ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

How does trying to make a positive impression on others impact the accuracy of impressions? In an experimental study, the impact of positive self-presentation on the accuracy of impressions was examined by randomly assigning targets to either “put their best face forward” or to a control condition with low self-presentation demands. First, self-presenters successfully elicited more positive impressions from others, being viewed as more normative and better liked than those less motivated to self-present. Importantly, self-presenters were also viewed with greater accuracy than control targets, being perceived more in line with their self-reported distinctive personality traits and their IQ test scores. Mediational analyses were consistent with the hypothesis that self-presenters were more engaging than controls, which in turn led these individuals to be viewed with greater distinctive self–other agreement. In sum, positive self-presentation facilitates more accurate impressions, indicating that putting one’s best self forward helps reveal one’s true self.
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://spp.sagepub.com/
Social Psychological and Personality Science
http://spp.sagepub.com/content/3/1/23
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/1948550611407689
2012 3: 23 originally published online 9 May 2011Social Psychological and Personality Science
Lauren J. Human, Jeremy C. Biesanz, Kate L. Parisotto and Elizabeth W. Dunn
Personality Impressions
Your Best Self Helps Reveal Your True Self: Positive Self-Presentation Leads to More Accurate
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Society for Personality and Social Psychology
Association for Research in Personality
European Association of Social Psychology
Society of Experimental and Social Psychology
can be found at:Social Psychological and Personality ScienceAdditional services and information for
http://spp.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://spp.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://spp.sagepub.com/content/3/1/23.refs.htmlCitations:
What is This?
- May 9, 2011 OnlineFirst Version of Record
- Dec 2, 2011Version of Record >>
by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from by guest on October 11, 2013spp.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Your Best Self Helps Reveal Your
True Self: Positive Self-Presentation
Leads to More Accurate Personality
Impressions
Lauren J. Human
1
, Jeremy C. Biesanz
1
, Kate L. Parisotto
1
, and
Elizabeth W. Dunn
1
Abstract
How does trying to make a positive impression on others impact the accuracy of impressions? In an experimental study, the
impact of positive self-presentation on the accuracy of impressions was examined by randomly assigning targets to either ‘‘put
their best face forward’’ or to a control condition with low self-presentation demands. First, self-presenters successfully elicited
more positive impressions from others, being viewed as more normative and better liked than those less motivated to self-
present. Importantly, self-presenters were also viewed with greater accuracy than control targets, being perceived more in line
with their self-reported distinctive personality traits and their IQ test scores. Mediational analyses were consistent with the
hypothesis that self-presenters were more engaging than controls, which in turn led these individuals to be viewed with greater
distinctive self–other agreement. In sum, positive self-presentation facilitates more accurate impressions, indicating that putting
one’s best self forward helps reveal one’s true self.
Keywords
accuracy, self–other agreement, self-presentation, person perception, first impressions
Man is least himself when he talks in his own person. Give him
a mask, and he will tell you the truth.
–Oscar Wilde
Individuals attempt to make positive impressions on others
in a range of social situations, from job interviews to first dates.
Interestingly, the very situations where individuals try the hard-
est to impress are those where accurate impressions are most
critical to the perceiver. Although traditionally and intuitively,
self-presentation has ‘‘evoked images of superficiality rather
than substance, and deception rather than authenticity’’
(Schlenker & Pontari, 2000, p. 199), day-to-day positive self-
presentation may not hinder the accuracy of first impressions
of personality but may actually enhance it.
Self-presentation is the goal-directed process of controlling
information about the self to influence others’ impressions
(Baumeister, 1982; Goffman, 1959; Schlenker, 1980). In posi-
tive self-presentation, the aim is to make a good impression on
others, through emphasizing one’s positive traits and minimizing
one’s negative traits. In the current study, we are particularly
interested in positive self-presentation without deception, which
is likely typical of most day-to-day self-presentation attempts. In
fact, self-presentation is often described as involving the dual
goals of making a good impression while remaining authentic
(Leary, 1995; Schlenker & Pontari, 2000). This is likely due to
the potential negative interpersonal consequences of one’s
deception being discovered—for instance, people respond nega-
tively to those whose actions differ from their words (Schlenker
& Leary, 1982). Further, deceiving others may have negative
personal consequences to one’s sense of authenticity, which
seems to be a primary motive for many people (Swann, Pelham,
& Krull, 1989). Indeed, even in online social networks and
web pages, where people are undoubtedly self-presenting, they
provide others with valid cues to their personalities and allow
them to form accurate impressions (Back et al., 2010; Vazire
& Gosling, 2004). At the same time, self-presentation attempts
in first impressions are also often successful in that people are
able to elicit the desired impression from others (e.g., Murphy,
2007; Paulhus, 1998), indicating that accuracy and positive bias
may coexist when self-presentation occurs. This is possible
1
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC,
Canada
Corresponding Author:
Lauren J. Human, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,
2136 West Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4
Email: lhuman@psych.ubc.ca
Social Psychological and
Personality Science
3(1) 23-30
ªThe Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1948550611407689
http://spps.sagepub.com
given that accuracy and positive bias can be independent in
personality impressions (e.g., Fletcher & Kerr, 2010; Funder &
Colvin, 1997).
However, not only is accuracy possible in the face of
self-presentation, we argue it is actually enhanced. Why would
self-presentation enhance accuracy in personality impressions?
According to Funder’s (1999) Realistic Accuracy Model
(RAM), there are four critical components to accurate impres-
sions: The target must make relevant cues available to others,
while the perceiver must detect and appropriately utilize these
cues. Given that we are investigating positive self-presentation
without deception, targets should still provide relevant, diag-
nostic cues to perceivers when self-presenting, enabling others
to form accurate impressions in the face of self-presentation.
However, we argue that self-presentation may actually enhance
accuracy through its impact on nonverbal behavior (DePaulo,
1992) and its corresponding impact on perceivers’ attention.
Specifically, positive self-presentation is likely to result in
more cheerful, engaging behaviors (e.g., Rosenfeld, 1966). In
turn, perceivers are likely to pay more attention to such plea-
sant individuals, just as they do with more attractive individuals
(Lorenzo, Biesanz, & Human, 2010). This enhanced motiva-
tion and attention should facilitate the cue detection and
utilization phases of RAM, thereby enhancing accuracy.
Indeed, greater motivation and information lead to more
accurate impressions (e.g., Biesanz & Human, 2010; Biesanz,
West, & Millevoi, 2007; Letzring, Wells, & Funder, 2006).
Thus, we predict that although self-presenters may not pro-
vide different verbal information than those less motivated
to self-present, they will behave in such a way so as to capture
more attention from others, and, as a result, be seen with more
distinctive accuracy.
There is preliminary empirical support that self-
presentational goals can enhance the accuracy of impression
formation. First, when motivated to advance their own agenda
during an interaction, targets, on average, are able to mitigate
perceivers’ experimentally induced negative bias (Smith,
Neuberg, Judice, & Biesanz, 1997). Thus, assuming self-
presenters are motivated to present both a positive and an
authentic picture to others, they may convey an even more
authentic picture of themselves to others than those who are
less explicitly motivated to do so. Second, there is evidence
that trait self-presenters, indexed by those who score highly
on the acting component of the Self-Monitoring Scale
(Snyder, 1987), agree more with close others about their char-
acteristics than those who score low on this scale (Cheek,
1982), suggesting that self-presenters may be viewed more
accurately by those who know them well. Finally, and most
directly, the specific self-presentational goal of appearing
smart does lead to more accurate impressions of an individu-
al’s intelligence (Murphy, 2007). However, whether more
general positive self-presentation leads to more accurate
broad personality impressions and the causal mechanisms
behind this process remain to be determined.
We will be examining two independent components of accu-
racy in the current study: distinctive and normative accuracy
(Biesanz, 2010; Furr, 2008), which are analogous to Cronbach’s
(1955) components of differential and stereotype accuracy,
respectively (for further details, see Biesanz, 2010). Distinctive
accuracy refers to understanding others’ unique profiles of per-
sonality traits, relative to the average person. Importantly, being
able to differentiate people from the average person implies an
ability to differentiate people from other specific people. As
such, distinctive accuracy can be interpreted both idiographi-
cally and nomothetically: It reflects both the extent to which per-
ceivers accurately discern the relative ordering traits within
people, for example, whether someone is more reliable than
sociable, and the extent to which perceivers accurately discern
differences between people on traits, for example, who is more
reliable than others (see Biesanz & Human, 2010, supplemental
appendix; Kenny & Winquist, 2001, pp. 275-278).
In the current study, we predominantly index distinctive
accuracy by examining distinctive self–other agreement across
the Big Five personality traits, using self-reported personality
traits as the accuracy benchmark for perceivers’ impressions.
Although the self may not always be the ideal accuracy criter-
ion (e.g., Vazire, 2010), self–other agreement is a common
index of accuracy (e.g., Funder & Colvin, 1997), and is quite
appropriate when the ‘‘other’’ is someone who has had minimal
access to information about the target person, as in the current
study. Nonetheless, because the trait of intelligence can be
measured more objectively than most other traits, we use stan-
dardized intelligence test scores in addition to self-reports as
accuracy criteria for the trait of intelligence. Overall, we use
the terms distinctive accuracy and distinctive self–other agree-
ment interchangeably, bearing in mind that the accuracy criter-
ion is generally the target’s self-reported personality traits, with
the exception of intelligence, for which we also have the stan-
dardized test scores.
Normative accuracy is the extent to which perceivers view
others as possessing a similar profile of traits as the average
person. Because the average person possesses a more positive
than negative personality profile (Borkenau & Zaltauskas,
2009; Edwards, 1957), being perceived normatively implies
being seen more positively. Given that the current study
involves an experimental manipulation, we can utilize norma-
tive accuracy as an index of positive bias: People randomly
assigned to self-present should not differ in their actual level
of similarity to the average person compared to those in the
control condition, so if perceivers see them more normatively,
then they are being viewed with positive bias. Nonetheless,
normative accuracy is a distinct concept from positivity, and
therefore we also index the positivity of impressions by exam-
ining whether self-presenters were viewed as more attractive
and better liked than those less motivated to self-present. In
sum, we hypothesize that self-presenters will be seen more
positively but also more accurately.
Positivity and distinctive accuracy can be independent
because positivity is reflected in the mean levels of personality
ratings while distinctive accuracy is reflected in the pattern of
ratings. For instance, a perceiver could rate an individual as
more sociable and reliable than he or she really is (reflecting
24 Social Psychological and Personality Science 3(1)
a positive impression), but still accurately determine that the
individual is more sociable than reliable (reflecting a distinc-
tively accurate impression). Equivalently, two self-presenters
could be seen as more sociable and reliable than they really are,
but also more accurately compared in terms of who is more
reliable than the other. Thus, greater distinctive accuracy would
enable perceivers to better differentiate among self-presenters,
understanding who might be better suited to a job where relia-
bility is critical, for instance.
In sum, we hypothesize that positive self-presentation
will enhance both the positivity and the accuracy of first impres-
sions. In the following experiment, we examined whether percei-
vers’ impressions of those who had been explicitly instructed to
self-present were more positive and accurate than impressions of
those in a self-presentation-minimizing control group. We then
examined the mechanisms behind these effects by examining
whether self-presenters were more attention-getting and enga-
ging than controls, and whether such engagement was in turn
associated with greater distinctive self–other agreement. Overall,
putting one’s best self forward is argued to capture others
attention, thereby allowing others to more accurately see
one’s true self.
Study
Method
Participants. A total of 66 University of British Columbia
(UBC) undergraduates (51 females, 15 males; mean age ¼
21.89, SD ¼5.73) participated in exchange for extra course
credits. All participants viewed videotapes of 24 individuals
(targets) and then rated their personalities on an abbreviated
21-item version of the Big Five Inventory (BFI; John &
Srivastava, 1999) plus 3 items assessing intelligence: ‘Is intelli-
gent,’’ ‘Is bright,’’ and ‘Receives good grades.’
1
Participants
also rated whether they thought the target was physically attrac-
tive and whether they liked each target on 1 (strongly disagree)
to 7 (strongly agree) scales. Roughly half of the targets were
instructed to self-present and half were given self-presentation
minimizing instructions.
Targets. Target stimulus materials consisted of 24 UBC
undergraduates who participated in a study ostensibly investi-
gating the effects of ‘‘digital communication’’ in exchange for
extra course credits. Of the 24 targets, 11 targets (7 female,
4 males; mean age ¼21.6, SD ¼4.25) had been instructed to
self-present, while 13 (8 female, 5 male; mean age ¼20.14,
SD ¼2.03) were given self-presentation minimizing instruc-
tions. All targets first completed self-report personality ratings
on the BFI (John & Srivastava, 1999) plus the three intelligence
items described above and completed the Wonderlic Personnel
Test (WPT), a 50-item, 12-minute timed test of intelligence
(test–retest reliability ranges from .82 to .94; Wonderlic, Inc.,
2002). Next, targets were randomly assigned to either the control
or self-presentation condition. All targets were told that they
were in the ‘‘digital’’ condition and would be left alone in the lab
to answer several getting-acquainted questions (e.g., ‘‘describe
two or three interests’’) provided on cue cards to the webcam
on the computer. At this point, they were not aware their
video would be shown to others. This cover story was pro-
vided to minimize self-presentational concerns for control
participants. All participants were asked to ‘‘respond
honestly and thoughtfully to the questions,’’ but control targets
were instructed to:
Keep in mind that we are not interested in your answers per se,
we are more interested in how it feels for you to answer them in
this format.
While targets in the self-presentation condition were asked to:
Also try to make a good impression when you answer the
questions, as you would if you were speaking to a person you
just met or had just started dating. Don’t role-play, or pretend
you are somewhere where you are not, but simply try to put
your best face forward.
The instructions in the self-presentation condition were adapted
from previous research and have been shown to produce heigh-
tened self-presentation (Dunn, Biesanz, Human, & Finn, 2007).
The instructions in the control condition were meant to minimize
self-presentation attempts. Directly after answering all ques-
tions, targets rated their mood and completed multiple measures
of general adjustment (see online supplementary appendix found
at http://spp.sagepub.com/supplemental). Importantly, the con-
trol and self-presenting targets did not differ significantly from
one another in terms of personality, IQ, adjustment, mood, or
length of video clip, all |t’s| < 1.02.
Two trained research assistants also coded the videos and
transcripts for information quantity, indexed by the number
of words spoken, the number of topics mentioned, the number
of sentences, speech rate, the amount of time looking at the
camera, the amount of time looking at the camera while
speaking, and the number of pauses (interrater reliability intra-
class correlations [ICCs] ranged from .83 to 1.00). Overall,
self-presenters and controls did not differ on these indices of
information quantity, indicating that targets in both conditions
provided an equivalent amount of information.
Coders. A total of 99 coders (86 female, 13 male) were later
recruited to rate our proposed mediator of how engaging and
attention-getting the targets were in exchange for extra course
credit. These coders watched each video clip and then rated the
extent to which each target ‘‘managed to hold my attention
throughout most of the video clip’’ on a 1 (strongly disagree)
to 7 (strongly agree) scale. These coders also rated the quality
of the information targets provided and a separate group of
coders rated the targets’ behaviors, described in detail in the
online supplementary appendix.
Analytical approach. To examine whether target self-
presentation enhanced distinctive and normative accuracy, we
estimated a multilevel model utilizing R’s lme4 package (Bates
& Sarkar, 2007; R Development Core Team, 2009) following
the social accuracy modeling procedures outlined by Biesanz
(2010; for empirical examples, see Biesanz & Human, 2010;
Human et al. 25
Chan, Rogers, Parisotto, & Biesanz, 2010; Lorenzo et al., 2010).
Specifically, in the within-perceiver part of the model (Level
1), perceivers’ ratings of each target on each item were
predicted simultaneously from the mean self-report on each
item and the target self-reports on each item, after subtract-
ing the mean self-report for that item. In order to get a more
reliable estimate of the mean self-report on each item, the
means were based on a larger set of self-reports (n¼273) from
similar participants, also UBC undergraduate students
recruited from the UBC human subject pool. Items were not
reverse coded prior to analysis. The relationship between the
means of each item and perceiver ratings reflects normative
agreement—the extent to which perceiver ratings correspond
to the average self-report on these personality dimensions.
By partialling out the mean self-report for each item, the rela-
tionship between target self-reports and perceiver ratings
reflects distinctive self–other agreement—the extent to which
perceivers’ ratings map on to the targets’ distinctive self-
reported personality profiles.
To examine the effect of self-presentation on distinctive and
normative agreement, target experimental condition was dummy
coded (0 ¼Control;1¼Self-presentation) and included as a
moderator of distinctive and normative agreement slopes in the
Level 2 part of the model. The critical parameters are the change
in distinctive and normative agreement slopes as a function of
experimental condition. A positive, significant interaction
between condition and normative agreement would demonstrate
that self-presentation successfully elicits more favorable
impressions. More interestingly, a positive interaction between
condition and distinctive agreement would show that self-
presentation promotes more accurate impressions. Individual
differences among perceivers and targets in intercepts and levels
of accuracy, as well as dyadic effects when required, were
allowed to vary randomly in the model.
Results
Accuracy of impressions. On average across condition,
perceivers viewed targets with significant levels of distinctive
self–other agreement, b¼.12, z¼3.48, p¼.0005. Impor-
tantly, as predicted, self-presenters were viewed with signifi-
cantly greater distinctive self–other agreement than controls,
change in b¼.14, d¼.71, interaction z¼2.22, p¼.026 (see
Figure 1). Note that this greater distinctive self–other agree-
ment indicates that self-presenters’ individual profiles of traits
(e.g., whether they reported being more sociable than reliable)
and that differences across self-presenters’ traits (e.g., who
reported being more sociable than others) were both more
accurately perceived relative to those in the control condition.
We were also able to examine distinctive accuracy in
impressions of a more objectively measured characteristic,
intelligence, utilizing targets’ WPT IQ test scores (M¼29.33,
SD ¼5.08) as predictors of perceivers’ ratings on the three intel-
ligence items (averaged to form a single composite intelligence
rating). On average, perceivers viewed targets’ IQ levels
accurately, b¼.03, z¼7.33, p< .001. Furthermore, in line
with our hypothesis and with the self–other agreement results,
perceivers were significantly more accurate in detecting self-
presenters’ than controls’ IQ scores, change in b¼.09,
interaction z¼2.18, d¼.89, p< .05. Thus, perceivers more
accurately detected self-presenting targets’ self-reported personal-
ity traits as well as their more objectively measured intelligence
levels.
Positivity of impressions. On average across conditions, targets
were viewed with significant normative accuracy, b¼.52,
z¼8.70, p< .0001. Importantly, as predicted, self-presenting
targets were seen as significantly more normative than control
targets, change in b¼.20, d¼.91, interaction z¼2.22, p¼.026.
Because self-presenting and control targets did not differ
significantly from one another in terms of personality traits, this
enhanced normative accuracy reflects positively biased
perceptions of self-presenters’ personalities. Further, although
self-presenters were not viewed as more physically attractive
by perceivers, b¼.04, z¼.14, ns, they were, controlling
for attractiveness, better liked than control targets, b¼.29,
z¼2.62, p¼.009. Thus, attempting to make a good impression
on others did successfully lead to more positive personality
impressions and greater liking.
Engagement/attention. As predicted, self-presenters were
rated as more engaging (M¼4.95, SD ¼.56) than
controls (M¼4.22, SD ¼.41), d¼.91, CI
.95
¼[0.03, 1.77],
t(21) ¼2.17, p¼.04. Further, being engaged was significantly
associated with greater normative accuracy, b¼.23, b¼.56,
z¼2.96, p¼.003, thus contributing to why self-presenters
were viewed more positively. Of primary interest, as illustrated
in Figure 2, being engaged was also significantly associated
with greater distinctive accuracy, b¼.13, b¼.53, z¼2.70,
p¼.008, resulting in a significant indirect effect, p¼.038 (test
Control Self-presentation
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7
Self−other agreement
Distinctive
agreement
Normative
agreement
Figure 1. Distinctive self–other agreement and normative agreement
as a function of self-presentation experimental condition. Error bars
represent standard errors.
26 Social Psychological and Personality Science 3(1)
of the indirect effect calculated using the partial posterior
pvalue; see Biesanz, Falk, & Savalei, 2010). Thus, self-
presenters were more engaging than controls, which led them
to be viewed with greater distinctive accuracy. Of note, being
perceived as more engaging and attention-getting was signifi-
cantly associated with behaving in a more confident, involved,
and positive manner, lending initial insight into how self-
presenters manage to capture others’ attention (see online sup-
plementary appendix).
General Discussion
Rather than leading perceivers astray, positive self-presentation
appears to provide perceivers with a more positive but also
more accurate picture of what a target is like. Specifically, per-
ceivers saw self-presenters with greater distinctive self–other
agreement. That is, perceivers’ better discerned self-
presenting individuals’ self-reported distinctive patterning of
traits, such as whether they reported being more sociable than
reliable, and better discerned which self-presenters reported
being more sociable and reliable than others. Further, percei-
vers also more accurately perceived the intelligence of the
self-presenting targets, as assessed by a standardized test, than
that of the control targets. This latter effect replicates Murphy’s
(2007) finding that trying to appear smart leads to more accu-
rate impressions of intelligence, but extends it by demonstrat-
ing that more general positive self-presentation instructions
lead to the same result. Furthermore, the fact that we see paral-
lel effects with both self-reports and more objectively mea-
sured accuracy criteria lends support to the interpretation of
distinctive self–other agreement as accuracy.
Overall, when comparing two individuals who are self-
presenting, such as two interviewees or first dates, this greater
distinctive accuracy will help perceivers distinguish the two
candidates’ personality profiles, potentially enabling better deci-
sions about whom to hire or date. Interestingly, it is actually
more difficult to compare people who are not self-presenting,
as their differential standing on traits is likely to be harder to per-
ceive. One must be particularly cautious if comparing individu-
als in contexts where self-presentation demands vary, as one is
likely to form a less accurate, as well as less positive, impression
about the individual who is not self-presenting.
It remains unclear from the current research whether self-
presenters were seen with greater self–other agreement across
all traits equally. Although distinctive self–other agreement
can be interpreted both idiographically and nomothetically
(e.g., Biesanz & Human, 2010; Kenny & Winquist, 2001), it
still only informs us of the average level of accuracy across
traits. Thus, it is quite plausible that this effect is stronger for
some and weaker for other traits, perhaps those that are less
immediately observable for instance (e.g., Funder & Dobroth,
1987; Human & Biesanz, in press). Directly examining this
question, however, would require far more targets in order
to attain adequate power. Nonetheless, the fact that this effect
emerges on average across the 24 items assessed suggests that
it is unlikely to be driven by just one or two primary traits.
Thus, we can conclude that the general goal of positive self-
presentation leads to greater self–other agreement regarding
targets’ overall personalities as well as greater accuracy in
detecting their intelligence.
Why were self-presenters viewed more accurately than those
less motivated to self-present? Quite simply, self-presenting tar-
gets were more engaging than those who were self-presenting
less, which in turn led to more accurate impressions. Presum-
ably, perceivers pay more attention to more engaging individu-
als, detecting more cues and thus forming more accurate
impressions. Why were self-presenters more engaging? The
behavioral analyses described in the online supplementary
appendix provide initial insight, demonstrating that more
involved, positive, and confident behaviors are all associated
with how engaging an individual was perceived to be. Further,
behaving in a confident manner was also directly associated with
being seen with greater distinctive accuracy.
These findings may extend more broadly to understanding
why some people generally tend to be more accurately under-
stood than others. For instance, perhaps, individuals who gen-
erally tend to be seen more accurately, such as those who are
physically attractive (Lorenzo et al., 2010) and those who are
well adjusted (Human & Biesanz, in press) and possess more
positive personality traits, such as extraversion and agreeable-
ness (Colvin, 1993), are also seen more accurately because they
are more interpersonally engaging and confident. Indeed, if
engagement is the larger mechanism at play, positive self-
presentation may not be the only way to achieve accuracy—
one might also more directly focus on being engaging, try to
be more extraverted, or perhaps even try to make a negative
impression on others and as a result keep their attention. Each
of these routes, however, seem either equivalent (behaving like
a well-adjusted, engaging, or extraverted individual is likely to
result in very similar behaviors to self-presentation), more dif-
ficult (becoming more physically attractive to others is no easy
task), or may carry negative consequences (one might be seen
more accurately trying to make a bad impression, but they are
also likely to be seen more negatively). Thus, although engage-
ment may be the larger process at play here, positive self-
presentation seems like a desirable and easy way to achieve it.
One alternative explanation is that self-presenters were
viewed more accurately not because they were more engaging
Self-Presentational
Goal (0 = No, 1 = Yes)
Engaging
d = .91* β = .53**
Distinctive self–
other agreement
d = .35, ns
Figure 2. Mediational model consistent with self-presentation leading
targets to be perceived as more engaging, resulting in greater distinctive
self–other agreement.
Human et al. 27
but because control targets were completely disengaged from
the task, to the point where perceivers had insufficient informa-
tion to form accurate impressions about them. This is plausible
given that controls were explicitly told that we were ‘‘not con-
cerned with their answers per se’’ in order to minimize
self-presentation concerns. As such, self-presentation in our
control condition may not mirror natural levels of self-
presentation, which are likely to be higher in most situations
involving impression formation. Nonetheless, there are several
indications that controls were still at least moderately engaged
in the task. Specifically, as noted above, controls provided an
equivalent quantity of information to self-presenters—if con-
trols were completely disengaged, they are unlikely to have
spoken for as long, said as many words, and mentioned as many
topics as the more engaged self-presenters. Further, although
controls were rated as being significantly less engaging than
self-presenters, their average rating on the engagement item
was still above the midpoint of the 1 to 7 scale at 4.22. Thus,
although indirect, these results point to the likelihood that con-
trols were not completely disengaged, but that self-presenters
were more engaged, and thus held perceivers’ attention better
and allowed them to see their unique personality traits and
intelligence levels more clearly.
An implication of these findings is that if perceivers are
motivated enough to pay attention to targets, then control tar-
gets’ lower engagement might be overridden and accurate
impressions could still be formed. Given the relative equality
in information quantity and quality, it does seem likely that if
perceivers could have stayed more attentive when viewing the
control targets they could have formed more accurate impres-
sions. Therefore, in an interview situation or first date, where
perceivers are highly motivated, a lack of self-presentation may
not always interfere with accuracy. Nonetheless, it seems likely
that even in these highly motivated contexts, perceivers may
not be able to fully control their attention, eventually (or even
quite quickly) losing interest in their interaction partner and
accordingly forming less accurate impressions relative to those
who self-present and maintain their attention. Thus, while self-
presentation may not be necessary to forming accurate impres-
sions, the current study suggests it should certainly facilitate it
by enhancing and maintaining perceivers’ attention.
Why were self-presenters viewed more positively than
those not explicitly motivated to self-present? Once again,
being engaging played a role, as did behaving in a more
involved, confident, and to a lesser extent, positive, manner
(see online supplementary appendix). Thus, consistent with
previous research (e.g., Rosenfeld, 1966), when given the
general instructions to makeagoodimpressiononothers,
individuals are able to adjust their behaviors in order to elicit
the desired impression from others.
While it may be comforting for perceivers to know that self-
presentation does not render impressions inaccurate and instead
enhances accuracy, what are the implications for targets?
Although the primary goal of self-presentation is to foster a
positive impression in others, the enhanced accuracy may ben-
efit the target as well, as people enjoy being seen in line with
their self-views, even when negative (Swann et al., 1989).
Combine these interpersonal benefits with the intrapersonal ben-
efits of self-presentation, namely, the elevated mood that stems
from engaging in positive self-presentation (Dunn et al., 2007),
and it becomes clear that positive self-presentation is an adaptive
interactional style.
There are several likely boundary conditions to this effect of
self-presentation. First, we have only examined the general
self-presentational goal of making a positive impression while
maintaining authenticity in a relatively stress-free environment.
Self-presentation in more stressful situations or without the
constraints of honesty may not facilitate greater accuracy or
self–other agreement, nor might more specific self-
presentation goals, such as to be modest or be respected. Further,
as noted throughout, accuracy was primarily defined here as dis-
tinctive self–other agreement; although this is an accepted index
of realistic accuracy (Funder & Colvin, 1997) and our effect was
paralleled with a more objective measure of intelligence, it
remains to be seen whether the same pattern of results would
hold for alternative accuracy validation measures, such as close
informant reports. Nonetheless, positive self-presentation is
clearly not the deceptive tendency it may at first appear to be.
Instead, by capturing others’ attention, self-presentation facili-
tates more accurate first impressions.
Authors’ Note
Portions of the coding analyses were conducted as part of Kate L.
Parisotto’s undergraduate honors thesis under the supervision of
Jeremy C. Biesanz.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the
research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research
was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council
of Canada Grant SSHRC 410-2008-2643 to Jeremy C. Biesanz.
Note
1. This 24-item version of the BFI has been used in several other pub-
lished papers (e.g., Biesanz, et al., 2011; Chan et al., 2010; Human
& Biesanz, 2011; Lorenzo et al., 2010). The specific items can be
found in Human (2009) or obtained from the first or second author.
Using a larger sample (N¼378) from the same population of UBC
undergraduates, each Big Five subscale demonstrated adequate
reliability (Extraversion, a¼.83; Neuroticism, a¼.73; Conscien-
tiousness, a¼.66; Agreeableness, a¼.59; Openness/Intelligence,
a¼.67).
References
Back, M. D., Stopfer, J. M., Vazire, S., Gaddis, S., Schmukle, S. C., &
Egloff, B, & Gosling, S. D. (2010). Facebook profiles reflect actual
personality, not self-idealization. Psychological Science,21, 372-
374. doi: 10.1177/0956797609360756.
28 Social Psychological and Personality Science 3(1)
Bates, D., & Sarkar, D. (2007). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models
using S4 classes (R package Version 0.9975-12) [Computer soft-
ware]. Retrieved April 11, 2010, from http://cran.r-project.org/
web/packages/lme4/index.html
Baumeister, R. F. (1982). A self-presentational view of social phenomena.
Psychological Bulletin,91, 3-26. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.1.3.
Biesanz, J. C. (2010). The social accuracy model of interpersonal per-
ception: Assessing individual differences in perceptive and expres-
sive accuracy. Multivariate Behavioral Research,45, 853-885.
doi: 10.1080/00273171.2010.519262.
Biesanz, J. C., Falk, C., & Savalei, V. (2010). Assessing mediational
models: Testing and interval estimation for indirect effects. Multi-
variate Behavioral Research,45, 661-701. doi: 10.1080/00273
171.2010.498292.
Biesanz, J. C., & Human, L. J. (2010). The cost of forming more accu-
rate impressions: Accuracy-motivated perceivers see the personal-
ity of others more distinctively but less normatively than perceivers
without an explicit goal. Psychological Science,21, 589-594.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610364121.
Biesanz, J. C., Human, L. J., Paquin, A.-C., Chan, M., Parisotto, K. L.,
Sarracino, J., & Gillis, R. L. (2011). Do we know when our impres-
sions of others are valid? Evidence for realistic accuracy awareness
in first impressions of personality. Social Psychological and Per-
sonality Science. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/
1948550610397211.
Biesanz, J. C., West, S. G., & Millevoi, A. (2007). What do you learn
about someone over time? The relationship between length of
acquaintance and consensus and self-other agreement in judgments
of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,92,
119-135. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.119.
Borkenau, P., & Zaltauskas, K. (2009). Effects of self-enhancement on
agreement on personality profiles. European Journal of Personal-
ity,23, 107-123. doi: 10.1002/per.707.
Chan, M., Rogers, K. H., Parisotto, K. L., & Biesanz, J. C. (2010).
Forming first impressions: The role of gender and normative accu-
racy in personality perception. Journal of Research in Personality,
45, 117-120. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.11.001.
Cheek, J. M. (1982). Aggregation, moderator variables, and the valid-
ity of personality tests: A peer-rating study. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,43, 1254-1269.
Colvin, C. R. (1993). ‘‘Judgable people:’’ Personality, behavior
and competing explanations. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,64, 861-873. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.861.
Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Process affecting scores on ‘‘understanding of
others’’ and ‘‘assumed similarity.’’ Psychological Bulletin,52,
177-193. doi: 10.1037/h0044919.
DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psy-
chological Bulletin,111, 203-243. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.203.
Dunn, E. W., Biesanz, J. C., Human, L. J., & Finn, S. (2007). Misun-
derstanding the affective consequences of everyday social interac-
tions: The hidden benefits of putting one’s best face forward.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,92, 990-1005. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.990.
Edwards, A. L. (1957). The social desirability variable in personality
assessment and research. New York, NY: Dryden.
Fletcher, G. J. O., & Kerr, P. S. G. (2010). Through the eyes of love:
Reality and illusion in intimate relationships. Psychological Bulle-
tin,136, 627-658. doi: 10.1037/a0019792.
Funder, D. C. (1999). Personality judgment: A realistic approach to
person perception. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1997). Congruence of others’ and self-
judgments of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.),
Handbook of personality psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Funder, D. C., & Dobroth, K. M. (1987). Differences between traits:
Properties associated with interjudge agreement. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,52, 409-418. doi: 10.1037/
0022-3514.52.2.409.
Furr, R. M. (2008). A framework for profile similarity: Integrating
similarity, normativeness, and distinctiveness. Journal of Person-
ality,76, 1267-1316. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00521.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden
City, NY: Doubleday, Anchor Books.
Human, L. J. (2009). The role of adjustment in accurate interpersonal
impressions. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of British
Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
Human, L. J., & Biesanz, J. C. (2011). Through the looking glass
clearly: Accuracy and assumed similarity in well-adjusted individ-
uals’ first impressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy,100, 349-364. doi: 10.1037/a0021850.
Human, L. J., & Biesanz, J. C. (in press). Target adjustment and
self-other agreement: Utilizing trait observability to disentangle
judgability and self-knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five taxonomy:
History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin
& O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research
(pp. 102-138). New York, NY: Guilford.
Kenny, D. A., & Winquist, L. A. (2001). The measurement of interper-
sonal sensitivity: Consideration of design, components, and unit of
analysis. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitiv-
ity: Theory and measurement (pp. 265-302). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Leary, M. R. (1995). Self-presentation: Impression management and
interpersonal behaviour. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Letzring, T. D., Wells, S. M., & Funder, D. C. (2006). Information
quantity and quality affect the realistic accuracy of personality
judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.91. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.111.
Lorenzo, G. L., Biesanz, J. C., & Human, L. J. (2010). What is
beautiful is good and accurately understood: Physical attractive-
ness and accuracy in first impressions of personality. Psychologi-
cal Science,21, 1777-1782. doi: 10.1177/0956797610388048.
Murphy, N. A. (2007). Appearing smart: The impression management
of intelligence, person perception accuracy, and behavior in
social interaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
33, 325-339. doi: 10.1177/0146167206294871.
Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of
trait self-enhancement: A mixed blessing? Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,74, 1197-1208. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.
74.5.1197.
Human et al. 29
R Development Core Team, R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
(2009). R: A language and environment for statistical computing
(Version 2.8.1) [Computer software]. Retrieved April 11, 2010,
from http://cran.rproject.org/bin/macosx/old/R-2.8.1.dmg
Rosenfeld, H. M. (1966). Instrumental affiliated functions of facial
and gestural expressions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology.4. doi: 10.1037/h0023514.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations of
the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology.57. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069.
Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept,
social identity, and interpersonal relations. Belmont, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Audiences’ reactions to
self-enhancing, self-denigrating and accurate self-presentations.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,18, 89-104.
Schlenker, B. R., & Pontari, B. A. (2000). The strategic control of
information: Impression management and self-presentation in daily
life. In A. Tesser, R. B. Felson, & J. M. Suls (Eds.), Psychological
perspectives on self and identity (pp. 199-232). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Smith, D. M., Neuberg, S. L., Judice, T. N., & Biesanz, J. C. (1997).
Target complicity in the confirmation and disconfirmation of erro-
neous perceiver expectations: Immediate and longer term implica-
tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,73, 974-991.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.974.
Snyder, M. (1987). Public appearances/private realities: The psychol-
ogy of self-monitoring. New York, NY: Freeman.
Swann, W. B. Jr., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1989). Agreeable
fancy or disagreeable truth? How people reconcile their self-
enhancement and self-verification needs. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology.57. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.782.
Vazire, S. (2010). Who knows what about a person? The self-other
knowledge asymmetry (SOKA) model. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology.98. doi: 10.1037/a0013314.
Vazire, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2004). e-Perceptions: Personality impres-
sions based on personal websites. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,87, 123-132. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.123.
Wonderlic, Inc. (2002). Wonderlic personnel test and scholastic level
exam, user’s manual. Libertyville, IL: Author.
Bios
Lauren J. Human received her BA and MA from the University of
British Columbia and is currently a PhD student in social and person-
ality psychology at the University of British Columbia.
Jeremy C. Biesanz received his BA from Cornell University, PhD
from Arizona State University, and is currently an assistant professor
at the University of British Columbia. His professional website is
http://www.socialaccuracy.com/.
Kate L. Parisotto received her BA (Hons.) from the University of
British Columbia and is currently studying law at the University of
British Columbia.
Elizabeth W. Dunn received her BA from Harvard University, PhD
from the University of Virginia, and is currently an associate professor
at the University of British Columbia. Her professional website is
http://dunn.psych.ubc.ca/.
30 Social Psychological and Personality Science 3(1)
... In employment settings, authenticity refers to aligning internal aspects of self, such as motivations, values and beliefs, with their external expression (Hewlin et al., 2020) or acting in line with one's true self or core values (Van den Bosch & Taris, 2018). This study views authenticity as involving the realignment of thoughts, emotions and behaviour (Kotzé & Nel, 2017) to one's best-self (Cableet al., 2013;Goodwin, 2019;Human et al., 2012;Roberts et al., 2005). Best-self is conceptualised as a person's self-portrait of their unique strengths and contributions, which is built from their interpretations of who they are in the world and how others perceive them (Roberts et al., 2005). ...
... Definitions include being one's true self (Kernis & Goldman, 2006), being true to one's real self (Sutton, 2020), or being one's ideal self (Lenton et al., 2013;Vainio & Daukantaité, 2016). In this study, we approach authenticity as congruence to one's best-self (Cable et al., 2013;Goodwin, 2019;Human et al., 2012;Roberts et al., 2005). As defined in the introductory section, a sense of best-self enhances authenticity and manifests when a person displays and is recognised for the strengths and qualities they perceive as unique to the self (Cable et al., 2013;Roberts et al., 2005). ...
... In the participants' stories that reflect the underlying self-appraisal dynamic (Barnard & Simbhoo, 2014), it is evident that self-appraisal often entails assessing the effects of their intrapersonal responses on the self and others in relation to the best-self enhancing strategies they choose to employ (theme 2). In this way, best-self enhancing strategies not only help to reveal authenticity to others (Human et al., 2012), but also serve as standards for being or standards for the best-self (Roberts et al., 2005). For example, after reflecting on her mood, PO1 foregrounds positivity in her appraisal of self: "There's always someone or something that gets you down, then you must mingle with positive people again." ...
... Behaviour is recognised as malleable and self-determined, and the self-integration process serves the pursuit of satisfying higher intrinsic goals or standards of being (Sheldon and Kasser, 1995) so that feeling authentic is expressive of the extent to which one feels aligned to one's ideal self (Lenton et al., 2013). The extent of alignment between the ideal self and the current self has been referred to as the space of the best-self (Boyatzis and Dhar, 2021) acknowledging that expressing one's best-self allows others to sense your true self more accurately (Human et al., 2012). State authenticity entails a self-verification process and implies an internal referent or benchmark against which one evaluates the extent of being true or congruent and results in a concomitant emotion (Lehman et al., 2019). ...
... This also reminds of Kernis and Goldman's (2006) view of authenticity as reflecting optimal self-esteem (feeling of high self-worth and acceptance) and relates to Goodwin's (2019, p. 33) use of best-self as the 'self-checking' point for women's sense of authentic leadership. As such, in this study authenticity is defined from an eudaimonic, existential, state perspective and define it as an individual difference construct (Kernis, 2003;Wood et al., 2008) formed by an ongoing process (Barnard and Simbhoo, 2014) of the realignment of thoughts, emotions and behaviour (Roberts et al., 2005;Kernis and Goldman, 2006) with notions of bestself (Roberts et al., 2005;Human et al., 2012). Best-self denotes people's cognitive construction of the qualities and characteristics they display when they deem themselves at their best (Roberts Frontiers Cable et al., 2013). ...
Book
Full-text available
This special issue explores women in various situations and contexts. We aim to demonstrate, through the various articles, the different ways in which women encounter, experience, and cope with stressful and challenging environments and events in various spheres of different societies. The articles collected here demonstrate how these experiences contribute to women’s distress, on the one hand, and to their growth, resilience, and leadership, on the other. These articles examine populations from many parts of the world, including China, India, Japan, Latin America, South Africa, Israel, Poland, and Spain, and describe studies based on different research methods. Ten of these studies were qualitative, seven were quantitative, and two were interpretive. The papers address themes such as employment (five articles), motherhood (five articles), armed conflict (three articles), and romantic partnership (two articles). Other articles in this issue concern attitudes toward lesbians, excluded young women, and the coping of an extraordinary woman.
... Behaviour is recognised as malleable and self-determined, and the self-integration process serves the pursuit of satisfying higher intrinsic goals or standards of being (Sheldon and Kasser, 1995) so that feeling authentic is expressive of the extent to which one feels aligned to one's ideal self (Lenton et al., 2013). The extent of alignment between the ideal self and the current self has been referred to as the space of the best-self (Boyatzis and Dhar, 2021) acknowledging that expressing one's best-self allows others to sense your true self more accurately (Human et al., 2012). State authenticity entails a self-verification process and implies an internal referent or benchmark against which one evaluates the extent of being true or congruent and results in a concomitant emotion (Lehman et al., 2019). ...
... This also reminds of Kernis and Goldman's (2006) view of authenticity as reflecting optimal self-esteem (feeling of high self-worth and acceptance) and relates to Goodwin's (2019, p. 33) use of best-self as the 'self-checking' point for women's sense of authentic leadership. As such, in this study authenticity is defined from an eudaimonic, existential, state perspective and define it as an individual difference construct (Kernis, 2003;Wood et al., 2008) formed by an ongoing process (Barnard and Simbhoo, 2014) of the realignment of thoughts, emotions and behaviour (Roberts et al., 2005;Kernis and Goldman, 2006) with notions of bestself (Roberts et al., 2005;Human et al., 2012). Best-self denotes people's cognitive construction of the qualities and characteristics they display when they deem themselves at their best (Roberts et al., 2005;Cable et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Law enforcement poses a difficult work environment. Employees’ wellbeing is uniquely taxed in coping with daily violent, aggressive and hostile encounters. These challenges are compounded for women, because law enforcement remains to be a male-dominated occupational context. Yet, many women in law enforcement display resilience and succeed in maintaining a satisfying career. This study explores the experience of being authentic from a best-self perspective, for women with successful careers in the South African police and traffic law enforcement services. Authenticity research substantiates a clear link between feeling authentic and experiencing psychological wellbeing. The theoretical assumption on which the study is based holds that being authentic relates to a sense of best-self and enables constructive coping and adjustment in a challenging work environment. A qualitative study was conducted on a purposive sample of 12 women, comprising 6 police officers and 6 traffic officers from the Western Cape province in South Africa. Data were gathered through narrative interviews focussing on experiences of best-self and were analysed using interpretive phenomenological analysis. During the interviews, participants predominantly described feeling authentic in response to work-related events of a conflictual and challenging nature. Four themes were constructed from the data to describe authenticity from a best-self perspective for women in the study. These themes denote that the participating women in law enforcement, express feeling authentic when they present with a mature sense of self, feel spiritually congruent and grounded, experience self-actualisation in the work–role and realign to a positive way of being. Women should be empowered towards authenticity in their world of work, by helping them to acquire the best-self characteristics needed for developing authenticity.
... As a result, consumers with high self-esteem present materialistic self-exhibition while on the other hand individuals' having lower self-esteem may involve in more defensive self-representation. Current studies on self-presentation reveal an interesting fact that, in comparison to protective self-presentation, materialistic self presentation brings instant poignant benefits because it propels upward temperament modifications and contribute to pleasant feelings in social communications (Dunn, Biesanz, Human, and Finn, 2007) and such individuals find themselves cheerful and satisfied most of the time (Human, Biesanz, Parisotto, and Dunn, 2012). For this mentioned reason acquisitive or materialistic self-presentation enhances feel-good factor and psychological wellbeing (Kim and Lee, 2011;Wojcik and Ditto, 2014). ...
... This hypothesis was based on the findings that cues of extraversion in a virtual environment are so pronounced that they can overshadow the digital traces of the other Big Five traits (Cochran et al., 2019), thus leading to a misjudgment of extraversion and a possiblly more positive distortion of judgment about it. Some authors have stressed that extraverted individuals tend towards a positive self-presentation, which in turn provides observers with more positive and more correct information of a judged target personality (Human et al., 2012). ...
Article
This study analyses the accuracy assessment of personality traits based on photos on the Instagram social network. The theoretical starting point is the Realistic Accuracy Model based on the correct detection and interpretation of information about individuals. The research sample consisted of a group of participants who provided their Instagram profiles for assessment (N = 4) aged M = 23.0, SD = 1.63 and a group of independent assessors (N = 342; 258 women). The accuracy of the assessment was based on the assessors' consensus and the degree of agreement between assessors and the accuracy criterion. The highest assessor consensus was for extraversion, and the most accurately assessed traits were neuroticism and agreeableness - with women being more accurate in their assessments.
... People possess and present multiple senses of their self [47]. There are mainly two aspects of self under a dual-goal approach: to make a good impression on others (i.e., ideal-self presentation) and to remain authentic (i.e., true-self presentation) [29,[47][48][49][50]. The goal of ideal-self presentation is to create a public image of the self that is consistent with what a person would like to be [51]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Communication through visual contents has become the new normal, and visual images and photographs are becoming the primary means of communication. People use Instagram not only for communication but also for identity seeking and self-expression. The casual sharing of digital photos on social networking sites has become a part of daily life, and there is a new phenomenon: people travel to take pictures to post on Instagram. In this circumstance, it is important to understand why people post photos on Instagram and who posts photos on Instagram. This study investigated the main and moderating effects of social–psychological motivations (i.e., social interaction, ideal-self presentation, true-self presentation) and an important personal trait, public self-consciousness, on the intention to post photos on Instagram. The study found that social–psychological motivations and public self-consciousness were positively associated with the intention to post photos on Instagram. This study also found a moderating role of public self-consciousness with social–psychological motivations on the intention to post photos on Instagram. Based on the study findings, the implications and directions for future research were discussed.
... Emotional stability was surprisingly accurately judged in our study compared with meta-analytic results and zeroacquaintance contexts in general (Connelly & Ones, 2010;Tskhay & Rule, 2014). Instagram's visual nature, encouraging users to upload appearance-focused images (Vandenbosch et al., 2021), constitutes a context that motivates positive self-expression, which may facilitate accurate personality judgments (Human et al., 2012) of certain traits (e.g., emotional stability). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: This study examined personality expression, impression formation, and the consensus and accuracy of zero-acquaintance personality judgments that were based on people's Instagram accounts. Method: Self- and informant reports of the Big Five personality traits, self-esteem, and narcissism were collected for 102 Instagram users. Screenshots were taken of Instagram users' profiles, including up to the 102 latest available Instagram posts. A number of Instagram cues were objectively retrieved, counted, and rated by independent trained cue coders from the screenshots. 100 unacquainted observers then judged the Big Five traits, self-esteem, and narcissism on the basis of Instagram screenshots only. Results: We identified Instagram account characteristics that were associated with users' personality traits (measured with self-reports, informant reports, and self-informant composites) and observers' zero-acquaintance personality judgments. Personality judgments that were based on Instagram accounts demonstrated consensus and significantly converged with Instagram users' Big Five traits, self-esteem, and narcissism across all three personality criteria. Averaged-observer accuracy correlations for self-informant composite scores ranged from r = .44 (p < .001) for extraversion to r = .25 (p = .013) for conscientiousness. Conclusions: Our findings provide insight into cue processes of online self-portrayal and impression formation on Instagram and the level of zero-acquaintance accuracy.
... According to Trottier (2012) and Belk (2013), friends can influence one's online image and may even behave like an extension of oneself. Therefore, Facebook users concerned with maintaining a career-oriented online image should disclose positive self-provided information on social networks as well as remove unwanted photos, posts and comments provided by others (Human et al., 2012;Lee-Won et al., 2014;Rui & Stefanone, 2013). In the employment context, job seekers who desire to build their brand online and network with professionals should convey a career-oriented image to potential recruiters (Bohnert & Ross, 2010;Schwämmlein & Wodzicki, 2012) because prospective employers are likely to review their online profiles/image (Curran et al., 2014;Root & McKay, 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Although Facebook is regarded as a common social network to connect everyone, it has not yet attracted much attention from researchers from the perspective of job searching. Based on the theories of job searching via social media networks, self-efficacy and social exchange, this study is conducted to investigate the impacts of social media self-efficacy and peer influence on perceiving Facebook as a job searching tool and maintaining a career-oriented online image by the youth in Vietnam. The empirical data collected from 317 respondents were examined using Covariance-based Structural Equation Modeling (CB-SEM) followed by Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA). The results show that social media self-efficacy and peer groups on Facebook motivate users' perception of Facebook as a job searching tool. In addition, this perception positively impacts the building and maintaining a career-oriented online image. Job seekers can use Facebook as a source of job search information, while organizations can use Facebook to review and recruit potential candidates.
Article
Full-text available
Does the common advice to “be yourself” lead people to reveal who they truly are? And what broader personal and social implications might this advice bear? In an experimental first-impression study, we examined whether a manipulation instructing some people to be themselves (vs. no explicit instructions) led targets to have their unique personality profiles more accurately perceived, and carried personal and social benefits. Specifically, 204 targets participated in a video interview, with half the targets told to “be yourself” before the interview. Then, 373 observers watched subsets of target video interviews. Overall, the manipulation led targets to be seen with greater distinctive accuracy, especially on their more observable and evaluative self-aspects. However, the manipulation did not significantly influence impression normativity, target likability, nor target post-interview well-being. In sum, being told to be oneself elicits more accurate first-impression perceptions but may not bear immediate personal or social consequences.
Article
Is what is beautiful good and more accurately understood? Lorenzo et al. (2010) explored this question and found that more attractive targets (as per consensus) were judged more positively and accurately. Perceivers’ specific (idiosyncratic) ratings of targets’ attractiveness were also related to more positive and accurate impressions, but the latter was only true for highly consensually attractive targets. With a larger sample (N=547), employing a round-robin study design, we aimed to replicate and extend these findings by 1) using a more reliable accuracy criterion, 2) using a direct measure of positive personality impressions, and 3) exploring attention as a potential mechanism of these links. We found that targets’ consensual attractiveness was not significantly related to the positivity or the accuracy of impressions. Replicating the original findings, idiosyncratic attractiveness was related to more positive impressions. The association between idiosyncratic attractiveness and accuracy was again dependent on consensual attractiveness, but here, idiosyncratic attractiveness was associated with lower accuracy for less consensually attractive targets. Perceivers’ attention helped explain these associations. These results partially replicate the original findings while also providing new insight: What is beautiful to the beholder is good but is less accurately understood if the target is consensually less attractive.
Article
Full-text available
Reactions to trait self-enhancers were investigated in 2 longitudinal studies of person.perception in discussion groups. Groups of 4-6 participants met 7 times for 20 rain. After Meetings 1 and 7, group members rated their perceptions of one another. In Study 1, trait self-enhancement was indexed by measures of narcissism and self-deceptive enhancement. At the first meeting, self-enhancers made positive impressions: They were seen as agreeable, well adjusted, and competent. After 7 weeks, however, they were rated negatively and gave self-evaluations discrepant with peer evaluations they received. In Study 2, an independent sample of observers (close acquaintances) enabled a pretest index of discrepancy self-enhancement: It predicted the same deteriorating pattern of interpersonal perceptions as the other three trait measures. Nonetheless, all self-enhancement measures correlated positively with self-esteem.
Article
Full-text available
Three studies asked why people sometimes seek positive feedback (self-enhance) and sometimes seek subjectively accurate feedback (self-verify). Consistent with self-enhancement theory, people with low self-esteem as well as those with high self-esteem indicated that they preferred feedback pertaining to their positive rather than negative self-views. Consistent with self-verification theory, the very people who sought favorable feedback pertaining to their positive self-conceptions sought unfavorable feedback pertaining to their negative self-views, regardless of their level of global self-esteem. Apparently, although all people prefer to seek feedback regarding their positive self-views, when they seek feedback regarding their negative self-views, they seek unfavorable feedback. Whether people self-enhance or self-verify thus seems to be determined by the positivity of the relevant self-conceptions rather than their level of self-esteem or the type of person they are. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Do people have insight into the validity of their first impressions or accuracy awareness? Across two large interactive round-robins, those who reported having formed a more accurate impression of a specific target had (a) a more distinctive realistically accurate impression, accurately perceiving the target’s unique personality characteristics as described by the target’s self-, parent-, and peer-reports, and (b) a more normatively accurate impression, perceiving the target to be similar to what people generally tend to be like. Specifically, if a perceiver reported forming a more valid impression of a specific target, he or she had in fact formed a more realistically accurate impression of that target for all but the highest impression validity levels. In contrast, people who generally reported more valid impressions were not actually more accurate in general. In sum, people are aware of when and for whom their first impressions are more realistically accurate.
Article
Because of special characteristics of nonverbal behaviors (e.g., they can be difficult to suppress, they are more accessible to the people who observe them than to the people who produce them), the intention to produce a particular nonverbal expression for self-presentational purposes cannot always be successfully translated into the actual production of that expression. The literatures on people's skills at using their nonverbal behaviors to feign internal states and to deceive are reviewed as they pertain to the question of whether people can overcome the many constraints on the translation of their intentions into expressions. The issue of whether people's deliberate attempts to regulate their nonverbal behaviors can be detected by others is also considered.
Article
Gender is associated with interpersonal sensitivity across different domains, with females, on average, demonstrating higher levels of interpersonal sensitivity than males. What underlies these gender differences in the accuracy of first impressions of personality remains unclear. Across two large video studies and a large round-robin design, perceivers’ gender was related to the accuracy of general personality trait impressions. Specifically, female perceivers achieved higher levels of accuracy, but only with respect to normative accuracy or perceiving what others are like in general. There were no significant gender differences in terms of distinctive accuracy or perceiving how others are different from the average person. Discussion considers how these findings relate to previously established gender differences in other domains of interpersonal sensitivity.