Content uploaded by Joyce Benenson
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Joyce Benenson on Feb 08, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org
Review
Cite this article: Benenson JF. 2013 The
development of human female competition:
allies and adversaries. Phil Trans R Soc B 368:
20130079.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0079
One contribution of 14 to a Theme Issue
‘Female competition and aggression’.
Subject Areas:
behaviour
Keywords:
female competition, development,
girls, women, strategies
Author for correspondence:
Joyce F. Benenson
e-mail: joyce.benenson@gmail.com
The development of human female
competition: allies and adversaries
Joyce F. Benenson
Emmanuel College, 400 The Fenway, Boston, MA 02115, USA
Throughout their lives, women provide for their own and their children’s
and grandchildren’s needs and thus must minimize their risk of incurring
physical harm. Alliances with individuals who will assist them in attaining
these goals increase their probability of survival and reproductive success.
High status in the community enhances access to physical resources and
valuable allies. Kin, a mate, and affines share a mother’s genetic interests,
whereas unrelated women constitute primary competitors. From early child-
hood onwards, girls compete using strategies that minimize the risk of
retaliation and reduce the strength of other girls. Girls’ competitive strategies
include avoiding direct interference with another girl’s goals, disguising
competition, competing overtly only from a position of high status in the
community, enforcing equality within the female community and socially
excluding other girls.
1. Introduction
Why should women differ from men in competitiveness? Past research focused on
the greater variance in men’s reproductive success and concomitantly women’s
lack of need to compete [1,2]. More recent research on non-human females
demonstrates that females do indeed vary significantly in reproductive success,
in some species as much or more than males do [3–5]. For humans, health, phys-
ical resources, alliances and community status influence reproductive success, but
women confront unique constraints.
2. Mothers’ worries
Like most female mammals, human mothers must remain healthy and safe
enough to protect their finite supply of oocytes, then later gestate and lactate.
They also must provide physical resources, including food, shelter and territories,
for themselves and their dependent offspring throughout their lifetimes [6– 10].
(a) Allies
Human mothers benefit more than most primates from allies who provide phys-
ical resources and child care [11– 13]. Compared with other great apes, human
females reproduce more rapidly [14,15] and produce unusually altricial offspring
[7], so assistance enhances reproductive success. Like most mammals, mothers
bear primary responsibility for unweaned infants [16,17], but in humans, allo-
mothers can care for older, but still dependent children [12,18]. Because women
often reside patrilocally, bilocally or neolocally [19], they typically disperse
from their natal families for part of their adult lives [20]. Therefore, kin are not
always available [21,22]. A mate and affines who share genes with a woman’s
children consequently provide valuable assistance. Besides a mother’s older
children and her own mother, a mother-in-law [12,13,23] and a long-term mate
[24– 27] can provide physical resources, child care for weaned children and
maintenance of a woman’s status in the community [16,17,28].
For both sexes, unrelated same-sex peers constitute primary competitors.
Women are less likely than men to engage in cooperative group activities
with unrelated same-sex peers beginning in childhood [29– 31] and continuing
through adolescence [32,33] into adulthood [34–39]. In hunter – gatherer,
&2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
agricultural and modern communities, young and middle-age
men benefit from the support and skills of similarly aged men
during hunting and warfare and other group pursuits. Men’s
hunting success and intergroup victories increase the reproduc-
tive success of the whole community [35,40]. By contrast, no
studyhas demonstrated the reproductive benefits of cooperation
with unrelated women as opposed to female kin.
Nonetheless, one or two carefully chosen unrelated female
friends can provide emotional support [41]; protection from
and assistance with competition against rivals [42] and low
cost aid with child care [38], although female kin can perform
similar functions. Trusted female friends however offer respite
from the high costs associated with kin, spousal and affinal
relationships and share a greater investment in competing
against same-age competitors by enforcing rules regulating com-
petition. Ultimately however, a woman invests first and foremost
in her own spouse, children and allomothers [12,13,38,43].
(b) Status
Across cultures, high status in the community increases both
women’s and men’s survival and reproductive success,
enhancing access to physical resources and valuable allies
for themselves and their children [44 – 47]. A high biological
market value in a community based on personal (e.g.
energy level, attractiveness, height, sociability, toughness
and generosity) and familial characteristics (e.g. resources,
number of kin, community connections and skills) determine
status by allowing an individual to reap benefits from and
dispense punishments to other community members [48].
Women typically occupy lower status positions in the com-
munity than men however, even in the most egalitarian
hunter–gatherer societies [49–51]. Forming a pair bond
with a higher status mate therefore increases a woman’s
status [24], especially if she lacks high-status kin [50]. High
status in the community allows a woman to increase her
own children’s success by reducing the reproductive poten-
tial of lower status women through harassment or harming
their children [52–54].
Within the female community, status is calculated differ-
ently. Generation and age determine status among female kin
[32]. By contrast, unrelated women enforce equality [41,55].
Unlike unrelated men, status is not earned through skills and
allegiance to a large interconnected group [56,57]. Instead, a
woman forms several separate, exclusive, one-on-one friend-
ships within the larger female community [41,58,59]. Female
friends prohibit competition by one another and other acquain-
tances by punishing superiority [58,60], requiring reciprocity
[59] and exhibiting a low threshold for dissolving relationships
when conflicts arise [61–63]. These prohibitions do not apply
to relationships with kin, a spouse or affines [59].
3. Principles of female competition
Women compete to acquire resources for their kin, spouse and
affines and a few trusted female friends; to protect their own
and threaten other women’s alliances, especially marriages,
given men’s greater interest in polygamy; and to prevent
other women from reducing their family’s market value and
opportunity for status enhancement in the larger community
[64]. Three principles appear to underlie female– female
competition. First, to protect their bodies for lifelong child
care, women employ competitive strategies that reduce the
probability of incurring physical harm through retaliation
[9,10]. Avoidance of direct interference competition, disguising
competition and competing only when there is little prob-
ability of retaliation, such as having high status in the
community, all minimize chance of injury.
Second, because unrelated females rarely cooperate as a
group, one woman’s success brings little benefit to the
female community. Demands for equality within the female
community function to impede individual women’s attempts
to gain additional physical resources, valuable allies or status
for themselves. High-achieving women have little incentive
to invest in other women, so most women benefit from pun-
ishing striving peers. By contrast, trusted, equal friends can
benefit from reciprocal investment.
Third, lack of group cooperation also underlies a more
extreme strategy: social exclusion [65,66]. Ultimately, with
the exception of a few selected friends, additional women
in the community are prime competitors for physical
resources, mates and status in the community [65]. Elimin-
ation of a non-friend by a coalition therefore reduces the
number of competitors. Proximately, a coalition minimizes
the probability of harmful retaliation by outnumbering the
victim [67]. Female friends likely serve most readily as
coalition partners in acts of social exclusion: a man would
be less willing to evict a potential sexual partner, whereas
female kin who differ in age from the victim would find
her less threatening.
Five competitive strategies result from these principles:
(i) avoid interference competition, (ii) disguise competition,
(iii) compete overtly only if high-ranked in the community,
(iv) enforce equality among female peers and (v) use social
exclusion. Girls employ these strategies against female
same-sex peers beginning early in childhood.
4. Development: who helps and who hinders?
(a) Infancy (0– 2 years): staying alive and connected to
a caregiver
Female infants are born more physically mature and less vul-
nerable than males to physical and social insults [68– 70], yet
most mothers prefer sons over daughters [71,72]. A mother
typically makes the final decision as to whether to keep a
newborn alive and how much to invest in it [73]. Although
either sex can be preferred [11,73], males’ higher status
translates into females’ greater vulnerability to infanticide.
Currently, more than 100 million girls have been murdered
in Asian and African countries [51,74]. Selective mistreatment
of female infants, from outright killing to deprivation of food
and medical care, continues to be widespread [51,75].
Paradoxically therefore, it is likely that a mother is both a
girl’s primary adversary and ally in infancy. While a father,
other kin and affines indirectly influence a mother’s decision
to invest in her daughter [11,73], it is an infant girl’s mother
who typically keeps her alive and healthy. Attracting her
mother’s investment therefore is absolutely critical [11]. A
girl’s direct competitors are current or future brothers, and
older sisters. Establishing a bond rapidly with a mother
and other caregivers is critical.
A female newborn may compete for her mother’s invest-
ment by being easier to care for and more interactive than her
brothers [76–78]. Parents tolerate crying less in female than
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
2
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
male infants: female infants are viewed as weak if they cry,
whereas the reverse is true for males [79]. From birth,
female neonates exhibit fewer negative emotions, less intense
activity [80–82] and greater self-regulation [83]. Further, com-
pared with males, female infants look more at a caregiver’s
face [84] and more accurately identify adult emotions [85].
They also smile more and make more eye contact [86,87].
When they become mobile, they remain in closer proximity
to their mothers [88,89]. By 14 months, female infants
comply more than males with parental requests [90– 92].
(b) Early childhood (2– 5 years): learning to assist with
and obtain assistance from kin
The relationship between a girl and her mother and other
female relatives may be the emotionally closest and most
enduring that exists [19,23,93– 96], especially if a girl lives uxor-
ilocally after marriage. Across diverse cultures, compared with
boys, in early childhood, girls offer more assistance to mothers
with child care and domestic responsibilities [21,95]. Beginning
at age 2, girls mind younger siblings, and at all ages girls are
more interested than boys in babies [97].
Reciprocally, in natural settings across cultures girls are
more likely than boys to seek assistance from mothers and
other adults [98]. Experimentally, 3- to 5-year-old girls requested
more assistance from an adult than boys on structured tasks
[99 –101]. Cross-culturally, mothers talk more and use more sup-
portive speech with daughters than sons [102], and daughters
remain physically closer to mothers than sons do [95,103]. For
girls, proximity to adults facilitates resource acquisition [104].
In preschool settings, the more same-sex peers present, the
closer 3- to 5-year-old girls remain to adults, whereas the con-
verse is true for boys [105]. A supportive mother or substitute
female adult therefore likely exerts the greatest influence on a
young girl’s ability to satisfy goals without incurring high costs.
When female adults are occupied, a girl spends time how-
ever with unrelated female peers owing to their common
interests and because unrelated boys can be dangerous
[106]. Further, from birth onwards, unrelated boys exhibit dif-
fering interests than females [84], so that mobile girls and
boys segregate themselves spatially [107]. Unrelated girls
who share goals and strategies, but no genetic interests,
constitute both a girl’s companions and primary competitors.
In naturalistic settings, after 2 years of age, girls often
avoid boys, most probably owing to boys’ rougher play styles
[107– 109]. At 33 months, when unfamiliar female and male
peers were paired, girls were more likely to withdraw, behave
passively, cry and seek proximity to their mothers when
playing with a boy than a girl [110]. Between 2 and 3 years,
cross-culturally boys begin to denigrate girls [95,111–113].
By age 4, despite similarities in size and strength, girls cede
physical resources to boys. In a study of resource utilization, a
child could view movies if another child pressed the light, and
a third child turned a crank [114]. When groups of two girls
and two boys were formed, girls viewed the movies less than
one-third as much as boys did. Even the toughest girls
viewed the movies less than boys. Boys’ physical attacks,
displacement, threats and commands deterred girls.
When mixed-sex pairs of 3- to 5-year-old children operated
the movie viewer, and no assistance with the light or crank was
required, girls likewise gained less viewing time [104]. When a
passive adult was present however, girls insisted on longer
turns. Young girls rely on adults to protect their interests
more than boys do, remaining closer to preschool teachers
[115] and mothers [103]. Ultimately, however, girls compete
against other girls, not boys.
(i) Avoid interference competition
Frequency of physical aggression peaks between 2 and 4 years
for both sexes, but girls are less aggressive [116–118]. Whereas
girls use physical force to resolve conflicts over possessions and
space earlier than boys, after 24 months girls use less bodily
force (pushing, pulling, kicking or hitting the peer’s body)
than boys in conflicts over resources or territory [119– 121].
Girls also are less likely than boys to directly verbally interfere
(command, threat, prohibit or heckle; [60,122,123]). Instead,
they use more subtle competitive strategies.
(ii) Disguise competition
Better disguised forms of competition minimize retaliation.
Girls use complex verbal and non-verbal tactics and physical
movements that appear non-competitive on the surface. By
age 3, girls far more than boys emphasize their caring and
egalitarian natures [124,125], and ‘niceness’, which means
concealing their goals [126]. ‘What girls learn to do with
speech is cope with the contradiction created by an ideology
of equality and cooperation and a social reality that includes
difference and conflict’ [60, p. 205].
Young girls’ ‘double voice discourse’ disguises conflicts
over resources by expressing anger quietly, using false voices
attributable to others to make demands, suggesting postpone-
ment of equal outcomes and shifting attention to cooperation
from conflict [127]. Smiles, politeness (‘I’m sorry’ ‘thank
you’), qualifiers (‘maybe’ ‘probably’ ‘yes, but’), questions
rather than direct commands (‘Will you do this?’), statements
that include tags (‘It is mine, right?’), inclusion of antagonists
(‘Let’s do this’) and other ways of mitigating or softening con-
flict constitute girls’ primary means of obtaining goals
[123,127]. Further, girls are more likely than boys to express
agreement, allow a speaker to finish, acknowledge the prior
speaker’s words and provide justifications that connect rather
than oppose others’ views [122,128]. Girls ‘possess verbal
negotiation skills that enable them to confront without being
very confrontational; to clarify without backing down; and to
use mitigators, indirectness, and even subterfuge to soften
the blow while promoting their own wishes’ ([127], p. 61).
To illustrate, a triad of 3-year-old girls was attempting to
gain a valuable resource, a pickle, in their preschool kitchen:
Sue: And strawberries for dinner, right?
Mary: And the- this for dinner. (Mary puts the pickle in a pot on
the stove)
Sue: And the pickle. Do you like pickle? (Sue takes the pickle
out of the pot)
Mary: And this (the hamburger) is for dinner. (Mary pulls the
hamburger and pickle out of Sue’s hand and puts them back in
the pot)
Sue: No, they aren’t for dinner, no, Lisa wants pickles. (Sue
tries to grab the hamburger and pickle back from Mary but she
holds on and puts them back in the pot) ([129], pp. 16–17)
By contrast, boys compete overtly:
Kevin: (at the table) Pickle. (takes the pickle)
Nick: I’m cutting- I’m cutting- No, I have to cut that! (Nick
tries to take pickle back from Kevin)
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
3
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
Kevin: No, I cut it.
Nick: No! No, no, no! You’re the children!
Kevin: No, I’m not! [129, p. 22]
(iii) Compete overtly only if high ranked in the community
Girls with high status in the community can compete more
directly, because the threat of retaliationis lowered. In preschool
dyadic resource conflicts, compared with boys, girls use less
physical aggression and submit more. A few girls with high
status in the class however refused to back down [130,131].
Likewise, when the movie viewer study was repeated with
4- to 5-year-old children in same-sex groups of four [132], girls’
and boys’ groups gained equal viewing time. Girls’ groups
were less egalitarian however than boys’ groups: one girl
dominated and another girl was not allowed to participate.
Dominant girls who viewed the most movies used more com-
mands, physical force, blocking, pushing and disruptions than
girls who never viewed the movies. By contrast, boys who
viewed the movies the most and least did not differ in verbal
or physical tactics. Overall, however, girls used less bodily
force or physical contact than boys did.
Similarly, high status more than low status 4- to 5-year-old
Turkish girls obtained the best roles during pretend play, then
issued more commands and directives to other girls [133].
American 3-year-old girls follow the same strategy: one gains
a superior ‘pretend’ role, then commands others [134]. Only a
high-status girl dispenses with egalitarian behaviour, which
she resumes when a similarly high-ranked girl enters [125].
(iv) Enforce equality among girls
Eliminating all uneven resource distributions and status differen-
tials provides an effective counterstrategy. Linguistic researchers
conclude that ‘a girl cannot assert social power or superiority as
an individual’ ([60], p. 205) without risking other girls’ denigra-
tion [133]. By age 3, girls enforce equality. Compared with boys,
across diverse cultures girls avoid employing signifiers of high
status with same-sex peers, including commands, boasts, pro-
vision of information or joke-telling [60,122,133]. Another
movie viewer study with same-sex groups of four 3- to 5-year-
olds demonstrates girls’ dislike of superior girls: those girls
who took control and viewed the movies longest were less
liked by all their female classmates than those girls who rarely
viewed the movies [135]. The opposite was true for the boys.
(v) Use social exclusion
Exclusion constitutes a more extreme strategy. By age 3, com-
pared with boys, girls more often disparage a same-sex peer
in conversations [136,137]. Experimentally, when same-sex
triads of 4-year-olds received only one valuable resource,
girls were more likely than boys to use social exclusion in
which two friends ganged up on the third. By contrast, boys
individually directly attempted to grab the resource [138].
(c) Middle childhood/juvenile period (5– 12 years):
developing an intimate, exclusive, reciprocal bond
with another girl
Cross-culturally, responsibility for the care of younger siblings
increases in middle childhood [95,139]. Further, when enough
unrelated children are present, universally girls interact almost
exclusively with other girls, and not boys [95,107,140].
Segregation by sex occurs partially because boys avoid
[113,141] and bully [142] girls. When unsupervised by adults,
girls allow boys to dominate them across domains and cultures
[143]. Othergirls also providea respite fromfamilial responsibil-
ities, share similar interests and provide emotional support [41].
Girls form an intimate, exclusive friendship with another
girl, isolated from other girls [55,144]. Two girls bond
through reciprocal verbal discussions of similar personal vul-
nerabilities, particularly concerns about alliances with other
girls and family members [41]. Unconditional acceptance,
exclusivity and loyalty further define the middle childhood
female friendship [63,145]. Imagining its potential dissolution
generates feelings of devastating loss [146], foreshadowing a
commitment to a long-term heterosexual bond.
Despite their appeal, isolated female pair bonds are not
destined to last and frequently endure for shorter periods
than male friendships that are embedded within a larger
group [144,146– 148]. Further, the intimate, repetitive discus-
sions of personal difficulties between girls can promote
negative emotions, including depression [149]. Consequently,
across cultures in middle childhood girls continue to rely on
and maintain proximity to mothers, female relatives, teachers
and other female adults [37,95,150,151].
Competitive strategies of early childhood become accentu-
ated between girls in middle childhood as they vie for a female
friend [152]. Initiation and preservation of female friendships
require continually expelling interlopers and enforcing loyalty,
exclusivity, reciprocity and equality. Girls tolerate conflicts less
than boys with same-sex friends, as they proceed from one
female partner to the next searching for another girl who
meets the requisite standards [61,63,153]. Even though
female bonds are the focus in middle childhood, the highest
status girls in the community begin to evaluate specific boys
as potential mates [141].
(i) Avoid interference competition
Across cultures, girls continue to use less direct physical and
verbal (yelling, cursing or extreme hostile language) aggression
than boys [116,118]. In school competitions, a girl competes
primarily against herself, as in jump rope, hopscotch, exercise
or academics, whereas boys engage in zero-sum activities
that require actively interfering with another’s goals [30,154].
Experimentally, when 10-year-old children played interfer-
ence and non-interference versions of the same game in groups
of four same-sex peers, compared with boys, girls avoided
interfering with another’s goals [155]. Girls physically removed
another child’s tokens, only when it was necessary to win a
prize, and then they averted their gaze. When winning did
not require taking another’s tokens, girls avoided it. By con-
trast, boys took their same-sex peers’ tokens, regardless of
whether it improved their own probability of winning. Like-
wise, when groups of four 6- or 9-year-old same-sex children
were asked to choose a leader, no sex differences occurred in
length of negotiations. Girls however exhibited more discom-
fort than boys during negotiations, and for girls, the longer
the negotiations, the greater the discomfort [156].
(ii) Disguise competition
When possible, disguising competition is safer than competing
overtly [60]. In same-sex conversations, compared with boys,
girls use half as many assertive or dominating speech acts
designed to control a same-sex peer’s behaviour [157]. Further,
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
4
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
girls use more affiliative speech acts than boys, including pro-
vision of information or invitations to play. Likewise, when
same-sex pairs of 10- to 14-year-old close friends played a
game with a same-sex confederate who was trained to criticize,
boast, boss and otherwise behave unpleasantly, girls made
fewer aggressive and assertive statements than boys to the con-
federate [158]. Instead, girls made more subtle aggressive non-
verbal facial and bodily non-contact gestures than boys.
Similarly, when 11- to 12-year-old Finnish children
described the behaviours of same-sex classmates whose goals
were thwarted, girls reported more instances than boys of dis-
guised competition, including telling an untruth behind the
target’s back, becoming another’s friend to take revenge on
the target, telling others not to be friends with the target, sulk-
ing, putting the target to the side, pretending not to know the
target and remaining angry [159]. By contrast, girls reported
fewer instances of direct interference with a target’s goals,
including tripping the target, taking the target’s objects, swear-
ing at the target, kicking, hitting or shoving the target, and
repeated expressions of anger. Likewise, in 17 classrooms of
11- to 15-year-old British schoolchildren, compared with
boys, girls reported more gossiping and dirty looks and less
making fun of others and hitting [160].
(iii) Compete overtly only if high ranked in the community
When overt bullying occurs, high-status girls bully lower status
girls, just as high-status boys bully lower status boys [142],
though girls are less likely to bully than boys [161]. More com-
monly, high-status girls use less overt strategies. Across diverse
cultures when girls assist mothers in domestic chores, ‘pro-
social dominance’ is used by older girls to control younger sib-
lings’ behaviours [95]. By extending this strategy to pretend to
hold a position of authority with unrelated same-age girls, a
girl can dominate others and pursue her own interests. For
example, ethnographic research in an African– American
working-class neighbourhood demonstrated that girls
attempted to control one another by assuming the mantle of
an authority figure, such as a mother or teacher [162].
(iv) Enforce equality among girls
In response, girls denigrate superior-acting girls. Girls evaluate
one another in terms of ‘niceness,’ that is lack of competitive-
ness, whereas boys like high-status, competitive boys
[56,163]. For example, 10- to 11-year-old American girls
engaged in activities where everyone played the same role,
such as turn-taking games or jump rope, with no winners
acknowledged, whereas boys played zero-sum team games,
with role and status differentiations, after which winners cele-
brated [30]. Likewise, working-class 8- to- 15-year-old
African– American girls disparaged superior-acting girls,
whereas boys continually vied to be superior [162]. Experimen-
tal investigation of 6- and 10-year-old children’s responses to
attaining higher in same-sex dyads showed that victorious
girls exhibited more discomfort than boys after winning a
game [156]. Most directly, when a same-sex confederate dis-
played high-status behaviours, such as boasting and bossing,
compared with boys, girls rated the confederate as meaner
and themselves as angrier at the confederate [158].
(v) Use social exclusion
Any lone girl risks ostracism, but superior-acting girls are
particularly attractive victims. Social exclusion in middle
childhood typically consists of female friends forming an alli-
ance to eject a lone target from the community [162]. A
longitudinal, naturalistic study of same-sex triads of 9- to 12-
year-old American schoolchildren demonstrated that more
female than male triads excluded one member to form a pair
bond [164]. Experimentally, when a same-sex newcomer was
introduced to pairs of 6-year-olds, initially girls were more
likely than boys to ignore, avoid and refuse to interact with
the newcomer [165]. Likewise, 8- to 12-year-old children nomi-
nated more girls than boys as using social exclusion and threats
to end a friendship [166]. Finally, in a studyof 10-year-old Brit-
ish children who competed to create a drama to win a
monetary award, more female than male groups ganged up
on a lone individual in their same-sex groups [167].
(d) Adolescence: finding a mate, bearing a child
Humans of both sexes experience an extended period of ado-
lescence [14,168]. Girls’ adolescence however is briefer
[15,169], requiring earlier acquisition of adult skills. In 40
developing countries, 50% of girls marry before age 18, and
for 75% first births follow within 2 years [170]. By contrast,
men often marry after age 30. Age at first marriage strongly
influences life histories of both a girl and her children [170],
in part because women are up to three times less likely
than men to re-marry [6,49,171]. Consequently, a woman’s
first marital partner is much more important than a man’s.
Further, unlike boys, girls appear sexually mature well
before they become fecund [15,169], increasing pressure on
girls to marry early [172].
In adolescence, girls’ primary allegiance switches from
natal kin to a husband and first child, with an intimate
female friend bridging the transition. Where a girl resides
matrilocally, the transition is eased [50]. Cross-culturally,
when possible girls and young women remain closer to their
mothers, other female kin and natal kin [32,95,96] and rarely
aggress against older kin or affines [54]. Across 173 cultures,
adolescent girls spent more time than boys in close proximity
to intergenerational familial members, assisting with domestic
chores [32]. In adolescence and adulthood, compared with
males, girls and women still view parents and parental substi-
tutes as more important than an unrelated close friend for
providing both instrumental and emotional assistance [37].
Adolescent girls more than boys ask medical and psychologi-
cal personnel, teachers and other authority figures for
assistance [173]. Even in polygynous marriages, a newlywed
female occasionally brokers an agreement with an older co-
wife, in which the new wife provides medical care to the
older wife in exchange for the older wife’s assistance [53]. A
few trusted female friends who meet the requisite standards
can provide emotional support [41] and serve as allies target-
ing rivals, or reciprocally as a bulwark from rivals’ attacks.
Nonetheless, in most societies, a husband becomes a girl’s
primary ally, providing physical resources, alliances and
community-wide status for a woman and her children.
Even though husbands dominate and even abuse women
[51], a woman’s reproductive success may be most influenced
by her husband’s connections and status. As researchers who
study the Tsimane conclude, ‘A good mate can mean the
difference between a lifetime of support vs. a lifetime of fend-
ing for one’s self and one’s children’ ([174], p. 203).
Accordingly, Ache women devoted 55% of their total time
foraging exclusively to helping their spouse [38].
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
5
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
As earlier, adolescent girls avoid competing against boys.
Experimentally, when early adolescent Hopi Indians and
African–Americans competed, girls lost to boys [175]. Like-
wise, older adolescent European – American girls let boys
dominate during dyadic [176] and group activities [177].
Instead, girls compete against other girls for a valuable
mate. Although two girls can support one another emotion-
ally, as well as form protection from and coalitions against
a rival, co-rumination about relationships increases
depression and anxiety markedly in adolescence [178].
Further, girls betray one another to form bonds with boys.
Girls report greater anger, hurt and jealousy than boys
towards close same-sex friends [179], especially if they were
abandoned for a romantic partner [174,180]. Across 137
societies, 91% of women’s competitive acts were targeted
towards women, most frequently over men’s attention,
emotional investment, goods, services and sexuality
[53,54,174], but also over broken social obligations, meat
theft and friendships in which one young woman allied
with another one against her former friend.
(i) Avoid interference competition
Adolescent girls across diverse cultures still rarely directly
interfere with another female’s pursuit of her goals [32,116],
even eschewing zero-sum sports games during exercise or
individual athletic activities [181]. Although serious physical
interference competition peaks for both sexes between 15 and
25 years, females use it 1/10 as often as males [106,116,182].
Young women’s use of violence across cultures typically
occurs when her own or her children’s survival is jeopardized
[54], in the absence of kin [50], when mates and resources are
in short supply [183– 185], when co-wives share a husband
[53] and where marriages are not arranged [52].
(ii) Disguise competition
More commonly, competition is disguised so thoroughly that
the competitors themselves are not even conscious they are
competing [126]. Girls compete frequently through attempts
to control other girls’ romantic relationships [186]. Across
cultures, girls’ manipulation of another girl’s alliances with
same-sex and other-sex peers increases in frequency and
intensity in adolescence compared with earlier and relative
to boys [116,187– 189]. Girls specifically attempt to end
romantic partnerships by maligning another girl [163],
often through denigration of the other girl’s sexual reputation
to a potential spouse [190,191]. Through denigration of
female peers, a girl reduces the number of serious competi-
tors for a mate. Records of conversations of pairs of older
adolescent girls and boys showed that a higher proportion
of girls’ conversations contained negative statements about
a same-sex peer [192]. Further, female conversational part-
ners were more likely than male partners to respond
positively to an initial denigration. Reputation denigration
of a female competitor provides a powerful tool to increase
a girl’s own chances of finding a valuable mate.
(iii) Compete overtly only if high ranked in the community
Across diverse cultures, attributes considered markers of
females’ reproductive success, including appearance and
energy level, as well as access to physical resources (high-
socioeconomic status), determine a girl’s attractiveness to
potential mates and define high status in the community
[24,163,193]. Studies of Canadian and American adolescents
showed that both high-status girls and boys used overt and
disguised forms of aggression more often [142,188,194] and
in the Netherlands were more likely to be bullies, although
girls bullied less than boys [195]. Similarly, high-status British
adolescent girls occasionally physically harmed and often
overtly manipulated the social relations of lower status girls
in order to control access to high-status boys [145].
(iv) Enforce equality among girls
High-status adolescent girls elicit more respect from other girls
than in earlier years, because the community of girls is more
spatially connected to the male community than before. Lower
status females who form alliances with high-status females
theoretically can increase their access to valuable mates. Adoles-
cent girls consequently compete more than boys to form bonds
with high-status same-sex peers [163,196]. A lower status girl
will surrender all her other friendships and accomplishments
in an attempt to form a bond with a high-status girl. Practically,
this rarely produces pay-offs, as a high-status girl profits little
from allying with anyone but another high-status girl [163,196].
This produces even stronger denigration of high-status
adolescent girls than earlier [196]. Within the girls’ peer
group, high-status girls who interfere with another girl’s
goals invite derision and social exclusion [162,163,193,196].
Niceness and equality remain the female community’s
norms. Girls’ primary justification for being mean is that
another girl is acting superior. Even in the rare girl gangs
that exist, the leader must behave as an equal [184].
In the Netherlands, in an implicit association computerized
task, girls approached the names of same-sex high-status early
adolescents more slowly than low status adolescents, whereas
the reverse was true for boys [195,197]. Likewise, high status
and liking were unrelated or negatively related for early and
middle adolescent American girls, but positively related for
boys [198,199]. Further, high status at ages 15–16 years pre-
dicted lack of liking 2 years later for girls, but liking for boys.
Likewise, Canadian girls reported feeling greater jealousy
than boys if a same-sex friend were more successful [179]. Reci-
procally, American [126] and Canadian [200] girls were more
likely than boys to believe that if they were more successful
than a close same-sex friend at making a new same-sex
friend, excelling in academics or athletics or forming a romantic
relationship, they would be less liked by their same-sex friend.
The sex difference was strongest for finding a romanticpartner.
(v) Use social exclusion
Girls with high status in the community can more easily socially
excludelower status girls, because they attract more female and
male allies. Ethnographic studies in American, English and
Australian schools demonstrate the common use of both overt
and disguised social exclusion by high-status adolescent girls
as they attempt to ostracize one another, lower status girls
and newcomers [145,201–203]. If lower status girls can form a
temporary large group however, they can exclude a lone
high-status girl [162]. This is even more true if they can elicit
the support of the larger community, meaning boys [201,202].
Experimentally, when pairs of same-sex American young
adolescent friends were introduced systematically to a same-
sex newcomer, compared with boys, girls rated the newcomer
less positively, took longer to speak to the newcomer and incor-
porated fewer of the newcomer’s ideas [204]. Likewise, older
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
6
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
adolescent French Canadian girls were more likely than boys to
use social exclusion ina computerized game [42], and Canadian
girls reported being socially excluded more frequently than
boys [65].
5. Conclusion
In summary, from early childhood through old age, human
females’ reproductive success depends on provisioning, pro-
tecting and nurturing first younger siblings, then their own
children and grandchildren. To safeguard their health over
a lifetime, girls use competitive strategies that reduce the
probability of physical retaliation, including avoiding direct
interference with another girl’s goals and disguising their
striving for physical resources, alliances and status. When a
girl has high market value in the community, she is afforded
greater protection and can compete more openly without fear
of retaliation. Within the female community, girls reduce
competition by demanding equality and punishing those
who openly attempt to attain more than others. Social exclu-
sion by several girls of lone female victims provides a safe
strategy for increasing physical resources, allies and status
opportunities by decreasing the number of competitors.
Girls reach adulthood earlier than boys, frequently marry-
ing and bearing their first child before age 20. Because they will
re-marry less often than boys, produce children faster and bear
primary responsibility for young children, girls have less
opportunity than boys to change the course of their reproduc-
tive careers. By late adolescence, a girl’s success in finding a
valuable spouse can influence her entire reproductive career.
Forming alliances with kin, a few trusted female friends, then
a spouse and affines, while reducing the power and number
of female competitors, enhance the probability that a
woman’s children and grandchildren will prosper.
References
1. Darwin C. 1871 The descent of man and selection in
relation to sex. London, UK: John Murray.
2. Trivers R. 1972 Parental investment and sexual
selection. Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company.
3. Clutton-Brock T. 2009 Sexual selection in females.
Anim. Behav. 77, 3–11. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.
2008.08.026)
4. Clutton-Brock T. 2007 Sexual selection in males and
females. Science 318, 1882– 1885. (doi:10.1126/
science.1133311)
5. Stockley P, Bro-Jørgensen J. 2010 Female
competition and its evolutionary consequences in
mammals. Biol. Rev. 86, 341– 366. (doi:10.1111/
j.1469-185X.2010.00149.x)
6. Lahdenpera
¨M, Lummaa V, Russell A. 2011 Selection
on male longevity in a monogamous human
population: late-life survival brings no additional
grandchildren. J. Evol. Biol. 24, 1053–1063.
(doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02237.x)
7. Lancaster JB, Lancaster CS. 1983 Parental
investment: the hominid adaptation. In How
humans adapt: a biocultural odyssey (ed. D Ortner),
pp. 33– 66. New York, NY: Smithsonian.
8. Ellison PT. 2001 Reproductive ecology and human
evolution. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
9. Campbell A. 1999 Staying alive: evolution, culture,
and women’s intrasexual aggression. Behav. Brain Sci.
22, 203 – 252. (doi:10.1017/S0140525X99001818)
10. Campbell A. 2002 A mind of her own: the
evolutionary psychology of women. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
11. Hrdy SB. 2009 Mothers and others: the evolutionary
origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA:
Belknap Press.
12. Lahdenpera M et al. 2004 Fitness benefits of
prolonged post-reproductive lifespan in women.
Nature 428, 178– 181. (doi:10.1038/nature02367)
13. Sear R, Mace R. 2008 Who keeps children alive? A
review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evol.
Hum. Behav. 29, 1–18. (doi:10.1016/j.
evolhumbehav.2007.10.001)
14. Bogin BA. 1997 Evolutionary hypotheses for human
childhood. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 28, 109– 153.
15. Bogin BA, Smith BH. 1996 Evolution of the human
life cycle. Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 40, 63– 89.
16. Konner M. 2005 Hunter– gatherer infancy and
childhood: the !Kung and others. In Hunter –
gatherer childhoods (eds BS Hewlett, ME Lamb),
pp. 19– 64. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction.
17. Konner M. 2010 The evolution of childhood:
relationships, emotion, mind. Cambridge, MA:
Belknap Press.
18. Marlowe FW. 2005 Who tends Hadza children. In
Hunter– gatherer childhoods: evolutionary,
developmental, and cultural perspectives (eds
BS Hewlett, ME Lamb), pp. 177–190. New
Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction.
19. Alvarez H. 2004 Residence groups among hunter–
gatherers: a view of the claims and evidence for
patrilocal bands. In Kinship and behavior in Primates
(eds B Chapais, C Berman), pp. 400 – 442. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.
20. Rodseth L et al. 1991 The human community as a
primate society. Curr. Anthropol. 32, 221– 254.
(doi:10.1086/203952)
21. Kramer KL. 2005 Children’s help and the pace of
reproduction: cooperative breeding in humans. Evol.
Anthropol. Issues News Rev. 14, 224– 237.
(doi:10.1002/evan.20082)
22. Hill KR et al. 2011 Co-residence patterns in hunter–
gatherer societies show unique human social structure.
Science 331, 1286. (doi:10.1126/science.1199071)
23. Gupta MD. 1995 Life course perspectives on
women’s autonomy and health outcomes. Am.
Anthropol. 97, 481– 491. (doi:10.1525/aa.1995.97.
3.02a00070)
24. Buss DM. 1989 Sex differences in human mate
preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37
cultures. Behav. Brain Sci. 12, 1– 49. (doi:10.1017/
S0140525X00023992)
25. Courtiol A et al. 2012 Natural and sexual selection
in a monogamous historical human population.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 8044– 8049.
(doi:10.1073/pnas.1118174109)
26. Dickemann M. 1979 The ecology of mating systems
in hypergynous dowry societies. Soc. Sci. Inf. 18,
163–195. (doi:10.1177/053901847901800201)
27. Hartung J et al. 1982 Polygyny and inheritance of
wealth [and comments and replies]. Curr.
Anthropol., 1– 12. (doi:10.1086/202775)
28. Hewlett BS. 1992 Father–child relations: cultural
and biosocial contexts. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine
Transaction.
29. Barbu S, Cabanes G, Le Maner-Idrissi G. 2011 Boys
and girls on the playground: sex differences in
social development are not stable across early
childhood. PLoS ONE 6, e16407. (doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0016407)
30. Lever J. 1978 Sex differences in the complexity of
children’s play and games. Am. Sociol. Rev. 43,
471–483. (doi:10.2307/2094773)
31. Benenson JF, Apostoleris NH, Parnass J. 1997 Age
and sex differences in dyadic and group interaction.
Dev. Psychol. 33, 538–543. (doi:10.1037/0012-
1649.33.3.538)
32. Schlegel A, Barry III H. 1991 Adolescence: an
anthropological inquiry. New York, NY: Free Press.
33. Savin-Williams RC. 1980 Social interactions of
adolescent females in natural groups. In Friendship
and social relations in children (eds HC Foot,
AJ Chapman, JR Smith), pp. 343 – 364. New
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
34. Bird R. 1999 Cooperation and conflict: the
behavioral ecology of the sexual division of labor.
Evol. Anthropol. Issues News Rev. 8, 65– 75. (doi:10.
1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1999)8:2,65::AID-EV
AN5.
3.0.CO;2-3)
35. Hill K, Kaplan H, Hawkes K. 1933 On why male
foragers hunt and share food. Curr. Anthropol. 34,
701–710. (doi:10.1086/204213)
36. Caldwell MA, Peplau LA. 1982 Sex differences in
same-sex friendship. Sex Roles 8, 721– 732.
(doi:10.1007/BF00287568)
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
7
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
37. Benenson JF et al. 2008 Sex differences in the value of
parents versus same-sex peers. Evol. Psychol. 6,13–28.
38. Hill K. 2002 Altruistic cooperation during foraging
by the Ache, and the evolved human predisposition
to cooperate. Hum. Nat. 13, 105– 128.
(doi:10.1007/s12110-002-1016-3)
39. Tiger L. 2005 Men in groups. Piscataway, NJ:
Transaction Publishers.
40. Bowles S. 2009 Did warfare among ancestral
hunter– gatherers affect the evolution of human
social behaviors? Science 324, 1293– 1298. (doi:10.
1126/science.1168112)
41. Buhrmester D, Prager K. 1995 Patterns and
functions of self-disclosure during childhood and
adolescence. In Disclosure processes in
children and adolescents (ed. K Rotenberg),
pp. 10– 56. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
42. Benenson JF et al. 2011 Under threat of social
exclusion, females exclude more than males.
Psychol. Sci. 22, 538–544. (doi:10.1177/
0956797611402511)
43. Crittenden AN, Marlowe FW. 2008 Allomaternal care
among the Hadza of Tanzania. Hum. Nat. 19,
249–262. (doi:10.1007/s12110-008-9043-3)
44. Sapolsky RM. 2005 The influence of social hierarchy
on primate health. Science 308, 648– 652. (doi:10.
1126/science.1106477)
45. Marmot MG et al. 1991 Health inequalities among
British civil servants: the Whitehall II study.
Lancet 337, 1387–1393. (doi:10.1016/0140-
6736(91)93068-K)
46. Marmot MG, Wilkinson RG. 2006 Social
determinants of health. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
47. Sapolsky RM. 2004 Social status and health in
humans and other animals. Annu. Rev. Anthropol.
33, 393–418. (doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.33.
070203.144000)
48. Barclay P. 2013 Strategies for cooperation in
biological markets, especially for humans.
Evol. Hum. Behav. 34, 164– 175. (doi:10.1016/
j.evolhumbehav.2013.02.002)
49. Marlowe F. 2010 The Hadza: hunter– gatherers of
Tanzania: origins of human behavior and culture.
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
50. Smuts B. 1995 The evolutionary origins of
patriarchy. Hum. Nat. 6, 1– 32. (doi:10.1007/
BF02734133)
51. Watts C, Zimmerman C. 2002 Violence against
women: global scope and magnitude. Lancet
359, 1232– 1237. (doi:10.1016/S0140-
6736(02)08221-1)
52. Geary D, Winegard B, Winegard B. In press.
Reflections on the evolution of human sex
differences: social selection and the evolution of
competition among women. In Evolutionary
perspectives on human sexual psychology and
behavior (eds VA Weekes-Shackelford, TK Shackelford).
New York, NY: Springer.
53. Jankowiak W, Sudakov M, Wilreker BC. 2005 Co-wife
conflict and co-operation. Ethnology 44,81– 98.
(doi:10.2307/3773961)
54. Burbank VK. 1987 Female aggression in cross-
cultural perspective. Behav. Sci. Res. 21, 70– 100.
55. Markovits H, Benenson J, Dolenszky E. 2001
Evidence that children and adolescents have
internal models of peer interactions that are
gender differentiated. Child Dev. 72, 879– 886.
(doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00321)
56. Benenson JF. 1990 Gender differences in social
networks. J. Early Adolesc. 10, 472 – 495.
(doi:10.1177/0272431690104004)
57. Vigil JM. 2007 Asymmetries in the friendship
preferences and social styles of men and women.
Hum. Nat. 18, 143– 161. (doi:10.1007/s12110-007-
9003-3)
58. Winstead B, Griffin J. 2001 Friendship styles. In
Encyclopedia of women and gender (ed. J Worell),
pp. 481– 492. Boston, MA: Academic Press.
59. Essock-Vitale SM, McGuire MT. 1985 Women’s lives
viewed from an evolutionary perspective. II.
Patterns of helping. Ethol. Sociobiol. 6, 155– 173.
(doi:10.1016/0162-3095(85)90028-7)
60. Maltz DN, Borker RA, Gumperz JA. 1982 A cultural
approach to male-female mis-communication. In
Language and social identity (eds L Monaghan,
JE Goodman, JM Robinson), pp. 195–216.
Marblehead, MA: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
61. MacEvoy JP, Asher SR. 2012 When friends disappoint:
boys’ and girls’ responses to transgressions of
friendship expectations. Child Dev. 83, 104 – 119.
(doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01685.x)
62. Benenson JF et al. 2009 Males’ greater tolerance of
same-sex peers. Psychol. Sci. 20, 184– 190. (doi:10.
1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02269.x)
63. Whitesell NR, Harter S. 1996 The interpersonal
context of emotion: anger with close friends and
classmates. Child Dev. 67, 1345– 1359.
(doi:10.2307/1131704)
64. Martin MC, Gentry JW. 1997 Stuck in the model
trap: the effects of beautiful models in ads on
female pre-adolescents and adolescents. J. Advert.
26, 19– 33. (doi:10.1080/00913367.1997.10673520)
65. Benenson JF et al. 2013 Social exclusion: more
important to human females than males. PLoS ONE
8, e55851. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055851)
66. Benenson JF. 2009 Dominating versus eliminating
the competition: sex differences in human
intrasexual aggression. Behav. Brain Sci. 32,
268– 269. (doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999046X)
67. Wrangham RW. 1999 Evolution of coalitionary
killing. Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 29, 1– 30. (doi:10.
1002/(SICI)1096-8644(1999)110:29+,1::AID-
AJPA2.3.0.CO;2-E)
68. Nagy E et al. 2001 Different emergence of fear
expressions in infant boys and girls. Infant Behav. Dev.
24, 189– 194. (doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(01) 00070-4)
69. Tanner J, Thomson A. 1970 Standards for
birthweight at gestation periods from 32 to 42
weeks, allowing for maternal height and weight.
Arch. Dis. Child. 45, 566–569. (doi:10.1136/adc.45.
242.566)
70. Tanner JM, Whitehouse R, Takaishi M. 1966
Standards from birth to maturity for height, weight,
height velocity, and weight velocity: British children,
1965. I. Arch. Dis. Child. 41, 454–471. (doi:10.
1136/adc.41.219.454)
71. Gupta MD et al. 2003 Why is son preference so
persistent in East and South Asia? A cross-country
study of China, India and the Republic of Korea.
J. Dev. Stud. 40, 153– 187. (doi:10.1080/
00220380412331293807)
72. Altekar AS. 1956/2009 The position of women in
Hindu civilization, from prehistoric times to the
present day. Delhi, India: Motilal Banarsidass.
73. Hrdy SB. 1999 Mother nature: a history of mothers,
infants, and natural selection. Politics Life Sci. 20,
107–252.
74. Sen A. 1990 More than 100 million women are
missing. NY Rev. Books 37, 123– 149.
75. Kristof N, Wudunn S. 2010 Half the sky: how to
change the world. London, UK: Virago.
76. Haviland JJ, Malatesta CZ. 1981 The development of
sex differences in nonverbal signals: fallacies, facts,
and fantasies. In Gender and nonverbal behavior
(eds C Mayo, NM Henley), pp. 183– 208. New York,
NY: Springer.
77. Kohnstamm GA. 1989 Temperament in childhood:
cross-cultural and sex differences. New York, NY:
John Wiley & Sons.
78. Prior M et al. 1993 Sex differences in psychological
adjustment from infancy to 8 years. J. Am. Acad.
Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 32, 291– 305.
(doi:10.1097/00004583-199303000-00009)
79. Teichner G, Ames E, Kerig P. 1997 The relationship
of infant crying and the sex of the infant to parents’
perceptions of the infant and themselves. Psychol.
Savannah 34, 59– 60.
80. Eaton WO, Enns LR. 1986 Sex differences in human
motor activity level. Psychol. Bull. 100, 19–28.
(doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.19)
81. Else-Quest NM et al. 2006 Gender differences in
temperament: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132,
33–72. (doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33)
82. Philips S, King S, DuBois L. 1978 Spontaneous
activities of female versus male newborns. Child
Dev. 49, 590–597. (doi:10.2307/1128225)
83. Schmid G et al. 2010 A prospective study on the
persistence of infant crying, sleeping and feeding
problems and preschool behaviour. Acta Paediatrica
99, 286– 290.
84. Connellan J et al. 2000 Sex differences in
human neonatal social perception. Infant Behav.
Dev. 23, 113–118. (doi:10.1016/S0163-
6383(00)00032-1)
85. McClure E. 2000 A meta-analytic review of sex
differences in facial expression processing and their
development in infants, children, and adolescents.
Psychol. Bull. 126, 424– 453. (doi:10.1037/0033-
2909.126.3.424)
86. Freedman DG. 1974 Human infancy: an
evolutionary perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: John Wiley &
Sons Inc.
87. Hittelman JH, Dickes R. 1979 Sex differences in
neonatal eye contact time. Merrill Palmer Q. J. Dev.
Psychol. 25, 171– 184.
88. Fagot BI. 1994 Peer relations and the development
of competence in boys and girls. New Dir. Child
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
8
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
Adolesc. Dev. 1994, 53– 65. (doi:10.1002/cd.
23219946506)
89. Goldberg S, Lewis M. 1969 Play behavior in the
year-old infant: early sex differences. Child Dev. 40,
21–31. (doi:10.2307/1127152)
90. Kochanska G, Aksan N. 1995 Mother – child
mutually positive affect, the quality of child
compliance to requests and prohibitions, and
maternal control as correlates of early
internalization. Child Dev. 66, 236–254.
(doi:10.2307/1131203)
91. Kochanska G et al. 2005 Pathways to conscience:
early mother–child mutually responsive
orientation and children’s moral emotion,
conduct, and cognition. J. Child Psychol.
Psychiatry 46, 19– 34. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.
2004.00348.x)
92. Power TG et al. 1994 Compliance and self-assertion:
young children’s responses to mothers versus
fathers. Dev. Psychol. 30, 980– 989. (doi:10.1037/
0012-1649.30.6.980)
93. Sear R et al. 2002 The effects of kin on child
mortality in rural Gambia. Demography 39, 43– 63.
(doi:10.1353/dem.2002.0010)
94. Troll LE. 1987 Gender differences in cross-generation
networks. Sex Roles 17, 751– 766. (doi:10.1007/
BF00287686)
95. Whiting BB et al. 1988 Children of different worlds:
the formation of social behavior. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
96. Youniss J, Smollar J. 1985 Adolescent relations with
mothers, fathers, and friends. Chicago, IL: University
of Chicago Press.
97. Berman PW. 1980 Are women more responsive than
men to the young? A review of developmental and
situational variables. Psychol. Bull. 88, 668– 695.
(doi:10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.668)
98. Whiting B, Whiting J. 1975 Children of six cultures:
a psycho-cultural analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
99. Benenson JF, Koulnazarian M. 2008 Sex differences
in help-seeking appear in early childhood.
Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 26, 163–169. (doi:10.1348/
026151007X231048)
100. Buss KA, Brooker RJ, Leuty M. 2008 Girls most of
the time, boys some of the time: gender differences
in toddlers’ use of maternal proximity and comfort
seeking. Infancy 13, 1– 29. (doi:10.1080/
15250000701779360)
101. Thompson RB. 1999 Gender differences in
preschoolers’ help-eliciting communication. J. Genet.
Psychol. 160, 357– 368. (doi:10.1080/002213
29909595405)
102. Leaper C, Anderson KJ, Sanders P. 1998 Moderators
of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: a
meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 34, 3– 27. (doi:10.
1037/0012-1649.34.1.3)
103. Benenson JF, Morash D, Petrakos H. 1998 Gender
differences in emotional closeness between
preschool children and their mothers. Sex Roles 38,
975–985. (doi:10.1023/A:1018874509497)
104. Powlishta KK, Maccoby EE. 1990 Resource utilization
in mixed-sex dyads: the influence of adult presence
and task type. Sex Roles 23, 223– 240. (doi:10.
1007/BF00290045)
105. Fabes RA, Martin CL, Hanish LD. 2003 Young
children’s play qualities in same, other, and mixed
sex peer groups. Child Dev. 74, 921– 932.
(doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00576)
106. Daly M, Wilson M. 1999 An evolutionary
psychological perspective on homicide. In Homicide:
a sourcebook of social research (eds MD Smith,
MA Zahn), pp. 58– 71. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications, Inc.
107. Mehta CM, Strough JN. 2009 Sex segregation in
friendships and normative contexts across the life
span. Dev. Rev. 29, 201–220. (doi:10.1016/j.dr.
2009.06.001)
108. Hinde RA et al. 1985 Incidence of ‘friendship’ and
behavior toward strong associates versus
nonassociates in preschoolers. Child Dev. 56,
234– 245. (doi:10.2307/1130190)
109. La Freniere P, Strayer FF, Gauthier R. 1984 The
emergence of same-sex affiliative preferences
among preschool peers: a developmental/
ethological perspective. Child Dev. 55, 1958–1965.
(doi:10.2307/1129942)
110. Jacklin CN, Maccoby EE. 1978 Social behavior at
thirty-three months in same-sex and mixed-sex
dyads. Child Dev. 49, 557–569. (doi:10.2307/
1128222)
111. Kyratzis A. 2001 Children’s gender indexing in
language: from the separate worlds hypothesis to
considerations of culture, context, and power.
Res. Lang. Soc. Interact. 34, 1– 13. (doi:10.1207/
S15327973RLSI3401_1)
112. Thorne B. 1993 Gender play: girls and boys in
school. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
113. Thorne B, Luria A. 1986 Sexuality and gender in
children’s daily worlds. Soc. Probl. 33, 176– 190.
(doi:10.2307/800703)
114. Charlesworth WR, La Freniere P. 1983 Dominance,
friendship, and resource utilization in preschool
children’s groups. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4, 175– 186.
(doi:10.1016/0162-3095(83)90028-6)
115. Lott B. 1978 Behavioral concordance with sex role
ideology related to play areas, creativity, and parental
sex typing of children. J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.36,
1087– 1100. (doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.10.1087)
116. Archer J. 2004 Sex differences in aggression in
real-world settings: a meta-analytic review.
Rev. Gen. Psychol. 8, 291– 322. (doi:10.1037/1089-
2680.8.4.291)
117. Archer J, Coyne SM. 2005 An integrated review of
indirect, relational, and social aggression. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. Rev. 9, 212– 230. (doi:10.1207/
s15327957pspr0903_2)
118. Maccoby EE, Jacklin CN. 1974 The psychology of
sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
119. Alink LR et al.2006 The early childhood aggression
curve: development of physical aggression in 10-to
50-month-old children. Child Dev. 77, 954–966.
(doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00912.x)
120. Baillargeon RH et al. 2007 Gender differences in
physical aggression: a prospective population-based
survey of children before and after 2 years of age.
Dev. Psychol. 43, 13– 26. (doi:10.1037/0012-1649.
43.1.13)
121. Hay DF et al. 2011 The emergence of gender
differences in physical aggression in the context of
conflict between young peers. Br. J. Dev. Psychol.
29, 158– 175. (doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.
02028.x)
122. Maccoby EE. 1990 Gender and relationships: a
developmental account. Am. Psychol. 45, 513.
(doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.4.513)
123. Sachs J. 1987 Preschool boys’ and girls’ language
use in pretend play. In Language, gender, and sex in
comparative perspective (eds S Philips, S Steele,
C Tanz), pp. 178–189. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
124. Kyratzis A, Guo J. 2001 Preschool girls’ and boys’
verbal conflict strategies in the United States and
China. Res. Lang. Soc. Interact. 34, 45– 74.
(doi:10.1207/S15327973RLSI3401_3)
125. Kyratzis A, Marx T. 2001 Preschoolers’
communicative competence: register shift in the
marking of power in different contexts of friendship
group talk. First Lang. 21, 387– 429. (doi:10.1177/
014272370102106308)
126. Tracy L. 1991 The secret between us: competition
among women. New York, NY: Little, Brown and Co.
127. Sheldon A. 1996 You can be the baby brother, but
you aren’t born yet: preschool girls’s negotiation for
power and access in pretend play. Res. Lang. Soc.
Interact. 29, 57– 80. (doi:10.1207/s15327973
rlsi2901_4)
128. Kyratzis A, Ross TS, Koymen SB. 2010 Validating
justifications in preschool girls’ and boys’ friendship
group talk: implications for linguistic and socio-
cognitive development. J. Child Lang. 37, 115– 144.
(doi:10.1017/S0305000908009069)
129. Sheldon A. 1993 Pickle fights: gendered talk in
preschool disputes. Gend. Conversational Interact.
13, 83– 109.
130. Strayer F, Strayer J. 1976 An ethological analysis of
social agonism and dominance relations among
preschool children. Child Dev. 47, 980– 989.
(doi:10.2307/1128434)
131. Strayer F, Noel J. 1986 The prosocial and antisocial
functions of preschool aggression: an ethological
study of triadic conflict among young children.
In Altruism and aggression: biological and social
origins (eds C Zahn-Waxler, EM Cummings,
RJ Iannotti), pp. 107– 131. New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
132. Charlesworth WR, Dzur C. 1987 Gender comparisons
of preschoolers’ behavior and resource utilization in
group problem solving. Child Dev. 58, 191– 200.
(doi:10.2307/1130301)
133. Kyratzis A, Tarım S¸D. 2010 Using directives to
construct egalitarian or hierarchical social
organization: Turkish middle-class preschool girls’
socialization about gender, affect, and context in
peer group conversations. First Lang. 30, 473– 492.
(doi:10.1177/0142723710370547)
134. Kyratzis A. 2007 Using the social organizational
affordances of pretend play in American
preschool girls’ interactions. Res. Lang. Soc.
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
9
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
Interact. 40, 321– 352. (doi:10.1080/
08351810701471310)
135. Sebanc AM et al. 2003 Gendered social worlds in
preschool: dominance, peer acceptance and
assertive social skills in boys’ and girls’ peer
groups. Soc. Dev. 12, 91– 106. (doi:10.1111/1467-
9507.00223)
136. Gottman JM. 1986 The world of coordinated play:
same- and cross-sex friendship in young children. In
Conversations of friends: speculations on affective
development (eds JM Gottman, JG Parker),
pp. 139– 191. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
137. Crick NR, Casas JF, Mosher M. 1997 Relational
and overt aggression in preschool. Dev.
Psychol. 33, 579– 588. (doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.
4.579)
138. Benenson JF et al. 2008 Sex differences in children’s
formation of exclusionary alliances under scarce
resource conditions. Anim. Behav. 76, 497– 505.
(doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.027)
139. Bove RB, Valeggia CR, Ellison PT. 2002 Girl helpers
and time allocation of nursing women among the
Toba of Argentina. Hum. Nat. 13, 457– 472.
(doi:10.1007/s12110-002-1003-8)
140. Maccoby EE. 1998 The two sexes: growing up apart,
coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/
Harvard University Press.
141. Adler PA, Kless SJ, Adler P. 1992 Socialization to
gender roles: popularity among elementary school
boys and girls. Sociol. Educ. 65, 169– 187.
(doi:10.2307/2112807)
142. Rodkin PC, Berger C. 2008 Who bullies whom?
Social status asymmetries by victim gender.
Int. J. Behav. Dev. 32, 473–485. (doi:10.1177/
0165025408093667)
143. Weisfeld CC. 1986 Female behavior in mixed-sex
competition: a review of the literature. Dev. Rev. 6,
278–299. (doi:10.1016/0273-2297(86)90015-8)
144. Parker JG, Seal J. 1996 Forming, losing, renewing,
and replacing friendships: applying temporal
parameters to the assessment of children’s
friendship experiences. Child Dev. 67, 2248–2268.
(doi:10.2307/1131621)
145. Duncan N. 2004 It’s important to be nice, but it’s
nicer to be important: girls, popularity and sexual
competition. Sex Educ. 4, 137–152. (doi:10.1080/
14681810410001678329)
146. Benenson JF, Christakos A. 2003 The greater fragility
of females’ versus males’ closest same-sex
friendships. Child Dev. 74, 1123–1129.
(doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00596)
147. Kon IS, Losenkov VA. 1978 Friendship in
adolescence: values and behavior. J. Marriage
Family 40, 143–155. (doi:10.2307/350615)
148. Benenson JF, Alavi K. 2004 Sex differences in
children’s investment in same-sex peers. Evol. Hum.
Behav. 25, 258– 266. (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.
2004.05.002)
149. Rose AJ, Carlson W, Waller EM. 2007 Prospective
associations of co-rumination with friendship and
emotional adjustment: considering the
socioemotional trade-offs of co-rumination.
Dev. Psychol. 43, 1019–1031. (doi:10.1037/
0012-1649.43.4.1019)
150. Benenson JF, Morganstein T, Roy R. 1998 Sex
differences in children’s investment in peers. Hum.
Nat. 9, 369–390. (doi:10.1007/s12110-998-1015-0)
151. Bryant BK. 1985 The neighborhood walk: sources of
support in middle childhood. Monogr. Soc. Res.
Child Dev. 50, 122.
152. Benenson JF, Heath A. 2006 Boys withdraw more in
one-on-one interactions, whereas girls withdraw
more in groups. Dev. Psychol. 42, 272– 282.
(doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.272)
153. Benenson JF, Sinclair N, Dolenszky E. 2006
Children’s and adolescents’ expectations of
aggressive responses to provocation: females predict
more hostile reactions in compatible dyadic
relationships. Soc. Dev. 15, 65– 81. (doi:10.1111/
j.1467-9507.2006.00330.x)
154. Ahlgren A, Johnson DW. 1979 Sex differences in
cooperative and competitive attitudes from the 2nd
through the 12th grades. Dev. Psychol. 15, 45– 49.
(doi:10.1037/h0078076)
155. Roy R, Benenson JF. 2002 Sex and contextual effects
on children’s use of interference competition.
Dev. Psychol. 38, 306– 312. (doi:10.1037/0012-
1649.38.2.306)
156. Benenson JF et al. 2002 Greater discomfort as a
proximate cause of sex differences in competition.
Merrill Palmer Q. J. Dev. Psychol. 48, 225– 247.
(doi:10.1353/mpq.2002.0010)
157. Leaper C, Smith TE. 2004 A meta-analytic review of
gender variations in children’s language use:
talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive
speech. Dev. Psychol. 40, 993– 1027. (doi:10.1037/
0012-1649.40.6.993)
158. Underwood MK et al. 2004 An observational study
of social exclusion under varied conditions: gender
and developmental differences. Child Dev. 75,
1538–1555. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.
00756.x)
159. Lagerspetz KMJ, Bjo
¨rkqvist K, Peltonen T. 1988 Is
indirect aggression typical of females? Gender
differences in aggressiveness in 11 to 12 year old
children. Aggress. Behav. 14, 403– 414. (doi:10.
1002/1098-2337(1988)14:6,403::AID-AB2480
140602.3.0.CO;2-D)
160. Coyne SM, Archer J, Elsea M. 2006 ‘We’re not
friends anymore! Unless...’: the frequency and
harmfulness of indirect, relational, and social
aggression. Aggress. Behav. 32, 294– 307.
(doi:10.1002/ab.20126)
161. Olweus D. 1993 Victimization by peers: antecedents
and long-term outcomes. In Social withdrawl,
inhibition, and shyness in childhood (eds KH Rubin,
JB Asendorpf), pp. 315 – 341. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
162. Goodwin MH. 1990 He-said-she-said: talk as social
organization among black children, vol. 618.
Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
163. Merten DE. 1997 The meaning of meanness:
popularity, competition, and conflict among junior
high school girls. Sociol. Educ. 70, 175– 191.
(doi:10.2307/2673207)
164. Eder D, Hallinan MT. 1978 Sex differences in
children’s friendships. Am. Sociol. Rev. 43,
237–250. (doi:10.2307/2094701)
165. Feshbach ND. 1969 Sex differences in children’s
modes of aggressive responses toward outsiders.
Merrill Palmer Q. J. Dev. Psychol. 15, 249– 258.
166. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. 1995 Relational aggression,
gender, and social– psychological adjustment. Child
Dev. 66, 710–722. (doi:10.2307/1131945)
167. Benenson JF et al. 2008 Human sexual differences
in the use of social ostracism as a competitive tactic.
Int. J. Primatol. 29, 1019–1035. (doi:10.1007/
s10764-008-9283-4)
168. Bogin B. 1999 Evolutionary perspective on human
growth. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 28, 109– 153.
(doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.109)
169. Hochberg ZE. 2011 Evo-devo of child growth: treatise
on child growth and human evolution. New York,
NY: Wiley-Blackwell.
170. Mensch BS, Singh S, Casterline JB. 2005 Trends in
the timing of first marriage among men and women
in the developing world. New York, NY: Population
Council, Policy Research Division.
171. Hetherington EM, Kelly J. 2003 For better or for
worse: divorce reconsidered. New York, NY: WW
Norton & Company.
172. Winking J et al. 2007 Why do men marry and why
do they stray? Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 1643– 1649.
(doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0437)
173. Seiffge-Krenke I, Shulman S. 1990 Coping style
in adolescence: a cross-cultural study. J. Cross
Cultur. Psychol. 21, 351–377. (doi:10.1177/
0022022190213006)
174. Rucas SL et al. 2012 Social aggression and resource
conflict across the female life-course in the Bolivian
Amazon. Aggress. Behav. 38, 194– 207. (doi:10.
1002/ab.21420)
175. Cronin Weisfeld C, Weisfeld GE, Callaghan JW. 1982
Female inhibition in mixed-sex competition among
young adolescents. Ethol. Sociobiol. 3, 29– 42.
(doi:10.1016/0162-3095(82)90028-0)
176. Morgan SW, Mausner B. 1973 Behavioral and
fantasied indicators of avoidance of success in men
and women. J. Pers. 41, 457–470. (doi:10.1111/j.
1467-6494.1973.tb00106.x)
177. Megargee E. 1969 Influence of sex roles on the
manifestation of leadership. J. Appl. Psychol. 53,
377–382. (doi:10.1037/h0028093)
178. Schwartz-Mette RA, Rose AJ. 2012 Co-rumination
mediates contagion of internalizing symptoms
within youths’ friendships. Dev. Psychol. 48,
1355–1365. (doi:10.1037/a0027484)
179. Benenson JF, Benarroch D. 1998 Gender differences
in responses to friends’ hypothetical greater success.
J. Early Adolesc. 18, 92– 208. (doi:10.1177/
0272431698018002004)
180. Parker JG et al. 2005 Friendship jealousy in young
adolescents: individual differences and links to sex,
self-esteem, aggression, and social adjustment.
Dev. Psychol. 41, 235–250. (doi:10.1037/0012-
1649.41.1.235)
181. Deaner RO et al. 2012 A sex difference in the
predisposition for physical competition: males play
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
10
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
sports much more than females even in the
contemporary US. PLoS ONE 7, e49168. (doi:10.
1371/journal.pone.0049168)
182. Moffitt TE et al. 2001 Sex differences in antisocial
behaviour: conduct disorder, delinquency, and
violence in the Dunedin longitudinal study.
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
183. Campbell A. 1995 A few good men: evolutionary
psychology and female adolescent aggression. Ethol.
Sociobiol. 16, 99– 123. (doi:10.1016/0162-
3095(94)00072-F)
184. Campbell A. 1992 The girls in the gang. Cambridge,
MA: Blackwell Publishers, Inc.
185. Campbell A. 2004 Female competition: causes,
constraints, content, and contexts. J. Sex Res. 41,
16–26. (doi:10.1080/00224490409552210)
186. Underwood MK. 2003 Social aggression among girls.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
187. Smith RL, Rose AJ, Schwartz-Mette RA. 2010
Relational and overt aggression in childhood and
adolescence: clarifying mean-level gender
differences and associations with peer acceptance.
Soc. Dev. 19, 243–269. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.
2009.00541.x)
188. Closson LM. 2009 Status and gender differences in
early adolescents’ descriptions of popularity. Soc. Dev.
18, 412– 426. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00459.x)
189. Bjo
¨rkqvist K. 1994 Sex differences in physical, verbal,
and indirect aggression: a review of recent research.
Sex Roles 30, 177– 188. (doi:10.1007/BF01420988)
190. Eder D. 1993 Go get ya a French! 55: romantic
and sexual teasing among adolescent girls.
In Gender and conversational interaction
(ed. D Tannen), pp. 17–31. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
191. Lees S. 1993 Sugar and spice: sexuality and
adolescent girls. New York, NY: Penguin Group.
192. Leaper C, Holliday H. 1995 Gossip in same gender
and cross gender friends’ conversations. Pers. Relat.
2, 237– 246. (doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1995.
tb00089.x)
193. Eder D, Kinney DA. 1995 The effect of middle school
extra curricular activities on adolescents’ popularity
and peer status. Youth Soc. 26, 298– 324.
(doi:10.1177/0044118X95026003002)
194. Rose AJ, Swenson LP, Waller EM. 2004 Overt and
relational aggression and perceived popularity:
developmental differences in concurrent and
prospective relations. Dev. Psychol. 40, 378– 387.
(doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.378)
195. de Bruyn EH, Cillessen AH, Wissink IB. 2010
Associations of peer acceptance and perceived
popularity with bullying and victimization in early
adolescence. J. Early Adolesc. 30, 543– 566.
(doi:10.1177/0272431609340517)
196. Eder D. 1985 The cycle of popularity: interpersonal
relations among female adolescents. Sociol. Educ.
58, 154– 165. (doi:10.2307/2112416)
197. Lansu TA, Cillessen AH, Karremans JC. 2012 Implicit
associations with popularity in early adolescence: an
approach– avoidance analysis. Dev. Psychol. 48,
65–75. (doi:10.1037/a0025681)
198. Cillessen AH, Mayeux L. 2004 From censure to
reinforcement: developmental changes in the
association between aggression and social status.
Child Dev. 75, 147– 163. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.
2004.00660.x)
199. Mayeux L, Sandstrom MJ, Cillessen AH. 2008 Is being
popular a risky proposition? J. Res. Adolesc. 18,
49–74. (doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2008.00550.x)
200. Benenson JF, Schinazi J. 2004 Sex differences in
reactions to outperforming same-sex friends.
Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 22, 317– 333. (doi:10.1348/
0261510041552729)
201. Simmons R. 2003 Odd girl out: the hidden
culture of aggression in girls. New York, NY: Mariner
Books.
202. Wiseman R. 2002 Queen bees and wannabes:
helping your daughter survive cliques, gossip,
boyfriends, and other realities of adolescence.
New York, NY: Three Rivers Press.
203. Owens L, Shute R, Slee P. 2000 ‘Guess what I just
heard!’: indirect aggression among teenage girls in
Australia. Aggress. Behav. 26, 67– 83. (doi:10.1002/
(SICI)1098-2337(2000)26:1,67::AID-AB6.3.0.
CO;2-C)
204. Feshbach N, Sones G. 1971 Sex differences
in adolescent reactions toward newcomers.
Dev. Psychol. 4, 381– 386. (doi:10.1037/
h0030986)
rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org Phil Trans R Soc B 368: 20130079
11
on October 30, 2015http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/Downloaded from
A preview of this full-text is provided by The Royal Society.
Content available from Philosophical Transactions B
This content is subject to copyright.