ArticlePDF Available

Gender differences in sexual motivation

Authors:
  • Tavistock Centre for Couple Relationships

Abstract

This article describes the ways in which arousal and sexual urges in women and men reflect sex differences in the links between brain responses and genital changes. Boys are more sexually motivated from puberty on, whereas girls tend to seek emotional connections as a precursor to sexual contact. Although feelings and thoughts are important for both sexes, women are not always psychologically aroused by genital vasocongestion, while the opposite tends to pertain to men. Lack of subjective arousal commonly leads to loss of sexual interest in women: negative emotions in the current relationship are often involved. For men, the causes of sexual withdrawal are more variable, and childhood family dynamics are a frequent, but unconscious, factor. The role of the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin, synthesised from oestrogen and testosterone respectively, is described as a biological underpinning for some observed gender differences. Oxytocin is released at orgasm by both women and men and promotes sexual pleasure and emotional bonding. Vasopressin, secreted during the male arousal phase, is linked to men's drive for sexual expression. This peptide may have the opposite effect on women and impair arousal and motivation due to the link between vasopressin release and aggression. Psychological treatment needs to explore the meaning of lack of sexual motivation for each person, and to address individual and couple issues. An integrated psychosexual model combining behavioural and psychodynamic approaches can alter feelings and relational patterns, and facilitate the return of sexual desire between partners in a committed relationship.
Practicing medicine
Keywords
Sexual motivation
Gender
differences
Oxytocin
Vasopressin
Arousal
Anger
Janice Hiller, BSc, MPhil
Psychological Services in
Sexual Health, Goodmayes
Hospital, Essex UK
E-mail:
janicehiller@hotmail.com
Online 27 July 2005
Gender differences in
sexual motivation
Janice Hiller
Abstract
This article describes the ways in whicharousal and sexual urges in women and men reflect sex differences in
the links between brain responses and genital changes. Boys are more sexually motivated from puberty on,
whereas girls tend to seek emotional connections as a precursor to sexual contact. Although feelings and
thoughts are important for both sexes, women are not always psychologically aroused by genital
vasocongestion, while the opposite tends to pertain to men. Lack of subjective arousal commonly leads
to loss of sexual interest in women: negative emotions in the current relationship are often involved. For
men, the causes of sexual withdrawal are more variable, and childhood family dynamics are a frequent, but
unconscious, factor. The role of the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin, synthesised from oestrogen
and testosterone respectively, is described as a biological underpinning for some observed gender
differences. Oxytocin is released at orgasm by both women and men and promotes sexual pleasure
and emotional bonding. Vasopressin, secreted during the male arousal phase, is linked to men’s drive for
sexual expression. This peptide may have the opposite effect on women and impair arousaland motivation
due to the link between vasopressin release and aggression. Psychological treatment needs to explore the
meaning of lack of sexual motivation for each person, and to address individual and couple issues. An
integrated psychosexual model combining behavioural and psychodynamic approaches can alter feelings
and relational patterns, and facilitate the return of sexual desire between partners in a committed
relationship. ß2005 WPMH GmbH. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
Introduction
Male sexuality has been the focus of much
popular and scientific interest since oral med-
ication for the erectile response was first intro-
duced. When used as part of a planned
therapeutic approach, pharmacological inter-
ventions can clearly be beneficial for a range of
erectile problems. But from the perspective of a
clinical psychologist working with individual
people and couples who are distressed by issues
of sexual motivation, it is also clear that sexual
behaviour between people is far more complex
than neurophysiology and endocrinology, for
men as well as for women. In particular, the loss
of desire, or motivation, for sexual contact with
a partner in an on-going relationship is a com-
mon presenting complaint, and remains one of
the most difficult to treat.
For effective clinical practice the ability to
respond sexually must be differentiated from
the motivation to be sexual with a specific
partner. Sexual interactions occur in social
and interpersonal settings that are mediated
by emotions and cognitions: what we feel and
think (consciously and unconsciously) about
a partner before and during love-making will
influence psychophysiological mechanisms
and alter our perception of sexual experiences.
Arousal and desire are interwoven in different
ways for women and men, neurochemically
and psychologically, as will be elaborated later
in this article. My aim here is to show that a
notion of sexuality occurring in an emotional
context between people, constructed in the
social environment, and also dependant on
biological underpinnings, can inform theore-
tical models and thereby guide our treatment
of sexual motivation issues.
How do our feelings and thoughts (brain
responses) interact, via bodily (genital) changes,
to lead to sexual expression? Although complex
connections are poorly understood in humans,
there is a wealth of literature on attachment
ß2005 WPMH GmbH. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 339–345, September 2005 339
patterns and sexual bonding in other mam-
mals, which deserves consideration. As
humans we are, of course, able to reect on
sensations, to consciously experience feelings,
and to make choices. At the same time our
sexual urges emanate from primitive subcor-
tical areas of the emotional limbic brain,
which are hormonally regulated and very simi-
lar to that of other mammals. Panksepp [1] has
described how neuropeptides released from
the pituitary gland, which are manufactured
from gonadal hormones, are essential for ero-
tic mood states, and suggests that cross-species
comparisons can be made, albeit with caution.
In contrast with the subcortical systems that
govern sexual urges, and can be investigated by
way of animal models, the evolutionarily mod-
ern neocortex (unique to humans) has been
shown by Damasio [2] to be the structure where
conscious reasoning, planning and mindful-
ness take place. Damasio [3] has also argued
that the neural patterns thatconstitute feelings
are based on the brains perception of emotions
(visceral states), and this occurs through a pro-
cess of dynamically updated evaluations of bod-
ily states including the musculoskeletal system,
the viscera and the internal chemical milieu.
Conscious decision-making about sexual beha-
viour will therefore interact with current bodily
states, personal history and the social context,
to varying degrees at different stages of the life
course, within a biopsychosocial model [4].
Recent neuroscientic evidence adds weight
therefore, to the need to integrate psychology
with biology, for a broader appreciation of
human sexual responsiveness. To what extent
do men and women differ in the links between
feelings (neocortical activity), genital responses
and sexual urges, and how can an understand-
ing of these inform clinical practice ? Simila-
rities and differences between men and women
which link psychological concepts or arousal
and motivation with the neuroendocrine sys-
tems that underpin sexual feelings and beha-
viours will be discussed.
Loss of sexual desire: disease entity
or psychological phenomenon
Since the concept of problems with sexual
desire was rst described by Kaplan [5],low
or absent motivation to be sexual has become a
well recognised and common presenting
issue. Generally it occurs in the context of a
committed relationship, when one partner
becomesreluctantorrefusestoengagein
sexual contact. The rejected partner often
feels hurt and confused and may withdraw
emotionally or become attacking, so that the
difculty is further compounded by dishar-
mony. The partner who has lost sexual interest
is diagnosed with hypoactive sexual desire
disorder according to DSM IV [6]. For clinicians
who provide therapeutic approaches based on
psychology, the notion of incompatibility of
sexual need [7] is more helpful. This shifts the
focus from a disease entity within one person
to a problem constructed between two people,
and indicates the necessity for a thorough
assessment of the psychological and relational
issues. Bancroft [8] pointsoutthatalossof
sexual interest often occurs with couples in an
unsatisfactory or damaging relationship, so
that withdrawal by one person represents an
adaptive response to couple difculties and is
psychologically understandable, rather than a
medical dysfunction. Moreover clinical prac-
tice indicates that sexual withdrawal in both
men and women is associated with negative
emotional states such as disappointment,
anger and fear. Although these emotions often
originate in conictual relationship issues or
current life stressors, they can also represent
unresolved experiences in the family-of-ori-
gin, that later become projected onto an
intimate relationship. Whether consciously
experienced as feelings that can be named
or remaining as unconscious factors, negative
emotions nevertheless exert a powerful inu-
ence on sexual urges and desire for intimacy
[9].
From a biological perspective, both women
and men require an adequate level of free
testosterone, which fuels the drive (or biologi-
cal) component of sexual desire. The willing-
ness to be sexual with a given partner is
inuenced by psychological and interpersonal
factors [10]. Men have 1020 times more tes-
tosterone than women and this androgen has a
clear role in male sexual interest and arousal
mechanisms [8]. Although circulating testos-
terone is related to womens sexual thoughts
and needs [11], research into the correlation
between womens sexual behaviour and testos-
terone levels has shown contradictory results
[8]. In the absence of organic causes, viewing
low or absent sexual motivation as emanating
Practicing medicine
340 Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 339345, September 2005
from a negative emotional state that has
intrapsychic and interpersonal signicance is
consistent with conceptualising the problem
as psychological in origin, rather than a dys-
function in the medical sense.
Motivation for sexual activity over
the life course
That women and men tend to differ in their
motivation for sexual activity, particularly in
the context of a long-term relationship, is a well-
observed phenomenon. Incompatible need for
sexual contact can occur despite partners
experiencing similar levels at the start of a
new relationship. More frequently it is a change
in the female partners desire for sex that causes
conict and tension, and leads the couple to
seek treatment. For example, 40% of the female
referrals at a London sexual problems clinic
were for low or absent sexual desire, compared
with 6% of the male referrals [12].
Boys and girls both recall their rst sense of
romantic attraction at around 10 years of age
whether for the same or opposite sex [13]. But
from puberty on, young men as a group are
more erotically motivated than women in
every area of sexual activity. Adolescent boys
masturbate more frequently than girls, mas-
turbate from a younger age, start having sex-
ual intercourse earlier and are more interested
in casual sex [14,15]. Sexual fantasies, which
imbue the situation with meaning, appear to
be equally frequent for girls and boys, but the
content of imagined sexual scenarios indicates
signicant gender differences. Whereas boys
are preoccupied with sexual acts, conquests
and achievements, girls fantasise about sex
as part of an emotional, faithful and romantic
relationship [16]. Evidence indicates that
women and men have similar levels of sexual
needs when they do occur, but men are more
likely to experience spontaneous and consis-
tent sexual urges irrespective of the nature of
the relationship. For women, feelings about a
particular partner tend to be more relevant for
sexual desire [17]. Discussing this gender dif-
ference can be helpful with couples for whom
the womans loss of sexual motivation is caus-
ing distress and conict. Clinical evidence
indicates that men who lose sexual motivation
feel embarrassed and confused at this atypical
pattern, and partners frequently blame them-
selves for being undesirable and can become
very depressed.
Difference in arousal stimuli for men
and women
Whereas the human sexual response cycle was
originally conceptualised as consisting of three
phases desire, followed by arousal, leading to
orgasm [5] recent advances in understanding
the complexity of sexual responding has high-
lighted the centrality of arousal in organising
sexual behaviour. For men, the erectile
response generally leads to subjective feelings
of arousal. By contrast a series of studies have
shown that womens subjective experiences of
arousal are not automatically linked with geni-
tal changes [18]. Blood ow to the genitals and
vaginal lubrication (unconscious physiological
responses) are necessary for comfortable penile
containment, but Basson [19] has demonstrated
that mental arousal and genital vasocongestion
are readily disconnected in women. Three sub-
types of female arousal disorders have now been
described. In ‘‘subjective sexual arousal disor-
der’’ a physical response is present with no
feelings of mental arousal; in ‘‘genital sexual
arousal disorder’’ subjective excitement occurs
with absent or impaired genital changes. The
third category of ‘‘combined genital and sub-
jective disorder’’ entails impairment of both
mental and genital responses [20]. Without feel-
ings of sexual arousal (brain responses) a desire
for sex will not be experienced, even if there is
genital vasodilation. Consequently, a difculty
with subjective arousal will often be a more
accurate explanation than ‘‘desire disorder’’
for a womans withdrawal of sexual interest.
Male arousal phase difculties, in the form of
erectile problems, are linked psychologically to
the intrusion of anxiety on arousal mechan-
isms. These have always been considered the
most prevalent sexual difculty for men,
whereby subjective arousal and the subsequent
desire for sex are present when erectile capacity
is not.
In the non-clinical population, men are able
to choose not to engage in sex despite genital
arousal and subjective excitement. Women, by
contrast, are able to choose to engage in sex for
reasons other than feeling psychologically or
physiologically aroused initially, such as avoid-
ing upsetting a partner or to become pregnant,
Practicing medicine
Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 339345, September 2005 341
and can then feel subjective arousal when
intimate contact has begun.
How do we understand this contrast
between men and women? As secondary sex-
ual characteristics develop at the start of pub-
erty, adolescent boys become aware of strong
genital responses to visual stimuli, accompa-
nied by a sense of urgency and craving for
sexual expression. They may also become
more aggressive and combative in their gen-
eral behaviour. Girlssexual feelings more
often derive from their emotional reactions
to a partner, which are heightened by the
romantic themes in lms and magazines,
ubiquitous in our culture, to which adolescent
girls are often drawn. Notwithstanding cultu-
ral norms, testosterone and oestrogen clearly
accompany these psychological changes. The
inuence of neuropeptides manufactured from
these gonadal hormones on feeling states will
be described below.
Whereas in our society the pace for sexual
contact is more often set by the contingencies
of male persistence and assertiveness (except
when the woman wants to conceive) it is gen-
erally the case that limit setting and emotional
closeness are predominantly female concerns.
For biological and evolutionary reasons this
makes sense, as women need to be aware of
careful partner selection for reproductive and
child-rearing purposes, rather than feeling dri-
ven by a pressing need for sexual contact when
physiologically aroused. Genetic predisposi-
tion and individual psychology will of course
vary, and for each individual person these will
interact with the broader sociocultural factors
that determine specic behaviour patterns
and the nature of each persons sexual expres-
sion over the lifecourse [4].
Neuropeptides, feelings and sexual
behaviour
Central to current thinking on the links
between gender differences in sexual beha-
viour, brain chemistry, and feeling states are
the neuromodulators vasopressin and oxyto-
cin. These peptide hormones, which are
synthesised in the hypothalamus and released
into the bloodstream during sexual activity,
are found exclusively in mammals. They can-
not pass the blood- brain barrier if taken orally.
Men and women secrete vasopressin and oxy-
tocin, (manufactured from testosterone and
oestrogen, respectively) in different amounts
during the sexual response cycle.
Two main areas of research have indicated
that vasopressin and oxytocin could provide
the neurochemical underpinnings that link
psychological experiences with physiological
responses in women and men. Firstly, labora-
tory studies with individual subjects found
that women secreted more oxytocin than
men during arousal (self-stimulation) and at
orgasm [21]. Moreover, increased oxytocin cor-
related in both groups with the intensity of
muscle contractions and the ratings of plea-
sure from sexual experiences [22]. In a study
with male subjects only, oxytocin was again
found to increase signicantly from baseline
to orgasm/ejaculation, but vasopressin showed
a different temporal pattern. Vasopressin rose
during the erectile response, then dropped
back to baseline levels at ejaculation, when
oxytocin remained elevated [23].
The signicance of these neuropeptides for
complex human interactions is indicated by
thesecondareaofresearch,namelysexual
activity and mate attachments in rodents.
Numerous studies have conrmed gender dif-
ferences in peptide secretion during copula-
tion and partner selection in prairie voles,
who form monogamous heterosexual attach-
ments for mating. Females release oxytocin
during cohabitation and mating, without
which they do not form attachments to a
mate, whereas males release vasopressin dur-
ing copulation, which is crucial for inter- male
aggression and mate-guarding [24,25].Vaso-
pressin is also associated with male persis-
tency for sexual behavior [1].Oxytocinhas
been identiedastheneuropeptideassociated
with the close emotional bonds that provide a
secure base for child-rearing purposes, and
for sexual pleasure in both women and men
[26,1].
Sexual responses, aggression and
vasopressin
What do we know about the effect on womens
sexual feelings when vasopressin is secreted?
The female brain has fewer vasopressin neu-
rons than the male brain, but as with men,
vasopressin is linked with aggression, social
memories and focused attention. Despite the
Practicing medicine
342 Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 339345, September 2005
lack of direct evidence from laboratory studies,
I have proposed that the central release of
vasopressin may have the opposite effect on
womens sexual urges and could actively
impair arousal mechanisms [27].Supportfor
this hypothesis derives from clinical data as
well as rodent research. Women who request
help for a loss of sexual interest with a specic
partner commonly describe anger, resent-
ment and hostility, emanating from the cou-
ples relationship, but also connected to
family-of-origin issues that may not be con-
sciously linked with sexual withdrawal. What-
ever the causes of negative emotional states
(i.e. stress, chores, life-events) feeling devalued
and undermined can inhibit womenssexual
functioning [17]. As mentioned above, lack
of a specic genital response is rarely involved
[20].
Loss of sexual interest after childbirth is
another common occurrence; a temporary
state that reverts with time and has obvious
evolutionary value. After female rats give birth
a natural increase in vasopressin is found in
brain circuits, which has been linked to the
neurochemistry of aggressive and hostile reac-
tions that enable the mother to protect her
offspring from danger [28]. Most signicantly,
when vasopressin is articially placed in a
precise area of the female rat brain the result
is an instant decline in acceptance of copula-
tion [29]. It is possible, therefore, that when
women have negative feelings such as disap-
pointment or anger in a couple relationship,
they are secreting vasopressin in the central
nervous system, which impairs sexual arousal
mechanisms and thereby inhibits the motiva-
tion for sex.
For men, rodent research is consistent with
laboratory ndings indicating that vasopres-
sin secretion during the erectile response is
linked with mens awareness of sensations
and, importantly with the drive for sexual
expression. Other gender differences may also
be associated, neurobiologically, with these
peptides. From adolescence on, testosterone-
regulated vasopressin secretion in the preoptic
area of the hypothalamus may underpin the
greater response men have to visual stimuli in
the creation of genital changes and sexual
sensations. Sex and aggression circuits are in
close proximity in the brain of male rodents,
and boys often become more territorial and
hostile in their teenage years.
Sexual responses, emotional bonds
and oxytocin
Although both men and women release oxyto-
cin during arousal, sufcient oxytocin levels
seem particularly important for womens
awareness of arousal mechanisms. If oxytocin
receptors are blocked in the ventromedial
hypothalamus of female rats, (the brain area
assocoated with many emotional and motiva-
tional processes), males who approach them
for copulation will be rejected or attacked [30].
When male rats palpate the anks of females,
oxytocin secretion is stimulated and erotic
states are induced to prepare the female psy-
chologically for sexual activity [1].
In the clinical population, women who are
diagnosed with a desire disorder, whose pri-
mary issue is absence of psychological arousal,
not only describe negative feelings about their
relationship or about life-events, but also com-
plain of a lack of emotional intimacy and
affectionate physical contact. A womans sex-
ual needs are embedded in fullling emotional
attachments with an appreciative and loving
partner [26], and intimate sensual touch
and social closeness are stimuli that release
oxytocin [31]. Therefore a relationship with
positive thoughts, feelings and contact will
set the scene for psychological arousal and
sexual expression. Female arousal and hence
the motivation for sex may be signicantly
impaired by insufcient oxytocin release
and/or a predominance of vasopressin. This
neurochemical balance is speculated to occur
when women feel neglected, undermined, or
hostile to a partner.
Loss of sexual interest and
negative emotions
Of particular relevance is the interaction
between oxytocin and the neurochemistry of
romantic attraction. Passionate feelings and
obsessivethinkingabouttheloveobject,
which often occurs when partners rst meet,
have been identied as a neurotransmitter
balance in which dopamine (DA) and noradre-
naline (NA) are elevated and serotonin is
decreased [32].AsDAandNAcanregulate
the release of neuropeptides in the central
nervous system [25] strong romantic attrac-
tion at the start of a relationship constitutes
Practicing medicine
Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 339345, September 2005 343
a brain state which facilitates sexual arousal
(in both sexes) through enhanced oxytocin
secretion. Arousal, pleasure and then motiva-
tion might, however, decrease in women if
initial passion and its neural correlates are
replaced by resentment and anger, the emo-
tional states (linked to lowered oxytocin and
increased vasopressin) that impair female
arousal responses.
From this perspective, changes to a relation-
ship over time are less likely to inhibit mens
desire for sex, due to the many visual stimuli in
everyday life that cause mental arousal, and
the experience of sexual need linked to vaso-
pressin secretion during the erectile response.
Some men do lose sexual interest for a partner
in a committed relationship, despite adequate
testosterone levels. This change is less common
in men, but the psychological causes seem
more complex. Clinical experience and
research on the inuence of thoughts and
feelings on mens desire for sex shows that
some men lose sexual motivation with low
mood and others do not. A paradoxical
increase of sexual interest amongst men with
negative mood states was found in research
using both quantitative and qualitative mea-
sures [33]. This unpredictability points to the
need to look beyond the relationship context,
and highlights the importance of developmen-
tal history in the shaping of mens sexual
response patterns. For women the research is
more clear-cut and reects clinical practice. In
a study assessing the predictors and prevalence
of distress about sexuality in women, it was
negative mental states and negative feelings
during sexual activity with a partner that was
found to lead to sexual distress in most cases
[34].
Developmental history, sociocultural fac-
tors, individual personality and relationship
issues are all relevant to the understanding of
sexual motivation problems in women and
men. Anxiety and fear during childhood
may be particularly signicant for the mean-
ing of sexual expression however, due to the
effect of stress hormones on the neural sys-
tems involved in sexuality, intimacy and
aggression. If circulating stress hormones (cor-
tisone and corticosterone) are consistently
high, the synthesis of receptors for oxytocin
and vasopressin may be impaired [25]:cogni-
tive and emotional links between intimate
contact and sexuality may be disrupted. Hence
a child who is exposed to situations such as
abuse, dysfunctional family dynamics or con-
sistent bullying may struggle to integrate
anxiety and anger into a later couple relation-
ship. Exploring this connection as part of a
therapeutic approach can increase under-
standing and alter feelings between partners,
facilitating the eventual return of motivation
for sexual contact.
Concluding comments
Within the framework outlined here, sexual
withdrawal is viewed as an adaptive mechan-
ism that protects from perceived threat or
harm. A review of the psychological conditions
that facilitate or impede sexual activity
between people points to the centrality of
negative emotions for womens lack of sexual
arousal and desire. Less is known about the
predictors of mens lack of motivation, but
clinical evidence suggests that damaging
developmental experiences (e.g. a boys inabil-
ity to cope with his mothers intense emo-
tional needs) can be especially signicant.
Unresolved issues from the family-of-origin
may be projected onto the current partner-
ship, and undermine desire [9].
Thorough assessment and psychological for-
mulation to uncover the meaning of sexual
withdrawal for each person is crucial for the
planning of appropriate treatment models.
Psychosexual therapy integrates behavioural
exercises using sensual and sexual touch and
stimulation with psychoeducation to help
partners understand the nature of the pro-
blem. In addition, object relations therapy
explores the re-enactment of past issues in
the present [9,35] and cognitive-behavioural
methods focus on dysfunctional beliefs and
assumptions about sexuality [36,37]. Other sys-
temic [38] and combined approaches [39] have
been described. An integrated psychosexual
model aims to develop understanding of the
difculty whilst facilitating a gradual non-
threatening approach to increasing sexual
intimacy. Exploring misunderstandings and
conict between people, and restructuring
emotional and physical contact may inuence
underlying physiology, and thereby provides a
scientically valid and clinically helpful
approach to sexual motivation problems in
women and men.
Practicing medicine
344 Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 339345, September 2005
Practicing medicine
For both men and women, feelings and thoughts affect sexual motivation.
Men and women differ in the links between brain responses (feelings and thoughts), genital changes and desire for sex.
Women are less easily aroused sexually because genital changes may not lead to subjective experiences of excitement.
Mens sexual arousal and desire is more spontaneous than that of women, for whom relational factors are more central.
Lack of sexual motivation is frequently psychological in origin and reects developmental and relationship issues.
Anger impairs sexual desire in women but not necessarily in men.
Oxytocin and vasopressin, neuropeptides that link brain and peripheral responses with sexual urges, may be associated
with gender differences in sexual motivation.
Psychological therapy aims to address partner-related factors and the meaning of sexual withdrawal for each person in
order to alter patterns of responding.
References
[1] Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: the
foundation of human and animal emotions.
New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.
[2] Damasio A. DescartesError: Emotion, Rea-
son and the Human Brain. Vintage; 1994.
[3] Damasio A. Looking for Spinoza. London:
William Heinemann; 2003.
[4] Hiller J. Sex, mind and emotion through the
lifecourse: a biopsychosocial perspective. In:
Hiller J, Wood H, Bolton W. (Eds.), Sex, Mind
and Emotion. Karnac Books 2005a, in press.
[5] Kaplan HS. Disorders of Sexual Desire. Bal-
liere Tindall; 1979.
[6] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic
& Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4
th
ed.) Washington DC.
[7] Crowe M, Ridley J. Therapy with Couples: A
Behavioral-Systems Approach to Marital and
Sexual Problems. Blackwell Scientic Publi-
cations; 2000.
[8] Bancroft J. The medicalization of female sex-
ual dysfunction: the need for caution.
Archives of Sexual Behaviour 2002;31(5):
4515.
[9] Hiller J. Loss of sexual interest and negative
states of mind. In: Hiller J, Wood H, Bolton
W. (Eds.), Sex, Mind and Emotion. Karnac
Books 2005b, In press.
[10] Levine S. The nature of sexual desire: a
clinicians perspective. Archives of Sexual
Behaviour 1992;32(3):27985.
[11] Riley A, Riley F. Controlled studies on
women presenting with sexual drive disor-
ders: I. endocrine status. Journal of Sex and
Marital Therapy 2000;26:26983.
[12] Hems SA, Crowe M. The psychosexual dys-
function clinic at the maudsley hospital,
London: a survey of referrals between Jan-
uary and December 1996. Sexual and Rela-
tionship Therapy 1999;14(1):1525.
[13] McClintock M, Herdt G. Rethinking puberty.
the development of sexual attraction. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science
1996;5:17883.
[14] Oliver MB, Hyde JS. Gender differences in
sexuality: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bul-
letin 1993;14:2951.
[15] Heaven PCL. The Social Psychology of Ado-
lescence. UK: Palgrave; 2001.
[16] Breakwell G. Adolescents and Emerging
Sexuality. In: Sherr L, editor. Aids and Ado-
lescents. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic
Publishers; 1997.
[17] Leiblum SR. Reconsidering gender differ-
ences in sexual desire: an update. Sexual
and Relationship Therapy 2002;17(1):
5867.
[18] Everaerd W, Laan E, Both S, Van der Velde J.
Female Sexuality. In: Szuchman LT, Muscar-
ella F. (Eds.), John Wiley and Sons Inc: New
York 2000.
[19] BassonR. Biopsychosocialmodelsofwomens
sexualresponse: applicationsto management
of ‘‘desiredisorders’’. Sexual and Relationship
Therapy 2003;18(1):10715.
[20] Basson R, Leiblum S, Brotto L, Derogatis L,
Fourcroy J, Fugl-Meyer K, et al. Denitions of
womens sexual dysfunction reconsidered:
advocating expansion and revision. Journal
of Psychosomatic Obstetrics-Gyneocology
2003.
[21] Carmichael MS, Humbert R, Dixon J, Palmi-
sano G, Greenleaf W, Davidson JM. Plasma
oxytocin increases in the human sexual
response. J Clin Endocrinology and Metabo-
lism 1987;64:2731.
[22] Carmichael MS, Warburton VL, Dixen J,
Davidson JM. Relationships among cardio-
vascular, muscular and oxytocin responses
during human sexual activity. Archives of
Sexual Behaviour 1994;23:5979.
[23] Murphy MR, Seckel JR, Burton S, Checkley
SA, Lightman SL. Changes in oxytocin and
vasopressin secretion during sexual activity
in men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology
and Metabolism 1987;68:73841.
[24] Insel TR. A neurobiological basis of social
attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry
1997;54:72635.
[25] Carter SC. Neuroendocrine perspectives on
social attachment and love. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology 1998;23(8):779818.
[26] Fisher H. The rst sex: the natural talents of
women and how they are changing the
world. New York: Ballantine; 1999.
[27] Hiller J. Speculations on the links between
feelings, emotions and sexual behaviour: are
vasopressin and oxytocin involoved? Sexual
and Relationship Therapy 2004;19(4):393
412.
[28] Landgraff R, Neumann I, Russell JA, Pittman
QJ. Push-pull perfusion and mircodialysis
studies of central oxytocin and vasopressin
release in freely moving rats during preg-
nancy, parturition and lactation. Ann N Y
Acad Science 1992;652:32639.
[29] Sodersten P, Henning M, Melins P, Ludin S.
Vasopressinalters female sexual behaviourby
acting on the brain independently of altera-
tions in blood pressure. Nature 1983;301:
60810.
[30] Caldwell JD, Johns JM, Faggin BM, Senger
MA, Pederson CA. Infusion of an oxytocin
antagonist into the medial preoptic area
prior to progesterone inhibits sexual recep-
tivity and increases rejection in female rats.
Horm Behav 1994;28:288302.
[31] Uvna¨s-Moberg K. Oxytocin may mediate
the benets of positive social interaction
and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology
1998;23:81935.
[32] Fisher HE, Aran A, Mashek D, Li H, Brown LL.
Dening the brain systems of lust, romantic
attraction and attachment. Archives of Sex-
ual Behaviour 2002;31(5):4139.
[33] Bancroft J, Janssen E, Strong D, Carnes L,
Vukadinovic Z, LongJS. The relation between
mood and sexuality in heterosexual men.
Archives of Sexual Behavior 2003;32(3):
21730.
[34] Bancroft J, Loftus J, Long JS. Distess about
sex: a national survey of women in hetero-
sexual relationships. Archives of Sexual
Behavior 2003;32(3):193208.
[35] Scharff D, Scharff JS. Object Relations Cou-
ple Therapy. London: Jason Aronson; 1991.
[36] Spence S. Psychosexual Therapy: A Cogni-
tive-Behavioural Approach. London: Chap-
man & Hall; 1991.
[37] McCabe MP. Evaluation of a cognitive beha-
vioural therapy program for people with
sexual dysfunction. Journal of Sex & Marital
Therapy 2001;27:25971.
[38] Schnarch D. Desire Problems: a Systemic
Perspective. In: Leiblum SR, Rosen RC, edi-
tors. Principles and Practoces of Sex Ther-
apy. The Guilford Press; 2000.
[39] Pridal CG, LoPiccolo J. Multielement Treat-
ment of Desire Disorders: Integration of Cog-
nitive, Behavioural, and Systemic Therapy.
In: Leiblum SR, Rosen RC, editors. Principles
and Practice of Sex Therapy. The Guilford
Press; 2000.
Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 339345, September 2005 345
... Yet at the present time, these gender differences appear somewhat difficult to interpret. As noted earlier, Carmichael et al. [8] found that women secreted higher levels of oxytocin than men during a sexual arousal task, and Hiller [25] argued that oxytocin may prove particularly important for women's psychological (as opposed to physiological) sexual arousal. This has important clinical implications, given that psychological arousal difficulties are much more prevalent in women than in men (e.g., [26,27]. ...
... This has important clinical implications, given that psychological arousal difficulties are much more prevalent in women than in men (e.g., [26,27]. Hiller [25] reviewed rodent studies which revealed that female rats whose oxytocin receptors were blocked tended to refuse copulation. Additionally, rodent models have shown that physical contact facilitates females' receptivity to sexual advances [25], and physical contact is known to trigger oxytocin release [28]. ...
... Hiller [25] reviewed rodent studies which revealed that female rats whose oxytocin receptors were blocked tended to refuse copulation. Additionally, rodent models have shown that physical contact facilitates females' receptivity to sexual advances [25], and physical contact is known to trigger oxytocin release [28]. Collectively, such findings support continued investigation of oxytocin's role in female's arousability. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review The purpose of the present review is to outline what is currently known about oxytocin and human sexual behavior. The present review utilizes existing research to outline an important question remaining in the field; does oxytocin facilitate sexual behavior or is it simply a result of the said behavior? Recent Findings Existing studies which measure oxytocin by way of blood or saliva reliably find elevations in oxytocin during sexual arousal and following orgasm. In contrast, studies which administer oxytocin intranasally report fewer significant results surrounding arousal and orgasm. Summary Existing research has yet to fully understand the function of oxytocin. Because of long-standing questions such as function, future research should pay close attention to the context of sexual activity (masturbation, partnered sex) and the specific types of pairings involved (sex with a long term versus a new partner and same-sex versus other-sex contact). Similarly researchers should carefully consider the mode of oxytocin assessment in consideration of question/s they wish to answer.
... This rather homogenous sample limits the generalizability of our findings. Existing evidence suggests that sexual motivations differ based on relational context and gender (e.g., Cooper et al., 2006Cooper et al., , 2011Hiller, 2005), and some have speculated that they may vary as a function of sexual orientation and culture (Hatfield et al., 2011). However, it is not yet known whether the effects of mindfulness differentially influence sexual motivations based on demographic factors or across different life and relationship stages. ...
Article
Individuals reporting greater insecure attachment are more likely to report maladaptive sexual motivations, such as sex to avoid negative relational and personal outcomes (e.g., conflict). Despite the costs of such sexual motivations, research is less clear regarding what might buffer the extent to which attachment insecurities manifest in such motives. The current study examined whether trait mindfulness moderates the links between attachment insecurity and maladaptive sexual motives. Participants (N = 194) completed measures of trait mindfulness, general sexual motivations, and attachment. As predicted, the links between attachment anxiety and having sex to cope and affirm the self were eliminated among individuals reporting higher levels of the acting with awareness facet of trait mindfulness. No such buffering effects were seen for attachment avoidance. Instead, acting with awareness mindfulness appeared to increase the extent to which more avoidantly attached individuals reported coping and self-affirmation-based sexual motives. These findings contribute to knowledge regarding the potential utility and limits of mindfulness in relational and sexual contexts, perhaps suggesting that mindfulness may help anxiously attached individuals manage the extent to which attachment concerns manifest in maladaptive sexual motivations. Findings of the study may inform both theory regarding mindfulness in interpersonal functioning and how mindfulness interventions might be deployed in sex therapy contexts.
... The physical aspects of sexual encounters are closely intertwined with the emotional aspects of sex, and both aspects impact the degree to which one is sexually satisfied (Haavio-Mannila & Kontula, 1997). When an individual has experienced a multitude of negative emotions (e.g., fear, anger, disappointment, etc.) in a relationship, that person has a tendency to withdraw sexually from his or her partner (Hiller, 2005). Women, particularly, are sexually shut down and inhibited when they feel dishonored, devalued, or emotionally hurt or stressed (Leiblum, 2002). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The effect of marital education on marital and sexual satisfaction.
... 49,50 In particular, oxytocin is important for the neuroendocrine mediation of the romantic love 51 playing a role in the processes related to the initial stages of the romantic passion. 52 This involvement could be explained in the light of the role played by the oxytocin in empathy, in the reward for positive social interaction [53][54][55] and the bonding creation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Despite its role in social cognition and affiliative behavior, less is known about the role played by oxytocin in human sexual behavior. Aim In the present systematic review, we aimed to find the levels of oxytocin related to human sexual arousal and orgasm. Methods We conducted the study according to the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) guidelines. We performed a systematic search in the principal databases for studies that reported collection of salivary or plasmatic samples, with dosage of oxytocin in relation to sexual activity during induction of sexual arousal and orgasm. Results 414 articles were obtained. After duplicates removal and the application of pre exclusion criteria, 16 articles were considered eligible and 13 articles were included with a Cohen's k of 0.827. Most of the studies used sexual self-stimulation and collected plasmatic or salivary samples to measure oxytocin. The sexual arousal and orgasm were assessed based on subjective reports. Main Outcome Measure The primary outcomes were the oxytocin levels collected during the induction of sexual arousal and orgasm. Conclusions Several studies collected only subjective reports about the sexual arousal and the orgasm. Most of the studies found higher levels of oxytocin during the orgasm or ejaculation. Given the sexual arousal evoked by self-stimulation in which sexual fantasies play an important role, it should be possible to postulate for a role of the oxytocin in sexual desire. In particular, we hypothesize a complex role of the oxytocin in the modulation of sexual fantasies and thoughts that are relevant in the sexual desire and help to trigger genital and sexual arousal.
... The brain gets signals from different senses, connects these signals with other experiences and sends the message to the penis to be ready to engage in the sexual act. The other part of the process is explained by hormones, peptides and other biochemical substances developed inside the brain and in other parts of the body, such as oxytocin (Carter, 1992;Keverne and Curley, 2004;Hiller, 2005), vasopressin (Keverne & Curley, 2004), acetylcholine and dopamine, which could act as neuromodulators or/and neurotransmitters. All of these are involved in the sexual activity and in reaching orgasm. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction:The effects of pornography consumption among men are revealed by both hundreds of internet testimonies and specialists who deal with such effects. This topicraises strong questions and determines the search for valid answers, as this behavior becomes addictive in some cases. There is a strong consideration among specialists that pornography consumption can be related to other problems. Objectives:The present paper aims to clarify some sexuality aspects in the pornography consumption context, in an attempt to understand both the brain mechanisms and the psychological factors involved in it.Methods:The method used was theinvestigation of the literature and the analysis ofsome clinical cases from our practice. Results:Pornography consumption influences the person’s behavior as he resorts to this type of stimulation to cope with life dissatisfaction. Even if the compulsive behavior fades to remission, the individual may relapse if the true cause that predisposes him to pornographic material consumption is not found. Therefore, it is needed to identify the psychological mechanisms that cause and maintain this behavior or that can favor relapse. Conclusion:Mental health specialists should take in consideration the possible effects of pornography consumption on men sexual behaviors, men sexual difficulties and other attitudes related to sexuality. Keywords: pornography consumption, sexuality, brain
Article
Sex under the influence of substances is a common sexual practice. Although they could explain engagement in sex under the influence of substances, few studies have explored sexual motivations driving substance use. To bridge this knowledge gap, this study aims to investigate differences in sexual motivations across genders and substance categories, and the link between substance use severity and sexual motivations. In all, 188 adults (aged 18–74) took part in a comprehensive assessment interview addressing substance use ( Évaluation intégrée spécialisée en dépendance), including three sexual motivation items for three substance categories (alcohol, cannabis, other substances), and substance use severity (WMH-CIDI). Wilcoxon rank-sum tests and generalized estimating equations were used to compare sexual motivations among substance categories between men and women. A logistic regression assessed whether substance use severity could influence the presence of sexual motivations when considering gender and age. Results revealed an interaction between gender and substance categories on sexual motivations. Men were more likely to present sexual motivations and exhibited greater sex-related substance use motivations than women for substances other than alcohol and cannabis. Moreover, higher substance use severity scores were associated with the presence of sexual motivations for substance use. This study underscores the need to develop gender-sensitive interventions that consider sexual motivations underlying substance use. Consideration of sexual motivations could help identify the needs fulfilled by substance use and prevent triggers arising from sexual situations. Being among the first studies on the subject, continuing research in this area to further investigate sexual motivations underlying substance use remains essential.
Article
The article presents a new Polish tool serving to measure the degree of sexual satisfaction achieved with a particular partner. It is a three-factor tool (measuring intimacy, petting, and sex) comprising 10 items that meet standardization requirements completely. The results of the performed factor validity analysis indicate that the three-factor model is the most valid for the purpose of description of sexual satisfaction measured with the SSQ (Sexual Satisfaction Questionnaire)—the aspects of satisfaction singled out in the model are strongly correlated. The three dimensions of satisfaction that were distinguished describe intimacy in a romantic relationship, satisfaction with one’s sex life, and satisfaction with mutual physical contacts other than sex. The cross-validation tests revealed that such a factor structure of the SSQ is stable and characteristic for the representatives of both of the sexes as well as married people. The three-factor structure significantly merges into a single global indicator only in the case of people maintaining partnerships, which advocates verification of a hypothesis regarding differences in the way married and cohabiting partners experience sexual satisfaction.
Article
Full-text available
This meta-analysis surveyed 177 usable sources that reported data on gender differences on 21 different measures of sexual attitudes and behaviors. The largest gender difference was in incidence of masturbation: Men had the greater incidence (d = .96). There was also a large gender difference in attitudes toward casual sex: Males had considerably more permissive attitudes (d = .81). There were no gender differences in attitudes toward homosexuality or in sexual satisfaction. Most other gender differences were in the small-to-moderate range. Gender differences narrowed from the 1960s to the 1980s for many variables. Chodorow's neoanalytic theory, sociobiology, social learning theory, social role theory, and script theory are discussed in relation to these findings.
Article
Full-text available
This paper addresses the links between subjective experiences and physiological responses during sexual activity, by exploring the neurobiology of the feelings and emotions that are associated with peripheral bodily changes. Although women and men differ in the brain structures and chemical pathways that govern sexuality, human sexual behaviour is part of interpersonal relationships and is cognitively mediated. Nevertheless comparisons can be made with other mammals whose subcortical systems are similar. Research into the neurophysiology of attachment, love and sexual bonding in rodents is reviewed, indicating that the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin, which are secreted from the pituitary gland, have a vital role in partner preference and copulation. Laboratory investigations into hormonal release during human sexual activity show that oxytocin is necessary for subjective pleasure during arousal and orgasm in both sexes, while vasopressin is released only during male arousal. Vasopressin is suggested to have the opposite effect on female sexuality, and to impair women's interest in sexual contact. As oxytocin and vasopressin are regulated by oestrogen and testosterone respectively, and are sensitive to developmental hormonal experiences, they could provide a link between biology and culture. It is also postulated that neuropeptide secretion in the human brain could account for some of the observed gender differences, such as the impact of puberty; aggressive and protective feelings; and consistency of sexual interest in committed relationships. Psychological approaches for relationships where negative emotional states inhibit responding, are considered more appropriate than pharmacological interventions at this stage, in view of the uniquely human need to regulate relationships and sex through awareness of mood states and feelings.
Book
Some investigators have argued that emotions, especially animal emotions, are illusory concepts outside the realm of scientific inquiry. With advances in neurobiology and neuroscience, however, researchers are proving this position wrong while moving closer to understanding the biology and psychology of emotion. In Affective Neuroscience, Jaak Panksepp argues that emotional systems in humans, as well as other animals, are necessarily combinations of innate and learned tendencies; there are no routine and credible ways to really separate the influences of nature and nurture in the control of behavior. The book shows how to move toward a new understanding by taking a psychobiological approach to the subject, examining how the neurobiology and neurochemistry of the mammalian brain shape the psychological experience of emotion. It includes chapters on sleep and arousal, pleasure and pain systems, the sources of rage and anger, and the neural control of sexuality. The book will appeal to researchers and professors in the field of emotion.
Article
The complexities of women's sexual response preclude useful management strategies which are modelled only on the traditional human sex response cycle beginning with sexual desire rather than other motivations. Both biological and psychological parameters affecting women's sexual motivation and arousability can be depicted in alternative models. These reflect that motivations to begin a sexual experience, although varied, are most often intimacy-based in women. The key role of subjective sexual arousal, which often precedes and then accompanies sexual desire, is clarified. A positive outcome emotionally and physically is necessary to achieve the original goal of enhancing emotional closeness, sense of being attracted and attractive, committed, loved—together with the goal (accessed en route) of sexual satisfaction. Innate, spontaneous desire may only occasionally augment the intimacy-based motivation. Thus therapy for ‘desire disorders’ may more profitably focus on the woman's problematic motivation, inadequate sexual stimuli and context, diminished arousability and/or unsatisfactory outcome to an experience. A model of subjective sexual arousal reflects the ongoing affective and cognitive modulation and variable input from physical genital congestion. Awareness of the poor correlation between genital vasocongestion and the subjective experience allows subtyping of arousal problems in order to facilitate their management.
Article
During breastfeeding or suckling, maternal oxytocin levels are raised by somatosensory stimulation. Oxytocin may, however, also be released by nonnoxious stimuli such as touch, warm temperature etc. in plasma and in cerebrospinal fluid. Consequently, oxytocin may be involved in physiological and behavioral effects induced by social interaction in a more general context. In both male and female rats oxytocin exerts potent physiological antistress effects. If daily oxytocin injections are repeated over a 5-day period, blood pressure is decreased by 10–20 mmHg, the withdrawal latency to heat stimuli is prolonged, cortisol levels are decreased and insulin and cholecystokinin levels are increased. These effects last from 1 to several weeks after the last injection. After repeated oxytocin treatment weight gain may be promoted and the healing rate of wounds increased. Most behavioral and physiological effects induced by oxytocin can be blocked by oxytocin antagonists. In contrast, the antistress effects can not, suggesting that unidentified oxytocin receptors may exist. The prolonged latency in the tail-flick test can be temporarily reversed by administration of naloxone, suggesting that endogenous opioid activity has been increased by the oxytocin injections. In contrast, the long-term lowering of blood pressure and of cortisol levels as well as the sedative effects of oxytocin have been found to be related to an increased activity of central α2-adrenoceptors. Positive social interactions have been related to health-promoting effects. Oxytocin released in response to social stimuli may be part of a neuroendocrine substrate which underlies the benefits of positive social experiences. Such processes may in addition explain the health-promoting effects of certain alternative therapies. Because of the special properties of oxytocin, including the fact that it can become conditioned to psychological state or imagery, oxytocin may also mediate the benefits attributed to therapies such as hypnosis or meditation. © 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
Article
tinct and significant studies have pointed to the age of 10 as the mean age of first sexual attrac tion?well before puberty, which is typically defined as the age when the capacity to procreate is attained (Timiras, 1972). These findings are at odds with previous
Article
A nearly universal stereotype holds that men are driven by their sexual impulses or drives while women rarely feel desire. This paper will consider the reported differences in sexual desire between men and women and the biological and social considerations fueling these differences. The effect of menstruation, gestation and lactation on female sexual desire will be briefly reviewed as well as the influence of estrogens and androgens on sexual drive. The suggestion will be made that, while differences in sexual drive and its behavioral expressions certainly exist, these gender differences may be narrowing as women gain political, social, economic and reproductive freedom. Finally, it will be argued that these differences may be precisely what makes sex interesting and meaningful and more than the simple release of biological tension.
Article
This paper describes the demographic characteristics, referral sources and types of referral problems of 135 patients referred to a psychosexual clinic located in an inner-city area of south London from January until December 1996. Results were comparable to those from another psychosexual clinic located in a similar inner-city area. Results from both these clinics differ from earlier reports of non-inner-city clinics in male to female distributions and most common referral source. The factors predictive of non-attendance were (a) non-return of a questionnaire asking for background information; (b) gender, with males being less likely to attend initial and treatment sessions; (c) place of birth, with non-UK born referrals being less likely to attend initial and treatment sessions; and (d) employment status, with the unemployed being less likely to attend treatment sessions. Identifying the characteristics of non-attenders can have important implications for the provision of cost-effective treatment and the optimal use of services.