ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Music and video are utilised by recreational gym users to enhance their exercise experience. Music and video have not been investigated for their combined ergogenic effect during high intensity exercise. To induce fatigue, this study was performed in warm (~26°C), moist conditions (~50%RH). Six, non-acclimated, male participants took part in the study. Each participant completed three 30-minute exercise bouts on a motorised treadmill under three counterbalanced conditions on separate days: control (CON), motivational music plus video intervention (M), non-motivational intervention (NM). They completed a warm-up (5 km·h-1 [5 minutes], 9km·h-1 [10 minutes]) followed by a maximal effort run (15 minutes). Participants did not receive any feedback of time elapsed, distance run or speed. Measures: Distance covered (metres), heart rate, blood lactate accumulation (Blac) and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE). Participants in the M condition ran significantly further than in the NM (M: 3524 [388]metres; NM: 3110 [561]metres; CON: 3273 [458]metres) and CON conditions, accumulated more Blac, but did not increase their peak RPE rating (p < 0.05). The M intervention improved tolerance of high intensity exercise in warm conditions. It was proposed that a change in attentional processing from internal (physical sensations) to external perspective (music and video) may have facilitated this improvement. These findings have strong implications for improving health, fitness and engagement in gym-based exercise programs
©Journal of Sports Science and Medicine (2009) 8, 435-442
http://www.jssm.org
Received: 13 February 2009 / Accepted: 19 June 2009 / Published (online): 01 September 2009
A motivational music and video intervention improves high-intensity exercise
performance
Martin J. Barwood , Neil J.V. Weston, Richard Thelwell and Jennifer Page
Department of Sport and Exercise Science, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK
Abstract
Music and video are utilised by recreational gym users to en-
hance their exercise experience. Music and video have not been
investigated for their combined ergogenic effect during high
intensity exercise. To induce fatigue, this study was performed
in warm (~26°C), moist conditions (~50%RH). Six, non-
acclimated, male participants took part in the study. Each par-
ticipant completed three 30-minute exercise bouts on a motor-
ised treadmill under three counterbalanced conditions on sepa-
rate days: control (CON), motivational music plus video inter-
vention (M), non-motivational intervention (NM). They com-
pleted a warm-up (5 km·h-1 [5 minutes], 9km·h-1 [10 minutes])
followed by a maximal effort run (15 minutes). Participants did
not receive any feedback of time elapsed, distance run or speed.
Measures: Distance covered (metres), heart rate, blood lactate
accumulation (Blac) and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE).
Participants in the M condition ran significantly further than in
the NM (M: 3524 [388]metres; NM: 3110 [561]metres; CON:
3273 [458]metres) and CON conditions, accumulated more Blac,
but did not increase their peak RPE rating (p < 0.05). The M
intervention improved tolerance of high intensity exercise in
warm conditions. It was proposed that a change in attentional
processing from internal (physical sensations) to external per-
spective (music and video) may have facilitated this improve-
ment. These findings have strong implications for improving
health, fitness and engagement in gym-based exercise programs.
Key words: Running, distraction, attention, lactate threshold.
Introduction
Music and video have separately been used to enhance
sports performance as part of preparatory routines (Szabo
et al., 1999; Templin and Vernacchia, 1995), skill acquisi-
tion (Onestak, 1997) or during the intervals between
games, halves or sequences (Bishop et al., 2007; Hall and
Erffmeyer, 1983). Considering the applied significance of
separate music and video interventions prior to and during
sports performance it is surprising that no studies have
examined the ergogenic effect of motivational music and
video interventions in combination. The applied efficacy
of combining music and video to influence sports per-
formance is appealing because, with the advancement of
communication technologies, both of these interventions
can easily be implemented at sporting venues and are
already utilised by recreational gym users to supplement
their exercise routines (Karageorghis et al., 2006a). In the
latter context it is likely that this type of intervention
could influence adherence and work intensity in an exer-
cise program culminating in health benefits as has already
been suggested with music alone (Karageorghis and
Terry, 1997).
The theoretical underpinning for the use of music
in this setting is more established than that of video (see
Bishop et al., 2007, Karageorghis et al., 2006a, Priest et
al., 2004). A greater number of studies have examined the
ergogenic effect of music, particularly prior to or during
high intensity exercise, during which, the synchronising
of physiological and motor responses during exercise with
music may be one means by which music is influential
(Brownley et al., 1995; Karageorghis, 2000; Simpson and
Karageorghis, 2006). The ergogenic effect of music on
high intensity exercise also extends to elite sports persons.
Elite runner, Heile Gebreselassie, used a high-tempo
popular music song to synchronise his strides in order to
optimise his pacing in winning a 5000m race in 2003
(Simpson and Karageorghis, 2006). Asynchronous music,
defined as not consisting of any conscious effort to syn-
chronize movements with music tempo, is also suggested
to carry this performance enhancing effect (Karageorghis,
2000).
The Brunel Music Rating Inventory-2 (BMRI-2;
Karageorghis et al, 2006a) and recently updated version
BMRI-3 (Karageorghis, 2008) have allowed researchers
to specifically identify music tracks that induce a motiva-
tional impact upon an athlete during sports performance.
Motivational tracks are thought to include a high tempo
beat (>120bpm), a strong rhythm, and to enhance energy
and induce bodily action (Karageorghis et al., 2006b).
Further influential characteristics of music have been
suggested to include the rhythm response, musicality,
cultural impact and association with the tracks (Karageor-
ghis et al., 1999). Therefore music tracks that encompass
all of these aspects would optimise moderate intensity
exercise performance. Bishop et al (2007) have recently
extended the study of music and performance providing a
rationale for the manipulation of emotional responses to
music in junior tennis players. Here it was suggested that
the right music can be used as a tool to ‘psych up’ in
preparation for performance (arousal regulation), shift
attentional focus (association/dissociation), boost self-
efficacy and encourage psychological skills usage (e.g.
mental imagery). Presumably during high intensity exer-
cise participants would aim to be psyched up, may wish to
dissociate from unpleasant exertion induced sensations
(untrained participants only; Brownley et al., 1995), feel
confident and employ appropriate psychological coping
strategies. Clearly the use of appropriately selected music
could induce an ergogenic effect.
Research article
Music, video and running performance
436
Theoretically, the supplementation of appropri-
ately chosen music with video footage has the potential to
enhance the beneficial effect of such interventions prior to
and during sports performance. Video footage of sports-
persons demonstrating mastery could manipulate the self-
efficacy of an individual as part of a combined music and
video intervention. This suggestion has already been
supported with the use of personal motivational videos
(PMVs) in competitive tennis players following a rela-
tively short intervention period (2 weeks; Bishop and
Forzoni, 2006). Self-efficacy is an individual’s belief
about their capabilities to produce designated levels of
performance that exercise influence over events that af-
fect their lives (Bandura, 1977). In relation to Bandura’s
(1977; 1997) self-efficacy theory, video footage of com-
petent sports performance may improve the performance
accomplishment (via self-reflection on previous success-
ful performances) and vicarious experience components
that are thought to comprise this construct thereby facili-
tating emotional control. It has already been suggested
that the combination of music with video can stimulate
positive images and help recreate feelings of confidence
and memories of previous performances (Forzoni, 2006).
The additive effect of music with images is further sup-
ported by evidence of stimulation of areas of the brain
with music and images that are thought to complete emo-
tional processing (Baumgartner et al., 2006). This evi-
dence appears to suggest that music and video combined
has the potential to stimulate self-efficacy and emotional
engagement and may therefore enhance motivation during
exercise.
Watching a video also has the potential to shift at-
tentional focus from internal stimuli to external cues.
Music researchers hypothesise this effect with asynchro-
nous music whereby attention is shifted from salient
physiological cues to the external components of music
particularly the rhythm and musicality components of a
given track (Karageorghis and Terry, 1997). Rejeski’s
(1985) parallel processing model explains this facilitative
change in attentional processing. The attentional field of
an individual is thought to narrow increasingly with ele-
vations in work intensity. During low and moderate exer-
cise external musical cues can prevail thereby reducing
RPE (Borg, 1982). However, this is only hypothesised to
the case at low/moderate intensities below the anaerobic
threshold above which salient physiological cues are
thought to dominate (Boutcher and Trenske, 1990). It is
possible that increasing the strength of external cues
through combining music and video may be sufficient to
influence RPE above the anaerobic threshold. If sup-
ported, this suggestion could have implications for trained
runners who are suggested to favour internal associative
perspectives whilst performing exercise (Morgan, 1977).
Evidence has been presented for the potential er-
gogenic effect of music and video on sports performance
which, if supported could have a more powerful influence
on attentional focus than video or no intervention alone.
This could have implications for those who exercise
whilst watching videos and listening to music. Recent
evidence has been presented demonstrating the efficacy of
other interventions, namely psychological skills training
(PST), in dealing with significant exertion induced physi-
cal sensations in hot conditions (Barwood, Thelwell and
Tipton, 2008). The aerobically trained participants in this
study showed an 8% (1.15km) improvement in distance
covered during a 90 minute time-trial run in hot (30°C),
moist conditions (50% relative humidity; RH) by sup-
pressing their temptation to reduce their work intensity
using a range of psychological skills. The authors sug-
gested that aspects of the PST (mental imagery, positive
self-talk) may have facilitated performance by distracting
the participant from unpleasant sensations elicited by
hyperthermia). Theoretically both music and video inter-
ventions may function in part in a similar way, that is by
distracting the individual from unpleasant sensations
elicited during high intensity exercise. This has yet to be
assessed under environmentally demanding temperature
conditions in which tolerance to unpleasant sensations
from exercise and heat will force performance to deterio-
rate at a faster rate (Tucker et al., 2004). This will provide
an improved backdrop for distinguishing the effects of
music and video interventions on performance.
Given the recent research and theoretical devel-
opments the aim of this study was to examine the benefi-
cial effect that a motivational music and video interven-
tion could have on high intensity exercise performance
under moderate environmental stress. The experiment
hypothesis was that a motivational music and video inter-
vention would significantly increase distance covered
during a time-trial running task and help participants
tolerate warm (~26°C), moist ambient conditions
(~50%RH). If the content of the video portion of the in-
tervention proved important, participants in a non-
motivational video condition would complete the shortest
distance.
Methods
Participants
The study protocol was approved in advance by the insti-
tutional ethics committee and the participants gave their
written informed consent to participate. Following medi-
cal checks, six healthy male, non-acclimated volunteers
acted as participants for the experiment (mean [s.d]; Age
20 [1] yrs; height 1.81 [0.04] m; mass 77 [10] kg). Par-
ticipants were non-smokers recruited from university
rugby and football teams, completed a minimum of four
hours of physical activity a week and were familiar with
treadmill running. Each participant refrained from strenu-
ous exercise, alcohol and caffeine consumption 24 hours
prior to participating in the study. The experimental pro-
cedures were conducted in accordance with the Declara-
tion of Helsinki, as adopted at the 52nd World Medical
Association general assembly, 2000.
Experimental design
Each participant performed three 30-minute (15 minute
warm-up, 15 minute self paced) maximal effort runs on a
treadmill in a climatic chamber held at a constant tem-
perature of ~26°C and ~50% relative humidity (RH).
During each run the participant underwent one of three
experiment manipulations comprising either a control
(CON) condition, motivational (M) or non-motivational
(NM) music and video intervention. To minimise any
Barwood et al.
437
order effects, the order of interventions were delivered in
a balanced crossover design. Prior to each run the partici-
pants were instructed to exert a maximal effort. Trials
were conducted at the same time of day on separate days
for each participant to minimise any circadian effects,
with a minimum of three days between each laboratory
visit. Following the completion of the study each partici-
pant completed a Social Validation Questionnaire (SVQ)
to measure his responses to each treatment intervention
delivered.
Procedure
The participants were required to visit the laboratory on 3
occasions. On arrival each participant changed into run-
ning clothes including shorts, socks, a running vest and
trainers; the same clothing was worn in each trial. Each
participant then attached a heart rate monitor (Team Po-
lar, UK) recording data every 6 seconds, to measure work
intensity during each run. Resting blood lactate (Blac)
concentration was measured (Biosen, C-line Sport, Ger-
many) via a finger prick blood sample taken by a quali-
fied phlebotomist. The difference between pre and post
exercise Blac (Blac) was used as an index of aerobic or
anaerobic metabolism during the maximal effort run. To
avoid the potential distraction by measurement equipment
(Karageorghis and Terry, 1997), no further measurement
devices were attached.
After instrumentation each participant entered a
climatic chamber The environmental conditions were
recorded each minute during the 30 minute trials by a
WBGT weather station (Grant Instruments, Cambridge,
U.K). Environmental conditions in the CON trial were
26.50 [1.20]°C and 45 [9]%, in the NM trial 26.60
[0.30]°C and 49 [4]% and the M trial 25.90 [0.70]°C and
45 [7]% RH and were not different from each other
(P>0.05). Such conditions are mild at rest but are suffi-
cient to provide a moderate thermal stimulus during high
intensity exercise producing a moderate risk of heat ill-
ness (Armstrong et al., 1996). The participant mounted a
treadmill (Powerjog GX200, Powerjog, U.K) and com-
pleted a standardised warm-up over a 15 minute period.
The warm-up comprised 5 minutes of exercise at 5 km·h-1
and a further 10 minutes at 9km·h-1 following which the
participant began his 15-minute maximal effort run. Dur-
ing this time the participant controlled his speed and did
not receive any feedback of time elapsed, distance cov-
ered or current running speed. Throughout the warm-up
and maximal effort run the treadmill incline was set to 1%
to reflect the metabolic energy demands of outdoor run-
ning (Jones and Doust, 1996). RPE (Borg, 1982) was
recorded at a minimum of 2-minute intervals; the fre-
quency of RPE measurement was limited to minimise any
temporal cues to the participant. Every effort was made
by the experimental team to eliminate temporal cues from
the immediate environment around the participant to
avoid anticipatory changes in work intensity toward the
end of the run. No verbal encouragement was given at any
stage. Following the cessation of the run the participant
slowed the treadmill to walking speed for 3 minutes and
exited the climatic chamber. A post exercise sample of
blood was drawn from the fingertip after 4 minutes for
blood lactate analysis. Participants were allowed to con-
sume normal tap water (19°C) ad libitum during each
trial. The distance covered in each trial was noted and
used as a performance indicator. In two of the three trials
the participants received the M video and music interven-
tion or NM video intervention projected (Sony SVGA,
VPL-DS100, Japan) onto the wall of the climatic chamber
~2 metres in front of the participant and through two
audio speakers (Goodmans 40W speakers, U.K) mounted
adjacent and above (1.80m), and below (0.40m) and be-
hind the treadmill respectively. The M and NM interven-
tions were controlled by a laptop computer (Toshiba Te-
cra M5, Japan) and were reproduced in each trial at the
same intensity (Goodmans MS 355, U.K) as measured by
a handheld sound level meter (Lutron SL-4001, Lutron
Electronic Enterpise Co, Taiwan); volume was 75db adja-
cent to the ear of the participant; >75db is classified as
loud music according to Karageorghis and Terry, 1997).
Development of Materials
Motivational Intervention
The music was selected by the experimenters on the basis
of tempo (e.g. Pendulum, ‘Slam’ 139 bpm), potential
affective content (e.g. Survivor, ‘Eye of the Tiger’
109bpm) or the inclusion of inspirational lyrics (e.g.
Lock, Stock and Barrel ‘Rise above it’). To avoid previ-
ous research criticisms for the arbitrary selection of music
in experiments using music interventions (Karageorghis
and Terry, 1997), participants rated the music portion of
the M intervention using the Brunel Music Rating Inven-
tory-3 (Karageorghis, 2008); this was undertaken follow-
ing completion of the study. Within the BMRI-3, the
motivational properties of each piece of music are rated
on a 7-point scale whereby the sum of the ratings indicate
the extent of the motivational properties (highly motiva-
tional rating 36-42; moderately motivating 24-35; <24
low/oudeterous). The warm-up section of the M interven-
tion was rated as 27 [4] (0-15 minutes) and the self-paced
exercise section of the M intervention was rated 36 [3]
(15-30 minutes). It has previously been noted that the
term ‘motivational music’ may oversimplify the effects
that music can have on exercise performance because of
the lack of distinction between the affective components
that music may include (Karageorghis et al., 2006a). Con-
sidering the exercise type (high intensity) high tempo
music (> 120 to 140bpm) was selected for the study with
tracks consistent with Gaston (1951) and Karageorghis et
al.’s (1999) suggestions of motivational music.
Controls were also implemented to account for po-
tential differences in the influence of the music portion of
the intervention between different age groups (participant
ages: 20 [1]), gender (male participants) and socio-
cultural background (Caucasians raised in the UK)
(Karageorghis et al, 2006a; Priest et al., 2004). Lastly,
although the music was of high tempo in nature it was not
designed to be synchronised with strides whilst partici-
pants were running. Asynchronous music has previously
been identified as background music similar to that pre-
sent in most gymnasia (Karageorghis et al., 2006b).
In order to stimulate the vicarious experience
component of self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997) the video and
picture portions of the M intervention included pictures
Music, video and running performance
438
and videos of influential World and British sporting feats
that the participants were likely to have seen in the media
previously. (e.g. Kelly Holmes’ 2004 Olympic 1500m
and 800m gold-medal winning races; Sir Steve
Redgrave’s coxless fours 2000 Olympic gold medal win).
The videos included moving pictures of further inspira-
tional sporting moments including England’s 2003 Rugby
Union World cup winning drop goal (Jonny Wilkinson)
and Michael Owen’s 2001 FIFA World Cup qualifying
hat-trick versus Germany.
Non-Motivational Intervention
The NM intervention consisted of 30 minutes of public
speaking from a recent political trial in the USA. The
footage included verbal communication of evidence from
the prosecution, defence interspersed with passages of
speech from a judge. At no point did this footage include
motivational slogans or any music or synchronous activ-
ity. This intervention did not include any of the compo-
nents that have previously been noted to have an er-
gogenic effect during exercise, therefore the term non-
motivational was used. The NM footage was selected on
this basis.
Control condition
Participants in the control condition completed their
warm-up and maximal effort run undergoing the same
physiological and psychological measurements but did
not watch or listen to any video or music whilst running.
Social validation questionnaire (SVQ)
To improve external validity, following completion of the
study each participant was informed of the distance they
covered in each trial and completed an SVQ asking for a
rating on a 7 point Likert scale (1 – not at all impor-
tant/significant/satisfied/useful to 7 extremely impor-
tant/significant/satisfied/useful) whether they rated an
improvement in performance as important to them,
whether they rated this improvement as significant, how
satisfied they were with the intervention programme and
whether the intervention proved useful. Consistent with
the recommendations of Tenenbaum et al. (2004), the
participants were invited to provide any subjective com-
ments on the perceived influences of the interventions
received.
Data analysis
The following variables were analysed: Distance covered
in each trial (m) and Blac, from each maximal effort run.
Data sets were analysed for differences between trial
using a Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
with repeated measures; significant effects were detected
using a post-hoc pairwise comparisons test with Bon-
ferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons. Statistical
findings are reported with their observed power (β) where
the chance of type II error is equal to 1- β. Where appro-
priate data sets were adjusted using a Greenhouse-Geisser
adjustment. For all statistical tests α level was set at 0.05.
Results
Performance indicators
Data are presented as mean [SD] where appropriate. Dis-
tances covered for each participant are presented in Figure
1. MANOVA indicated a significant difference between
the CON, NM and M conditions (F = 3.68, p =0.023, β =
0.77). The univariate statistic (F(2,10) = 11.29, p = 0.003, β
= 0.96) indicated that participants ran significantly fur-
ther in one trial with the post-hoc analysis showing that
participants covered a greater distance in the M condition
than both the NM (p = 0.049) and the CON (p = 0.019)
condition. There were no differences between the CON
and NM condition in distance covered (p > 0.05). On
average the participants ran 415 m (13%) and 251 m (8%)
further in the M than NM and CON respectively.
Figure 1. Individual and mean distance covered (m) during each 15 minute maximal effort run in participants 1-6.
Barwood et al.
439
Table 1. Blac accumulation, heart rate and RPEpeak in the CON, NM and M conditions.
Blac (mmol·L-1) Mean HR (bt·min-1) RPEpeak
Participant CON NM M‡ CON NM M CON NM M
1 11.34 7.67 8.66 181 182 170 19 18 19
2 1.50 4.36 8.62 197 192 190 19 19 19
3 3.85 1.55 6.91 168 160 177 16 12 19
4 7.96 5.43 6.81 188 182 192 20 19 19
5 3.01 1.81 5.28 180 170 185 17 19 18
6 3.70 -.09 8.42 171 161 183 17 19 16
Mean 5.23 3.46 7.45 181 174 183 18 18 18
† denotes M> CON and NM condition, ‡ denotes M>NM only.
Indicators of exercise intensity
Indicators of exercise intensity are presented in Table 1.
Univariate analysis indicated differences in Blac accumu-
lation between trials (F(2,10) = 5.33, p = 0.026, β = 0.70).
Post-hoc analysis indicated Blac accumulated to a signifi-
cantly greater extent (p = 0.05) in the M compared to the
NM condition. There were no further differences between
the other conditions (p > 0.05). On average, participants
produced an additional 4 mmol·L-1 of Blac in the M condi-
tion. Mean heart rate during maximal effort running is
displayed in Figure 2. Four of the six participants dis-
played elevated heart rates in the M compared to the NM
condition. Heart rate during treadmill running had a ten-
dency to be higher throughout the maximal effort run in
the M condition (Figure 2). RPE peak was similar during
each of the experimental manipulations and averaged 18
in each condition.
Social validation data, rated on a 7-point Likert
scale, indicated that the participants rated the improve-
ments in performance during the M intervention as impor-
tant (6 [0]), rated their improvement in performance as
significant (6 [1]), were satisfied with the intervention (6
[1]) and found the intervention useful (6 [2]).
Discussion
This study examined the effect a motivational music and
video intervention on high intensity exercise performance
in warm conditions. It was hypothesised that, since the
combination of music and video included more external
stimuli to induce a dissociative attentional strategy and
increase tolerance, that these interventions combined
would increase distance covered during a 15 minute
treadmill maximal effort run trial. The first hypothesis
was supported. The video content clearly played a role in
determining participants’ effort as they covered the least
distance in the NM conditions in which stimulating cues
were minimised. We also suggested that the magnitude
and number of external distractions caused by the combi-
nation of music with video would lead to a lowered RPE
rating despite running further. This is partially supported
by the unchanged RPE, having run 13% further, in the
motivational music and video intervention. In the NM
condition RPE was also not different raising the possibil-
ity of a change in perception.
Previous studies using only music as an interven-
tion have indicated that music is most efficacious during
low and moderate work intensities or in untrained partici-
pants (Boutcher and Trenske, 1990; Brownley et al.,
1995). The current study demonstrated that music with
video extended the beneficial effects of audiovisual inter-
ventions to include high intensity exercise. Participants
receiving the M intervention worked at a higher heart rate
and accumulated more Blac post-exercise. Boutcher and
Figure 2. Mean heart rate during each 15 minute maximal effort run in participants (n = 6).
Music, video and running performance
440
Trenske (1990) suggested that the physiological cues at
high exercise intensities (~170bpm), similar to those ob-
served in our study (~180 to 190bpm), were too strong for
their participants to ignore thereby restoring RPE to the
levels observed in a control condition. The current study
provides some tentative support for a change in percep-
tion and consequent benefit to exercise performance when
many stimulating distractions were present in the exercise
environment.
Rejeski’s (1985) parallel information processing
theory has consistently been used to interpret the er-
gogenic influence of music on performance at a range of
intensities (e.g. Simpson and Karageorghis, 2006; Carr et
al., 2006; Tenenbaum et al., 2004). This model could also
provide insight into the findings of the current study.
Rejeski (1985) suggests that both physiological and psy-
chological afferent inputs are processed preconsciously
and in parallel. During high intensity exercise, the band-
width for attentional processing narrows and only the
most salient cues are processed which are often, because
of their overwhelming strength, physiological in nature
(e.g. high heart rate, respiratory rate and raised Blac). The
inclusion of video may have provided some competition
for preconscious processing. In untrained runners this
could have a beneficial impact on exercise intensity and
adherence to exercise programmes potentially constituting
a health benefit. However, this could ultimately be dam-
aging for trained participants who favour an internal at-
tentional perspective, particularly during high intensity
exercise, because such cues carry important details of
pacing strategy for training or competition (Morgan,
1977). Music and video may force trained participants to
miss important pacing cues . The cohort of participants in
the current study were physically active but were not
trained runners per se. Further studies using trained run-
ners would elucidate the relationship between training
status and music and video interventions.
Conversely, the NM intervention may have in-
duced fewer stimulating distractions culminating in the
poorest maximal effort performance but interestingly it
did not differ in RPE rating. The subjective comments
made following the NM trial suggest that the exercise
intensity was perceived as harder because of the boredom
experienced. This emphasises the specificity of the con-
tent of the motivational music and video intervention in
the current study. Where possible the appropriate research
tools were used to create the music in the M intervention
(BMRI-2; Karageorghis et al, 2006a; BMRI-3; Karageor-
ghis, 2008) and an equivalent tool to develop the video
interventions may have proved useful. Some tentative
suggestions have been made in developing this personal
motivational videos (Forzoni, 2006), although these sug-
gestions have not been assessed empirically.
Indeed, the combination of music with video may
have functioned in other ways to facilitate high intensity
exercise performance. Rendi et al. (2008) suggested that
high-tempo music may only facilitate arousal during high
intensity exercise performance with no change in atten-
tional processing. Rendi et al. also reported unchanged
RPE ratings during rowing exercise but linked the facilita-
tive effect of music to a greater stroke frequency aug-
mented by elevations in arousal. As already noted, music
in combination with images stimulates many brain struc-
tures involved in emotional processing (Baumgartner et
al., 2006) and it appears feasible that the M intervention
facilitated arousal in the this manner. Some support for
this suggestion can be found in the post-trial comments
made in the current study where some participants felt
that they “wanted to run faster in the warm-up because of
the music and video” in the M condition whereas they
“wanted to slow the speed down because the trial felt
harder” in the NM condition. Interestingly, heart rate
appeared higher at the start of exercise (Figure 2) in the M
condition despite completing the same warm-up as in
other conditions suggesting some impact upon arousal.
Risk-taking images (e.g. climbing, surfing, parachuting)
have also been noted to carry an affective component (at
rest) and may also have contributed to the elevated heart
rate in the M condition (Gomez et al., 2005). Conversely
heart rate was lowest prior to the start of the maximal
effort run in the NM condition (Figure 2).
We can only assume that the video portion of the
M intervention boosted self-efficacy and future studies
should measure this possibility. It was proposed that vi-
carious experience and performance accomplishment may
be augmented by watching video footage including mas-
tery of skill in elite and sub-elite sports persons. This
could be further enhanced by the creation of personal
motivational videos which can be tailored to carry infor-
mation specific to the performer providing behavioural
reinforcement, augmenting psychological skills usage and
optimising the emotive response prior to or during exer-
cise (Forzoni, 2006). This represents an important future
direction for research in this area as a study using PMVs
in this setting will help develop a rationale for the use of
effective tools for sport psychology practitioners.
This study presents a number of interesting further
questions to be answered with subsequent studies. It is
difficult to separate out specifically how the video inter-
vention improved performance and some measure of the
participant reactions to the treatment interventions, over
and above the SVQ used here, would clarify this issue.
Similarly, further treatment conditions including only
music and only video would help to separate the additive
effect that these interventions combined may have but
these have been studied in part elsewhere (e.g. Bishop and
Forzoni, 2006; Brownley et al., 1995). The findings of the
current study provide a strong rationale to test this hy-
pothesis. This study does provide some ecological valid-
ity for the appeal of video and music in combination as an
ergogenic aid to exercise of short duration. Testing these
beneficial effects over longer duration may also prove
interesting as there may be a greater number physiological
inputs that comprise fatigue and compete for attentional
processing over longer exercise of greater duration, par-
ticularly in warm conditions (Tucker et al., 2004; Davis
and Bailey, 1997; Nybo and Nielsen, 2001). In relation to
ecological validity, the conditions of the study may be
similar to those experienced in non-air-conditioned car-
diovascular exercise suites of recreational gyms. Likewise
the intervention itself was highly rated according to the
SVQ in participants similar to those who may comprise a
Barwood et al.
441
population of gym users. The SVQ showed that the par-
ticipants in the current study were highly motivated, rated
their improvements as significant, were satisfied with the
intervention thereby indicating they would use this kind
of intervention in future.
Conclusion
In conclusion, this study showed that a combined music
and video intervention has a beneficial effect on exercise
of high intensity in conditions that may induce premature
fatigue. Video and music may primarily be of use to non-
competitive, recreational gym users who are more likely
to select dissociative attentional strategies to elongate or
tolerate a high intensity exercise workload. Future studies
should assess the efficacy of this intervention in trained
runners. In order to establish suitably motivating video
components to such interventions, research tools, similar
to the BMRI-3 (Karageorghis, 2008) should be developed
to aid practitioners and researchers in subsequent studies.
Acknowledgment
The authors would like to acknowledge the contributions of Mr Matthew
Panton, Mr Daniel Isaac, Mr Daniel Burns and Mr Phil Cable for their
assistance in the data collection process. The authors would also like to
acknowledge the participants for their reliability and forbearance and
our technicians for their help in setting up the laboratory.
References
Armstrong, L.E., Crago, A.E., Adams, R., Roberts, W.O. and Maresh,
C.M. (1996) Whole-body cooling of hyperthermic runners:
Comparison of two field therapies. American Journal of Emer-
gency Medicine 14, 355-358.
Bandura, A. (1977) Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behav-
ioural change. Psychological Review 84, 191-215.
Bandura, A. (1997) Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York:
Freeman.
Barwood, M.J., Thelwell, R.C. and Tipton, M.J. (2008) Psychological
skills training improves exercise performance in the heat.
Medicine and Science in Sport and Exercise 40(2), 387-396.
Baumgartner, T., Lutz, K., Schmidt, C.F. and Jancke, L. (2006) The
emotional power of music: How music enhances the feeling of
affective pictures. Brain Research 1075, 151-164.
Bishop, D. and Forzoni, R. (2006) The use of music video in sport
psychology interventions: empirical and anecdotal considera-
tions. In: British Association of Sport and Exercise Sciences
Annual Conference, September 11-13, University of Wolver-
hampton, England. 4.
Bishop, D.T., Karageorghis, C.I. and Loizou, G. (2007) A grounded
theory of young tennis players’ use of music to manipulate
emotional state. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 29,
584-607.
Borg, G.A.V. (1982) Psychophysical basis of perceived exertion. Medi-
cine and Science in Sports and Exercise 14, 377-381.
Boutcher, S.H. and Trenske, M. (1990). The effects of sensory depriva-
tion and music on perceived exertion and affect during exer-
cise. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 12, 167-176.
Brownley, K.A., McMurray, R.G. and Hackney, A.C. (1995) Effects of
music on physiological and affective responses to graded
treadmill exercise in trained and untrained runners. Interna-
tional Journal of Psychophysiology 19, 193-201.
Carr, S., Elliott, D. and Savage, D. (2006) The influence of rhythm and
personality on the endurance response to motivational asyn-
chronous music. Journal of Sports Sciences 24, 187-195.
Cervone, D., Kopp, D., Schaumann, L. and Scott, W. (1994) Mood, self-
efficacy and performance standards: Lower moods induce
higher standards for performance. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 67, 499-512.
Davis, M. and Bailey, S.P. (1997) Possible mechanisms of central
nervous system fatigue during exercise. Medicine and Science
in Sports and Exercise 29, 45-57.
Easterbrook, J.A. (1959) The effect of emotion on the utilization and
organization of behavior . Psychological Review 66, 183-201.
Forzoni, R. (2006) Personal motivational videos: So where’s the down-
side? The Sport and Exercise Scientist 7, 10-11.
Gaston, E.T. (1951) Musical factors influencing social behaviour. Kan-
sas Welfare Digest 10, 2-4.
Gomez, P., Zimmermann, P., Guttormson-Schär, S.,and Danuser, B.
(2005) Respiratory responses associated with affective process-
ing of film stimuli. Biological Psychology 68, 223-235.
Hall, E.G. and Erffmeyer, E.S. (1983) The effect of visuo-motor behav-
iour rehearsal with videotape modelling on free throw accuracy
of intercollegiate female basketball players. Journal of Sport
Psychology 5, 343-346.
Jones, A.M. and Doust, J.M. (1996) A 1% treadmill incline most accu-
rately reflects the energetic cost of outdoor running. Journal of
Sports Sciences 14, 321-327.
Karageorghis, C.I. (2008) The scientific application of music in sport
and exercise. In: Sport and Exercise Psychology. Ed: Lane, A.
London: Hodder Education. 109-138
Karageorghis, C.I. (2000) The effects of synchronous and asynchronous
music in cycle ergometry. Journal of Sports Sciences 18, 16.
Karageorghis, C.I. and Terry, P.C. (1997) The psychophysical effects of
music in sport and exercise: a review. Journal of Sport Behav-
ior 20, 54-68.
Karageorghis, C.I., Jones, L. and Low, D.C. (2006b) Relationship
between exercise heart rate and music tempo preference. Re-
search Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 77(2), 240-250.
Karageorghis, C.I., Terry, P.C. and Lane, A.M. (1999) Development and
initial validation of an instrument to assess the motivational
qualities of music in exercise and sport - The Brunel Music
Rating Inventory. Journal of Sports Sciences 17, 713-724.
Karageorghis, C.I., Priest, D.L., Terry, P.C., Chatzisarantis, N.L.D. and
Lane, A.M. (2006a). Redesign and initial validation of an in-
strument to assess the motivational qualities of music in exer-
cise: The Brunel Music Rating Inventory-2. Journal of Sports
Sciences 24, 899-909.
Morgan, W.P. (1977) Psychological characterizations of the elite dis-
tance runner. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
301, 383-403.
Nybo, L. and Nielsen, B. (2001) Perceived exertion is associated with an
altered brain activity during exercise with progressive hyper-
thermia. Journal of Applied Physiology 91, 2017-2023.
Onestak, D.M. (1997) The effect of visuo-motor behaviour rehearsal
(VMBR) and videotape modelling (VM) on free throw per-
formance of intercollegiate athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior
20, 185-198.
Priest, D.L., Karagheorghis, C.I. and Sharp, N.C.C. (2004) The charac-
teristics and effects of motivational music in exercise settings:
the possible influence of gender, age frequency of attendance,
and time of attendance. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physi-
cal Fitness 44, 77-86.
Rejeski, W.J. (1985) Perceived exertion: an inactive or passive process?
Journal of Sport Psychology 7, 371-378.
Rendi, M., Szabo, A. and Szabo, T. (2008) Performance enhancement
with music in rowing sprint. The Sport Psychologist 22, 175-
182.
Simpson, S.D. and Karageorghis, C.I. (2006) The effects of synchronous
music on 400-m sprint performance. Journal of Sports Sciences
24, 1095-1102.
Szabo, A., Small, A. and Leigh, M (1999) The effects of slow and fast
rhythm classical music on progressive cycling to voluntary
physical exhaustion. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical
Fitness 39, 220-225.
Templin, D.P. and Vernacchia, R.A. (1995) The effect of highlight
music videotapes upon the game performance of intercollegiate
basketball players. The Sport Psychologist 9, 41-50.
Tenenbaum, G., Lidor, R., Lavyan, N., Morrow, K., Tonnel, S.,
Gershgorgen, A., Meis, J. and Johnson, M (2004) The effect of
music type on running perseverance and coping with effort
sensations. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 5, 89-109.
Tucker, R., Rauch, L., Harley, Y.X.R. and Noakes, T.D. (2004) Antici-
patory regulation of skeletal muscle recruitment and reduced
exercise in the heat. Pflugers Archive 448, 442-430.
Music, video and running performance
442
Key points
The study examined the ergogenic effect of a moti-
vational (M) video and music intervention on high-
intensity exercise performance in comparison to a
non-motivational (NM) condition and a control
(CON).
Participants in the M condition ran significantly further
than in the NM (M: 3524 [388]metres; NM: 3110
[561]metres; CON: 3273 [458]metres) and CON condi-
tions, accumulated more Blac, but did not increase their
peak RPE rating (p < 0.05).
It was proposed that a change in attentional processing
from internal (physical sensations) to external perspective
(music and video) may have facilitated this improvement.
These findings have strong implications for improving
health, fitness and engagement in gym-based exercise
programs.
AUTHORS BIOGRAPHY
Martin BARWOOD
Employment
Senior Lecturer, Department of Sport and
Exercise Science, University of Ports-
mouth
Degree
BSc, PhD
Research interest
Human and applied physiology.
E-mail: martin.barwood@port.ac.uk
Neil WESTON
Employment
Principal lecturer Department of Sport and
Exercise Science, University of Ports-
mouth
Degree
BSc, PhD
Research interest
Sport and exercise psychology.
E-mail: neil.weston@port.ac.uk
Richard THELWELL
Employment
Principal lecturer Department of Sport and
Exercise Science, University of Ports-
mouth
Degree
BSc, PhD
Research interest
Sport and exercise psychology.
E-mail: richard.thelwell@port.ac.uk
Jennifer PAGE
Employment
Senior lecturer Department of Sport and
Exercise Science, University of Ports-
mouth
Degree
BSc, MSc
Research interest
Sport and exercise psychology.
E-mail: jenny.page@port.ac.uk
Martin J. Barwood
Department of Sport and Exercise Science, University of Ports-
mouth, Portsmouth, UK
... On the other hand, in a study conducted by Copeland and Franks [41], investigating the effects of different types of music on HR, RPE, and time to exhaustion during treadmill work in three different treatments, the results showed that peak HR and HR in the minute preceding the maximum were higher in testing with soft, slow, and popular music (approximately 100 bpm) than in conditions without music. In a study examining the effects of using motivational music with a video recording on participants, the results showed a significant increase in HR and accumulated lactate during the motivational intervention compared to non-motivational and control conditions [42] as well as an increase in ergogenic effects when using motivational music compared to neutral and control conditions [40]. ...
... Previous research findings show varying results in subjective exertion. In some studies, RPE remained unchanged in all testing conditions [40,42,43], while in others, it was lower in conditions of exercise with applying motivational music and even in conditions of neutral music [44]. ...
... The obtained results are consistent with the results of previous research. In the aforementioned studies, the results showed that testing lasted longer, and participants covered a greater distance in motivational conditions compared to non-motivational and control conditions [40,42,44]. Additionally, studies that tested walking speed as a measure of endurance for a certain time or distance showed that participants moved significantly faster when exposed to any music condition compared to the control intervention, with musical conditions also being associated with a higher state of motivation, i.e., they swam faster [45] and cycled 10 km faster with music, even while anticipating its playback [43]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The aim of this study was to examine the effect of applying synchronized music and appropriate music selection on motivation for exercise and achieving better results in individuals of different fitness levels. Methods: The study included a total of 20 female participants, who for certain analyses were divided into two groups with slightly different levels of aerobic fitness: students from the Faculty of Sport and Physical Education (n = 10, age 23.0 ± 2.8), and middle-aged adult women exercising recreationally (n = 10, age 38.3 ± 11.6). Cardiopulmonary exercise testing (CPET) was conducted using a treadmill and gas analysis equipment, and motivational music qualities were assessed using the BMRI-2 questionnaire. The procedure included an initial maximal CPET test, echocardiography, and spirometric tests, followed by an interview to select preferred music tracks. A second CPET test was then performed with the chosen motivational music. The Borg Rating of Perceived Exertion (RPE) scale was used in both tests. Results: The internal consistency of the questionnaire was confirmed with a Cronbach’s Alpha of 0.982. The synchronized motivational music significantly improved cardiopulmonary parameters such as peak oxygen consumption (peak VO2), oxygen consumption (VO2) at the second ventilatory threshold (VT2), peak heart rate (peak HR), test duration, and reduced perceived exertion (RPE) at the beginning of the test and at the intensity level corresponding to the VT2. Negligible differences were noted between students and recreational athletes, so it can be assumed that music had an equal impact on these two groups of subjects. Conclusions: The study concluded that synchronous motivational music significantly enhances cardiopulmonary performance and reduces perceived fatigue during physical exertion by serving as a key motivational element and facilitating more economical movement.
... Another resource used before, during, and after exercise is watching videos with or without audio (video clips) (Loizou & Karageorghis, 2015). Because of the separation of concentration induced by both aural and visual stimulation, this technique has demonstrated efficacy in improving performance (even more so than music alone) (Barwood, et al., 2009). Loizou and Karageorghis (2015) also found that videos can increase athletes' motivation before exercise. ...
... The time-to-exhaustion approach was employed to analyze performance in this study, and statistically significant variations were found across the various scenarios. Similar research using audiovisual resources (Barwood, et al., 2009;Lin & Lu, 2013) supports this. Other studies, unlike ours, found a decrease in RPE and an increase in performance when simple music was employed (Waterhouse, et al., 2010;Coelho Silva, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
The aural and visual environment in which people exercise is described as being significant to their affective responses to exercise and possibly their degree of adherence. Research investigating the use of music and video during moderate-intensity exercise is needed because they are inexpensive, simple to adopt, and can reflect activity adherence and reduce dropout rates. This study examined the acute effects on psychophysiological variables and the time for exhaustion on a cycle ergometer during a moderate-intensity workout of listening to music, watching nature videos, or doing both in the standard population. Thirty physically active young male adults (19.46 ± 0.87 years, 62.94 ± 11.86 kg; 169.23 ± 7.21 cm; 21.9 ± 3.14 kg/m2) exercised moderately to exhaustion or until 20 minutes in four different conditions: three experimental conditions (music only, video only, and music and video) and one control condition (no music and visually sterile). The results showed that the mean and standard deviation of changes in time to exhaustion and rate of perceived exertion (RPE) during the second and fourth minutes of exercise differed. Finally, the major purpose of this study was to investigate how people responded to different audiovisual stimuli while exercising on a bike ergometer. The stimuli consisted of music, video, and music-video. These findings are still being debated in the research and may be attributable to the type of exercise, intensity of the exercise, and preference for audiovisual support. As a result, understanding the cus-tomer's and/or athlete's choice for these aspects is critical for achieving better results during training sessions, particularly in the moderate-intensity domain.
... Nidhal Jebabli 1, 2* , Abderraouf Ben Abderrahman 1 INTRODUCTION Il a été bien documenté que la manière dont les athlètes distribuent leurs charges de travail, de façon consciente et/ou inconsciente, pendant un exercice (stratégie d'allure) a pour objectif de limiter la fatigue prématurée et augmenter la probabilité de gagner un évènement sportif (Abiss et al., 2008). Des rares études ont montré que l'écoute de la musique peut influencer sur la stratégie d'allure (Barwood et al., 2009;Lim et al., 2009). Cependant, à notre connaissance, l'effet de la musique préférée et leurs moments sur la variation de la stratégie d'allure reste encore manquantes dans la littérature. ...
... Nos résultats sont en ligne avec des études antérieurs qui ont montré des effets bénéfiques de la musique pendant un échauffement (Macone et al., 2006) ou pendant l'exercice (Barwood et al., 2009) sur les performances aérobies (> 3 min). Cependant, d'autres études n'ont montré aucun effet (Aburto-corona et al., 2021). ...
... Preliminary evidence indicates that music could promote flow during exercise (Pates et al., 2003;Karageorghis et al., 2008;Schuetz and Roetters, 2019). Barwood et al. (2009) and Karageorghis et al. (2012) reported measurable and relatively consistent effects of music on the behavioral and psychological states of exercisers. Laukka and Quick (2013) concluded that regularly listening to music during preparation and training could increase athletes' positive emotions, motivation, performance, and flow. ...
... In this study, the G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) program was used to perform a priori power analysis at a significance level of 0.05 (α = 0.05), a statistical power (1-β) of 0.80, a medium effect size (f = 0.25; Faul et al., 2009), and a high correlation value of 0.65 between each repeated measure (Barwood et al., 2009;Karageorghis et al., 2012). Besides, the repeated measures ANOVA (analysis of variance) -within factors was used, the Nonsphericity correction was set to the default value (ε = 1), then the computed total sample size was 20. ...
Article
Full-text available
There has been much controversy over the effects of music tempo on movement flow. In this study, a single-factor repeated-measurement design was used to explore the effect of music tempo (fast, slow, and no music control) on movement flow by measuring both subjective experiences and objective electroencephalographic (EEG) characteristics during brisk walking. In the experiment, 20 college students walked briskly on a treadmill using EEG equipment. Each participant walked for 10 min on three different days. Their brain waves were recorded during brisk walking on a treadmill. After each walk, the participants completed a form of short flow state scale-2 (S FSS-2), which covered nine major aspects of flow. The results showed that music tempo had a significant effect on subjective experiences and objective physiological characteristics; that is, a higher subjective flow level for fast-tempo music in brisk walking and a significant enhancement of mean power values in the subconscious brain waves of the delta, theta, alpha, and beta bands for fast tempo music were observed. A fast tempo facilitated the movement flow. The findings of this study can be instructive for the use of music in exercises to improve sports training outcomes.
... Understanding how key variables like VO2 max, heart rate and stroke volume are impacted by anaerobic contributions allows coaches and athletes to fine-tune training. Properly balancing aerobic development with targeted anaerobic training can optimize these physiological factors to improve performance over middle distance race distances [17] . ...
... Alguns autores (BISHOP;LOIZOU, 2007;LOW, 2006) reportam que este tipo de intervenção pode influenciar a percepção de esforço e desconforto durante exercícios de alta intensidade, fator que já foi referido como limitador da prática deste tipo de exercícios. Em um estudo com jovens jogadores de tênis, esporte este composto por momentos de esforços repetidos intensos,Bishop;Loizou (2007) sugerem que a música correta pode ser usada como uma ferramenta para melhor preparo psicológico, controlando a excitação, mudando o foco atencional e a autoeficácia.Desta forma, durante um exercício de alta intensidade, os participantes devem estar mais focados mentalmente para talvez se dissociarem das sensações induzidas pelo esforço desagradável e assim se sentirem mais confiantes e motivados(BARWOOD et al., 2009;MARQUES;STAIBANO;FRANCHINI, 2022). Contudo, o controle do foco de atenção pode não ser simples em sujeitos não atletas ou em condição psicológica desfavorável (p.ex., em estado de fadiga mental) ou ainda, pouco motivados a desempenhar a tarefa.Consequentemente, não pairam muitas dúvidas acerca dos efeitos ergogênicos da música no exercício físico (KARAGEORGHIS; PRIEST, 2012a; STORK et al., 2017), contudo no EIAI esse efeitos podem ser menos evidentes, com estudos apresentando resultados conflitantes (BALLMANN et al., 2019; CHTOUROU et al., 2012; HUTCHINSON et al., 2011; KARAGEORGHIS; PRIEST, 2012a; STORK et al., 2015). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
A fadiga mental é um estado psicobiológico induzido por demanda cognitiva caracterizada por esforço mental e falta de energia que tem sido associada a efeitos negativos no desempenho físico durante o exercício contínuo. No entanto, no exercício intermitente de alta intensidade (EIAI), poucos estudos analisaram tal interferência. Além disso, o estímulo de motivação durante o exercício físico (por exemplo, ouvir música) poderia mitigar esses efeitos negativos. Contudo, o conhecimento do efeito da música como agente motivacional no estado de fadiga mental durante o desempenho de EIAI ainda é desconhecido. O objetivo do estudo 1 e do estudo 2 foi investigar os efeitos da fadiga mental e uma intervenção com estímulo musical sobre o desempenho durante a EIAI. No estudo 1, sete adultos jovens fisicamente ativos do sexo masculino (média ± desvio padrão; 30,3 ± 5,8 anos; 76,4 ± 6,8 kg; 175,1 ± 5,2 cm) participaram de 3 condições experimentais: 60 minutos de Stroop teste seguido de EIAI; 60 minutos de teste de Stroop seguido de EIAI ouvindo música; 60 minutos assistindo um documentário emocionalmente neutro seguido do EIAI (8x15s na maior intensidade possível em cicloergômetro contra uma resistência fixa de 9% de massa corporal intercalados por 120s de recuperação passiva entre os esforços. Potência pico e média, índice de fadiga e trabalho total foram aferidos neste estudo. No estudo 2, dez adultos jovens fisicamente ativos do sexo masculino (média ± desvio padrão; 26,4 ± 4,4 anos; 73,0 ± 9,7 kg; 168,0 ± 5,7 cm) participaram das mesmas 3 condições experimentais, contudo o modelo de EIAI compreendia 8x15s de corrida na maior intensidade possível intercalados por 120s de recuperação passiva entre os esforços. Distância percorrida por série e total, velocidade máxima, média e mínima foram aferidas neste estudo. Em ambos os estudos, respostas psicológicas (percepção de fadiga mental, percepção de esforço físico, índice de carga de trabalho) ao EIAI foram coletadas. Em ambos os estudos a fadiga mental não provocou efeitos deletérios nas variáveis de desempenho e nem na percepção subjetiva de esforço (p > 0,05 para todas as comparações). A percepção de fadiga mental aumentou do momento pré para o pós Stroop teste em ambos os estudos (p < 0,001 para ambas as comparações). O índice de carga de trabalho também apresentou diferenças estatísticas do momento pré para o momento pós Stroop teste (p < 0,05 em ambos os estudos para as diferentes comparações). A música não afetou as variáveis de desempenho e psicológicas. A fadiga mental parece não promover efeitos negativos na percepção subjetiva de esforço e no desempenho em protocolos de EIAI baseado em cicloergômetro e baseado em corrida adultos jovens fisicamente ativos. A música não alterou a percepção subjetiva de esforço e o desempenho em protocolos de EIAI tanto em cicloergômetro quando em corrida adultos jovens fisicamente ativos. Por fim, parece ser importante que mais dados sejam coletados com a mesma população e com a música selecionada com critérios mais precisos.
... Researchers have also integrated a range of audio-visual stimuli within the exercise environment, such as music-videos (Pottratz et al., 2021), sporting highlights (Barwood et al., 2009) and film scenes (Bigliassi et al., 2019). This represents a shift in focus towards technologies that fall along the y-axis of the EPI Cube (see Figure 1; Flavián et al., 2019), albeit exercisers' perceptions of presence are rarely measured by investigators (Chow & Etnier, 2017). ...
... Tracey (2011) plantea que los videos podrían mejorar la regulación emocional, datos obtenidos desde una evaluación a un ciclista profesional. Asimismo, Barwood y Weston (2009) concluyeron que los videos podrían mejorar el rendimiento y la percepción de esfuerzo en sujetos que realizaban ejercicios de alta intensidad cuando eran expuestos a videos con imágenes y música previamente seleccionadas. Hutchinson et al. (2015), a su vez, evaluaron la influencia de estímulos auditivos y visuales en variables psicológicas y psicofisiológicas y obtuvieron, de igual manera, que la percepción de activación, motivación y emociones mejoraban a partir del experimento. ...
Article
Full-text available
Las charlas inspiradoras, previas a una competición, son una herramienta que se utiliza a menudo en el ámbito deportivo. El desarrollo tecnológico sumó a estas charlas, refuerzos visuales y auditivos, que provocaron un avance en los ya conocidos discursos de entrenadores o referentes del equipo. En esta investigación se propuso como objetivo determinar la influencia de los videos de inspiración sobre la atención y activación de patinadoras de roller derby. Para ello, se evaluó la atención y la autopercepción de activación en 30 patinadoras, pre y post tratamiento. Para llevar a cabo la condición experimental, se dividió aleatoriamente la muestra en dos partes, y se expuso a un grupo al video de inspiración (grupo experimental) y al otro, a un video de habilidades básicas (grupo control). Los resultados mostraron diferencias significativas, entre el pre y post tratamiento del grupo experimental en dos dimensiones de la variable activación (tono hedónico y activación energética). Se discuten los efectos de los videos inspiracionales y su utilidad para el trabajo de entrenadores, deportistas y profesionales de la psicología del deporte, como herramienta para optimizar el rendimiento deportivo.
... study proved that music conditioned High intensity exercise showed lower RPE as compared to control and non-motivational intervention high intensity exercise. [32] There is no difference in RPE during warm up , rest 1 , rest 2 , rest 3 , cool down , this might be due to use of Borg 6-20 category scale , which showed 6 as the lowest point on scale on exertion . So there might be a scope for future studies to continue the same trial by using the Borg CR -10 scales. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is increase in physical inactivity. Lack of time is been the most common reason for physical inactivity. High intensity interval training (HIIT) is the most time efficient exercise strategy. But HIIT involves high negative affect and high Rate of perceived exertion. Music is thought to affect senses allowing the separation of thought from feeling, thereby altering ones perception of unpleasant feeling. The purpose of this study was to find the effect of music on perceived exertion, enjoyment, mood and affect during high intensity interval training.30 participants were selected as per the inclusion and exclusion criteria. They were divided into 2 groups: group A performed with music and group B performed without music condition HIIT. To control for order effects, trial order was counterbalanced. Mood was assessed pre and post using Abbreviated Profile Of Mood State (POMS), RPE and affect were taken over time using the Borg CR-10 scale and the Feeling scale respectively , enjoyment was assessed post HIIT using the Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale (PACES). From the present study it was concluded that music had positive effect on enjoyment , affect and rate of perceived exertion and has no effect on mood in high intensity interval training exercise when compared to without music conditioned High intensity interval exercise , and thus may increase the exercise adherence to it .
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to find the improvement in the performance of the Kickboxing Individuals by assessing Speed, Power, Agility, Quickness and Reaction time. This study was carried out on 40 Professional Kickboxing individuals age ranging between 20- 30 years. Prior to the study performance testing of the all the individuals of Experimental and Control group were noted. A 4 week Strong by Zumba training program was given at their respective training centers. Post program all the performance tests were done of Experimental and Control group. The results were analyzed using Paired and Unpaired sample‘t’ test. Results obtained showed there was significant improvement in Speed and Power (p< 0.009), Agility and Quickness (p<0.0002) and not significant for Reaction time (p<0.4).
Article
Full-text available
Examined the effects of induced mood on personal standards for performance and judgments of one's performance capabilities, or self-efficacy judgments. Three experiments involving 208 undergraduates were conducted. In Exp 1, standards and self-efficacy judgments were assessed on common social and academic tasks. In Exp 2, these variables were assessed on 2 novel tasks. In both experiments, negative mood induced higher standards for performance. Induced mood had no effect on perceived self-efficacy. Negative mood Ss thus held minimal standards for performance that significantly exceeded the levels of performance they judged they actually could attain. Exp 3 provided support for the hypothesis that negative mood raises standards by lowering evaluations of prospective outcomes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Subjective estimates of physical work intensity are considered of major importance to those concerned with prescription of exercise. This article reviews major theoretical models which might guide research on the antecedents for ratings of perceived exertion (RPE). It is argued that an active rather than passive view of perception is warranted in future research, and a parallel-processing model is emphasized as providing the needed structure for such reconceptualization. Moreover, existing exercise research is reviewed as support for this latter approach and several suggestions are offered with regard to needed empirical study.
Article
The purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of fast- and slow-tempo music on 500-m rowing sprint performances. Twenty-two rowers performed 500-m sprints 3 times: rowing without music, rowing to slow music, and rowing to fast tempo music. Strokes per minute (SPM), time to completion, (TTC), and rated perceived exertion (RPE) were recorded. Although RPE did not differ between the rowing conditions, TTC was shortest in the fast music condition. Further, shorter TTC was observed in the slow music condition in contrast to the control condition, indicating that slow music also enhanced performance. The strongest treatment effects emerged, however, in the examination of the SPM that were significantly higher during rowing to fast music in comparison with rowing to slow music or no music. These results suggest that fast music acts as an external psyching-up stimulus in brief and strenuous muscle work.
Article
Article
24 female undergraduates performed 3 18-min sessions on a cycle ergometer at light, moderate, and heavy workloads during which perceived exertion, affect, and heart rate were monitored. Each S participated in a control, deprivation, and music condition. No significant differences were found in heart rate between conditions. Significantly lower perceived exertion existed during the music compared with the deprived condition at the low workload. There was lower perceived exertion during the music compared with the control condition at the moderate workload. Significantly greater levels of affect were observed during the music compared with the deprived condition at the moderate and heavy workloads. The influence of music and deprivation on perceived exertion and affect appears to be load dependent. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
10 female college basketball players received 5 daily 30-min training sessions in progressive relaxation and visual imagery related to foul shooting. Over the next 5 days of training, Ss continued relaxation and visualization exercises or also observed a videotaped model. Following a 5-day fading period to reduce Ss' dependence on the videotape, Ss' foul shooting performance was compared to preintervention baseline. Ss in the modeling condition showed a significant improvement in performance relative to Ss in the relaxation/visualization-only condition. All Ss in the modeling condition also reported feeling kinesthetic sensations in association with visual imagery. Findings indicate that the use of visual cues through modeling may provide a means of enhancing visuo-motor behavior rehearsal and support the suggestion that most forms of mental rehearsal are more effective for performers with relatively high levels of skill. (10 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Examined the effectiveness of highlight peak performance music videotapes on competitive offensive field goal percentage using 5 male intercollegiate basketball players. Videotapes incorporating each S's best and most effective plays were supplemented by inspirational music and were viewed by the Ss throughout the competitive season. Treatment effects were determined by a single-S multiple-baseline-across-Ss design. Although a causal relationship between highlight videotapes and offensive field goal percentage was not established, the results did demonstrate a mean increase of 4.7% in overall field goal percentage for 3 of the 5 Ss. Social validity was explored through the use of imagery evaluation sheets, individual player logs of introspective thought, and personal interviews. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Objectives. To examine: (a) the effect of music type on running time and on sensations and thoughts experienced by the runners under high physical exertion, and (b) the role that music plays in the use of two distinct self-regulation techniques during high exertion, namely dissociative and motivational.Design and procedure. Three studies were conducted. In Study 1 and Study 2, performed in the laboratory, participants ran at 90% of their maximal oxygen uptake on a motorized treadmill four times, once each with rock, dance, and inspirational music, and once without attending to music. Ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) and heart rate (HR) were monitored during the run, and discomfort symptoms and music-specific questions were examined. In Study 3, performed in the field, participants ran a hilly course eight times, four under a competitive-pair condition, and four under a single-mode condition. Running time was the dependent variable.Results. Music failed to influence HR, RPE, and sensations of exertion in the three studies. However, about 30% of the participants indicated that the music helped them at the beginning of the run. The participants stated that music both directed their attention to the music and motivated them to continue. Despite the heavy workload reported by the runners, running with music was perceived as beneficial by many.Conclusions: People engaged in high intensity running may benefit from listening to music, but may not increase their ability to sustain that effort longer than they could without music. Further research that incorporates personal music type and rhythm preferences should be carried out in order to advance this line of inquiry.