ArticlePDF Available

Motivational Function of Plans and Goals

Authors:

Abstract

The academic literature shows that by helping tune out distractions, goals can get individuals' to try harder, work longer, and achieve more. Goals that people set for themselves and that are devoted to attaining mastery are usually healthy. But goals imposed by others—sales targets, quarterly returns, standardized test scores—can sometimes have dangerous side effects (Pink, 2009). Because understanding action demands understanding intention, the idea of motivation is natural and readily expressed in everyday language. Cognitive mental events like goals and expectancies can function as a “spring to action”, a moving force that energizes and directs action in purposive ways (Reeve, 2005). Cognitive studies of motivation are dealing with relationship between cognition and action. Literature is indicating a few cognitive elements that can have motivational significance. The article presents the overview of theory and research about the motivational function of plans and goals, according to Goal setting theory (Locke & Latham, 1990) and Self-determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Suggestions for additional research are also indicated.
Theoretical Analyses
Motivational Function of Plans and Goals
Sabina Alispahić*a
[a] University of Sarajevo, Sarajevo, Bosnia and Herzegovina.
Abstract
The academic literature shows that by helping tune out distractions, goals can get individuals' to try harder, work longer, and achieve more.
Goals that people set for themselves and that are devoted to attaining mastery are usually healthy. But goals imposed by others—sales
targets, quarterly returns, standardized test scores—can sometimes have dangerous side effects (Pink, 2009). Because understanding action
demands understanding intention, the idea of motivation is natural and readily expressed in everyday language. Cognitive mental events like
goals and expectancies can function as a “spring to action”, a moving force that energizes and directs action in purposive ways (Reeve, 2005).
Cognitive studies of motivation are dealing with relationship between cognition and action. Literature is indicating a few cognitive elements
that can have motivational significance. The article presents the overview of theory and research about the motivational function of plans and
goals, according to Goal setting theory (Locke & Latham, 1990) and Self-determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Suggestions for additional
research are also indicated.
Keywords: plans, goals, motivation, goal setting theory, Self-determination Theory
Psychological Thought, 2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203, doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Received: 2013-06-21. Accepted: 2013-08-10. Published (VoR): 2013-10-25.
*Corresponding author at: University of Sarajevo, Obala Kulina Bana br 7/II, 71 000 Sarajevo, Bosnia and Herzegovina. E-mail: sabina_alispahic@hotmail.com
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited.
The word “motivation” captures a number of distinct but closely related meanings. Because of this, when social
scientists have asked, “what is motivation?” the question they have really been asking is, “what do we mean when
we say ‘X was motivated to do Y’?” This is a question that has deep implications for social science and yet remains
largely unresolved, though new techniques and technologies may finally allow us to answer the question far more
meaningfully than has ever before been the case (Vecera, Miller, & Marcus, 2008).
The contemporary cognitive study of motivation began when a trio of psychologists - George Miller, Eugene
Galanter, and Karl Pribram who investigated how plans motivate behavior (Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960;
Reeve, 2005). According to these pioneers, people have mental representation of the ideal states of their behavior,
environmental objects and events. People are also aware of the present state of their behavior, environment, and
events. Any mismatch perceived between one’s present state and one’s ideal state instigates an experience of
incongruity” which has motivational properties. By suffering from incongruity, people formulate a plan to remove
that incongruity. The incongruity acts as the motivational “spring to action” (provides energy) and plan is the cog-
nitive means for advancing the present state toward an ideal state (provides direction). Incongruity is a fundamental
motivational principle. There are several ways to eliminate the incongruity: a person can change the plan, change
behavior (increase effort), or withdraw from the plan altogether (Reeve, 2005).
Psychological Thought
psyct.psychopen.eu | 2193-7281
Goals
AGoal is whatever an individual is striving to accomplish in their life (Locke, 1996;Reeve, 2005). Just like plans,
goals generate motivation by focusing people’s attention on the discrepancy (or incongruity) between their present
and ideal level of accomplishment. Researchers refer to this discrepancy as a “goal-performance discrepancy
(Locke & Latham, 1990;Reeve, 2005). Generally speaking, people with goals are more successful than people
without goals. Goal setting generally improves performance, but the type of goal a person sets for himself is a
key determinant in the extent to which a goal translates into performance gains. As to types of goals, goals vary
in how difficult they are and how specific they are.
Goal difficulty refers to how hard a goal is to accomplish. The more difficult a goal is, the more it energizes the
performer. According to Locke (1996), the more difficult the goal, the greater the achievement is. This finding may
seem surprising in view of the more intuitively appealing inverse-U function, predicted by Atkinson (1958) and
others (Locke, 1996). However, it is almost impossible to replicate Atkinson's original finding (Locke & Latham,
1990). Locke’s (1996) linear function assumes that the individual is committed to the goal and possesses the re-
quisite ability and knowledge to achieve it. Without these, performance does drop at high goal levels.
Goal specificity refers to how clearly a goal informs the performer precisely what he is to do. The more specific
or explicit the goal, the more precisely performance is regulated. High goal specificity is achieved mainly through
quantification (increase sales by 10%) or enumeration (here is a list of tasks to be accomplished). Thus it reduces
variance in performance, providing the individual control of performance. This is not to say that specificity is always
desirable (it may not be in some creative innovation situations), but only that it has certain effects (Locke, 1996).
Goal Setting Theory (Locke & Latham, 1990)
The suggested goals do not always enhance performance. According to the Goal Setting Theory (Locke & Latham,
1990;Webb & Sheeran, 2005), only those goals that are difficult and specific do enhance performance. There is
a motivational reason for that: Difficult goals energize the performer (increase effort and persistence) and specific
goals direct toward a particular course of action.
It is stated that an additional variable is crucial in making goal setting effective and that is feedback (Erez & Kanfer,
1983;Reeve, 2005). Goal setting is most effective when there is feedback showing relation to the goal. For people
to pursue goals effectively, they need some means of checking or tracking their progress toward their goal (Locke,
1996). Without feedback, performance can be emotionally unimportant and uninvolving. Goal attainment breeds
emotional satisfaction, while goal failure breeds emotional dissatisfaction (Bandura, 1991).
In addition to goals needing to be 1. Difficult and specific and 2. Coupled with feedback, a third condition is neces-
sary before goals translate into performance gains: goal acceptance (Erez & Kanfer, 1983). Only internalized
(i. e., accepted) goals improve performance, because goal acceptance breeds goal commitmenti. Four factors
determine whether an externally set goal will be accepted or rejected by the individual:
Perceived difficulty of the imposed goal
Participation in the goal-setting process
Credibility of the person assigning the goal
Extrinsic incentives (“E-Portfolio: Motivation and Emotion,” 2011).
Psychological Thought
2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203
doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Alispahić 197
In other words, easy-to-accomplish goals generally breed goal acceptance, whereas difficult goals breed goal
rejection. Generally, people do not accept the goals that others try to impose on them, but will accept them when
they themselves participate in goal setting (Pink, 2009). The greater the credibility of the person who inflicts the
goals, the greater the chance that these goals will be accepted. Awards such as money, public recognition and
scholarships contribute positively to a performer’s willingness to accept a goal, regardless of its difficulty, origin,
or the credibility of the person assigning the goal (Reeve, 2005). Overall, goal acceptance is highest when goals
are perceived to be easy or moderately difficult; goals are self-set (or at least negotiated to the performer’s satis-
faction); goals are assigned by credible and trustworthy others, and promise forthcoming personal benefit.
Guidelines for successful goal setting, which were formulated by Gauggel & Hoop (2003) and Locke (1996) are
shown in the list below:
1. The more difficult the goal, the greater the achievement.
2. The more specific or explicit the goal, the more precisely performance is regulated.
3. Goals that are both specific and difficult lead to highest performance.
4. Commitment to goals is most critical when goals are specific and difficult.
5. High commitment to a goal is achieved when:
a. the individual is convinced that the goal is important
b. the individual is convinced that the goal is attainable.
6. In addition to having a direct influence on performance, self-efficacy influences:
a. the difficulty level of the goal chosen or accepted
b. the commitment to goals
c. the response to negative feedback or failure
d. the choice of task strategies.
7. Goal setting is most effective when there is feedback showing progress in relation to the goal.
8. Goals affect performance by affecting the direction of action, the degree of effort exerted, and the persistence
of action over time.
Implementation Intentions
Goal setting seems promising as a motivational intervention strategy for helping people to accomplish the sort of
things they wish to accomplish. The self-help books advise readers to set goals and to focus their full attention
on these goals. If a person wants to make better grades, lose 10 pounds, save some money, or be successful in
private life and work, then he or she must visualize the goal they want. Unfortunately, motivational processes are
not that simple. The gap between goal-directed thinking and goal-direction action can be a wide one (Reeve,
2005). In a series of studies, where it is examined whether the "imagining success" really leads to achieving
success, it was found that focusing on the goal actually interfered with goal attainment! Focusing on how to ac-
complish the goal, however, did facilitate goal attainment (Reeve, 2005).
These findings are important because they draw out the distinction between the content of the goal and process
of goal striving, and also because they explain that once a goal has been set, it does not automatically translate
Psychological Thought
2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203
doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Motivational Function of Plans and Goals 198
itself into effective performance. In other words, visualizing fantasies of success (i. e., wishful thinking) does not
produce productive behavior (Oettingen, Grant, Smith, Skinner, & Gollwitzer, 2006).
Formulating Implementation Intentions
According to the Formulating Implementation Intentions, it is common for individuals’ to set goals, but fail to follow
through them (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). An example would be that New Year’s resolutions where people set high
goals but often fail to achieve (Sheldon & Kasser, 1998). Unfortunately, this phenomenon does not apply only to
the New Year’s period, but also to the rest of the year, when people often do not achieve the goals, for a number
of reasons.
Once a goal is set, the following volitional problems emerge:
1. How to start, despite the daily distractions?
2. How to persist in spite of difficulties and setbacks?
3. How to resume, once a disruption occurs? (“Implementation Intentions,” 2002).
In that sense, Implementation intention is such a plan - when, where, how and for how long goal-directed action
will occur. People who develop a plan of actions that lead to achievement of goals, have more chances to achieve
those goals (Reeve, 2005).
Despite the large number of convincing studies supporting the goal-setting approach, there are several problems
with goal-setting theory. To begin with, studies on the quality of goals have been ignored. Both quantity and
quality are important components of performance in many jobs or daily life situations. Second, goal-setting theory
does not consider goal conflicts, although they occur in many daily life situations and may have dysfunctional effects
on. Third, the influence of goal difficulty and specificity has been investigated mainly in its effects on intensity of
behavior. No study has investigated direction, intensity, or persistence simultaneously. Fourth, the nature of cri-
teria used in goal-setting research is limited. In order to meet the specificity requirement of goal-setting, performance
measures generally take the form of countable criteria. Experimental tasks (e.g., solving anagrams or sorting
cards into piles) yield concrete scores, such as “number of cards sorted” or “number of errors made.” However,
real life criteria are less clear and sometimes very subjective. Finally, although numerous studies have found that
goal-setting leads to performance improvement, there are only a few studies that have tried to explain how goal-
setting works by analyzing the dynamics responsible for goal-setting effects, e.g., the process by which task–goal
attributes affect performance (e.g., Gauggel & Hoop, 2003;Schmidt, Kleinbeck, & Brockmann, 1984).
Self-Determination Theory: Goals
Self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000) has differentiated from the concept of goal-directed behavior,
yet it has taken a very different approach. SDT differentiates in the content of goals or outcomes and the regulatory
processes through which the outcomes are pursued, making predictions for different contents and for different
processes. Further, it uses the concept of innate psychological needs as the basis for integrating the differentiations
of goal contents and regulatory processes, and the predictions that results from those differentiations (Vansteenkiste
et al., 2004).
Psychological Thought
2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203
doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Alispahić 199
The theory postulates that a critical issue in the effects of goal pursuit and attainment is the degree to which people
are able to satisfy their basic psychological needs, as they pursue and attain their valued outcomes. The concept
of needs was once widely employed in empirical psychology to organize the study of motivation.
Although variously defined at the physiological or psychological levels and as innate or learned, the concept of
needs specified the content of motivation and provided a basis for the energy and direction of action.
SDT posits that individuals’ have three basic psychological needs: needs for autonomy, competence, and related-
ness (Deci & Ryan, 2000). According to SDT, the satisfaction of these psychological needs is essential for psy-
chological growth optimal functioning, and well-being.
SDT claims that it is not only the quantity of goals that matters but also their quality. It is argued that people pursue
qualitatively different types of goals, which will lead to considerably different outcomes (Deci & Ryan, 2000).
Intrinsic goals such as community feelings, affiliation, health, and self-development are distinguished from extrinsic
goals such as image, financial success, and appearing physically attractive (Kasser & Ryan, 1996).
In line with an organismicii way of thinking, intrinsic goals are theorized to promote psychological well-being because
intrinsically oriented individuals’ are more likely to satisfy their basic psychological needs along the path toward
goal attainment (Deci & Ryan, 2000).
In contrast, extrinsic goals often entail the contingent reactions of others and are associated with more stressful
interpersonal comparisons. They reduce opportunities to engage in basic psychological need satisfying experiences,
which in turn lead to less subjective wellbeing (Vansteenkiste et al., 2004).
Kasser and Ryan (1996) examined seven life goals including accumulation of wealth and material possessions,
social recognition and fame, being physically attractive and having an appealing image, personal growth and de-
velopment, meaningful affiliation and close relationships, generativity and community involvement, and being
physically healthy—and proposed that those aspirations were likely to fall into two general categories. The re-
searchers found that money, fame, and image loaded on one factor, which they labeled extrinsic, whereas per-
sonal growth, close relationships, community involvement, and physical health loaded on another factor, which
they labeled intrinsic.
The intrinsic factor was labeled because its goals are expressive of humans inherent growth tendency and are
conducive to satisfaction of the basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness; the factor
was labeled extrinsic because its goals typically are pursued as a means to some separable outcome and are
not directly linked to satisfaction of the basic psychological needs (Kasser & Ryan, 1996). Kasser and Ryan (1996)
reported that the importance of pursuing intrinsic aspirations related positively to various indicators of well-being,
including positive affect, vitality, and self-actualization, and related negatively to various indicators of ill-being, in-
cluding depression, anxiety, and health problems. Similar results have been found in diverse countries and
working with adults as well as college students (e.g., Niemiec, Ryan, & Deci, 2009;Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Deci,
2006). According to Duriez, Meeus, and Vansteenkiste (2012), association between individuals’ type of goal and
their well-being was found in various nations and age groups, and with different measures of both goals and well-
being. In addition to displaying well-being decrements, people who place a relatively high value on extrinsic relative
to intrinsic goals were found to perform less well in academics (Tabachnick, Miller, & Relyea, 2008;Vansteenkiste,
Psychological Thought
2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203
doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Motivational Function of Plans and Goals 200
Lens, & Deci, 2006), to be less persistent in physical exercising (Sebire, Standage, & Vansteenkiste, 2009) and
to be more prone to bulimic symptoms (Verstuyf, Vansteenkiste, & Soenens, 2012).
It is evident that the plans and goals have an important role in human behavior. Goals are important regulators
of human action as they set the standards to which we compare our perceptions and expectations. Because of
that, goals are central constructs of many psychological theories, helping to explain why people differ in their
choices, persistence and intensity in behavior. Planning and setting goals for the future increases well-being,
because it provides meaning and structure to human lives, but also motivation for any activities people embark
on.
Conclusion
By focusing on individuals’ plans and goals, motivation researchers have learned much about the reasons why
individuals’ choose to engage or disengage in different activities, and how individuals’ plans and goals relate to
their achievement behaviors. Various theoretical perspectives on these issues are flourishing, and motivation re-
search remains very active.
We conclude by noting two important issues that need further study. First, although various theoretical models
are present, there is a need for theoretical integration in the field, particularly with respect to the models that incor-
porate plans and goals constructs. Although there are some differences across these constructs, the similarities
likely outweigh the differences. The proliferation of different terms (and measures) for similar constructs makes
theoretical integration more difficult. Second, the focus on plans and goals constructs has led to important advances
in the field of motivation. Therefore, it is important for motivation researchers to investigate such processes more
fully.
Notes
i) The degree to which the person is genuinely attached to and determined to reach the goals (Locke, 1996).
ii) Self-determination Theory is an organismic dialectical approach. It begins with the assumption that people are active
organisms, with evolved tendencies toward growing, mastering ambient challenges, and integrating new experiences into a
coherent sense of self. Thus, it is the dialectic between the active organism and the social context that is the basis for SDT's
predictions about behavior, experience, and development (What is Self-Determination Theory?, n.d.).
References
Atkinson, J. W. (1958). Towards experimental analysis of human motivation in terms of motives, expectancies and incentives.
In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action and society (pp. 288–305). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2),
248-287. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90022-L
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "what" and "why" of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior.
Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227-268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01
Duriez, B., Meeus, J., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2012). Why are some people more susceptible to ingroup threat than others? The
importance of a relative extrinsic to intrinsic value orientation. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 164-172.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.003
Psychological Thought
2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203
doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Alispahić 201
E-Portfolio: Motivation and Emotion. (2011). Retrieved August 10, 2013, from the Wikiversity:
http://en.wikiversity.org/wiki/User:AriWright
Erez, M., & Kanfer, F. H. (1983). The role of goal acceptance in goal setting and task performance. Academy of Management
Review, 8(3), 454-463.
Gauggel, S., & Hoop, M. (2003). Goal-setting as a motivational technique for neurorehabilitation. In W. M. Cox & E. Klinger
(Ed.), Handbook of motivational counseling: Concepts, approaches, and assessment (pp. 439-456). Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley-Blackwell.
Implementation Intentions. (2002). Class handout for the course “Introduction to Human Motivation”. Retrieved from
http://www.uiowa.edu/~c07p111/Classhandouts/implementationshowto.htm
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(3), 280-287. doi:10.1177/0146167296223006
Locke, E. A. (1996). Motivation through conscious goal setting. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 5(2), 117-124.
doi:10.1016/S0962-1849(96)80005-9
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and
Winston.
Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The path taken: Consequences of attaining intrinsic and extrinsic aspirations
in post-college life. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(3), 291-306. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.09.001
Oettingen, G., Grant, H., Smith, P. K., Skinner, M., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2006). Nonconscious goal pursuit: Acting in an
explanatory vacuum. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(5), 668-675. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.10.003
Pink, D. H. (2009). Drive: The surprising truth about what motivates us. New York, NY: Riverhead Books.
Reeve, J. (2005). Understanding motivation and emotion (4th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Schmidt, K.-H., Kleinbeck, U., & Brockmann, W. (1984). Motivational control of motor performance by goal setting in a dual-task
situation. Psychological Research, 46(1-2), 129-141. doi:10.1007/BF00308598
Sebire, S. J., Standage, M., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2009). Examining intrinsic versus extrinsic goals: Cognitive, affective, and
behavioral outcomes. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31(2), 189-210.
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(3), 482-497. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.3.482
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress, but not all progress is beneficial.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(12), 1319-1331. doi:10.1177/01461672982412006
Tabachnick, S. E., Miller, R. B., & Relyea, G. E. (2008). The relationships among students’ future-oriented goals and subgoals,
perceived task instrumentality, and task-oriented self-regulation strategies in an academic environment. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 100(3), 629-642. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.629
Psychological Thought
2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203
doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Motivational Function of Plans and Goals 202
Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic vs. extrinsic goal contents in Self-determination theory: Another
look at the quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41(1), 19-31. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4101_4
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., Matos, L., & Lacante, M. (2004). Less is sometimes more: Goal content
matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 755-764. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.755
Vecera, V., Miller, J. M., & Marcus, G. E. (2008). Conceptualizing motivation. Paper presented at the Annual meeting of the
Midwest Political Science Association. Retrieved from
http://citation.allacademic.com//meta/p_mla_apa_research_citation/2/6/8/3/4/pages268347/p268347-1.php
Verstuyf, J., Vansteenkiste, M., & Soenens, B. (2012). Is eating regulation always related to bulimic symptoms? The differential
correlates of health-focused and appearance-focused eating regulation. Body Image, 9(1), 108-117.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.09.003
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2005). Integrating concepts from goal theories to understand the achievement of personal goals.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(1), 69-96. doi:10.1002/ejsp.233
What is Self-determination Theory? (n.d.). Retrieved August 10, 2013, from Opentopia Encyclopedia:
http://www.selfdeterminationtheory.org
About the Author
Sabina Alispahić, PhD is a teaching assistant, working at the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy
in Sarajevo. Her research interests are: motivation, human needs, Maslow’s theory of motivation, and Self-determ-
ination Theory.
PsychOpen is a publishing service by Leibniz Institute
for Psychology Information (ZPID), Trier, Germany.
www.zpid.de/en
Psychological Thought
2013, Vol. 6(2), 196–203
doi:10.5964/psyct.v6i2.80
Alispahić 203
... As the result suggests, students who pursued their intended college course had higher academic performance than those who failed to adhere to their college course choice. Taking the result into consideration, the personally defined-goals may have an important role in academic performance (Alispahić, 2013). In the academic context, studies on goal concordance suggest consistent results. ...
Article
Full-text available
Grounded in self-determination theory (SDT), this study had two purposes: (a) examine the associations between intrinsic (relative to extrinsic) exercise goal content and cognitive, affective, and behavioral Outcomes: and (b) test the mediating role of psychological need satisfaction in the Exercise Goal Content -> Outcomes relationship. Using a sample of 410 adults, hierarchical regression analysis showed relative intrinsic goal content to positively predict physical self-worth, self-reported exercise behavior, psychological well-being, and psychological need satisfaction and negatively predict exercise anxiety. Except for exercise behavior, the predictive utility of relative intrinsic goal content oil the dependent variables of interest remained significant after controlling for participants' relative self-determined exercise motivation. Structural equation modeling analyses showed psychological need satisfaction to partially mediate the effect of relative intrinsic goal content Oil the outcome variables. Our findings support further investigation of exercise goals commensurate with the goal content perspective advanced in SDT.
Article
Full-text available
An integrative model of the conative process, which has important ramifications for psychological need satisfaction and hence for individuals’ well-being, is presented. The self-concordance of goals (i.e., their consistency with the person’s developing interests and core values) plays a dual role in the model. First, those pursuing self-concordant goals put more sustained effort into achieving those goals and thus are more likely to attain them. Second, those who attain self-concordant goals reap greater well-being benefits from their attainment. Attainment-to-well-being effects are mediated by need satisfaction, i.e., daily activity-based experiences of autonomy, competence, and relatedness that accumulate during the period of striving. The model is shown to provide a satisfactory fit to 3 longitudinal data sets and to be independent of the effects of self-efficacy, implementation intentions, avoidance framing, and life skills.
Article
Full-text available
Whereas an individual differences perspective recently pointed to the importance of a relative extrinsic to intrinsic value orientation in the prediction of outgroup attitudes, the intergroup relations perspective stresses the importance of threat. This study investigates the interplay of both perspectives. A scenario study among high-school students showed that only people who attach greater relative importance to extrinsic values react with a negative attitude towards an outgroup that is portrayed as threatening. A longitudinal study among university students then showed that people with a relatively greater extrinsic value orientation are not only more likely to react to threat but also to perceive threat. Specifically, cross-lagged analyses showed that a relatively greater extrinsic value orientation predicted over-time increases in threat perceptions.
Article
Full-text available
Examination of motivational dynamics in academic contexts within self-determination theory has centered primarily around both the motives (initially intrinsic vs. extrinsic, later autono- mous vs. controlled) that regulate learners' study behavior and the contexts that promote or hin- der these regulations. Less attention has been paid to the goal contents (intrinsic vs. extrinsic) that learners hold and to the different goal contents that are communicated in schools to in- crease the perceived relevance of the learning. Recent field experiments are reviewed showing that intrinsic goal framing (relative to extrinsic goal framing and no-goal framing) produces deeper engagement in learning activities, better conceptual learning, and higher persistence at learning activities. These effects occur for both intrinsically and extrinsically oriented individu- als. Results are discussed in terms of self-determination theory's concept of basic psychologi- cal needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness.
Article
Full-text available
The authors performed path analysis, followed by a bootstrap procedure, to test the predictions of a model explaining the relationships among students' distal future goals (both extrinsic and intrinsic), their adoption of a middle-range subgoal, their perceptions of task instrumentality, and their proximal task-oriented self-regulation strategies. The model was based on R. B. Miller and S. J. Brickman's (2004) conceptualization of future-oriented motivation and self-regulation, which draws primarily from social-cognitive and self-determination theories. Participants were 421 college students who completed a questionnaire that included scales measuring the 5 variables of interest. Data supported the model, suggesting that students' distal future goals (intrinsic future goals in particular) may be related to their middle-range college graduation subgoal, to their perceptions of task instrumentality, and to their adoption of proximal task-oriented self-regulation strategies. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Whether you're a manager, company psychologist, quality control specialist, or involved with motivating people to work harder in any capacity—Locke and Latham's guide will hand you the keen insight and practical advice you need to reach even your toughest cases. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Although goal theorists have speculated about the causes and consequences of making progress at personal goals, little longitudinal research has examined these issues. In the current prospective study, participants with stronger social and self-regulatory skills made more progress in their goals over the course of a semester. In turn, goal progress predicted increases in psychological well-being, both in short-term (5-day) increments and across the whole semester; At both short- and long-term levels of analysis, however, the amount that well-being increased depended on the "organismic congruence" of participants' goals. That is, participants benefited most from goal attainment when the goals that they pursued were consistent with inherent psychological needs. We conclude that a fuller understanding of the relations between goals, performance, and psychological well-being requires recourse to both cybernetic and organismic theories of motivation.
Article
Empirical research and organismic theories suggest that lower well-being is associated with having extrinsic goals focused on rewards or praise relatively central to one's personality in comparison to intrinsic goals congruent with inherent growth tendencies. In a sample of adult subjects (Study 1), the relative importance and efficacy of extrinsic aspirations for financial success, an appealing appearance, and social recognition were associated with lower vitality and self-actualization and more physical symptoms. Conversely, the relative importance and efficacy of intrinsic aspirations for self-acceptance, affiliation, community feeling, and physical health were associated with higher well-being and less distress. Study 2 replicated these findings in a college sample and extended them to measures of narcissism and daily affect. Three reasons are discussed as to why extrinsic aspirations relate negatively to well-being, and future research directions are suggested.
Article
A profitable way of seeing 'Plans,' therefore, is as an act of persuasion, an attempt to show that a scientifically acceptable language could discuss real human functions such as those disturbed in brain injury; but without unwarranted assumptions about the actual physiological mechanisms. From this aspect, notice in the following pages the clear, simple, and appealing accounts of early cybernetic work. The analysis of computability by Turing, of neural nets by McCulloch and Pitts, of precise formulations of grammar by Chomsky, and of concept learning by Bruner, Goodnow, and Austion, had already appeared. . . . The summaries of this work in 'Plans' have probably never been bettered either for accuracy or for level of literary quality. The readers then, and the readers now, could see without effort what was being claimed, why it was important, and the promise it gave for the future. If it were only a summary of pre-existing ideas, however, the book might merely have had its persuasive effect and then been forgotten. It added other ideas as well, less visible in earlier work. One in particular, the concept of the TOTE [Text-Operate-Text-Exit] unit, is probably the most frequently cited by later writers. . . . In addition to the TOTE unit, there were a number of other ideas that are not usually quoted as coming from this book; but whose influence is clearly visible in later research. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)