyes) aged 60–70 years; and group
5, 33 patients (66 eyes) aged 71–85 years. In this
study, visual acuity, transparency of the cornea and
the lens, and the fundus were investigated in the
patients. Biomicroscopy was performed in order to
assess corneal and lenticular transparency. Noncor-
rected and best-corrected visual acuity (measured in
decimals from 0.1 to 1.0) was evaluated using Land-
olt’s rings (C optotypes) by the Snellen’s test at a
5-m distance from the chart.
The lenses were evaluated by biomicroscopy. The
lenses were examined using a slit lamp, positioning
the illumination source at a 45° angle and the light
beam being split to a 2-mm width.
During each examination, refraction was per-
formed, intraocular pressure was measured, and the
iris color was noted using the slit lamp. The pupils
of the subjects were dilated with 1% tropicamide or
1% cyclogyli. After the dilation of the pupils, fundos-
copy was performed with an ophthalmoscope of the
direct monocular type and the slit lamp, using a dou-
ble aspheric lens of +78 diopters. Peripheral retinal
examination was performed using an indirect oph-
thalmoscope. The results of the ophthalmic examina-
tion were recorded on standardized forms that were
developed for this study. Stereoscopic color fundus
photographs of the macula were obtained at 45° and
30° to the fovea for a detailed fundus analysis.
The inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) age of
30 to 85 years; 2) no other eye disorders found dur-
ing a detailed ophthalmological examination; and 3)
consent to participate.
The exclusion criteria were as follows: 1) related
eye disorders (high refractive error, cloudy cornea,
opacity of the lens [nuclear, cortical, and posteri-
or subcapsular cataract], keratitis, acute or chronic
uveitis, glaucoma, neovascular age-related macular
degeneration or geographic atrophy, diseases of the
optic nerve, etc.); 2) systemic illnesses (diabetes
mellitus, oncological diseases, systemic tissue dis-
orders, chronic infectious diseases, conditions after
organ or tissue transplantation, etc.); 3) nongraded
color images of the fundus because of obscuration
in the eye optic system or because of the quality of
fundus photography; and 4) functional acuity con-
trast test (FACT) value of 0.
Contrast sensitivity was measured by employ-
ing a Ginsburg Box, VSCR-CST-6500 with a FACT
chart at photopic (in the daytime, 85 cd/m2) and
mesopic (in the nighttime, 3 cd/m2) luminance with
and without glare at 5 standard spatial frequencies:
1.5, 3, 6, 12, and 18 cycles per degree (6). Contrast
sensitivity testing was performed in the presence of
best-corrected visual acuity.
Statistical analysis was performed using the
SPSS/W 13.0 program (Statistical Package for the
Social Sciences for Windows, Inc., Chicago, Illinois,
USA). The χ2 test was used for comparison of the
frequencies of qualitative variables. A statistically
signicant difference was considered if P<0.05.
Results
Table 1 shows noncorrected and best-corrected
visual acuity by age groups. Noncorrected visual
acuity was signicantly better in the group 2 than
in the group 3 (P=0.018). Moreover, noncorrected
and best-corrected visual acuity was better in the
group 4 compared with the group 5 (P<0.001 and
P<0.005, respectively) (Table 1).
The comparison of contrast sensitivity at the
nighttime without glare between the groups 1 and 2
showed that contrast sensitivity was signicantly
Parameter Age Groups, Years
30–39 40–49 50–59 60–70 71–85
Noncorrected visual acuity
Best-corrected visual acuity
0.89 (0.28)
0.98 (0.14)
0.86 (0.28)
0.98 (0.13)
0.69 (0.33)
0.97 (0.11)
0.52 (0.35)
0.9 (0.21)
0.35 (0.28)
0.69 (0.27)
Values are mean (standard deviation).
Table 1. Patients Groups and Results of Visual Acuity
Rasa Liutkevičienė, Džastina Čebatorienė, Giedrė Liutkevičienė, et al.
275
Medicina (Kaunas) 2013;49(6)
worse in the group 2 at the spatial frequencies of
3, 12, and 18 cycles per degree (P=0.001, P=0.05,
and P=0.01, respectively) (Table 2). At the daytime
without glare, no signicant differences in contrast
sensitivity between the groups 1 and 2 were docu-
mented at all spatial frequencies. Similar compari-
sons of contrast sensitivity at the nighttime and day-
time with glare revealed that the patients in the group
2 had signicantly worse contrast sensitivity at the
spatial frequencies of 1.5, 12, and 18 cycles per de-
gree (P=0.054, P=0.04, and P=0.01 and P=0.011,
P=0.031, and P=0.011, respectively) (Table 3).
The comparison of contrast sensitivity between
the groups 2 and 3 showed signicant differences
at the nighttime with glare at a spatial frequency of
3 cycles per degree (P=0.001) and at the daytime
without glare at the spatial frequencies of 6 and 12
cycles per degree (both P=0.05) (Tables 2 and 3).
The greatest differences in contrast sensitivity
were observed comparing the groups 4 and 5 (Ta-
bles 2 and 3). Contrast sensitivity was 2 to 4 times
better in the group 4, i.e., younger group, than the
group 5. All the differences in contrast sensitivity at
the nighttime and daytime with and without glare
were signicant.
Discussion
It has been reported that a decrease in contrast
sensitivity is directly associated with patients’ age
and visual acuity. The FACT is a very sensitive
method used for the evaluation of the visual system
and may help detect the onset of the disease even
when visual acuity is still intact. This examination
method also allows observing changes in the dis-
ease, such as progression or recovery. In some coun-
tries, the FACT is used to diagnose visual disability.
There are many studies (2, 7–13) analyzing age-
related changes in contrast sensitivity, but to our
knowledge, age-related changes in contrast sensitiv-
ity with and without glare in the Lithuanian popula-
Contrast Sensitivity Age Groups, Years
30–39 40–49 50–59 60–70 71–85
At nighttime at different spatial frequencies,
cycles per degree
1.5
3
6
12
18
81.8 (20.6)
100.8 (45.6)
75.8 (42.1)
28.8 (14.2)
16.1 (15.1)
73.4 (26.9)
88.4 (50.3)*
74.9 (48.6)
23.9 (20.3)*
10.4 (7.1)*
69.4 (23.2)
85.3 (41.1)
72.8 (46.5)
22.5 (14.0)
9.02 (6.2)
68.2 (36.4)
80.4 (53.9)
57.7 (51.3)
15.2 (13.7)
6.3 (6.2)
26.7 (25.2)†
25.3 (23.4)†
8.2 (6.8)†
2.3 (2.3)†
1.6 (1.2)†
At daytime at different spatial frequencies,
cycles per degree
1.5
3
6
12
18
71.9 (22.8)
106.5 (39.2)
111.0 (42.7)
45.5 (23.5)
24.0 (16.2)
66.0 (26.0)
104.7 (44.6)
106.5 (56.7)
45.3 (30.9)
20.0 (17.3)
64.0 (22.9)
97.4 (40.8)
96.9 (53.7)‡
37.8 (34.9)‡
16.7 (9.2)
61.2 (30.6)
90.4 (50.8)
82.6 (55.8)
30.2 (29.3)
12.2 (5.3)
31.5 (28.8)†
38.4 (38.6)†
22.4 (20.4)†
6.3 (6.1)†
1.2 (1.1)†
Values are mean (standard deviation).
*P<0.05 as compared with the 30–39-year age group. P<0.05 as compared with the 40–49-year age group.
P<0.05 as compared with the 60–70-year age group.
Table 2. Contrast Sensitivity at the Nighttime and Daytime Without Glare According
to the Functional Acuity Contrast Test by Age Groups
Contrast Sensitivity Age Groups, Years
30–39 40–49 50–59 60–70 71–85
At nighttime at different spatial frequencies,
cycles per degree
1.5
3
6
12
18
77.7 (23.8)
95.3 (43.8)
69.0 (32.5)
25.2 (16.8)
10.8 (9.6)
68.8 (27.6)*
91.8 (48.5)
64.0 (52.8)
22.2 (17.5)*
6.7 (3.7)*
66.9 (30.1)
79.9 (49.0)‡
62.6 (49.3)
18.8 (6.0)
5.9 (4.9)
61.3 (33.2)
74.5 (53.3)
45.7 (37.5)
10.3 (7.1)
2.2 (2.1)
26.6 (24.2)†
24.5 (24.7)†
7.2 (4.3)†
2.2 (1.6)†
0.4 (0.3)†
At daytime at different spatial frequencies,
cycles per degree
1.5
3
6
12
18
84.3 (19.8)
111.8 (39.9)
117.6 (45.4)
53.6 (26.9)
25.3 (19.3)
74.6 (29.2)*
105.2 (47.1)
111.3 (55.8)
42.5 (36.4)*
18.4 (18.4)*
72.5 (23.2)
102.9 (35.4)
108.7 (26.9)
41.8 (35.9)
16.3 (13.1)
64.4 (30.7)
89.6 (51.2)
83.3 (59.2)
31.2 (23.4)
14.5 (8.1)
31.2 (26.4)†
35.7 (32.3)†
25.4 (23.2)†
4.1 (4.2)†
2.2 (2.1)†
Values are mean (standard deviation).
*P<0.05 as compared with the 30–39-year age group. P<0.05 as compared with the 40–49-year age group.
P<0.05 as compared with the 60–70-year age group.
Table 3. Contrast Sensitivity at the Nighttime and Daytime With Glare According
to the Functional Acuity Contrast Test by Age Groups
Associations Between Contrast Sensitivity and Aging
276
Medicina (Kaunas) 2013;49(6)
tion by the different age groups have been evaluated
for the rst time. Our study showed that contrast
sensitivity in the 40–49-year age group decreased
at high spatial frequencies as well; it remained very
similar in the age groups of 40–49 and 50–59 years
and was much worse in the oldest group, i.e., patients
aged 71–85 years. Our results are in agreement with
those of the study performed by Owsley et al. (2).
This study revealed that contrast sensitivity began
decreasing at the age of 40 to 50 at higher spatial
frequencies (2). Shahina et al. carried out a study of
younger and older patients’ groups and reported that
contrast sensitivity decreased with older age, too (7).
Nio et al. examined 100 healthy persons aged 20
to 69 years and conrmed that contrast sensitivity
showed age-related changes at a spatial frequency of
8 cycles per degree (9). A Japanese study evaluated
contrast sensitivity in a group of patients aged 40
to 79 years, whose visual acuity was 1.0 or better,
and reported that 9.4% of the patients with intact
visual acuity had lower contrast sensitivity (9). Hoh-
berger et al. conrmed a signicant relationship be-
tween age and a decrease in contrast sensitivity (10).
Crassini et al. examined groups of younger persons
(mean age, 20.4 years) and older persons (mean age,
64.4 years) when visual acuity and the visual system
function were intact (11). The study results suggest-
ed that the contrast sensitivity of younger persons
was better compared with older persons, but only
at high spatial frequencies (11). Our results are also
in agreement with those of the study by Robert et
al., who investigated FACT changes in 2 groups of
healthy persons: a younger group with a mean age of
18.5 years and an older group with a mean age of 73
years (12). The authors demonstrated that contrast
sensitivity at low and medium spatial frequencies
was 3 times better in the group of younger patients
than in the group of older patients (12). Greene et
al. investigated the relationship between aging and
visual acuity as well as between aging and contrast
sensitivity (13). The authors examined a group of
young persons (mean age, 19.5 years) and a group
of older persons (mean age, 68.4 years) and showed
that contrast sensitivity signicantly decreased in
the group of older patients (13). Such results imply
that the FACT is useful when evaluating age-related
changes in the visual system (13). Ross et al. evalu-
ated 2 age groups: one with participants aged 20 to
30 years and the other with participants aged 50 to
87 years (8). Older observers had reduced contrast
sensitivity at all spatial frequencies when compared
with their younger counterparts. This was particular-
ly obvious at medium and high spatial frequencies.
In the age range 50 to 87 years, there was a linear
decline in contrast sensitivity with age at medium
and high spatial frequencies. Within this age range,
the responses to low spatial frequencies appeared to
be independent of age (8). Our results are in dis-
agreement with those of the study by Ross et al.,
because our results revealed that the FACT results
were worse at low spatial frequencies (1.5 cycles per
degree) in the younger age groups and in the old-
est age group, especially, when we evaluated con-
trast sensitivity in the nighttime and daytime with
glare, but when contrast sensitivity without glare
was evaluated, only one statistically signicant re-
sult at a medium spatial frequency was found, i.e., in
the nighttime without glare, the results were worse
in 40–49-year age group at a spatial frequency of 3
cycles per degree. Deterioration of contrast sensi-
tivity at medium spatial frequencies may be due to
an impact of glare at younger age and due to lens
opacities at older age (14). Rouhiainen et al. have
reported a signicant association between contrast
sensitivity and lens opacication at low and medium
frequencies in posterior subcapsular opacities (14).
Therefore, it seems likely that cataract (cortical or
posterior subcapsular) should be primarily respon-
sible for the trend of decreasing contrast sensitivity
with age in subjects with good visual acuity (14). It
has also been determined that glare reduces contrast
sensitivity in older persons, especially the ones who
are diagnosed with cataract (15). The study by Hoh-
berger et al. also proved a close relationship between
decreasing contrast sensitivity and age and contrib-
uted to the existing knowledge by determining and
describing the inuence of cataract on decreasing
contrast sensitivity (10).
Conclusions
Contrast sensitivity decreased with age, and it is
attributed to age-related changes in the optical sys-
tem.
Statement of Conflict of Interest
The authors state no conict of interest.
References
1. Sekuler R, Sekuler AB. Age-related changes, optical fac-
tors, and neural processes. In: Kazdin AE, editor. Encyclo-
pedia of Psychology American Psychological Association.
Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. Vol. 8. p. 180-3.
2. Owsley C, Sekuler R, Siemsen D. Contrast sensitivity
throug hout adulthood. Vision Res 1983;23:689-99.
3. Abraitis R, Cibas P, Gronow G, Gutmanas A, Illert M,
Hultborn H, et al. Žmogaus ziologija. (Human physiol-
ogy.) Kaunas: KMU; 1999. p. 87-8.
4. Ginsburg AP. Contrast sensitivity and functional vision. Int
Ophthalmol Clin 2003;43:5-16.
5. Žaliūnienė D, Glebauskienė B, Liutkevičienė R, Špečkauskas
M. The comparison of visual acuity and contrast sensitiv-
ity in patients with cataract and age-related macular degen-
eration implanted with foldable or rigid intraocular lenses.
Rasa Liutkevičienė, Džastina Čebatorienė, Giedrė Liutkevičienė, et al.
277
Medicina (Kaunas) 2013;49(6)
Medicina (Kaunas) 2006;42:975-82.
6. Ginsburg AP. Contrast sensitivity: determining the visual
quality and function of cataract, intraocular lenses and re-
fractive surgery. Curr Opin Ophthalmol 2006;17:19-26.
7. Shahina P. Contrast sensitivity loss with aging: sampling ef-
ciency and equivalent noise at different spatial frequen-
cies. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 2004;21:169-75.
8. Ross JE, Clarke DD, Bron AJ. Effect of age on contrast
sensitivity function: uniocular and binocular ndings. Br J
Ophthalmol 1985;69:51-6.
9. Nio YK, Jansonius NM, Fidler V, Geraghty Enorrby S,
Kooijman AC. Age-related changes of defocus-specic
cont rast sensitivity in healthy subjects. Ophthalmic Physiol
Op tics 2000;20:323-34.
10. Hohberger B, Laemmer R, Adler W, Juenemann AG, Folk-
ert K, Horn FK. Measuring contrast sensitivity in normal
subjects with OPTEC 6500: inuence of age and glare.
Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2007;245:1805-14.
11. Crassini B, Brown B, Bowman K. Age-related changes in
contrast sensitivity in central and peripheral retina. Percep-
tion 1988;17:315-32.
12. Robert S, Picciano LH. Spatial vision and aging. I: Contrast
sensitivity. J Gerontol 1980;35(5):692-9.
13. Greene HA, Madden DJ. Adult age differences in visual
acuity, stereopsis, and contrast sensitivity. Am J Optom
Phy siol Opt 1987;64:749-53.
14. Rouhiainen P, Rouhiainen H, Salonen JT. Contrast sensi-
tivity in different types of early lens opacities. Acta Oph-
thalmol Scand 1996;74:379-83.
15. Aslam TM, Haider D, Murray IJ. Principles of disability
glare measurement: an ophthalmological perspective. Acta
Ophthalmol Scand 2007;85:354-60.
Received 21 February 2012, accepted 30 May 2013
Associations Between Contrast Sensitivity and Aging
... With senility, however, the processing of the visual image by humans becomes less clear. Large, close objects can generally be seen clearly, however, elderly people tend to have difficulty seeing smaller elements (Liutkevičiene et al., 2013). The visual system is influenced by several factors such as gender, geographic location, eating habits, as well as climatic aspects. ...
... According to Snellen, VA can be expressed in spatial frequencies; however, higher spatial frequencies are assessed by standard VA testing only under conditions of maximum contrast. Therefore, contrast sensitivity is partially assessed (Liutkevičiene et al., 2013). ...
... The Functional Acuity Contrast Test (FACT) is recognized as a highly informative and precise tool for assessing visual functions. It's a particularly sensitive method that can effectively evaluate the visual system and identify early signs of disease, even when visual acuity remains relatively intact (Liutkevičiene et al., 2013). ...
Article
Contrast sensitivity is a visual function associated with recognizing objects in low light. Objective: to review the contrast sensitivity function (CSF) in the elderly, the changes related to it with aging and the impact of its decline on daily activities. Methods: Search in PubMed/MEDLINE and SciELO, using the descriptors “contrast sensitivity” AND “seniors” OR “Elder*” OR “older adults” OR aged. Inclusion criteria: age ≥ 60 years or “elderly” and reference to CSF assessment. Results: elderly people have reduced CSF, leading to the risk of falls. CSF is useful for balance, performance on digital tasks, movement processing and quality of life, as well as it is important in cognitive and neuropsychological tests. Impairment of CSF is associated with loss of driving performance of motor vehicles, especially at low luminance. Elderly people who have a decrease in CSF have less social interaction and a lower quality of life. Conclusion: the reduction in CSF occurs with aging and is associated with postural imbalance, in addition to an increase in fall rates and impact on the activities of daily living of the elderly, influencing mobility and social interaction.
... Previous studies have demonstrated that the deterioration of monocular contrast sensitivity usually begins in higher spatial frequencies (where refractive changes may play a larger role) around the age of 40-50. The deficit then extends down to a wider range of spatial frequency in later life (Derefeldt et al., 1979;Owsley et al., 1983;Liutkeviciene et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Changes in vision that occur in normal healthy aging can be seen in fundamental measures of monocular vision. However, the nature of the changes in binocular vision with age remain unclear. Methods A total of 28 older (53–66 years) and 28 younger adults (20–31 years) were enrolled in this study. We performed a battery of tests to assess differences in monocular contrast thresholds and various binocular visual functions including dichoptic masking weight and strength, the binocular balance point for fused stimuli, and stereoacuity in the aging and control groups. Results Aging significantly increased monocular contrast thresholds (p < 0.001). Although this suggests that aging reduces the effective “input gain” to vision, we also found a significantly elevated contribution of those weaker signals to interocular suppression (p < 0.001). Consequently, there was no significant net difference in the strength of interocular suppression (p = 0.065). We did not find a significant difference of absolute balance point between the two groups (p = 0.090). Lastly, the mean stereoacuity was worse in the older group compared to the younger group (p = 0.002). Discussion Our findings confirm previous results showing differences in contrast sensitivity and stereoacuity with aging. Furthermore, we find a change in interocular suppression that is a possible consequence of the change in contrast sensitivity. It is suggestive of a cortical system that maintains a homeostatic balance in interocular suppression across the lifespan.
... Frontiers in Psychology 06 frontiersin.org function declines around this age range, and the population over 65 years old can be considered as a homogeneous group (Ross et al., 1985;Elliott, 1987;Liutkevičienė et al., 2013;Pelletier et al., 2016). It is noteworthy that subjects in the FH+ group had a younger age range because they were those whose parents had suffered from dementia, and although they had predisposing genetic traits, cognitive decline had not yet developed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia affecting the central nervous system, and alteration of several visual structures has been reported. Structural retinal changes are usually accompanied by changes in visual function in this disease. The aim of this study was to analyse the differences in visual function at different stages of the pathology (family history group (FH+), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), mild AD and moderate AD) in comparison with a control group of subjects with no cognitive decline and no family history of AD. Methods We included 53 controls, 13 subjects with FH+, 23 patients with MCI, 25 patients with mild AD and, 21 patients with moderate AD. All were ophthalmologically healthy. Visual acuity (VA), contrast sensitivity (CS), colour perception, visual integration, and fundus examination were performed. Results The analysis showed a statistically significant decrease in VA, CS and visual integration score between the MCI, mild AD and moderate AD groups compared to the control group. In the CS higher frequencies and in the colour perception test (total errors number), statistically significant differences were also observed in the MCI, mild AD and moderate AD groups with respect to the FH+ group and also between the control and AD groups. The FH+ group showed no statistically significant difference in visual functions compared to the control group. All the test correlated with the Mini Mental State Examination score and showed good predictive value when memory decline was present, with better values when AD was at a more advanced stage. Conclusion Alterations in visual function appear in subjects with MCI and evolve when AD is established, being stable in the initial stages of the disease (mild AD and moderate AD). Therefore, visual psychophysical tests are a useful, simple and complementary tool to neuropsychological tests to facilitate diagnosis in the preclinical and early stages of AD.
... Only those diagnoses made under international criteria or certified by autopsy reports of the relative were accepted. In addition, in order to avoid the influence of the variable "age, " which greatly affects visual function, especially contrast sensitivity [28][29][30], the sample was divided into a subsample ranging between 40 and 60 years of age, and a subsample over 60 years of age. Finally, each subsample was stratified according to allelic characterization for the APOE ɛ4 gene. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The earliest pathological features of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) appear decades before the clinical symptoms. The pathology affects the brain and the eye, leading to retinal structural changes and functional visual alterations. Healthy individuals at high risk of developing AD present alterations in these ophthalmological measures, as well as in resting-state electrophysiological activity. However, it is unknown whether the ophthalmological alterations are related to the visual-related electrophysiological activity. Elucidating this relationship is paramount to understand the mechanisms underlying the early deterioration of the system and an important step in assessing the suitability of these measures as early biomarkers of disease. Methods In total, 144 healthy subjects: 105 with family history of AD and 39 without, underwent ophthalmologic analysis, magnetoencephalography recording, and genotyping. A subdivision was made to compare groups with less demographic and more risk differences: 28 high-risk subjects (relatives/APOEɛ4 +) and 16 low-risk (non-relatives/APOEɛ4 −). Differences in visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and macular thickness were evaluated. Correlations between each variable and visual-related electrophysiological measures (M100 latency and time–frequency power) were calculated for each group. Results High-risk groups showed increased visual acuity. Visual acuity was also related to a lower M100 latency and a greater power time–frequency cluster in the high-risk group. Low-risk groups did not show this relationship. High-risk groups presented trends towards a greater contrast sensitivity that did not remain significant after correction for multiple comparisons. The highest-risk group showed trends towards the thinning of the inner plexiform and inner nuclear layers that did not remain significant after correction. The correlation between contrast sensitivity and macular thickness, and the electrophysiological measures were not significant after correction. The difference between the high- and low- risk groups correlations was no significant. Conclusions To our knowledge, this paper is the first of its kind, assessing the relationship between ophthalmological and electrophysiological measures in healthy subjects at distinct levels of risk of AD. The results are novel and unexpected, showing an increase in visual acuity among high-risk subjects, who also exhibit a relationship between this measure and visual-related electrophysiological activity. These results have not been previously explored and could constitute a useful object of research as biomarkers for early detection and the evaluation of potential interventions’ effectiveness.
... the retinal pigment epithelium (RPE), hemorrhages, and/or scars (Liutkevičienė, Čebatorienė, Liutkevičienė, Jašinskas, & Žaliūnienė, 2013). The prevalence of early AMD in persons 30-40 years of age is 1.3 ± 0.3%, and increases with age to 8.0% ± 5.5% in the ≥81-year-olds group (Nangia, Jonas, Kulkarni, & Matin, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Age‐related macular degeneration (AMD) is the leading cause of blindness in the elderly individuals. The etiology of AMD includes environmental and genetic factors. Methods We aimed to determine the association between CETP (rs5882; rs708272; rs3764261; rs1800775; rs2303790), AGER (rs1800624; rs1800625), and CYP4F2 (rs1558139) gene polymorphisms and development of atrophic AMD. About 52 patients with atrophic AMD and 800 healthy control subjects were evaluated. The genotyping of single‐nucleotide polymorphisms in CETP , AGER, and CYP4F2 was carried out using the real‐time‐PCR method. Results Genetic risk models in the analysis of CETP rs5882 revealed statistically significant variables with increased risk of atrophic AMD in the codominant (p < .001), dominant (p < .001), recessive (p < .001), and additive (p < .001) models with the highest 25.4‐fold increased risk of atrophic AMD in the codominant model (p < .001). The AGER rs1800625 was associated with a highly increased risk of atrophic AMD in the codominant (p < .001), recessive (p < .001), and additive (p < .001) genetic models. Conclusion We identified two polymorphisms with a higher risk of atrophic AMD (CETP rs5882 and AGER rs1800625).
... Quick-CSF (Quick CSF) promises complete testing in a matter of minutes (2-5min) with fully automated means (handled via a tablet). test [54][55][56][57][58][59][60]. Finally using the well-known web browser, Firefox, an innovative CSF Nearby SPARCS test, uses a computer screen and the gray gradations it can perform Figure 8. ...
Article
Background: In an experimental model, telomere shortening inhibits neovascularization. It is thus possible that telomere shortening might have a role in the pathogenesis of geographic atrophy in case of age-related macular degeneration (AMD). This is why we aimed to find any associated differences of telomere length and genetic variants in telomere-related genes (TERT, TERT-CLPTM1, TRF1, TNKS2, and TRF2) in patients with atrophic AMD compared to healthy controls. Methods: The study enrolled patients with atrophic AMD (n = 56) and healthy (n = 73) controls. Samples of DNA from peripheral blood leukocytes were extracted by DNA salting-out method. The genotyping of TERT rs2736098, rs401681 in TERT-CLPTM1 locus, TRF1 rs1545827, rs10107605, TNKS2 rs10509637, rs10509639, and TRF2 rs251796 and relative leukocyte telomere length (T/S) measurement were carried out using a real-time polymerase chain reaction method. The results were assessed using the statistical analysis method of “IBM SPSS Statistics 20.0“. Results: We found statistically significantly higher T/S in atrophic AMD patients than in healthy controls (T/S, median (IQR): 1.638 (1.110) vs. 0.764 (0.801), p < .001). Also, statistically significant differences were found in TRF1 rs10107605 allele (A and C) distributions between the atrophic AMD and control groups (88.36% and 11.64% vs. 95.54% and 4.46%, respectively, p = .041), as well as between the short telomere and long telomere groups (86.92% and 13.08% vs. 96.09% and 3.91%, respectively, p = .008). Conclusions: Our research revealed the leukocyte telomere length having a role in atrophic AMD development, also the association between TRF1 rs10107605 and the telomere length.
Article
Purpose: To report late postoperative opacification of a hydrophobic acrylic intraocular lens (IOL) AcryNovaTMPC 610Y as well as the clinical consequences in patients 10 years after uncomplicated cataract surgery. Materials and Methods: Medical records were reviewed of 23 patients (26 eyes) with AcryNovaTMPC 610Y implantated between years 2005 and 2007. Next clinical examination was performed 10 years after surgery. We assessed best corrected distance visual acuity (BCDVA), contrast sensitivity (CSV-1000E) and relative opacity of IOL material, (OCULUS Pentacam HR). Results of BCDVA and Pentacam were analysed statistically. One explanted IOL was analysed using anterior segment OCT in vitro and spectroscopic method EDX (Energydispersive X-ray spectroscopy). Results: Opacification led to a statistically significant reduction in the best corrected distance visual acuity (BCDVA) = (0,95 ± 0,10) versus (0,87 ± 0,20) and to increase of IOL opacity only in some lenses but statistically significant in the average (6,37 ± 2,16)% versus value of (14,22 ± 5,87)%. In the explanted IOL we have documented structural changes of primarily hydrophobic raw material leading to property of hydrophilic one. Conclusion: Some batches of AcryNovaTMPC 610Y were produced from raw material of poor quality which is the cause of its structural changes and its progressive opacification.
Article
Full-text available
Monocular and binocular contrast sensitivity function for a range of spatial frequencies was measured in two groups of subjects with normal vision. Statistically significant differences in performance between the younger group (age 20-30 years) and the older group (age 50-87 years) were found at all spatial frequencies sampled between 0.40 and 19 X 25 cpd. In the age range 50-87 years there was a linear decline in contrast sensitivity with age for medium and high spatial frequencies, but sensitivity for low spatial frequencies was independent of age.
Article
Full-text available
The relative contributions of optical and neural factors to the decrease in visual function with aging were investigated by measurement of contrast detection at three different spatial frequencies, in the presence of external noise, on young and older subjects. Contrast detection in noise functions allows two parameters to be measured: sampling efficiency, which indicates neural changes, and equivalent noise, which demonstrates optical effects. Contrast thresholds were measured in the presence of four levels (including zero) of externally added visual noise. Measurements were obtained from eight young and eight older visually normal observers. Compared with young subjects, older subjects showed significantly ( p < 0.05 ) lower sampling efficiencies at spatial frequencies of 1 and 4 cycles per degree (c/deg) and significantly higher equivalent noise levels for gratings of 10 c/deg. Neural and optical factors affect contrast sensitivity loss with aging differently, depending on the spatial frequency tested, implying the existence of different mechanisms.
Article
Seeing begins with information contained in the light reflected from objects in the environment (Sekuler & Blake, 1994). The visual system's subsequent capture, transduction, and analysis of this light-borne information depend on a variety of optical factors and neural processes, all of which are subject to change with age. Changes in the Eye The eye makes two different but equally important contributions to vision: optical and neural. In the first contribution, various surfaces and structures admit light into the eye and focus it on the retina's specialized photosensitive elements (rods and cones). In the second contribution. a complex, organized network of neurons processes signals generated by photoreceptor transduction of light, extracting biologically significant information that is transmitted via the optic nerve to the brain.
Article
Eight young (average age 20.4 years) and eight elderly (average age 64.4 years) observers took part in three experiments designed to study age-related changes in peripheral retinal function. A further eight young (average age 22.3 years) and eight elderly (average age 63.8 years) observers took part in a replication of experiment 3. All observers had normal or better-than-normal visual acuity and no evidence of ocular pathology. All testing was monocular and the eye with better visual acuity was used. In the first experiment contrast sensitivity was measured in central retina and 10 deg temporally, at spatial frequencies of 0.2, 0.8, 2.0, and 5.0 cycles deg-1. Young observers had better contrast sensitivities than older observers, but only at higher spatial frequencies (2.0 and 5.0 cycles deg-1). For both groups, contrast sensitivity was poorer with peripheral presentation of stimuli than with central presentation, but not for the lowest spatial frequency used (0.2 cycle deg-1). In the second experiment observers had to detect the presence of a sharp edge (square-wave luminance profile), while in the third and fourth experiments the target was a "fuzzy' edge (sine-wave profile). Edges were again presented centrally or 10 deg temporally. As expected from the data of experiment 1, young observers were better able to detect the sharp edge than were the older observers in both central and peripheral viewing conditions. For both age groups, edge detection was better during central viewing than during peripheral viewing. However, contrary to expectations based on the results of experiment 1, detection of the fuzzy edge was better for central than for peripheral viewing for both age groups in experiments 3 and 4. The apparent (and expected) equality of performance found in experiment 3 for young and elderly observers in detecting the fuzzy edge was shown to be due to the range of contrast values used. When appropriate contrast values were used in experiment 4, young observers detected fuzzy edges presented in central retina better than did elderly observers. The results of experiment 1 show sparing of the ability to process low spatial frequencies across (i) age and (ii) retinal location, and are discussed in terms of the notion of (i) models of age-related loss of visual function and (ii) cortical magnification. The results of experiments 2, 3, and 4 provide some support for the proposition that the contrast sensitivity of observers may be used to predict their performance on other visual tasks.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
Article
We investigated adult age differences in four measures of visual function: distance acuity, near acuity, stereopsis, and contrast sensitivity. Twenty-four young adults (mean age 19.5 years) and 24 older adults (mean age 68.4 years) participated. Age differences were present in each of the four measures. A stepwise discriminant analysis performed on the four measures revealed that, when the correlations among the measures were taken into account, only contrast sensitivity significantly discriminated young and older adults' performance. The strength of the correlations among the four measures was greater for older adults than for young adults. The results indicated that contrast sensitivity is a useful measure for detecting age-related changes in visual function and that a common mechanism may underlie age differences on various visual tests.
Article
Previous studies of spatial contrast sensitivity in adulthood have produced conflicting results. To clarify the situation, we measured contrast sensitivity functions on a large sample of adults (n = 91), ranging in age from 19 to 87. All observers were free from significant ocular pathology and were individually refracted for the test distance. Sensitivity for stationary gratings of low spatial frequency remained the same throughout adulthood. At higher spatial frequencies, sensitivity decreased with age beginning around 40 to 50 years. When a low spatial frequency grating was drifted, young adults' sensitivity improved by a factor of 4-5 over sensitivity to a static grating; this motion enhancement was markedly diminished in adults over 60 years, implying an impairment of temporal processing in the elderly. Reduced retinal illuminance characteristic of the aged eye could account for a large part of older adults' deficit in spatial vision, but appeared to play little role in their deficit in temporal vision.
Article
Contrast sensitivity functions (CSFs) were measured for two groups of healthy observers, a younger group with mean age 18.5 years and an older group with mean age 73 years. Although the two groups had virtually identical sensitivities to high spatial frequencies (gratings with narrow bars), the younger observers were three times more sensitive at low and intermediate spatial frequencies (gratings with wider bars) than were the older observers. Groups did not differ in relative sensitivity to gratings flickered at two rates, .33 and 6 Hz. The reduced sensitivity of older observers to low spatial frequencies cannot be explained in terms of optical factors or as the result of ocular pathology; it probably reflects a loss in that visual sub-system responsible for detecting low spatial frequency, temporally transient targets.
Article
The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the relationship of various types of early lens opacities with contrast sensitivity at different spatial frequencies. The Lens Opacities Classification System II and Lensmeter 701 were used in the grading of the lens status. The contrast sensitivity was tested with the Vistech VCTS 6500 distance test with optimal correction of refractive errors. Data from 995 eyes were available for the present study. Contrast sensitivity decreased by increasing lens opacification graded with the Lens Opacities Classification System II and also by increasing Lensmeter 701 reading. When the statistical model was adjusted for age and best corrected visual acuity, nuclear opacities and nuclear colour were not associated with decreased contrast sensitivity, however, there was a weak, but statistically significant correlation between contrast sensitivity reduction and cortical opacities at high spatial frequencies, and between contrast sensitivity and posterior subcapsular opacities at low and medium spatial frequencies.
Article
To investigate the effect of defocus on contrast sensitivity as a function of age in healthy subjects, the through focus contrast sensitivity was measured in 100 healthy subjects aged 20–69. Defocus-specific changes in contrast sensitivity reflect age-related changes in the optics of the eye. Tests were performed in cycloplegic eyes varying artificial pupil size (2, 4 and 6 mm), defocus (−1 to +2 D), and spatial frequency (1–16 cpd). Integrated contrast sensitivity was taken as a measure for the total amount of visual information transferred by the optical media. At optimal focus, integrated contrast sensitivity and log contrast sensitivity at 8 cpd showed a significant age-related decline. The log contrast sensitivity at 1 cpd appeared to be independent of age. The depth of focus for a 4-mm pupil increased significantly with age, even though contrast sensitivity at +2 D defocus decreases with age too, but not as much as the contrast sensitivity at optimal focus. Our study indicates that the effect of defocus on contrast sensitivity decreases with age; this was attributed to age-related changes in the optical media.
Article
Contrast sensitivity continues to emerge as a powerful tool for clinicians better to understand and offer treatment that directly relates to patients' functional vision and quality of life. Understanding the limitations of Snellen acuity to measure quality of vision necessitates sine wave grating contrast sensitivity as a standard adjunct vision test. The ability of sine wave contrast sensitivity to offer the most sensitive and comprehensive measure of contrast sensitivity, coupled with its ability to measure visual function and relate directly to wavefront analysis, promises an exciting tool for clinicians and the ophthalmic community with which to provide better patient care.