ArticlePDF Available

Understanding the means and objects of addiction: Technology, the Internet, and gambling

Authors:

Abstract

This article describes how using new computer technology and the Internet for gambling can represent both the means and object of addiction. However, these technological factors do not represent the cause of addictive behavior. Given the widespread availability of computer technology and the remarkable expansion of the Internet, it is not surprising, however, that these technological advances have become associated with intemperate gambling activities. By discussing the concept of addiction and its associated social setting, neurochemistry, and gaming characteristics, this article suggests that addiction is the result of shifts in subjective experience and that new technology and the Internet can provide relatively reliable and potent contemporary vehicles for changing emotional states.
Understanding the Means and Objects of Addiction:
Technology, the Internet, and Gambling
Howard J. Shaffer, Ph.D., C.A.S.
Harvard Medical Schoo~ Division on Addictions
This article describes how using new computer technology and the Internet for
gambling can represent both the means and object of addiction. However, these tech-
nological factors do not represent the cause of addictive behavior. Given the wide-
spread availability of computer technology and the remarkable expansion of the In-
ternet, it is not surprising, however, that these technological advances have become
associated with intemperate gambling activities. By discussing the concept of addiction
and its associated social setting, neurochemistry, and gaming characteristics, this m-ti-
de suggests that addiction is the result of shifts in subjective experience and that new
technology and the Intemet can provide relatively reliable and potent contemporary
vehicles for changing emotional states.
New computer technology is the means by which gambling is de-
livered to homes, businesses, and airplanes. Computer users require
this new technology to access and maintain the Internet, which is a vast
network that serves as both a communication system and a repository
of information. During 1995, the Internet came to be regarded as a new
"mainstream" communication vehicle for the general public (Chapman,
1996). Researchers estimate that there are 13 to 15 million Internet
users in North America and 20 to 25 million users around the world
Special thanks are extended to Joni Vander Bih, Matthew Hall, and Gregory O'Donohue for
their helpful comments on earlier versions of this article.
Send reprint requests and correspondence to Dr. Howard J. Shaffer, Division on Addictions,
Harvard Medical School, 220 Longwood Avenue, Boston, MA 02115.
Journal of Gambling Studies Vol 12(4), Winter 1996
9 1996 Human Sciences Press, Inc. 461
462
JOURNAL OF GAMBLING STUDIES
(Chapman, 1996). By combining an expansive number of Internet
users, credit cards, and new computer technology, access to gambling
has become easy. The Internet permits casino type gambling to become
ubiquitous.
Without a specific jurisdiction responsible for its activities, Inter-
net gambling is difficult to regulate (National Association of Attorneys
General, 1996). "Gambling once involved clandestine dealing with un-
savory bookmakers .... Innovators are using technology to extend the
frontiers of gambling-often to the frustration of regulators" (Bulkeley,
1995, p. 1). In addition to regulatory problems, there is a growing pub-
lic concern (e.g., Jabs, 1996; O'Neill, 1995) that new technology may in-
crease gambling addiction. Murray (1996) described the general prob-
lems of computer addiction for students. This article will explore the
relationship between technology and addiction by using the Internet
and gambling as contemporary examples of the means and the object,
respectively, of potentially self-destructive, repetitive behaviors.
UNDERSTANDING THE CONCEPT OF ADDICTION
To begin this exploration, we must first take a digression and come
to some agreement about what is meant by addiction. This is a daunt-
ing task, however, since there is considerable confusion about the na-
ture of addiction (e.g., Shaffer, 1986; Shaffer, in press). Most theoreti-
cal explanations of addiction are provincial, attending only to the
unique characteristics associated with specific objects (e.g., alcohol,
heroin, cocaine, lottery) and a narrow pattern of behavior (e.g., alco-
holism, heroin dependence, cocaine dependence, pathological gam-
bling). As the opportunities to engage in potentially addicting activities
(e.g., gambling) become more prevalent because of widespread access,
a more cosmopolitan model of addiction will be necessary to help us un-
derstand the role that new and emerging technology plays in initiating,
expediting and sustaining addictive patterns.
Currently, there is conceptual chaos surrounding the notion of ad-
diction (e.g., Shaffer, 1986; Shaffer, in press). Clinicians, researchers,
and social policy makers are without a shared definition of addiction.
For example, addiction does not appear as a concept in any contempo-
rary diagnostic manuals. Absent a mutually agreed upon definition of
addiction, clinicians and social policy makers often are left to debate
whether patients who use drugs also "abuse" drugs. Similarly, "heavy"
HOWARD J. SHAFFER 463
gamblers remain difficult to distinguish from "pathological gamblers."
Treatment programs regularly mistake drug users and "abusers" for
those who are drug dependent. Similarly, bad luck "heavy" gamblers can
be mistaken too easily for pathological gamblers. Too often the result
of this confusion is unnecessary hospitalization, increased medical
costs, and patients who learn to distrust health care providers. Alterna-
tively, absent a precise definition of addiction, some people fail to re-
ceive the care they require. As a result of these complex conceptual con-
ditions, practice guidelines in the addictions are equivocal and health
care systems experience management and reimbursement chaos.
Addiction with Dependence and Without Dependence: Substances and
Processes.
Even under most established constructions of addiction, not
all drug dependent patients evidence addictive behavior. 1,2 For exam-
ple, in most civilized countries, under nearly all traditional circum-
stances, people who are nicotine dependent do not evidence addiction.
In spite of nicotine dependence, when cigarettes are readily available,
smokers rarely express themselves with anti-social behavior patterns or
resort to illicit activities. However, when tobacco is recast as a socially
unsavory or legally illicit substance, addictive behavior patterns have
emerged (e.g., Reuters News Service, 1992). Similarly, many post-oper-
ative patients receive sufficient quantities of narcotics as analgesic med-
ications to produce some level of physical dependence. However, in
spite of their neuroadaptive experiences (i.e., tolerance and with-
drawal), the overwhelming majority of these patients never display ad-
dictive behavior toward narcotics.
Complicating matters, physical dependence is not necessary for the
notion of addiction to apply. For example, upon stopping, pathological
gamblers who do not use alcohol or other psychoactive drugs can reveal
physical symptoms that appear to be very similar to either narcotics,
stimulants, or poly-substance withdrawal (e.g,, Shaffer, Hall, Walsh, &
Vander Bilt, 1995; Wray & Dickerson, 1981). Perhaps the patterns and
frequency of excessive behaviors are more important than the objects
of these acts.
1Although a full discussion of this matter is beyond the scope of this article, it also is important
to note that not all people with addiction are impaired in every aspect of their daily life. Despite some
exceptions, substance addictions tend to be more broad spectrum disorders while pathological gam-
bling tends to be a more narrow spectrum disorder.
'~'his notion of addiction includes a shifting priority of behaviors so that those once having pri-
ority lose value while others gain ascendancy;, in addition, since these new patterns include anti-social
behaviors, illicit activity is present on occasion.
464 JOURNAL
OF
GAMBLING STUDIES
If addiction can exist both with and without ingesting exogenous
drugs--as is the case with gambling-then the concept of addiction must
be sufficiently broad to include human predicaments that are related to
either substances or activities (i.e., process addiction). Although it is
possible to debate whether we should include both excessive substance
or process disorders within the conceptual domain of addiction, tech-
nically there is little choice.Just as exogenous substances precipitate im-
postor molecules vying for receptor sites within the brain, other activi-
ties (i.e., not related to drug taking) can also stimulate naturally
occurring neurotransmitters (e.g., Hyman, 1994; Hyman & Nestler,
1993; Milkman & Sunderwirth, 1987). The effects of these naturally oc-
curring psychoactive substances likely will be determined as the medi-
ating cause of many process addictions as neuroscience researchers in-
vestigate the biochemistry of the brain.
THE NEUROCHEMISTRY OF ADDICTION:
SHIFTING SUBJECTIVE STATES
If addiction can exist both with and without physical dependence,
then we can advance addiction theory by considering the objects of ad-
diction to be those things or activities that reliably and robustly shift
subjective experience. The most reliable and robust "shifters" hold the
greatest potential to stimulate the development of addictive disorders.
Psychoactive drugs, gambling, and certain other activities will correlate
highly with shifting subjective states because these activities reliably shift
subjective states. The strength and consistency of any activity to shift
subjective states will vary across individuals. In spite of this variation,
psychoactive drug use and gambling are sufficiently reliable shifters of
subjective states that this model will continue to rank these experiences
high among the array of activities that can become the objects of ad-
dictive behavior.
OBJECTS OF ADDICTION:
CAUSE, CONSEQUENCE, OR RELATIONSHIP
To this point, I have implied tacitly that simply using drugs or
gambling does not cause addiction. Now let me be explicit: from a
HOWARD J. SHAFFER 465
logical perspective, the objects of addiction cannot cause addictive be-
havior patterns. The teleological aspects of the current conventional
wisdom about addiction theory and practice contribute much to con-
temporary conceptual chaos. To illustrate, if using a drug was a nec-
essary and sufficient cause for addiction, then addiction would occur
every time this drug using pattern was present. Similarly, if drug using
was the
only
necessary and sufficient cause for addiction, addictive be-
haviors would be absent every time drug using was missing. Neverthe-
less, as I described before, both neuroadaptation and pathological
gambling are often present when drug using is absent. Therefore, drug
using is neither a necessary nor a sufficient cause to produce addic-
tion. Furthermore, it may not even be the primary cause of addiction.
Even though drug using is highly correlated with addiction, because
psychoactive substances reliably shift subjective experience, drug tak-
ing does not
cause
addiction. As in the case of pathological gambling
and excessive sexual behaviors that do not fall within the domain of
obsessive compulsive disorders, addiction can exist without taking
drugs. This observation provides insight into the necessity for consid-
ering a more complex relationship between a person who might de-
velop addiction and the object of their dependence.
It is the
relationship
of the addicted person with the object of their
excessive behavior that defines addiction. It is the confluence of psy-
chological, social and biological forces that determines addiction. No
single set of factors can define addiction precisely (e.g., Shaffer, 1987,
1992; Zinberg, 1984). Unfortunately, the concept of relationship is dif-
ficult to define. Therefore, until experience provides more insight into
the synergistic nature of these factors and helps us to determine the in-
teractive threshold(s) that may apply, we operationally define addiction
so that researchers, clinicians and policy makers can share a common
perspective (Shaffer, 1992; Shaffer & Robbins, 1991; 1995).
THE SUBJECTIVE EFFECTS OF GAMBLING:
GAME, SET, SETTING
The most common conceptual error committed by clinicians, re-
searchers and social policy makers is to think that addiction resides as
a latent property of an object (e.g., a drug or game of chance). Con-
ventional wisdom refers to "addictive drugs" or "addictive gambling."
466 JOURNAL OF GAMBLING STUDIES
However, addiction is not the product of a substance, game or tech-
nology, though each of these things has the capacity to influence human
experience. Experience is the currency of addiction. Addiction is the de-
scription of a relationship between organisms and objects within their
environment. Consequently, the causes of addiction are multifactorial
(e.g., Zinberg, 1984).
The experience of gambling--whether buying lottery tickets at a
convenience store, placing a bet on the floor of a casino, placing a wager
on the next race, or using credit cards to gamble at a web site on the In-
temet--is a function of the psychological "set" of the gambler, the "set-
ting" within which the gambling occurs and the "game" that is played. In
this case, set refers to the psychological expectations of the gambler, in-
cluding their personality and cognitive style. The setting refers to the
immediate social environment within which one gambles as well as the
influences exerted by the more distant social mores, folkways and cul-
tural context. The game refers to the specific activities involved in play-
ing a particular game (e.g., the speed, rules of play, articles, risk reward
ratio, player demands, etc.). These three interactive factors determine
the subjective experience of every gambling activity. There is consider-
able subjective variation among gamblers. For example, some people
only like table games and others only sports betting. Perhaps more in-
teresting is the variation that occurs within an individual: the same per-
son, playing the same game, will experience different subjective states
on various occasions because their set and setting vary. This circum-
stance makes it difficult to predict with precision who will develop ad-
dictive disorders.
In every instance of shifting technology, people require the op-
portunity to adapt to the unexpected subjective states associated with
using the new technology. The invention of the hypodermic syringe dur-
ing the middle 19 ta century ushered in an era of narcotics addiction
(Courtwright, 1982); when users developed the technology that permit-
ted smoking instead of chewing cocaine, they increased the potency of
this stimulant and started an era of cocaine abuse. When tobacco be-
came available in pre-rolled cigarettes and no longer required ritualis-
tic preparation, excessive smoking patterns increased (e.g., Zinberg &
Fraser, 1989; Zinberg, 1984). Now, as computers attract more people to
the Internet for collecting information or gambling, this technology is
changing how people learn and play; these new activities mark the be-
ginning of unaccustomed experiences. These new experiences are dif-
HOWARD J. SHAFFER 467
ficult to regulate precisely because the subjective state is novel. New op-
portunities to gamble using the Internet, computer technology and
other electronic means require people to adapt to these unprecedented
experiences. Adaptation to Promethean events requires time. Just as it
has taken decades to begin adjusting to the adverse consequences of
post-prohibition alcohol use, it will take time to learn how to relate to
computer technology with temperance. In spite of the inevitable prob-
lems associated with the use of new technology, we should not mistake
the technological means of gambling with the objects of addiction. We
also should not think that addiction is a property of computers, tech-
nology, or electronics.
The final pathway for all of the addictions is not the object of ad-
diction (e.g., as many believe drugs are "addictive"). The final common
pathway for addiction is always subjective experience. The organ of sub-
jective experience is the brain with all of its associated neurochemistry.
Activities that reliably shift human experience hold addictive potential.
This will vary from person to person and from moment to moment for
the same person. Epstein (1989)described the subjective experience of
shifting brain neurochemicals (e.g., increased dopamine) by gambling.
"Gambling is personal. It is associated with our status in life. No activity
has been more rationalized than gambling--odds figured, probabilities
worked out, point spreads meticulously established--and no activity,
surely, is finally more irrational. In this essentially irrational activity, the
first item that must be fixed, and with some precision, is the stake.
Above all, it cannot be too little; it must be enough to stimulate what-
ever those spiritual glands are that gambling calls into action. The pun-
ishment must fit the crime; the agony of losing must be roughly equiv-
alent to the ecstasy of winning. In this sense, it becomes clear that no
bet can ever be too large; and herein lies the madness inherent in gam-
bling, for the more you have, the more you need to risk" (Epstein, 1989,
p. 100).
Human vulnerabilities for consciousness shifting have been identi-
fied. For example, chronic and acute emotional pain enhances factors
that encourage the dissociative, pain relieving, characteristics of gam-
bling. In addition, exciting new findings reveal a genetic propensity for
novelty-seeking personality traits (Benjamin et al., 1996; Ebstein et al.,
1996). The interactive characteristics of novelty seeking as an analgesic
consciousness changing activity increase the potency and reliability of
the Internet as a shifter of subjective experience.
468
JOURNAL OF GAMBLING STUDIES
HOW DOES GAMBLING, COMPUTER TECHNOLOGY, AND THE
INTERNET INFLUENCE SUBJECTIVE EXPERIENCE
One of the great allures of gambling is its capacity to excite, en-
tertain, and help people forget their everyday problems. In this capac-
ity, Jacobs (1989) considered gambling to be a dissociative experience.
Computers have the capacity to stimulate the intellect and excite the
spirit. The Internet is a connecting web of computer sites. It is a system
of technological bridges woven together electronically to provide an
enormous repository of information. The Internet is a vehicle for gath-
ering resources and communicating with others. Using a computer, like
narcotics, often accelerates the subjective passage of time. Many begin-
ning users find themselves spending more time than anticipated in
front of a keyboard and monitor. Technological neophytes and experi-
enced users alike know the feeling of getting lost in the intellectual sub-
stance of cyberspace. Accessed by computer, the Internet provides users
with the opportunity to escape from one state of consciousness and visit
different worlds of information with all the euphoric mind-bending
properties associated with new adventures and problem solving.
Accessing and navigating the Internet provides users with a sense
of competency and control as they explore new worlds of information.
With remarkable swiftness, visitors to the Internet can take on them-
selves the roles of explorers who sail ("surf") the high seas in search of
new territories. The Internet has no inherent addictive properties; how-
ever, it does have the capacity to stimulate our senses and shift our sub-
jective state. The results of these experiential shifts can be positive (e.g.,
education) or negative (e.g., addiction).
REFERENCES
Benjamin, J, Lin, L., Patterson, C., Greenberg, B. D., Murphy, D. L., & Hammer, D. H. (1996).
Population and familial association between the D4 dopamine receptor gene and measures
of novelty seeking.
Nature Genetic.s,
12(January), 81-83.
Bulkeley, W. M. (1995, August 16). Feeling Luck? Electronics is bringing gambling into homes,
restaurants and planes.
Wall StreetJourna~
pp. 1, A7.
Chapman, G. (1996). The Internet: Promise and peril in cyberspace. In
Microsoft Enearta96 Enty-
elopedia.
Redmond, CA: MicrosoR Corporation.
Courtwright, D. T. (1982).
Dark paradise. Opiate addiction in America before 1940.
Cambridge: Har-
vard University Press.
Ebstein, R. P., Novick, O., Umansky, R., Priel, B., Osher, Y., Blaine, D., Bennett, E. R., Nemanov,
L., Katz, M., & Bdmaker, R. H. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymor-
phism associated with the human personality trait of novelty seeking.
Nature Genetics, 12(Jan-
uary), 78-80.
HOWARD J. SHAFFER 469
Epstein, J. (1989). Confession of a low roller. In R. Atwan (Ed.), The best American easays 1989 (pp.
98-113). New York: Ticknor & Fields.
Hyman, S.E. (1994). Why does the brain prefer opium to broccoli? Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 2,
43-6.
Hyman, S.E., & Nestler, EJ. (1993). The molecularfimrul~ztion.s oJ'p~ychiatry. Washington, D.C.:
American Psychiatric Press.
Jabs, C. (1996, May 21). Fatal distraction? HOMEPC, 3, 66-70, 76, 78.
Jacobs, D. (1989). A general theory of addictions: rationale lor and evidence supporting a new ap-
proach for understanding and treating addictive behaviors. In Shaffer, H.J., Stein, S., Gam-
bino, B., & Cummings, T. N. (Eds.). CtJmpuLsive gamblingr, the~Jry, research & practice, (pp.
36-64). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Marlatt, G.A. (1988). Matching clients to treatment: treatment models and stages of change. In
Donovan, D.M. & Marlatt, G.A. (Eds.) Asw.gsrae.nt cJJ" addictive behaviors, (pp. 474-484). New
York: Guilford Press.
Marlatt, G.A., Baer, J.S., Dovovan, D.M., & Kivlahan, D.R. (1988). Addictive behaviors: Etiology
and treatment. Annual Review tJf P.vycholo~, 39, 223-252.
Milkman, H.B., & Sunderwirth, S.G. (1987). Craving fiJr erataay: the cortwiousne.s~ & chemistry of es-
cape. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Murray, B. (1996). Computer addictions entangle students. The APA Monitor, 27(6), 3839.
National Association of Attorneys General. (1996, May/June). Executive summary of Internet
gambling report. NAAG Gaming Developments Bulletin, 12.
O'neill, M. (1995, March 8). The lure and addiction of life on line. The New York Time.s, pp. C1,
C6.
Reuters News Service. (1992, November 25). Ciffarette shortage in Italy draws tobacconist protest.
Boston Glr
Shaffer, H.J. (1996). Conceptual crises and the addictions: a philosophy of science perspective.
Jourru~l of Substance Almse Treatment, 3, 285-296.
Shaffer, H.J. (1987). The assessment and diag'nosis of addictive disorders: The use of clinical rt.~
tlection and hypotheses testing. Psychiatric Clinics o]'NrJrth America, 4, 103-113.
Shaffer, H.J. (1992). The psychology of stage change: the transition from addiction to recovery, in
J.H. Lowinson, P. Ruiz, R.B. Millman (Eds.), CompreherLsive Textbook of Substance Abuse-2nd
edition. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 100-105.
Shaffer, H.J. (1993). Denial, ambivalence and countertransference hate, in J.D. Levin &: R. Weiss
(Eds.), Alcoholism: Dynamir,~ and Treatment. Northdale, New Jersey: Jason Aronson Inc.,
421-437.
Shaffer, H.J. (in press). The most important unresolved issue in the addictions: conceptual chaos.
International Journal ~Jf the Addictiort~.
Shaffer, H.j. & Burglass, M.E. (Eds.) (1981). Classic contributiorL~ in the tuldictiort~. New York: Brun-
ner/Mazel.
Shaffer, H.J., & Gambino, B. (1983). Addiction Paradigms Ill: From Theory-Research to Practice
and Back. Advanc.e in Alcohol and Substance Abzt~e, 3, 135-152.
Shaffer, H.J., & Robbins, M. (1991). Manufacturing multiple meanings of addiction: time-limited
realities, Contemporary Family Therapy: An lnternationalJourna~ 13(5), 387-404.
Shaffer, H.J., & Robbins, M. (1995). Psychotherapy for Addictive Behaviors: A Stage Change Ap-
proach to Meaning Making, in A. M. Washton (Ed.), P~ychotherapy arul Suhstance AbtL~e: A Prac-
titioner's Harulbook. New York: Guilford Publications, 103-123.
Shaffer, H.J., Hall, M.N., Walsh, J.S., & Vander Bilt, J. (1995). The psychosocial consequences of
gambling. In R. Tannenwald (Ed.), CasiruJ development: Hoto would casinos affect New Englarul's
economy. Special Report No. 2. Boston: Federal Reserve Bank of Boston, 130-141.
Wray, I., & Dickerson, M. (1981). Cessation of high frequency gambling and "withdrawal" symp-
toms. British Journal of Addiction, 76, 401-405.
Zinberg, N.E. (1984). Dru/# set, and setting7, the basis fi~r controUed intoxicant use. New Haven: Yale
University Press.
Zinberg, N. E., & Fraser, K. M. (1979). The role of the social setting in the prevention and treat-
ment of alcoholism. InJ. Mendelson & N. Mello (Eds.), The Diagnosis & Treatment of Alco-
holisra (pp. 359-385). New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company.
... Reilly (1996) reported 'Internet addiction: a new disorder enters the medical lexicon', where he mentioned Young's still unpublished research on internet addiction. Shaffer (1996) elaborated the ' reward deficiency hypothesis' stating that people who were not getting the natural rewards turn to internet usages for immediate gratification similar to alcohols or drugs. Brenner (1997) after a survey conducted with 563 users reported that this addiction happens with the management of time along with normal problems as that of different types of addictions. ...
Article
Full-text available
A detailed literature review on Internet addiction from 1995 to 2023 has been reported in this article. Many psychologists were of the opinion that there might be some existing psychopathologies in people which prompted them to Internet addiction. This observation is extended into viewing the possibility that internet addiction might be masking other issues that need to be dealt with within therapy.
... As gambling became a readily available digital commodity, examining and regulating the influence of technology on gambling behavior has emerged as an important agenda for harm reduction research [1]. In the early days of the internet, Shaffer [2] noted that the innate nature of the "new" technology has the potential to become a potent vehicle to introduce certain biases in gambling, resulting in a shift in the subjective experiences of gamblers. Almost 30 years later, and with online gambling becoming commonplace, the extant literature has seemingly reached a consensus that technology-enabled gambling platforms with engaging and persuasive features can facilitate attention bias, impulsive betting, or longer time-on-devices, which can ultimately trigger or maintain addictive behavior [3]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Technology has significantly reshaped the landscape and accessibility of gambling, creating uncharted territory for researchers and policy makers involved in the responsible gambling (RG) agenda. Digital payment solutions (DPS) are the latest addition of technology-based services in gambling and are now prominently used for deposit and win withdrawal. The seamless collaboration between online gambling operators and DPS, however, has raised concerns regarding the potential role of DPS platforms in facilitating harmful behavior. Objective Using a focus group session with problem gamblers, this study describes a preliminary investigation of the role of DPS in the online gambling context and its influence on players’ gambling habits, financial behavior, choices of gambling environment, and the overall outcome of gambling subjective experiences. Methods A total of 6 problem gamblers participated in a one-and-half-hour focus group session to discuss how DPSs are integrated into their everyday gambling habits, what motivates them to use DPS, and what shifts they observe in their gambling behavior. Thematic analysis was used to analyze the empirical evidence with a mix of inductive and deductive research approaches as a knowledge claim strategy. Results Our initial findings revealed that the influence of DPSs in online gambling is multifaced where, on the one hand, their ability to integrate with players’ existing habits seamlessly underscores the facilitating role they play in potentially maximizing harm. On the other hand, we find preliminary evidence that DPSs can have a direct influence on gambling outcomes in both subtle and pervasive ways—nudging, institutionalizing, constraining, or triggering players’ gambling activities. This study also highlights the increasingly interdisciplinary nature of online gambling, and it proposes a preliminary conceptual framework to illustrate the sociotechnical interplay between DPS and gambling habits that ultimately capture the outcome of gambling’s subjective experience. Conclusions Disguised as a passive payment enabler, the role of DPS has so far received scant attention; however, this exploratory qualitative study demonstrates that given the technological advantage and access to customer financial data, DPS can become a potent platform to enable and at times trigger harmful gambling. In addition, DPS’s bird’s-eye view of cross-operator gambling behavior can open up an opportunity for researchers and policy makers to explore harm reduction measures that can be implemented at the digital payment level for gambling customers. Finally, more interdisciplinary studies are needed to formulate the sociotechnical nature of online gambling and holistic harm minimization strategy.
... Traditionally, the concept "addiction" was based on a medical model and was reserved for physical and psychological dependence on a substanceand not a behavioral pattern. But recent research suggests that addiction should be widened to cover a broader range of behaviors [12][13][14][15]. ...
... Regarding body image dissatisfaction, depression, social anxiety, emotional abuse, and neglect were positively correlated with problematic smartphone use both directly and indirectly [6]. Smartphone usage has many other problems too, for example, emotional abuse and neglect were directly and indirectly related positively to problematic smartphone use via body image dissatisfaction, depression, and social anxiety [7]. In addition to providing enjoyment and lowering pain and stress, using a smartphone can also result in failure to limit usage despite detrimental effects on one's especially adolescent's health, finances, physical well-being, and relationships with others [8]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Smart phone use is on the rise globally, which may have an impact on people's health. The second-largest country in terms of mobile phone usage is India. However, there aren't many studies that have been done in India to evaluate its health impacts. Therefore, it is of interest to assess the effectiveness of codex on the impact of smart phone use on various dimensions of health status among adolescence. The pre-test mean score for knowledge regarding the impact of smart phones on physical health was 1.92, while the post-test mean score was 3.75. The pre-test and post-test standard deviations were 0.91 and 0.93, respectively, and the mean deviation score was the ‘t’-value that was 11.000 and Significant at the p<0.001*** level. The research was conducted in higher secondary school, and 60 students participated. The research design was a pre-experimental design; a self-administered questionnaire was used to assess the knowledge of Students on the Impact of mobile phone use on various dimensions of health. Most people, on average, spend 3 hours and 15 minutes on their phones each day. As adolescents are engaged in smart phone use, it disturbs their sleep patterns, adversely impacting their short-term memory .
... Habitual behavior is defined as an automatic behavior triggered by both external cues (e.g., sound, objects, places, people, and preceding actions) and internal cues (e.g., emotional state, urge) (Verplanken and Melkevik, 2008;Kurz et al., 2015). There is overlap in the characteristics of PSU and habitual smartphone use, with habitual smartphone use appearing to be an important contributor to PSU (Shaffer, 1996;Huisman et al., 2000;Van Deursen et al., 2015). However, unlike PSU, habitual behavior does not necessarily lead to negative consequences, nor does it necessarily produce pleasure or relieve pain or stress. ...
Article
Full-text available
Excessive smartphone use may be habitual behavior induced by cues associated with the phone. Habitual behavior occurs outside of awareness and is characterized by lack of control. It is not like problematic smartphone use (PSU) (Brand et al., 2016), which is used to either limit behavior or produce pleasure and relieve feelings of pain, stress, and failure despite significant harmful consequences. 62 college students participated in experiments to test the effects of visual cues and self-control, which are the important characteristic of habitual behavior in smartphone-related behavior. The results showed that a significantly larger amount of cue-related phone use behavior occurred in the setting where participants (a) had their smartphones in sight and (b) were given no instructions to exert self-control, compared to when neither of the two conditions was imposed. The habitual model is a useful framework for understanding PSU and can help people avoid it with less stress. The results provide substantial implications for reducing the frequency and duration of smartphone use among college populations.
... These include compulsion, functional impairment and tolerance (Lin et al., 2014), psychological symptoms (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005), and human-machine interaction (Griffiths, 1995). It is cited that smartphone use reduces stress, but it leads to addiction creating harmful consequences in financial, physical, psychological, and social aspects of life (Shaffer, 1996;Van Deursen et al., 2015;Young, 1999). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to recognize the patterns of smartphone addiction among post-graduate management students in the North-East Region of India. The validated Smartphone Addiction Proneness Scale (SAPS) was administered to the respondents, and two different methods, namely SAPS method and median-based scoring method, were used to measure smartphone addiction. The measurement results of smartphone addiction show evidence that the student respondents are not vulnerable to smartphone addiction. Principal component analysis with promax rotation (Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure = 0.84; Bartlett’s test of sphericity = 0.000) demonstrated four crucial components that signify smartphone addiction which are habitual issues (issues relating to regularly or repeatedly doing or practicing something), withdrawal anxiety, tolerance, and usage outcomes. Gender was not seen to play a significant role in these components. The duration of use of a smartphone was seen to have a significant relationship with the component of habitual issues but not with the other components.
... Smartphone addiction is considered as the inability to control the smartphone use despite negative eff ects on users. The use of a smartphone not only produces pleasure and reduces feelings of pain and stress but also leads to failure to control the extent of use despite signifi cant harmful consequences in fi nancial, physical, psychological, and social aspects of life [45,55,56]. Addiction to media has been characterized as excessive or poorly controlled preoccupations, and compulsive needs or behaviors that lead to impairment [57,58]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The screen whether it is mobile, computer and tablet is a symbol of our modern age. For our children the” digital natives” who have grown up surrounded by digital information and entertainment on screens. Screen Time has become major part of contemporary life. There has been growing concern about the impact of screens on children and young people’s health. So, we plan to study the pattern of mobile phone use and reason behind the mobile use along with the various effects of mobile phones amongst the urban children and follow up after 3 months. Physical inactivity and sedentary behavior have been independently associated with wide range of negative health indicator including obesity, poor cardio metabolic and psycho social health. Total sedentary time can be classified by variety of specific sedentary behavior such as reading, playing quietly, watching television. Mobile phones use which comes under screen time takes almost 1/3rd of total sedentary time. Mobile phone use has become the major part of screen time use. Pediatric organizations recommend no more than 1-2 hour of daily screen time for age 2-5 years and discourage screen time below 2 years of age. Our aim is to systematically examine the pattern of mobile phone use and reason behind the use along with the effect on children in urban population and follow up after 3 months either in person or telephonically.
... Smartphone addiction is considered as the inability to control the smartphone use despite negative eff ects on users. The use of a smartphone not only produces pleasure and reduces feelings of pain and stress but also leads to failure to control the extent of use despite signifi cant harmful consequences in fi nancial, physical, psychological, and social aspects of life [45,55,56]. Addiction to media has been characterized as excessive or poorly controlled preoccupations, and compulsive needs or behaviors that lead to impairment [57,58]. ...
Book
Full-text available
TEKNOLOJİNİN RUH SAĞLIĞINA YANSIMALARI VE BAĞIMLILIK
Article
Full-text available
Human personality traits which can be reliably measured by any of a number of rating scales, show a considerable heritable component. The tridimensional personality questionnaire (TPQ) is one such instrument and was designed by Cloninger to measure four distinct domains of temperament - Novelty Seeking, Harm Avoidance, Reward Dependence and Persistence-that are hypothesized to be based on distinct neurochemical and genetic substrates. Cloninger proposed that individual variations in the Novelty Seeking trait are mediated by genetic variability in dopamine transmission. Individuals who score higher than average on the TPQ Novelty Seeking scale are characterized as impulsive, exploratory, fickle, excitable, quick-tempered and extravagant, whereas those who score lower than average tend to be reflective, rigid, loyal, stoic, slow-tempered and frugal. We now show that higher than average Novelty Seeking test scores in a group of 124 unrelated Israeli subjects are significantly associated with a particular exonic polymorphism, the 7 repeat allele in the locus for the D4 dopamine receptor gene (D4DR). The association of high Novelty Seeking and the 7-repeat allele was independent of ethnicity, sex or age of the subjects. This work, together with the accompanying confirmations in this issue, provides the first replicated association between a specific genetic locus involved in neurotransmission and a normal personality trait.
Article
Full-text available
This article explores how clinicians, patients, and addiction as a youthful science manufacture meaning. Explanations of addictive behavior and addictive disease are viewed as constructs, ideologies, or ways of making meaning. This analysis considers the controversy—over whether addiction qualifies as a disease—to be primarily a matter of epistemology and social perception. Since there is no widespread agreement about what isease means in general, it is not possible to determine whether or not addiction is a disease in particular. The discussion also envisions how treatment for addicted clients might be reformulated as a transactional process utilizing the range of relevant theoretical models as time-limited reframes of addictive reality instead of dichotomizing addiction as either a disease or not.
Article
treatment models in addiction stages of change: implications for matching treatment matching: a graded-intensity approach (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
a general theory of addictions is proposed, using the compulsive/pathological gambler as the prototype subject / addiction is defined as a dependent state acquired over time by a predisposed person in an attempt to relieve a chronic stress condition two interacting sets of factors [an abnormal physiological resting state, either hypertensive or hypotensive, and childhood experiences that have produced a deep sense of personal inadequacy and rejection] . . . predispose persons to addictions addictions are hypothesized to follow a three-stage course, (that is discovery, resistance to change, and exhaustion) outlines the author's rationale for developing a general theory of addictions and offers evidence from a new line of inquiry that supports a central position of the theory (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
SummaryA retrospective survey of high frequency gamblers (members of Gamblers Anonymous) suggested that, on ceasing to bet, some 30–40 per cent reported significant disturbance of mood and/or behaviour. The content and frequency of these reported disturbances were similar to those reported by high frequency alcohol drinkers on cessation of drinking and it was suggested that in the context of a study of alcohol and related problems they would be labelled ‘withdrawal’ symptoms. Greater disturbance reported by individual gamblers was associated with aspects of previous gambling such as, betting just at the ‘off, or when a favourable price was quoted, being ‘extremely excited’ while listening to a race, exhibiting more than one behaviour out of the following, pacing around, fidgeting, shouting out, and ‘often’ changing his selection at the last moment. The level of disturbance was also associated with the difficulty experienced in suddenly trying to stop betting. The implications of these tentative findings were briefly explored in relation to the concept of addiction.