ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Previous studies have shown that Asian American or Chinese individuals value low-arousal positive affect and a harmonious state of happiness more than European Americans do. However, the affective component of subjective well-being has mostly been defined as the presence of positive affect and the absence of negative affect. This definition emphasizes the importance of hedonic pleasure but fails to include the affect valued in Chinese culture. The present study developed the construct of peace of mind to describe the affective well-being valued in Chinese culture. Peace of mind was defined as an internal state of peacefulness and harmony. To develop a measure to assess peace of mind, three studies were conducted. Study 1 developed the Peace of Mind Scale (PoM), Study 2 established its validity as an affective well-being measure, and Study 3 found that individuals from Chinese cultures score higher on this scale than those from Western cultures. The results indicate that the PoM has good reliability and validity for measuring affective well-being. The cross-cultural validation also found that Taiwanese individuals scored higher on the PoM than European Americans, which provides further evidence of good construct validity of the PoM.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH PAPER
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind
Yi-Chen Lee ·Yi-Cheng Lin ·Chin-Lan Huang ·Barbara L. Fredrickson
Published online: 26 May 2012
©Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012
Abstract Previous studies have shown that Asian American or Chinese individuals value
low-arousal positive affect and a harmonious state of happiness more than European
Americans do. However, the affective component of subjective well-being has mostly been
defined as the presence of positive affect and the absence of negative affect. This definition
emphasizes the importance of hedonic pleasure but fails to include the affect valued in
Chinese culture. The present study developed the construct of peace of mind to describe
the affective well-being valued in Chinese culture. Peace of mind was defined as an
internal state of peacefulness and harmony. To develop a measure to assess peace of mind,
three studies were conducted. Study 1 developed the Peace of Mind Scale (PoM), Study 2
established its validity as an affective well-being measure, and Study 3 found that indi-
viduals from Chinese cultures score higher on this scale than those from Western cultures.
The results indicate that the PoM has good reliability and validity for measuring affective
well-being. The cross-cultural validation also found that Taiwanese individuals scored
higher on the PoM than European Americans, which provides further evidence of good
construct validity of the PoM.
Y.-C. Lee
National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
Y.-C. Lee (&)
Department of Psychology, Chung Yuan Christian University, 200, Chung Pei Rd., Chungli 32023,
Taiwan
e-mail: ycl@cycu.edu.tw
Y.-C. Lin
Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
C.-L. Huang
Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, National Taiwan University of Science and
Technology, Taipei, Taiwan
B. L. Fredrickson
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
123
J Happiness Stud (2013) 14:571–590
DOI 10.1007/s10902-012-9343-5
Keywords Peace of mind · Subjective well-being · Low-arousal positive affect ·
Chinese culture
1 Introduction
What constitutes “happiness” has long been debated (Ryff 1989; Ryan and Deci 2001).
Subjective well-being (SWB) has emerged as one of the most effective ways of measuring
happiness in everyday life (Ryan and Deci 2001; Kahneman et al. 1999). Derived from the
hedonic view of happiness, which emphasizes the experience of maximum levels of
pleasure, SWB has been indexed by the presence of positive affect (PA), absence of
negative affect (NA), and cognitive component of life satisfaction (Ryan and Deci 2001;
Kahneman et al. 1999). Of these components of SWB, affective SWB, which can be
understood as the extent to which one experiences PA over NA, most closely reflects the
hedonic view of happiness (Andrews and Robinson 1991; Andrews and Withey 1976;
Diener 2000). Affective SWB is measured by the Affect Balance Scale (ABS; Bradburn
1969), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al. 1988), and Daily
Mood Scale (Diener and Larsen 1984). In accordance with the hedonic view of happiness,
these measures only cover the moderate-arousal positive (MAP) affect (e.g., happy, joyful,
and pleased) and high-arousal positive (HAP) affect (e.g., excited, enthusiastic, and alert)
of positive feelings. Thus, higher affective SWB scores indicate more MAP and HAP
affects and less NA in daily life. Such measures of affective SWB have been administered
in various cultures in order to investigate the cultures that report being the happiest. These
studies found that Asians (or Asian Americans) had lower affective SWB scores than
Westerners (Diener et al. 1995; Okazaki 2000; Oishi 2001; Suh 2002).
Nonetheless, these results may not accurately reflect whether Asians (or Asian Amer-
icans) are actually less happy than Westerners. In fact, cultural influences can shape the
subjective meaning of happiness and thereby influence an individual’s emotional experi-
ence of happiness. Lu and Gilmour (2004) compared participants’ responses to the
question “what is happiness?” in Chinese and European Americans. With the exception of
some commonalities between the two groups regarding their concept of happiness (i.e.,
happiness as a mental state of satisfaction and contentment), large discrepancies were
found between cultures. For example, Chinese individuals often used the terms “harmony”
and “balance” (which in Chinese culture refers to a state of homeostasis both within one’s
mind as well as between an individual and his/her surroundings) to define happiness,
whereas European Americans never used these terms. In another study, Tsai et al. (2006)
investigated how culture influences ideal affect (how people want to feel) and found that
both Asian Americans and Hong Kong Chinese placed more value on the low-arousal
positive (LAP) affect (i.e., peaceful, calm) than European Americans. By contrast, Euro-
pean and Asian Americans placed more value on the HAP affect (i.e., excited, elated) than
the Hong Kong Chinese. The results provide evidence that people influenced by Eastern
and Western cultures value different positive feelings. In summary, the happiness valued
by Chinese individuals may well be different from the hedonic view of happiness that
emphasizes experiencing maximum levels of pleasure.
In addition, previous studies also found that the discrepancy between ideal and actual
affect (how people actually feel) correlates positively with depression and negatively with
life satisfaction (Kampfe and Mitte 2009; Tsai et al. 2006). Specifically, the further an
572 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
individual’s emotional experiences depart from the emotions he or she values, the lesser
well-being that individual experiences. As culture plays an important role in shaping ideal
affect, people who experience emotions that are in line with those valued by their culture
should enjoy an increased sense of well-being. Therefore, measuring the extent to which one
experiences MAP or HAP affects over NA should be an appropriate measure to reflect
affective well-being in Western cultures. However, applying these measures within cultures
that value LAP affects along with the mental states of “harmony” and “balance” could be
problematic. Given that measures of affective well-being derived from Chinese culture are
lacking, it is necessary to develop a new construct and measurement tool that appropriately
reflects Chinese’ affective well-being, one that reflects the importance of LAP affect and
mental harmony. Next, we review the ancient philosophical thought patterns on achieving
happiness—from Confucianism, Taoism, and Buddhism—that justify our approach.
2 Affective Well-being in Chinese Culture
2.1 Confucianism
Confucianism was founded by Confucius, who was the first Chinese philosopher to for-
mulate a thought system that provided guidance on managing life. In the representative
book of Confucianism, “Lun Yu” (論語), Confucius emphasized the importance of
benevolence (). Rather than fulfilling one’s desires, benevolence, defined as controlling
one’s desires to conform to the decorum valued by society, is encouraged more in the
Chinese culture. In another seminal book of Confucianism, “Zhong Yong” (), the
internal states of equilibrium and harmony were emphasized. The text states:
While there are no stirrings of pleasure, anger, sorrow, or joy, the mind may be said
to be in the state of equilibrium (). When those feelings have been stirred, and they
act in their due degree, there ensues what may be called the state of harmony ().
This equilibrium is the great root from which grow all the human actions in the
world, and this harmony is the universal path that they all should pursue (translation:
Wong and Leung 2001).
To summarize, in Confucianism, behavioral ideals require the individual not only to
control his/her desires in order to comply with the pivotal virtue of benevolence but also to
maintain equilibrium, a peaceful state of mind without overly positive or negative emo-
tions, in their mental state.
2.2 Taoism
The main idea of Taoism is Tao (), which can be understood in many different ways. It
can be seen as the mystical power of the universe, the way in which the world functions, or
the way in which people speak about reality (Kohn 2001). Lao-tzu was the philosopher
who first introduced the concept of Tao. In his book “Tao Te Ching” (), he
described Tao as a state of perfect harmony. The Taoist harmony is largely based on the
Chinese yin-yang theory. Yin and Yang stand for the paired opposites in the world (e.g.,
light/dark; good/bad; hot/cold), and the Taoist harmony captures the dynamics of balance
between opposing elements (Delle et al. 2011; Lee et al. 2009). To maintain harmony, Lao-
tzu taught people to accept the way things are:
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 573
123
The Tao never does anything, yet through it all things are done. If people could
center themselves in it, the whole world would be transformed by itself, in its natural
rhythms. People would be content with their simple, everyday lives, in harmony, and
free of desire. When there is no desire, all things are at peace (translation: Mitchell
1988).
The text clearly describes the ideal lifestyle and mental state valued by Lao-tzu. In
contrast to the hedonic view of happiness, which encourages people to pursue maximum
levels of pleasure, the ultimate goal of Taoism is to achieve inner peace by maintaining
internal harmony.
2.3 Buddhism
Buddhism originated in India, where Gautama Buddha was enlightened under the Bodhi
tree and then set forth to spread his message of enlightenment, compassion, and peace.
Buddhist teachings and practices have greatly influenced Chinese culture and religion for
millennia (Mitchell 2002). The fundamental concerns of Buddhism are as follows: To
identify the inner causes of suffering, embrace the possibility of freedom from suffering,
and explore the means by which one can realize such freedom (Wallace and Shapiro 2006).
Through Buddhist practice, people could gradually approach the ultimate goal of life.
Gautama Buddha described the ultimate goal, Nirvana, as the complete cessation of
craving (Mitchell 2002). Another word the Buddha used to describe Nirvana was peace-
fulness, which refers to a status of being “unmoved” by life events that would ordinarily
shift one into negative states of mind (Mitchell 2002). It was believed that even though a
nirvanic person is not immune to things such as sickness or death, he or she can experience
a “peaceful and calm” state of mind when facing ills. Therefore, according to Buddhism,
maintaining inner peacefulness is the ideal mental state that one should long for.
As can be seen, all three schools of ancient teaching that have influenced Chinese
culture emphasize the importance of maintaining inner peace and harmony. This accords
with previous findings that Chinese people value LAP affect (Tsai et al. 2006) and a
harmonious state of happiness (Lu and Gilmour 2004). Therefore, we introduce a new
construct, peace of mind, to describe the type of affective well-being valued within Chinese
culture. Peace of mind is characterized by the affective states of internal peace and har-
mony. The experience of internal peace captures the states of mind included in the LAP
affect (e.g., peaceful, calm, and serenity), whereas internal harmony captures the states of
mind that include balance and harmony. According to previous research, emotions can be
differentiated into “cognitive-independent” and “cognitive-dependent” emotions (see
reviews of Niedenthal et al. 2006). The former indicates the set of “basic” emotions that
are fundamental elements of emotional life, such as emotions classified in the affect
circumplex (e.g., LAP affect); the latter indicates more complicated emotions that rely on
cognitive evaluation before they emerge. We further regard internal peace as a cognitive-
independent emotion because it includes emotions that cover LAP affect. On the other
hand, in Taoism, harmony stands for the dynamic balance between opposing elements (Lee
et al. 2009) which we suggest depends on cognitive evaluation, and as such, we regard
internal harmony as reflective of cognitive-dependent emotions. We suggest that the two
aspects of PoM are reciprocally related such that people can either achieve internal peace
through the process of harmony, or achieve inner harmony through maintaining a peaceful
state of mind. We therefore suggest that individuals with peace of mind would experience
both internal peace and harmony as one coherent state.
574 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
The aim of the three studies presented is to develop and test a new measure that can
accurately assess the construct of peace of mind as a measure of affective well-being. In
Study 1, items of the Peace of Mind Scale (PoM) were generated on the basis of the definition
of peace of mind. Both the factor structure and psychometric properties of the PoM were also
tested. In Study 2, the PoM and conceptually related self-report measures were administered
to examine the criterion-related and discriminant validity of the PoM. In Study 3, a cross-
cultural study was designed to further examine the construct validity of the PoM.
3 Study 1: Development of the Peace of Mind Scale
3.1 Method
3.1.1 Participants
Two sample groups were recruited for Study 1. Sample A, which included a total of 239
undergraduate students (90 men and 149 women) from Chung Yuan Christian University,
1
was recruited to explore the factor structure of the PoM. The ages of the participants
ranged from 18 to 29, with a mean age of 20. Sample B was a student sample recruited to
perform confirmatory factor analysis. Sample B included a total of 139 undergraduate
students (69 men and 70 women) from National Taiwan University (NTU). Participants
ranged in age from 18 to 26, with a mean age of 20.5.
3.1.2 Materials
3.1.2.1 Peace of Mind To develop a self-report measure of peace of mind, we began by
generating items for the PoM written in Chinese. Based on the definition of peace of mind,
which was defined as the extent to which one experiences inner peace and harmony, a list of
emotional words related to peace and harmony was collected to facilitate item generation.
The word list included words collected from the circumplex model of LAP emotions
(Russell 1980; Tsai et al. 2006; Watson and Tellegen 1985), including peaceful, serene,
calm, at ease, and comfortable. Words such as harmonious, balanced, or settled were also
included to reflect the internal harmony element of the peace of mind construct. Given our
view that people with peace of mind experience both internal peace and internal harmony at
the same time, instead of targeting the specific meanings of peace and harmony within
different items, each item was designed to capture internal peace conjoined with internal
harmony. Additionally, unlike the measure of actual LAP affect that uses emotional
adjectives (Tsai et al. 2006), we used sentences that included words describing both internal
peace and internal harmony. Using the word list, a total of 10 sentence items were drafted to
reflect the experiences of internal peace and harmony in general terms (e.g., I have peace
and harmony in my mind) as well as in everyday circumstances (e.g., I feel content and
comfortable with myself in daily life). Because words related to both peace and harmony
were included in each item, a single factor model was predicted. Because we believe that the
PoM measures affective well-being, and frequency scales have been demonstrated as being
useful for capturing affective well-being (Diener et al. 1991), we chose frequency assess-
ment for the PoM. The instructions ask participants to indicate how often they experience
1
Chung-Yuan Christian University is one of the general universities in Taiwan. Students are neither
required to be members of the religion nor expected to study or practice the tenets of Christianity.
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 575
123
the internal states described in each of the items on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (all the time).
All participants in the two samples were administered the PoM in group settings.
3.2 Results and Discussion
3.2.1 Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA)
EFA was conducted using the 10 items we generated. The principal axis factoring method
was chosen for factor extraction. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy
(KMO) was .91. The number of factors to be extracted was determined by inspection of the
scree plot of eigenvalues, which revealed a single factor solution. The first factor accounted
for 63.58 % of the total variation, with an eigenvalue of 6.36. The second factor accounted
for 9.95 % of the total variation, with an eigenvalue of 1.00. EFA using the maximum-
likelihood method for factor extraction also showed a strong single factor structure and
similar factor loadings. Thus, only items with high factor loadings on the first factor were
retained. One item was dropped due to a low factor loading. In addition, because of the
high semantic similarity of some of those items,
2
two items were deleted on the basis of
high inter-item correlations (r[.8) resulting in a seven-item scale. Item characteristics for
the seven-item PoM are presented in Table 1. All of the factor loadings derived from
principal axis factoring estimation were above .30, and the average factor loading was .78.
The item–total correlations ranged from .76 to .85, and the average item–total correlation
was .81. The mean for scores on the scale was 3.19, and the mean standard deviation was
.91. The alpha reliability coefficient of the PoM was .91.
3.2.2 Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)
CFA was conducted using data from Sample B to test the single-factor model revealed by
the exploratory analysis. We chose maximum-likelihood estimation and the LISREL 8.80
program (Joreskog and Sorbom 1996) to conduct the CFA. Because the sample size was
small, a Satorra-Bentler (SB) scaled Chi-squared statistic, which was considered as a
robust estimator in small sample size, was used (Satorra and Bentler 2001). In addition, fit
Table 1 Means, standard deviations, factor loadings, and item–total correlations for the PoM
M SD F I–T
1. My mind is free and at ease 3.32 .86 .84 .83
2. I feel content and comfortable with myself in daily life 3.31 .82 .79 .81
3. My lifestyle gives me feelings of peace and stability 3.16 .91 .78 .82
4. I have peace and harmony in my mind 3.11 .85 .84 .83
5. It is difficult for me to feel settled (Reverse) 3.13 1.06 .69 .76
6. The way I live brings me feelings of peace and comfort 3.24 .89 .82 .85
7. I feel anxious and uneasy in my mind (Reverse) 3.08 .98 .69 .76
Total score 3.19 .91
Ffactor loadings, ITitem–total correlations
2
The items of the PoM were originally written in Chinese. In order to develop the English version of the
PoM for the Study 3, semantic similarity of items was considered to avoid duplicity which can improve the
equivalence of translation.
576 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
indices including root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit
index (CFI), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) were used to evaluate
the fit of each model. General cutoffs for model acceptance, including RMSEA less than or
equal to .05, CFI greater than or equal to .95, and SRMR less than .08, were adopted (Hu
and Bentler 1999). The fit indices of the model indicated that the correspondence between
the single-factor model and the sample covariance matrix was satisfactory (RMSEA =.00;
CFI =1; IFI =1.00, SRMR =.03; SB-X
2
(14) =11.68, p[.05). All seven items of the
scale were significantly related to the latent factor (all ps\.01). The alpha reliability
coefficient was .94.
In summary, the PoM was found to be a measure with good factor structure and
reliability. In Study 2, we further tested the validity of the PoM. In order to establish the
criterion-related validity, several measures that were expected to be highly correlated with
the PoM were chosen as validation criteria, including conventional indices of SWB,
psychological ill-being indices, and LAP affect. The discriminant validity of the PoM
versus measures of related constructs was also tested using LAP affect or psychological ill-
being indices as validation criteria to show that the PoM differs significantly from con-
ventional indices of SWB and LAP affect.
4 Study 2: Criterion-Related and Discriminant Validity
4.1 Method
4.1.1 Participants
A total of 137 undergraduate students (69 men and 68 women) from NTU completed a
packet of questionnaires that were written in Chinese in two waves over a 2-week period
during a general psychology course. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 32, with a mean
age of 19.5 years.
4.1.2 Materials
4.1.2.1 Peace of Mind The seven-item PoM developed in Study 1 was administered in
this study. The alpha reliability (α=.90) and test–retest reliability over a 2-week period
(r=.75, p\.05) of the PoM was adequate in the current sample.
4.1.2.2 Positive and Negative Affect This assessment was used to measure conventional
indices of affective SWB, including affect balance (Bradburn 1969), positivity ratio
(Fredrickson and Losada 2005), and LAP affect (Tsai et al. 2006). The affect assessment
that we used was primarily based on the items developed by Tsai et al. (2006); however,
additional items were added according to the items listed in the affect circumplex in order
to represent the emotions in each octant (Larsen and Diener 1992; Russell 1980; Ressell
and Carroll 1999; Watson and Tellegen 1985). The assessment construct was chosen
because it fairly reflects the positive and negative affect of East Asians as it captures low
arousal affect. As shown in Table 2, a total of 24 items was used to sample six of the
octants of the affect circumplex. Octants of high arousal neutral affect (e.g., aroused,
surprised) and low arousal neutral affect (e.g., quiet, still) were not included in the
assessment because the main concerns of the present work focus on affect with positive or
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 577
123
negative valence. Again, frequency assessment was chosen to capture affective well-being
(Diener et al. 1991); participants were instructed to indicate how often they had experi-
enced each of the emotions during the past month on a scale of 1 (never) to 5 (always). The
internal consistency in each octant was satisfactory (see Table 2).
Using this assessment, the affect balance score was calculated by subtracting the
summation score of NA from PA. For the positivity ratio, following the scoring procedures
reported by Fredrickson and Losada (2005), we used different thresholds for different
valences by tallying the number of positive emotions that were experienced at least often
(3) and the number of negative emotions experienced at least sometimes (2). Then, the
positivity ratio was calculated by the ratio of the number tallied for PA over the number
tallied for NA. In addition, an index of LAP affect was also calculated by averaging the
scores on items measuring LAP affect.
4.1.2.3 Satisfaction with Life The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al.
1985) was employed to measure participants’ global evaluation of life satisfaction, the
cognitive component of SWB. The SWLS consists of five items. The items included, for
example, “In most ways my life is close to my ideal” and “So far I have gotten the
important things I want in life.” Participants were asked to indicate agreement with each
item on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The alpha reliability was .88
in the current sample.
4.1.2.4 Happiness The short version (20 items) of the Chinese Happiness Inventory
(CHI; Lu and Shih 1997) was used to reflect the general SWB in Chinese culture. The
development of CHI was primarily based on the Oxford Happiness Inventory (OHI; Argyle
et al. 1989); however, it includes items that describe the aspects of Chinese happiness that
are not covered by the OHI. CHI is a composite index of SWB as it reflects PA, (lack of)
NA, and life satisfaction. Example items were “I feel I have a worse life than others” and “I
am never in a state of joy or elation.” The alpha reliability was .91 in the current sample.
4.1.2.5 Depression The 20-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-
D) scale was employed to measure depressive symptoms in the general population (Radloff
1977). The items were, for example, “I was bothered by things that usually don’t bother
me” and “People were unfriendly.” Participants were asked to rate how often they had
experienced each item in the last week, using a 1 (hardly) to 4 (most of the time) scale. The
alpha reliability of the scale was .88 in the current sample.
4.1.2.6 Anxiety The current study used the state format of the State Trait Anxiety
Inventory (STAI; Spielberger 1983), which consists of 20 items. The state format was
Table 2 Items of the affect assessment and internal consistency in each octant
Valence Octant Items Alpha
Positive affect High-arousal positive (HAP) Excited, enthusiastic, energetic, elated .80
Moderate-arousal positive (MAP) Happy, satisfied, content, delighted .84
Low-arousal positive (LAP) Calm, tranquil, serene, relaxed .80
Negative affect High-arousal negative (HAN) Nervous, fearful, hostile, distressed .85
Moderate-arousal negative (MAN) Sad, lonely, blue, gloomy .79
Low-arousal negative (LAN) Bored, tired, sleepy, sluggish .67
578 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
chosen because we assumed that the PoM captures the extent to which people maintain
internal peace and harmony in response to daily events, and the state form of STAI should
best reflect the opposite nature of maintaining peace of mind. Participants were asked to
indicate the strength of their feelings at the moment, using a scale of 1 (not at all) to 4 (very
much so). The items were, for example, “I feel secure” and “I feel nervous.” The alpha
reliability of the scale was .91 in the current sample.
4.1.2.7 Entrapment The 16-item entrapment scale (Gilbert and Allan 1998) was given to
participants. This scale measures the extent to which people have a strong motivation to
take flight from things triggered by internal feelings or external world events; however, this
motivation is blocked. The measure was chosen because it provides an important aspect of
psychological health, which is distinct from depression and anxiety. Example items were “I
would like to escape from my thoughts and feelings” and “I feel powerless to change
things.” Participants were asked to indicate the degree to which the items represented their
thoughts and feelings, using a scale of 1 (not at all like me) to 5 (extremely like me). The
alpha reliability of the scale was .95 in the current sample.
4.1.2.8 Social Desirability A short version (10 items) of the Taiwanese Social Desir-
ability Scale (Liao 2000) was given to participants. This scale was developed on the basis
of the construct of the Marlowe-Crown social desirability scale (Crowne and Marlowe
1960), and the items were generated by Taiwanese participants to reflect the tendency for
social desirability in Taiwanese culture. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to
which they agree with each item, using a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly
agree). The items included, for example, “I make excuses for my mistakes sometimes” and
“I pry into the private business of others sometimes.” The alpha reliability of the scale was
.75 in the current sample.
4.2 Results and Discussion
4.2.1 Criterion-Related Validity
In order to establish the criterion-related validity of the PoM, measures of SWB, psycho-
logical ill-being, and LAP affect were chosen as criterion-related validation indices.
Correlations between the PoM and these measures are presented in Table 3. As expected,
the PoM was found to have highly positive correlations with indices of affective well-being,
including the affect balance score and the positivity ratio. The correlation coefficients
between the PoM and indices of affective well-being were high enough to demonstrate that
they are similar constructs, but modest enough to demonstrate that they are not measuring
the same construct. Moreover, as expected, the PoM was found to have significant positive
correlations with the cognitive aspect of SWB, measured by the SWLS, and the composite
index of SWB, measured by CHI. The results indicate that people who score high on the
PoM also experience high life satisfaction and happiness within their daily lives.
In addition to the relatedness between the PoM and SWB measures, indices of psy-
chological ill-being were expected to negatively correlate with the score on the PoM. As
predicted, the PoM was found to have significant negative correlations with the tendency
for depression, state anxiety, and entrapment. In summary, the results show that the PoM
can be seen as an important index for mental health because it effectively predicted both
positive and negative aspects of psychological well-being.
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 579
123
In the circumplex model of affect, affective adjectives can be categorized into different
octants (Russell 1980; Tsai et al. 2006; Watson and Tellegen 1985). As shown in Table 3,
significant positive correlations were found between the PoM and the three octants of PA.
Among those octants, as expected, LAP showed the highest correlations with the PoM,
followed by MAP, whereas HAP showed only a moderate correlation with the PoM.
Furthermore, significant negative correlations were found between the PoM and the three
octants of NA. Among those octants, the correlation between the PoM and moderate-
arousal negative (MAN) showed the highest negative correlation in comparison with high-
arousal negative (HAN) affect and low-arousal negative (LAN) affect. The results show
that people who score high on the PoM are less likely to experience NA in their daily lives.
In summary, these results show that people who have higher peace of mind experience
positive emotions more frequently and experience negative emotions less frequently.
In order to ensure that the PoM was not contaminated by social desirability, a Pearson
correlation coefficient was calculated between the scores of the PoM and the Taiwanese
Table 3 Correlations of the PoM with related measures
PoM SWLS CHI Affect
balance
Positivity
ratio
Indices of subjective well-being
Satisfaction with Life Scale
(SWLS)
.54***
(.47***)
Chinese Happiness
Inventory (CHI)
.52***
(.49***)
.55***
(.51***)
Affect balance (PA – NA) .65***
(.63***)
.57***
(.54***)
.61***
(.59***)
Positivity ratio (PA/NA) .47***
(.44***)
.20* (.15) .43***
(.41***)
.65***
(.63***)
Frequency judgment of
emotions
1. High-arousal Positive
(HAP)
.32** (.32**) .34***
(.35***)
.46***
(.46***)
.66***
(.66***)
.42***
(.42***)
2. Moderate-arousal
positive (MAP)
.49***
(.49***)
.54***
(.54***)
.55***
(.55***)
.76***
(.76***)
.45***
(.44***)
3. Low-arousal Positive
(LAP)
.60***
(.57***)
.37***
(.31**)
.40***
(.36***)
.41***
(.38***)
.21* (.17)
4. High-arousal Negative
(HAN)
.50***
(.45***)
.34***
(.27**)
.30***
(.26**)
.63***
(.61***)
.45***
(.42***)
5. Moderate-arousal
negative (MAN)
.52***
(.48***)
.39***
(.34***)
.41***
(.38***)
.77***
(.76***)
.51***
(.49***)
6. Low-arousal Negative
(LAN)
.32**
(.22*)
.36***
(.27**)
.27**
(.21*)
.46***
(.42***)
.22** (.16)
Indices of psychological ill-being
CES-D .58***
(.54***)
.43***
(.37***)
.44***
(.40***)
.52***
(.48***)
.35***
(.32***)
STAI-state .57***
(.52***)
.31**
(.24**)
.24**
(.19*)
.35***
(.31***)
.32***
(.28**)
Entrapment Scale .61***
(
.58***)
.43***
(.37***)
.65***
(.63***)
.61***
(.58***)
.48***
(.45***)
*** p\.001, ** p\.01, * p\.05. Values in parentheses are correlation coefficients after controlling for
the effects of social desirability
580 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
social desirability scale. The results showed that the PoM was positively correlated with
social desirability (r=.35, p\.001). In order to control the effects influenced by social
desirability, we calculated the correlation coefficients between the PoM and indices of SWB
and psychological ill-being when the effects of social desirability are controlled (see Table 3).
The pattern of correlations largely remained unchanged: The PoM was still highly correlated
with indices of SWB and psychological ill-being after controlling for shared variance with
social desirability. These findings suggest that, although experiencing peace of mind can be
regarded as a socially desirable state by Chinese people, the PoM still showed good criterion-
related validity when social desirability was factored out. Finally, gender differences in the
PoM were also examined, and no gender differences were found (t=.54, p[.05).
4.2.2 Discriminant Validity
In order to establish the discriminant validity of the PoM, we first investigated whether the
PoM has a stronger association with LAP affect than the conventional SWB measures did.
Table 3shows that even after the effects of social desirability were controlled, the cor-
relation coefficient between the PoM and LAP affect is higher than the coefficients
between conventional indices of SWB (including affect balance, positivity ratio, SWLS,
and CHI) and LAP affect. The results show that although all the conventional measures of
SWB, especially affect balance and positivity ratio, are conceptually related to LAP affect,
the PoM shares more variance with LAP affect than with the other indices. In addition, we
used multiple regression analysis to examine the unique contribution of the PoM when
predicting LAP affect in comparison to conventional SWB measures. Table 4shows that,
when predicting LAP affect, the regression coefficient of the PoM remains significant even
when the effects of the other SWB measures were controlled, whereas the coefficients for
all other SWB measures become non-significant. These results further indicate that the
PoM is a measure of well-being that predicts low-arousal positive affect better than con-
ventional measures of SWB.
The incremental validity of the PoM was assessed by hierarchical regression analysis to
examine whether the PoM would add to the prediction of psychological ill-being above and
beyond the existing measures that evaluate similar constructs, including conventional
measures of SWB and LAP affect. Three series of regressions were conducted, each
predicting one of the three psychological ill-being indices, including depression, state
anxiety, and entrapment. For each analysis, predictors were entered in three steps. In Step
1, age, gender, and social desirability were entered as control variables. Subsequently, in
Step 2, we entered one of the indices that shares conceptual space with the PoM (i.e., the
SWLS, CHI, affect balance, positivity ratio, and LAP affect). Following this, in Step 3, we
entered the PoM.
Table 5presents the summary of all the regression models. In all the regression models,
the change statistics (R
2
change and Fchange) of the PoM reached the .001 significance
level, which reveals that the PoM significantly predicted each of the psychological ill-
being indices after the variances of gender, age, and social desirability were controlled in
Step 1, and one of the related constructs was controlled in Step 2. The results show that the
PoM can explain important variance in each psychological ill-being index above and
beyond conventional measures of SWB, including SWLS, CHI, affect balance, and posi-
tivity ratio, which strengthens the evidence that the PoM is a distinct and valuable measure
of affective well-being. In addition, the PoM predicts psychological ill-being indices more
powerfully than LAP affect, whereas the PoM can significantly predict all of the
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 581
123
psychological ill-being indices when the effects of LAP affect were controlled (see
Table 5); LAP affect does not reliably predict indices of psychological ill-being when the
effects of the PoM are controlled (CESD: β=.03, ns; STAI-state: β=.16, p\.05;
Entrapment: β=.03, ns.). These results further demonstrate that even though the PoM
shares conceptual space with LAP affect, it captures independent variances when pre-
dicting psychological ill-being and is arguably a better index of well-being than LAP
affect.
In addition to demonstrating the criterion-related and discriminant validity of the PoM,
a cross-cultural study was performed in Study 3 to provide further evidence of the con-
struct validity of the PoM. Since peace of mind is a construct of well-being especially
emphasized in Chinese culture, we expected that people from a Chinese culture would
experience peace of mind more frequently than people from Western culture.
5 Study 3: Cross-Cultural Validation of the Peace of Mind Scale
5.1 Method
5.1.1 Participants
European American and Taiwanese participants were selected to represent people from
Western and Chinese cultures, respectively. The cultural values of Chinese culture can be
ascribed to Taiwanese participants because Taiwan and China have been culturally bonded
for hundreds of years. Taiwanese participants included 84 (42 men and 42 women)
undergraduate students from NTU. All of the Taiwanese participants were included in the
final data because they were all born and raised in Taiwan. The age of the participants
ranged from 19 to 23, with a mean age of 20.2. Participants from the United States were
140 undergraduate students from the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. In order
to exclude influences from non-Western cultures, only those who were born and raised in
the United States or Canada and whose race/ethnicity was white/Caucasian were selected
for data analysis. A total of 91 European American participants (29 men and 62 women)
were included in the final data analysis. The ages of the participants ranged from 18 to 23,
with a mean age of 18.9.
5.1.2 Item Translation of the PoM
Because the items of the PoM were originally written in Chinese, they were translated into
English before administration in the US. The translation work involved translating the
Chinese version of the PoM into English and then back translating it into Chinese. This was
done by two bilingual Chinese-English speakers (one born in Taiwan and raised in the US,
Table 4 Standardized regression
coefficients in multiple
regressions predicting the LAP
affect from the PoM and
conventional measures of SWB
βtp
PoM .54 5.71 \.001
Affect balance .11 .98 .330
Positivity ratio .14 1.52 .131
SWLS .15 1.60 .111
CHI .03 .33 .745
582 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
Table 5 Correlations of the PoM with indices of psychological ill-being before and after controlling for
related constructs
Step Predictors Final βR
2
R
change
2
F
change
Sig. of F
change
CES-D
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .05, .09, .07 .10 .10 4.95 \.01
2 SWLS .17 .24 .14 23.63 \.001
3 PoM .48*** .38 .14 29.49 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .04, .10, .11 .11 .11 4.79 \.01
2 CHI .18* .26 .15 23.91 \.001
3 PoM .47*** .40 .14 27.52 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .04, .09, .10 .10 .10 4.95 \.01
2 Affect balance .28** .33 .23 46.19 \.001
3 PoM .37*** .40 .07 15.49 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .03, .07, .09 .09 .09 4.51 \.01
2 Positivity ratio .11 .19 .10 15.52 \.001
3 PoM .49*** .36 .17 33.71 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .05, .07, .10 .10 .10 4.95 \.01
2 LAP .04 .17 .07 10.53 \.01
3 PoM .57*** .36 .19 39.40 \.001
STAI-state
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .03, .07, .08 .08 .08 4.07 \.01
2 SWLS .01 .14 .05 8.24 \.01
3 PoM .53*** .32 .18 34.80 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .05, .12, .10 .10 .10 4.30 \.01
2 CHI .08 .14 .04 5.45 \.05
3 PoM .59*** .37 .23 42.35 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .03, .07, .08 .08 .08 4.07 \.01
2 Affect Balance .06 .20 .11 18.42 \.001
3 PoM .49*** .32 .12 23.98 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .02, .07, .08 .08 .08 3.60 \.05
2 Positivity ratio .08 .15 .07 11.39 \.01
3 PoM .43*** .31 .16 29.23 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .004, .06, .08 .08 .08 4.07 \.01
2 LAP .19* .24 .16 27.34 \.001
3 PoM .41*** .34 .10 20.19 \.001
Entrapment Scale
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .12, .04, .07 .12 .12 6.11 \.01
2 SWLS .14 .26 .14 24.06 \.001
3 PoM .52*** .43 .17 38.95 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .04, .02, .03 .09 .09 3.87 \.05
2 CHI .45*** .46 .37 80.33 \.001
3 PoM .37*** .55 .09 23.57 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .11, .04, .09 .12 .12 6.11 \.01
2 Affect balance .31*** .39 .27 57.81 \.001
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 583
123
the other born and raised in Singapore; both were currently living in the US). Disagree-
ments between translators were resolved by discussion.
5.1.3 Materials and Procedure
Taiwanese participants completed the PoM in Chinese with a paper-based questionnaire.
Participants from the United States completed the PoM in English through online surveys
in two waves at an interval of 2 weeks.
5.2 Results and Discussion
5.2.1 Mean-Level Differences with Similar Factor Structure
Before comparing the mean scores on the PoM, EFA was performed to ensure a similar
factor structure of the PoM across the two cultural groups. The principal axis factoring
method was used for factor extraction. In the Taiwanese sample, the same single factor
structure of Study 1 was found. All seven items of the PoM loaded on the same latent
variable, and the alpha reliability of the seven-item scale was .88. However, in the US
sample, two latent factors were found using the scores on the PoM measured at time 1. Five
items loaded high on the first factor, whereas the two reverse-scored items (item 5 and item
7) loaded high on the second factor. The results indicated that the US sample may have
understood the two negatively-worded items differently from the other five items. In order
to have a compatible factor structure across the two groups, we excluded the two items and
compared the mean score using the new, five-item PoM. Table 6shows the item charac-
teristics for the five-item PoM in the two samples. The alpha reliability of the five-item
PoM was .88 in the Taiwanese sample and .90 in the US sample. The test–retest reliability
of the US sample over a 2-week interval was also good (r=.78, p\.001). The ttest
statistic between the two mean scores was significant (t=3.59, p\.001), revealing that
Taiwanese participants scored higher than European Americans on the PoM.
5.2.2 Mean-Level Differences with Measurement Invariance
In order to compare the mean scores of the PoM in the two cultures using stricter criteria,
we conducted means and covariance structure analysis (MACS; see Cheung and Rensvold
Table 5 continued
Step Predictors Final βR
2
R
change
2
F
change
Sig. of F
change
3 PoM .39*** .47 .08 19.22 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .10, .03, .09 .11 .11 5.40 \.01
2 Positivity ratio .21** .27 .16 28.88 \.001
3 PoM .48*** .43 .16 36.38 \.001
1 Gender, age, & social desirability .12, .02, .09 .12 .12 6.11 \.01
2 LAP .03 .20 .08 12.56 \.01
3 PoM .61*** .41 .22 48.52 \.001
*** p\.001, ** p\.01, * p\.05. CES-D Center for Epidemiological Study-Depression Scale,
STAI-state State Trait Anxiety Inventory-state subscale, Sig. of F
Change
significance of F
Change
584 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
1999; Little 1997; Vandenberg 2002; Whiteside-Mansell and Corwyn 2003). The purpose
of applying MACS is to test whether the participants from different cultural groups
ascribed the same meanings to the survey items (Cheung and Rensvold 1999). When
comparing the PoM in different cultural groups, MACS can provide fit indices for three
nested models at different constraint levels to test different levels of measurement
equivalence (Cheung and Rensvold 1999). We used LISREL 8.80 (Joreskog and Sorbom
1996) to perform MACS. Because of the small sample size of the present data, we used a
Satorra-Bentler (SB) scaled Chi-squared statistic to evaluate the fit of each model. We also
conducted the difference tests for comparing nested models using the SB scaled Chi-
squared statistics (Satorra and Bentler 2001). In addition to the Chi-squared test, we used
fit indices of RMSEA, CFI, and SRMR. We used the following general cutoffs for model
acceptance: less than or equal to .05 for the RMSEA, greater than or equal to .95 for the
CFI, and less than .08 for the SRMR (Hu and Bentler 1999).
We began by examining the fit of the baseline model. The fit indices of the five-item
PoM across the two cultural groups supported the baseline model, which showed that the
structure was similar for the two groups (RMSEA =.00; CFI =1; SRMR =.04;SB-X
2
(10) =7.44, p[.05). Next, equality of factor loadings was imposed to test the weak
factorial invariance model. The fit indices of the model also revealed acceptable fit
(RMSEA =.00; CFI =1; SRMR =.06; SB-X
2
(14) =12.29, p[.05). The Chi-squared
difference test between the baseline model and the weak invariance model was not sig-
nificant (SB-X
2
(4) =8.12, p[.05), revealing that weak invariance was supported.
This indicated that the factor structures of the two cultural groups were similar. Then,
equality of intercepts across time was imposed to test strong factorial invariance. Some fit
indices of the strong invariance model showed acceptable fit (CFI =.98; SRMR =.07);
however, the indices of RMSEA and SB-X
2
test rejected the model (RMSEA =.10; SB-X
2
(18) =33.84, p\.05). The Chi-squared difference test between the weak invariance and
strong invariance was significant (SB-X
2
(4) =79.87, p\.01), showing that strong
invariance was not supported.
We then tested the partial strong factorial invariance by freeing constraints of the strong
factorial invariance until the partial strong invariance model did not have significant dif-
ferences from the weak invariance model. The purpose of this method was to find items
that are eligible for the mean level comparison test for the present study. Through this
procedure, the intercept constraint of item 1 (see Table 6) was dropped according to the
modification index, and the other four items were retained in the partial strong invariance
Table 6 Means, standard deviations, factor loadings, and item–total correlations for the PoM in the U.S.
and Taiwan
U.S. Taiwan
M SD F I–T M SD F I–T
1. My mind is free and at ease 2.45 0.89 .71 .78 3.21 0.84 .69 .75
2. I feel content and comfortable with myself in daily life 3.22 0.90 .76 .82 3.32 0.82 .76 .79
3. My lifestyle gives me feelings of peace and stability 2.91 1.13 .81 .86 3.37 0.89 .81 .83
4. I have peace and harmony in my mind 2.74 1.05 .89 .90 3.14 0.79 .73 .77
6. The way I live brings me feelings of peace and comfort 2.97 1.10 .84 .87 3.36 0.82 .79 .81
Total score 2.86 0.86 3.23 0.69
Ffactor loadings, ITitem–total correlations
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 585
123
model. The fit indices of the partial strong invariance model were satisfactory
(RMSEA =.002; CFI =1; SRMR =.05; SB-X
2
(17) =17.01, p[.05), and the Chi-
squared difference test between the weak invariance model and partial strong factorial
model was not significant (SB-X
2
(3) =7.04, p[.05), revealing that partial strong
invariance was supported. The latent factor mean of Taiwanese (M=3.35, SD =.48) and
European Americans (M=2.99, SD =.82) was compared on the basis of the partial strong
invariance model. The ttest statistic between the two means was significant (t=3.61,
p\.001), revealing that Taiwanese have higher scores than European Americans on at
least four items of the PoM.
In summary, the mean level of the PoM was higher among Taiwanese than among
European Americans, regardless of comparing scores on the five-item PoM that have
similar factor structures or scores on the four-item PoM selected through constraining
measurement invariance across groups. Even with only four or five items, the PoM still
capture the core ideas of peace of mind because each item of the PoM captures both the
internal states of peacefulness and harmony. These results provide evidence for the cultural
specificity of the PoM.
6 General Discussion
The purpose of the present work was to develop a valid and reliable measure that accu-
rately assesses the construct of peace of mind as an affective well-being measure that is
sensitive to Chinese cultural values regarding happiness. Study 1 provided evidence for the
psychometric properties of the newly developed PoM. The PoM was found to have reliable
factor structures across different samples. In Study 2, we further demonstrated that the
PoM had high criterion-related validity and good discriminant validity. The PoM was
highly correlated with indices of SWB, and psychological ill-being and was shown to
uniquely contribute to the LAP affect even when the effects of the conventional measures
of SWB were controlled for. The results indicate that the PoM can be regarded as a
measure of well-being that subsumes the low-arousal aspects of positive affect and is
independent from the conventional measures of SWB. In Study 3, the cross-cultural val-
idation of the PoM showed that Taiwanese participants scored higher than European
American participants on the PoM, which suggests that the PoM reflects a form of affective
well-being that is valued by Chinese cultures. Taken together, these results provide evi-
dence for the reliability and construct validity of the PoM to capture the harmonious and
low-arousal aspects of affective well-being that is neglected by conventional measures of
SWB.
6.1 Peace of Mind Differs from Hedonic Happiness
A fundamental argument of the present work is that happiness as valued by people
influenced by Chinese culture is very different from the hedonic view of happiness. In
order to complement the conventional indices of affective SWB, which consider happiness
to be a state where pleasure is maximized and pain is minimized, we developed the PoM as
a measure of the experiences of internal peace together with harmony. Using the LAP
affect as a validation criterion, the present work also evidenced that peace of mind is
distinct from hedonic pleasure. When controlling for the effects of the conventional
measures of SWB that reflect hedonic happiness, including affective, cognitive, and
586 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
composite indices of well-being, the PoM contributes uniquely to the prediction of the LAP
affect. By contrast, all the conventional measures of SWB do not predict the LAP affect
above and beyond the PoM. In addition, the results show that, when controlling for con-
ventional measures of SWB, the PoM uniquely contributes to the prediction of
psychological ill-being. This suggests not only that the PoM is a robust index of well-being
but also that the association between PoM and psychological ill-being is not fully
explained by conventional measures of SWB. The results further support our argument that
hedonic happiness, as captured by conventional measures of SWB, does not sufficiently
reflect well-being within cultures influenced by Chinese values. Therefore, the PoM
contributes distinct and valuable information regarding the contours of affective well-being
in such cultures.
6.2 Peace of Mind is More than Low-Arousal Positive Affect
Inspired by the cross-cultural finding that Chinese and Asian Americans value the LAP
affect to a greater degree than European Americans (Tsai et al. 2006), the emotions
included in the LAP affect were adopted as part of the definition of peace of mind and
subsequently in the development of the PoM. Due to the high correlation between the
construct of peace of mind and the LAP affect, the LAP affect was used as a validation
criterion for the PoM. The results not only show the PoM to be highly correlated with the
LAP affect but also reveal several distinctions between the two measures. First, the con-
struct of peace of mind includes internal states of peacefulness together with harmony. By
contrast, although Tsai et al.’s (2006) measure of the LAP affect includes items related to
peacefulness (e.g., peacefulness, serenity, relaxation), it does not include items related to
harmony (e.g., harmony, balance, settled). Therefore, the PoM reflects a more complex and
balanced state of mind. Second, unlike the measure of the LAP affect, which uses emo-
tional adjectives to capture the extent to which it is experienced (Tsai et al. 2006), the PoM
uses sentences to measure peace of mind. Since peace of mind is considered a composite
emotional state that includes both internal peace and internal harmony, sentences are better
able to capture the simultaneous experiences of peace and harmony within each individual
item.
Furthermore, our findings also support the premise that peace of mind and LAP affect
should be treated as distinct constructs. In Study 2, the PoM significantly predicted psy-
chological ill-being indices when we controlled the effects of LAP, whereas LAP affect
only partially predicted psychological ill-being when we controlled the effects of the PoM.
This finding shows that the PoM is a better indicator of mental health compared to LAP
affect. Overall, as compared to the measures of LAP affect, the PoM provides a more
complete picture of affective well-being that reflects the values of Chinese Culture.
6.3 Limitations and Future Work
The PoM is a tool that has shown both good reliability and validity for measuring internal
states of peace of mind. However, as a self-report measure, it has limitations. First, the
PoM asks people to report how often they feel internal peace in daily life. However, people
can find it difficult to accurately retrieve and assess the frequency of mood experiences in
their daily lives (Robinson and Clore 2002). Second, evaluation of the PoM may be easily
influenced by one’s attitudes toward experiencing peace of mind. People who value the
internal state of peace of mind over hedonic happiness may pay more attention to this state
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 587
123
and may therefore tend to report higher frequency of peace of mind than people who value
peace of mind to lesser degrees. Third, each item of the PoM was designed to capture
internal peace conjoined with internal harmony. As such, analyses using the scale are not
able to show the very different psychological processes that may underlie internal peace
versus harmony. It will be valuable to develop other ways to capture peace of mind in the
future. For example, in our future work, we plan to measure participants’ mood fluctuations
using an experience sampling method to better observe the frequency of experiencing
peace of mind in daily lives. Our aim is to capture both the state of emotional peacefulness
and the processes of emotional balancing in people’s online mood reports. Moreover,
future studies could also consider the use of priming in terms of the cognitive and affective
components of peace of mind to examine how they may influence each other. Despite the
limitations outlined above, the PoM is reliable in measuring individual differences in
experiences of peace of mind, and this can be regarded as a first step toward investigating
this new construct.
Since peace of mind is regarded as a state of well-being different from hedonic hap-
piness, the process of achieving peace of mind should also be different from that involved
in achieving hedonic happiness. According to previous studies, approaching pleasure and
avoiding pain have been regarded as the fundamental principles of achieving a good
affective life (Higgins et al. 1999; Kahneman 1999; Peterson 1999). However, maximizing
pleasure and minimizing pain should not be the critical process of achieving peace of mind.
Instead, we argue that the process of achieving peace of mind might involve balancing
between the experiences of pleasure and pain. In future studies, it would be worthwhile to
investigate the mood-regulation processes that allow people to revert to their equilibrium
state of peace of mind. Furthermore, it would also be helpful to explore the mood-regu-
lation strategies for attaining peace of mind, which may point to useful strategies for
people to practice in daily life.
6.4 Conclusion
We propose that the construct of peace of mind captures the affective experiences most
valued within Chinese cultural contexts and offers an index of affective well-being that is
different from conventional measures. Through three studies, we showed the new Peace of
Mind Scale, or PoM, to be a valid index of this distinct form of affective well-being, with
convergent, divergent, and predictive validity as well as the predicted differences between
Chinese and Western cultures. Therefore, peace of mind is a construct that is worthy of
further investigation, and we now have a tool with which this investigation can be
conducted.
Acknowledgments This study was supported in part by a grant of National Science Council, Taiwan
(NSC 99-2410-H-033-056).
Appendix: The Peace of Mind Scale
How often do you feel internal peace and ease in your daily life? Use the following scale to
indicate your response.
1=Not at all
2=Some of the time
588 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
3=Often
4=Most of the time
5=All of the time
____ 1. My mind is free and at ease.
____ 2. I feel content and comfortable with myself in daily life.
____ 3. My lifestyle gives me feelings of peace and stability.
____ 4. I have peace and harmony in my mind.
____ 5. It is difficult for me to feel settled. (–)
____ 6. The way I live brings me feelings of peace and comfort.
____ 7. I feel anxious and uneasy in my mind. (–)
Notes. Items marked (–) should be scored in reverse. The average of the item scores is
an overall measure of peace of mind, with high scores indicating greater peace of mind.
References
Andrews, F. M., & Robinson, J. P. (1991). Measures of subjective well-being. In J. P. Robinson, P. Shaver,
& L. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. New York: Plenum Press.
Argyle, M., Martin, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Happiness as a function of personality and social
encounters. In J. P. Forgas & J. M. Innes (Eds.), Recent advances in social psychology: An international
perspective (pp. 189–203). North-Holland: Elsevier.
Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine.
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (1999). Testing factorial invariance across groups: A reconceptualization
and proposed new method. Journal of Management, 25, 1–27.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.
Delle Fave, A., Brdar, I., Freire, T., Vella-Brodrick, D., & Wissing, M. P. (2011). The eudaimonic and
hedonic components of happiness: Qualitative and quantitative findings. Social Indicators Research,
100, 185–209.
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index.
American Psychologist, 55, 34–43.
Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1984). Temporal stability and cross-situational consistency of affective,
behavioral, and cognitive responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 871–883.
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Pavot, W. (1991). Happiness is the frequency, not the intensity, of positive versus
negative affect. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An interdisci-
plinary perspective (pp. 119–139). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.
Diener, E., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (1997). Recent findings on subjective well-being. Indian Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 24, 25–41.
Diener, E., Suh, E., Smith, H., & Shao, L. (1995). National differences in reported subjective well-being:
Why do they occur? Social Indicators Research, 34, 7–32.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing.
American Psychologist, 60, 678–686.
Gilbert, P., & Allan, S. (1998). The role of defeat and entrapment (arrested flight) in depression: An
exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychological Medicine, 28, 585–598.
Higgins, E. T., Grant, H., & Shah, J. (1999). Self-regulation and quality of life: Emotional and non-
emotional life experiences. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foun-
dations of hedonic psychology (pp. 244–266). New York: Russell Sage foundation.
Hu, L., & Bentler, O. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Coventional
criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: Users reference guide. Mooresville, IN: Scientific
Software, Inc.
Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being:
The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 3–25). New York: Russell Sage Foundation Press.
Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (1999). Well-being: Foundations of hedonic psychology.
New York: Russell Sage Foundation Press.
The Construct and Measurement of Peace of Mind 589
123
Kampfe, N., & Mitte, K. (2009). What you wish is what you get? The meaning of individual variability in
desired affect and affective discrepancy. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 409–418.
Kohn, L. (2001). Daoism and Chinese culture. Cambridge: Three Pines Press.
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Problems and promises with the circumplex model of emotion. Review of
Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 25–59.
Lee, Y. T., Yang, H. G., & Wang, M. (2009). Daoist harmony as a Chinese philosophy and psychology.
Peace and Conflict Studies, 16, 68–71.
Liao, L. Y. (2000). The new scale of social desirability responding in Taiwan and psychological process
analysis. Unpublished master’s thesis, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.
Little, T. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analyses of cross-cultural data: Practical and
theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 53–76.
Lu, L., & Gilmour, R. (2004). Culture, self and ways to achieve SWB: A cross-cultural analysis. Journal of
Psychology in Chinese Societies, 5, 51–79.
Lu, L., & Shih, J. B. (1997). Sources of happiness: A qualitative approach. Journal of Social Psychology,
137, 181–187.
Mitchell, S. (1988). Tao te ching. New York: HarperCollins.
Mitchell, D. W. (2002). Buddhism: Introducing the Buddhist experience. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Niedenthal, P. M., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2006). Self-conscious emotions. In P. M. Niedenthal, S.
Krauth-Gruber, & F. Ric (Eds.), Psychology of emotion (pp. 77–114). New York: Psychology Press.
Oishi, S. (2001). Culture and memory for emotional experiences: On-line vs. retrospective judgment of
subjective well-being (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2001). Dis-
sertation Abstracts International, 61, 5625.
Okazaki, S. (2000). Asian American and White American differences on affective distress symptoms: Do
symptom reports differ across reporting methods? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 603–625.
Peterson, C. (1999). Personal control and well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 288–301). New York: Russell Sage foundation.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general popu-
lation. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: An accessibility model of emotional self-report.
Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934–960.
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39,
1161–1178.
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological
Bulletin, 125, 3–30.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and
eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.
Ryff, R. M. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-
being. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference Chi-square test statistic for moment structure
analysis. Psychometrika, 66, 507–514.
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). PaloAlto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 86, 1378–1391.
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 90, 288–307.
Vandenberg, R. (2002). Toward a further understanding of and improvement in measurement invariance
methods and procedures. Organizational Research Methods, 5, 139–158.
Wallace, B., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). Mental balance and well-being: Building bridges between Buddhism
and western psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 690–701.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive
and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98,
219–235.
Whiteside-Mansell, L., & Corwyn, R. F. (2003). Mean and covariance structures analyses: An examination
of the Rosenberg self-esteem scale among adolescents and adults. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 63, 163–173.
Wong, Y. H., & Leung, T. K. (2001). Guanxi: Relationship marketing in a Chinese context. NewYork:
Haworth Press.
590 Y.-C. Lee et al.
123
... Drawing from Chinese cultural tradition, Lee et al. (2013) developed a positive emotion construct, peace of mind (PoM), to describe the distinctive affective well-being valued in Chinese culture. PoM refers to an affective state of inner peace and internal harmony. ...
... Internal harmony captures the state of harmony and balance, which refers to the process of self-control which can help individuals deal with feelings that are excessively intense, such as anger, sorrow, and enthusiasm, and ensure their emotional state fluctuates within a moderate range. The two aspects of PoM are intertwined such that individuals can either attain inner peace through the process of harmony or achieve harmony via maintaining a peaceful state of mind, thus constituting a coherent emotional state (Lee et al., 2013). The socioculturally oriented conception of inner peace and harmony has been embedded in traditional Chinese philosophy, including the principle of equilibrium (zhong) in Confucianism, the balance between Yin and Yang (opposite forces) advocated by Taoism, and the state of cessation of desire in Buddhism (Lee et al., 2013). ...
... The two aspects of PoM are intertwined such that individuals can either attain inner peace through the process of harmony or achieve harmony via maintaining a peaceful state of mind, thus constituting a coherent emotional state (Lee et al., 2013). The socioculturally oriented conception of inner peace and harmony has been embedded in traditional Chinese philosophy, including the principle of equilibrium (zhong) in Confucianism, the balance between Yin and Yang (opposite forces) advocated by Taoism, and the state of cessation of desire in Buddhism (Lee et al., 2013). The present study aims to investigate the low-arousal emotion of PoM in the Chinese workplace context with respect to how it may affect employees' work engagement in times of the COVID-19 pandemic, in contrast to high-arousal positive (HAP) emotions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: The prevention and control of the COVID-19 pandemic has taken on a "New Normal" form, which necessitates a calm and peaceful social mentality. This study delves into the Chinese socioculturally oriented emotion construct of peace of mind (PoM) with regard to how it may affect employees' work engagement in times of the pandemic. Based on the conversation of resource (COR) theory, we develop a model in which the relationship between PoM (i.e., a low-arousal positive affective state) and work engagement and the relationship between career calling (i.e., a high-arousal positive state) and work engagement are both mediated by social support. Methods: A total of 292 employees from 18 companies in Wuxi and Dalian, China, were surveyed at two different time points during the COVID-19 pandemic. Results: The results show that both relationships were mediated by social support; furthermore, after the mediating effect of social support on the relationship between PoM and work engagement was controlled for, the relationship between career calling and social support failed to reach significance. Discussion: The findings attest to the unique advantages of PoM in boosting employees' resource conservation and interpersonal communication in public crises. Possible implications on applying the incentive mechanism of PoM in the workplace are discussed.
... In the literature, we have identified at least other three existing measures of harmony. One is the Peace of Mind Scale (Lee et al., 2013) which assesses the affective wellbeing valued in Chinese culture. This measure includes feelings such as peace and stability (e.g., "I have peace and harmony in my mind"). ...
... As this is the first study evaluating the factor structure and internal consistency of the MHS, we recommend the use of our revised version in future studies. These results also provide evidence of a priori incremental validity of the MHS-R in comparison with the Peace of Mind Scale (Lee et al., 2013) and the Harmony in Life Scale (Kjell & Diener, 2021) since it has shown measurement invariance across more groups than the latter scales (across age groups, clinical/non-clinical sample, and five countries). These findings support the meaningfulness of the comparisons of the MHS-R scores between these populations. ...
... Moreover, there was a missing criterion measure of harmony (e.g., the Peace of Mind Scale, Lee et al., 2013, or the Harmony in Life Scale, Kjell et al., 2016) to test convergent validity of the MHS-R as a measure of inner harmony. This happened because the original data collection belongs to a larger research project and an additional measure would have made the survey excessively long. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study aimed to evaluate the psychometric properties of the Mature Happiness Scale, a measure focused on inner harmony. Mature happiness is achieved when a person can live in balance between both positive and negative aspects of their life. A total sample of 2,130 participants from five countries (Canada: n = 390, United States: n = 223, United Kingdom: n = 512, Spain: n = 724, and Hungary: n = 281) responded to an online survey including the original Mature Happiness Scale, the PERMA-Profiler, and the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses yielded a one-factor solution with seven positive items (non-reversed). We called this new version of the questionnaire the Mature Happiness Scale-Revised (MHS-R). Measurement invariance was found across countries, age groups, gender, and mental disorder diagnosis. Internal consistency and test–retest reliability were high. Older people, males, and people without a mental disorder diagnosis scored higher in mature happiness than younger ones, females, and those with a mental health disorder diagnosis, respectively. Mature happiness showed strong positive associations with various subscales of the PERMA-Profiler, specifically with positive emotions and meaning in life. In addition, mature happiness was strongly correlated with less negative affect and inner conflict and lower psychological inflexibility, whereas it was moderately correlated with lower loneliness. This validity evidence supports the cross-cultural use of the MHS-R in the aforementioned countries to reliably measure happiness among adults. With its holistic approach, the MHS-R may be a unique complement to other well-being measures, particularly to better predict mental health problems.
... Peace of mind (PoM) was defined as a state of inner peace and harmony (Lee et al., 2013). In its definition, inner peace captures low-arousal positive affect such as feelings of peacefulness, calmness, and serenity. ...
... In its definition, inner peace captures low-arousal positive affect such as feelings of peacefulness, calmness, and serenity. Internal harmony, on the other hand, is characterized by a state of balance and harmony (Lee et al., 2013). PoM is an important affective well-being valued by Chinese culture (Lee et al., 2013). ...
... Internal harmony, on the other hand, is characterized by a state of balance and harmony (Lee et al., 2013). PoM is an important affective well-being valued by Chinese culture (Lee et al., 2013). All three schools of ancient teaching (i.e., Confucianism, Taoism, and Buddhism) that have shaped Chinese culture place a great emphasis on pursuing inner peace and harmony (Lee et al., 2013). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Peace of mind is an important affective well-being valued in Chinese culture. Mindfulness and self-compassion had the potential to promote peace of mind. However, the mechanisms underlying these effects were not well understood. The current cross-sectional study aimed to investigate whether nonattachment explained the benefits of mindfulness and self-compassion on peace of mind. A sample of 364 Chinese adults was recruited from WeChat, a popular social media platform in China. Participants filled out an online survey including measures of dispositional mindfulness, self-compassion, nonattachment, and peace of mind. Correlation analyses showed that all of these variables were significantly and positively associated with each other. More importantly, nonattachment significantly mediated the associations between dispositional mindfulness and self-compassion with peace of mind. These findings suggest that nonattachment may be a potential mechanism by which mindfulness and self-compassion promote peace of mind among Chinese adults. If the mediating effects are further confirmed in future longitudinal and experimental studies, mindfulness and self-compassion interventions can emphasize nonattachment to optimize their effects on peace of mind.
... The Cronbach's α value of the SwLS in the current study was 0.923.Peace of mind (PoM) scale.PoM is defined as an internal state of peacefulness and harmony. The construct was originally developed based on the description of well-being valuation in Chinese culture25 . A cross-cultural validation of this scale was conducted and showed cultural differences, with a higher PoM score for Taiwanese individuals than for Europeans or Americans. ...
Article
Full-text available
Psychosocial factors, including life satisfaction, resilience, and social support, have been proposed to influence depressive symptom severity in adults because the age of onset of depressive disorders, i.e. adolescence to early adulthood, is associated with various impairments in psychosocial functioning. In this study, a psychosocial model was constructed to verify these relationships to prevent depression. For this study, 370 participants were recruited from the community via poster or online advertisements. They completed several questionnaires to assess depressive symptom severity: the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC), Satisfaction with Life Scale (SwLS), Peace of Mind (PoM) scale, Social Support Questionnaire (SSQ), and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II). A negative association was found between depressive symptom severity and all other variables, including PoM and CD-RISC scores, life satisfaction, and social support. Such factors can be considered protective against increased depressive symptom severity. In addition, indirect effects of PoM and resilience on the negative association between SwLS scores and depressive symptom severity were observed. Moreover, social support was found to mediate the correlation between PoM and resilience, implying that social support mediates the relationship between state of mind and resilience. The psychosocial model suggested that depressive symptom severity is influenced by internal factors (an individual’s state of mind, subjective view of events and their life) and external factors (including social support).
... Inner peace has the basic building blocks of love, awareness, and compassion (Davis & Thompson, 2013) and encourages individuals to develop a more flexible perspective toward life and time as well as helps people reduce negative cognition and increase positive emotions (Ge et al., 2020). In Buddhism, inner peace is essential in people's lives (Liu et al., 2015), while in Taoist practice, achieving inner peace has been stated as the ultimate intention (Lee et al., 2013). When looking at inner peace from an existential perspective, the most effective way to handle death can be said to be living a satisfying and meaningful life; in other words, it means living a good life and dying with inner peace rather than dealing defensively with the looming threat of death by denying it (Van Der Kaap-Deeder et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Sense of peace, inner balance, and harmony are aspects of inner peace. The purpose of this study is to translate the Inner Peace Scale (IPS; Xi & Lee, 2021) into Turkish, test its psychometric features, and conduct validity and reliability tests on a Turkish population. The study sample is comprised of 291 students at universities in Konya, of whom 233 are women (80%) and 58 are men (20%). The participants’ ages range between 18-35. The translation process was done using translation and back-translation stages. The original version of the scale has three sub-dimensions according to the CFA results (i.e., acceptance of loss, inner balance and calmness, transcending hedonism and materialism). The inner peace scale and similar instruments have been demonstrated to have significantly positive relationships, and the study’s findings are discussed within the context of the literature.
... A revised Peace of Mind Scale (Lee et al., 2013) was developed to be appropriate for the age group. One item was removed to promote brevity, for a total of 6 items, and language was simplified (for example, 'My mind was free and at ease' was changed to 'I felt calm inside'). ...
Article
Research on interventions that aim to cultivate character strengths, or virtues, has been conducted primarily among highly resourced, predominantly White communities, and the interventions have been developed to reflect the values of those communities. The purpose of this study was to use a participatory action research approach to develop a virtue intervention focused on addressing the community-identified problem of violence in a predominantly Black community, and to test its effectiveness in a pilot study. Participants were 37 youth (M age = 12.1 years old; 97% Black) who attended summer programs in two sites (on intervention, one control condition). Participants in the intervention group had greater increases in effective nonviolent behavior than participants in the comparison group according to teacher report. Overall, the results of this pilot test suggest it is possible to build a feasible, attractive, evidence-based intervention to promote character strengths using participatory action research.
Article
Character strengths research has the potential to imply that youth have character deficits or moral failings that cause their problematic behavior. This ignores the impact of context, especially for youth who are members of historically marginalized groups in under resourced communities. On the other hand, framing youth who are members of underrepresented groups solely as products of oppression undermines their agency and the power of collective action. It may be possible to promote character development in a contextually relevant, culturally grounded way through a transdisciplinary, participatory action approach. We engaged in this work in three phases: goal specification; logic model identification; and intervention development and testing. Lessons learned included the need for intensive trust-building and identifying the limitations in scope and capacity. Overall, a transdisciplinary, participatory action approach is a promising strategy to overcome the existing bias in virtue development research and to leverage positive psychology to address health disparities.
Article
Peace of mind is a feeling of calm or not being worried. Bhatnagar (2019) wrote that “Peace of mind is free of costs, but only for positive people; whereas it's most expensive things for the negative people.” The conditions aroused due to the covid-19 pandemic affecting mental peace. Therefore the researcher decided to know the Relationship between Self-reported peace of mind and self-reported spirituality during the covid-19 pandemic crisis. Rigorous review of related literature revealed that there is no study with reference to the association between peace of mind and spirituality during covid-19 pandemic crisis. The presented study was related to explain the present condition i.e. "What is" therefore the survey method under descriptive research method was used for the presented research study. A survey form, constructed by the researcher on Google form, was used to collect data. The survey form consisted of the items related to the demographic data and a statement related to self-reported peace of mind and self-reported spirituality. There were two options of the items regarding the self-reported peace of mind and self-reported spirituality Agree and Disagree. The language medium of the survey form was Hindi. The data was collected between 25th of April 2020 to 20th of June 2020. The Google survey form link was distributed to various WhatsApp groups and individuals with a request to complete the survey and distribute it to others. In this way a total of 120 respondents from Lucknow district (India) fully completed the survey form. After collection, the data was organized and statistically treated with the help of frequencies, percentage, χ2 (chi-square) test to study the self-reported peace of mind with respect to age, the level of education, employment status, residential status and φ (phi) coefficient measure of the association between two dichotomous variables i.e. self-reported peace of mind and self-reported spirituality. The study showed that Self-reported peace of mind of the individuals during crisis of the covid-19 pandemic was not differing with respect to age, the level of education, employment status, and residential status and, it was strongly associated with the self-reported spirituality.
Article
Tourism has been widely recognized as a powerful tool for economic growth in rural communities around the world. The growth of rural tourism has always been a significant start in targeting poverty alleviation and promoting economic change in rural China. Rural tourism development is the participation of rural people in tourism and full integration into tourism development. Rural tourism development depends on the participation and support of rural residents. Therefore, this research explores the influencing factors of rural tourism residents’ happiness and their influence on tourism support. The sample was selected from Anren Town, Chengdu City, China, and the research subjects were residents of Anren Town. In this study, data were collected from 370 residents of Anren Town via a questionnaire survey. The results show that Community Involvement (CI) and Community Attachment (CA) positively affect residents’ Authentic Happiness (AH). This research found that improving residents’ support for tourism development can promote the development of rural tourism. The study finding provides a new perspective for the treatment of residents’ problems in tourism development and has guiding significance for the improvement of residents’ happiness index in tourism destinations. If the community is better, the happiness of the residents will be stronger.
Article
Friendship alienation is a common dilemma experienced by many Chinese people in life. It especially has negative impacts on the mental health of the alienated party, which cannot be ignored. The role of “yuanfen” ( 緣分) in adapting to relationship dilemmas has already been put forward and yuanfen has recently been subdivided into beliefs in “yuan” ( 緣 ) and “fen” ( 分 ). However, yuan and fen beliefs were for the most part hypothesized in the past to have respective effects on psychological adaptation. This paper first proposes the hypothesis that yuan and fen belief can work together (interact) to buffer the negative effects of friendship alienation on mental health. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to explore the roles of the yuan and fen beliefs following friendship alienation, including the effect of “friendship alienation” on “mental health” and the moderating role of yuan and fen beliefs. In this study, adults aged 18 and above were adopted as subjects. Data was collected through online questionnaires. Participants were requested to fill out assessment scales measuring friendship alienation experiences, their yuan and fen beliefs, sense of loneliness, satisfaction with life, subjective happiness, etc. The results show that compared with those without experiences of friendship alienation, people who had experienced estrangement from their friends within two years indeed showed poorer mental health. The research results with greater significance show that 1) If the yuan and fen beliefs operate simultaneously, they play a buffering role on the negative impacts of alienation experience on mental health. If they both operate independently however, they do not have a buffering effect on the negative impacts of the alienation experience on mental health; and 2) It is worth noting that in situations where belief in yuan is low, and the belief in fen is higher, the more likely an experience of alienation would jeopardize subjective happiness and satisfaction with life. Overall, the joint operation of the beliefs in yuan and fen may play a buffering role in the negative effects of friendship alienation. Nevertheless, it is also worth noting that under specific circumstances, yuan and fen do not always have positive effects on negative relationships as mentioned in the preceding literature. Finally, the research results were targeted for further discussion. The research limitations and directions were also indicated.
Article
Full-text available
This review organizes a variety of phenomena related to emotional self-report. In doing so, the authors offer an accessibility model that specifies the types of factors that contribute to emotional self-reports under different reporting conditions. One important distinction is between emotion, which is episodic, experiential, and contextual, and beliefs about emotion, which are semantic, conceptual, and decontextualized. This distinction is important in understanding the discrepancies that often occur when people are asked to report on feelings they are currently experiencing versus those that they are not currently experiencing. The accessibility model provides an organizing framework for understanding self-reports of emotion and suggests some new directions for research.
Article
Full-text available
Based on Lee's prior research on Daoism (Lee, 2003; Lee, 2004; Lee, Han, Byron and Fan, 2008; Lee and Hu, 1993; Lee, Norasakkunkit, Liu, Zhang and Zhou, 2008), this article first introduces Laozi, Dao, De and Daoism in relation to harmony. Then, Daoist harmony is elaborated in the following areas: (1) the yin-yang oneness, (2) the way it is (natural), (3) wei-wu-wei (or nonintervention), (4) water-like characteristics, (5) love for peace, and (6) tolerance and appreciation of differences. The article concludes with a suggestion for harmony with the external world as well as with fellow human beings.
Conference Paper
All individuals have multiple views of themselves. Whereas the consistency among the different aspects of identity is emphasized in Western cultures, the "multiple selves" are often viewed as coexisting realities in East Asian cultures. This research revisits the classic thesis in psychology that identity consistency is a prerequisite condition of psychological well-being. Between individuals (Study 1), people with a more consistent self-view had a more clear self-knowledge, were more assertive, and, most notably, had self-experiences that were less affected by the perspectives of others. Compared with North American participants. (Study 2), Koreans viewed themselves more flexibly across situations, and their subjective well-being was less predictable from levels of identity consistency. Also, consistent individuals received positive social evaluations from others in the United States but not in Korea.
Chapter
Consistency and stability of feelings were examined in reports that were completed on 3,512 occasions randomly sampled from the lives of 42 subjects. The stability and consistency of responses depended on the situations, individuals, and responses involved. High degrees of consistency were unusual for single responses, although mean levels of responding tended to be both highly stable and consistent. The consistency and stability of variables covaried, suggesting a connection between the two. Persons who were more consistent across one pair of situations tended to be more consistent across other situational pairs. The results indicate that the question of whether personality consistency exists does not have a simple answer, and requires knowledge of the persons, situations, responses, and level of analysis involved.