Content uploaded by Florian Krause
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Florian Krause
Content may be subject to copyright.
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 1
Different brains process numbers differently:
structural bases of individual differences in spatial and
non-spatial number representations
Florian Krause1, Oliver Lindemann2, Ivan Toni1, Harold Bekkering1
1Radboud University Nijmegen, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, The Netherlands
2Division of Cognitive Science, University of Potsdam, Germany
Florian Krause (corresponding author)
Address: P.O. Box 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Phone: +31 24 36 11027
Email: f.krause@donders.ru.nl
In press: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
This manuscript may not exactly replicate the final published version.
It is not a copy of record.
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 2
Abstract
A dominant hypothesis on how the brain processes numerical size proposes a spatial
representation of numbers as positions on a 'mental number line'. An alternative hypothesis
considers numbers as elements of a generalized representation of sensorimotor-related
magnitude which is not obligatorily spatial. Here we show that individuals' relative use of
spatial and non-spatial representations has a cerebral counterpart in the structural organization
of the posterior parietal cortex. Inter-individual variability in the linkage between numbers
and spatial responses (faster left responses to low numbers and right responses to high
numbers; SNARC effect) correlated with variations in grey matter volume around the right
precuneus. Conversely, differences in the disposition to link numbers to force production
(faster soft responses to low numbers and hard responses to high numbers) were related to
grey matter volume in the left angular gyrus. This finding suggests that numerical cognition
relies on multiple mental representations of analogue magnitude using different neural
implementations that are linked to individual traits.
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 3
Introduction
Dealing with numerical information is an integral part of our modern society. Numbers
occur throughout all aspects of every day life; they depict information about prices and values
and allow us to count occurrences and entities. During a single day we probably process
several thousand numbers (Butterworth, 1999). Yet, our ability to deal with them varies
greatly across individuals (Butterworth, 2010; Adams, 2007). It is therefore important to
understand how individuals represent numbers and how their brains process this information.
The most influential model of number processing, the triple-code model, dissociates between
three different numerical representations: An Arabic code for digit processing, a verbal code
for retrieval of arithmetic facts and verbal counting, and an analogue magnitude code for the
processing of numerical size (Dehaene, 1992). While the first two representations are
notation- and modality-dependent, an analogue magnitude representation is thought to be
abstract in nature and is thus assumed to be independent of notation and modality (Cohen
Kadosh & Walsh, 2009). The current study seeks to investigate two different manifestations
of this analogue magnitude representation of numerical size.
Over the last decades, several studies on number cognition have provided abundant
empirical support for the hypothesis that numerical size is spatially represented in the brain
(Dehaene, 2009; de Havia, Vallar & Girelli, 2008; Hubbard, Piazza, Pinel & Dehaene, 2005).
This hypothesis assumes that we derive the size of a number from its position on an ordered
'mental number line' on which small numbers are represented on one side and large numbers
on the other (Moyer & Landauer, 1967). For instance, the so-called effect of
Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) shows a linkage between
numerical information and spatial responses (Dehaene, Bossini & Giraux, 1993). When
participants are asked to judge the parity of Arabic digits between 1 and 9 by a left or right
response, the numerical size of the digit interferes with the execution of the spatial responses,
with faster left responses to small numbers and faster right responses to large numbers. This
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 4
spatial number-response interference effect has been interpreted as evidence for a shared
representation of spatial response codes and the ordinal position of the number representation
in mental space.
More recently, the assumption that spatial codes become obligatorily activated when
processing numerical size has been questioned by several authors (e.g. Fischer, 2006; Santens
& Gevers, 2008). It has been argued instead that information about numerical size are
mapped onto representations of other size-related sensorimotor information, within a system
that processes generalized analogue magnitude (Walsh, 2003). According to this hypothesis,
number meaning is conceptualized by recruiting the same mechanisms that allow us to
experience and control other behaviorally relevant magnitudes in daily life. Evidence for this
notion comes from several studies showing associations between numbers and other types of
magnitude information in action and perception, like physical size (Tzelgov & Henik, 1992),
temporal duration (Oliveri, Vicario, Salerno, Koch, Turizziani, Mangano, Chillemi &
Caltagirone, 2008), grip aperture (Lindemann, Abolafia, Girardi & Bekkering, 2007), object
graspability (Badets, Andres, Di Luca, & Pesenti, 2007) and tactile sensation (Krause,
Bekkering, & Lindeman, 2013). Crucially, the associated sensorimotor magnitudes can be
entirely non-spatial in nature. For instance, a link between numerical information and force
production has been reported (Vierck & Kiesel, 2010), which we will refer to as
Force-Numerical Association of Response Codes (FoNARC). When participants are asked to
judge the parity of Arabic digits between 1 and 9 by a soft or hard response on a single
button, the numerical size of the digit interferes with the execution of force responses, with
faster soft responses to small numbers and faster hard responses to large numbers.
Importantly, the procedure to quantify a FoNARC effect is identical to the procedure to
quantify a SNARC effect except that required motor responses for the latency measurement
do not differ spatially. Due to the homogeneity of all spatial response components, it can be
excluded that the number-response interference effect observed under these condition is
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 5
driven by a spatial representation of numbers on a mental number line. The observation of a
FoNARC effect consequently has to be interpreted as a within-magnitude inference between
numerical information and the control of motor force, which in turn suggests the existence of
non-spatial sensorimotor-related representations of numbers.
With the apparent coexistence of both spatial and non-spatial representations of
numerical size the question arises in which way the two representations contribute to
numerical cognition. It has been suggested that multiple representations of the same
numerical information rely on different neural implementations and that the weights of their
contribution are simply determined by the requirements of the situation or task at hand
(Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel & Cohen, 2003). A numerical task with a spatial component would
lead to a stronger activation of posterior superior parietal lobe. In contrast, a number task
without any spatial component (e.g. force production) is expected to engage primarily inferior
parietal regions (cf. Dehaene et al., 2003).
However, task demands might not be the only factor to determine how numerical
information are processed. For instance, the general disposition to associate numbers with
space has been shown to vary strongly between individuals (for a review see Wood, Nuerk &
Willmes, 2008) and might even depend on personal preferences to code numerical
information (Fischer, 2006). The same might hold for linking numbers to non-spatial
sensorimotor-related magnitude, as this disposition might be related to the individual's bodily
competence and experience of dealing with magnitudes and sizes in everyday life
(Lindemann, Rueschemeyer, & Bekkering, 2009; see also Barsalou, 2008). Here we assess
whether those inter-individual differences reflect stable individual traits, rather than
stochastic noise or task demands.
This issue was addressed by combining a double dissociation approach with the method
of Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM; Ashburner and Friston, 2000). The rationale of this
approach is to isolate differential structural variances across two behavioural indexes of
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 6
numerical cognition, intrinsically controlling for confounds correlated with both indexes. We
tested whether inter-individual variation in anatomical brain structure explains individual
differences in spatial (SNARC) and non-spatial (FoNARC) number-response interference
effects – reflecting a spatial and non-spatial representation of numerical size, respectively.
VBM was used to measure variability in local grey matter volume in the posterior parietal
cortex, a site consistently associated with numerical representations (see Arsalidou & Taylor,
2011 for a review). Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) was used to map the
spatial distribution of task-related activity across the posterior parietal cortex.
Method
Participants
A total of 33 students (20 female) between 18 and 34 years of age (mean age 21.33,
SD=3.28) participated in the experiment in return of 20 Euro or course credits. All of them
had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were of general health, with no known
neurological or psychological disorders. The study was approved by the local ethics
committee and participants gave their written consent prior to the experimental procedure.
Setup
Stimuli consisted of the Arabic digits 1 to 9, except 5, depicted in white colour (visual
angle: ~1.26 degrees vertical & ~0.53 degrees horizontal) centrally on a black background.
Participants viewed the screen via a mirror attached to the Magnetic Resonance (MR)
scanner's receiver head-coil.
Responses were recorded using MR-compatible button boxes with either spatially
arranged buttons that had to be pressed with the right index and right middle finger, or with a
single isometric force-transducer button which had to be pressed with the right thumb. The
force sensitive button box was a cylinder grasped between the thumb and the remaining
fingers.
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 7
Procedure
The data collection was done in the context of a larger functional Magnetic Resonance
Imaging study and was thus performed while subjects were lying inside the MR-scanner.
Participants were engaged in two consecutive number parity judgement tasks in which the
presented digits had to be classified as odd or even. Importantly, both tasks differed only in
the required responses. In the spatial task, number parity had to be indicated by a right index
finger (“left”) or middle finger (“right”) response. That is, each response involved the flexion
and extension of either one of the two fingers. In the non-spatial task, responses were given
with the right thumb and involved applying either a small force (>500 N, “soft” responses) or
a large force (>2500 N, “hard” responses).
Each trial started with the presentation of a white fixation cross for 500 ms, followed by
the target stimulus. Participants had to respond within 1000 ms after stimulus presentation. If
it took them too long to respond, or their response was incorrect, an auditory error signal was
played back to them via headphones. After the response was given, a dark grey fixation cross
was presented for a variable time between 2000 and 4000 ms, before the next trial started.
Before the actual experiment, participants were given verbal instructions and practised
the task for about 5 minutes outside the MR scanner. The response mapping to indicate the
parity (i.e., left or right response for odd numbers and soft or hard response for odd numbers)
was reversed in the middle of each task block. The order of mappings as well as the order of
the spatial and non-spatial task were balanced between participants. Eighteen participants
performed 320 trials, 15 participants performed 288 trials. The order of trials was
randomized.
MRI data acquisition
For each participant a high-resolution anatomical MR image was recorded using a
T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence with a GRAPPA acceleration factor of 2 (TR/TE =
2300/3.03 ms, voxel size = 1x1x1 mm). Anatomical images were recorded directly after both
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 8
tasks were conducted. Due to technical problems, MR images of two participants were
acquired 12 and 5 weeks after the behavioural test, respectively. For one participant, a 7
months older MR image was used. All images were recorded on the same 3 Tesla Siemens
Magneton Trio MR scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) in combination with the same
32-channel receiver head-coil.
Functional images were acquired using a multi-echo gradient echo planar T2*-weighted
sequence sensitive to blood oxygen level-dependent contrast (TR = 2390 ms; TE = 9.4, 21.2,
33.0, 45.0 and 56.0 ms; FA = 90°; field of view = 224 x 224 mm; number of slices = 31; slice
thickness = 3 mm; resolution = 3.5 x 3.5 x 3.0 mm).
Behavioural data analysis
The behavioural data of each participant were analyzed separately for the spatial and the
non-spatial task, to estimate effect sizes for both, a SNARC effect in the spatial task, as well
as a FoNARC effect in the non-spatial task. Only trials with correct parity judgments within
1000 ms were included in the analysis. Effect sizes were calculated as suggested by Fias,
Brysbaert, Geypens and Ydewalle (1996). First, the difference in the median reaction times
between left and right responses (spatial task) and soft and hard responses (magnitude task)
was calculated for each presented digit. Then, individual linear regressions between these
response time differences and the digits were calculated. The resulting regression coefficients
were used to characterize the size of the SNARC or FoNARC effect in each participant.
VBM analysis
MR image preprocessing and statistical testing was done using Statistical Parametric
Mapping 8 (SPM8, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and the integrated DARTEL toolbox
(Ashburner, 2007).
Each anatomical image was segmented into grey and white matter images and resampled
to 1.5 mm isotropic resolution. Afterwards, nonlinear deformations for warping all grey and
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 9
white matter images to each other were determined by iterative template creation (7 steps;
Ashburner, 2007). Modulated warped grey matter images were created by smoothing with a
Gaussian kernel of 10 mm and normalizing to the Montreal Neurology Institute (MNI)
coordinate space.
An anatomical mask was created using the SPM8 Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff, 2005),
including portions of the superior parietal cortex (areas 7A, 7PC, 7M, 7P; Scheperjans,
Hermann, Eickhoff, Amunts, Schleicher, & Zilles, 2008a; Scheperjans, Eickhoff, Hömke,
Mohlberg, Hermann, Amunts, & Zilles, 2008b), the inferior parietal cortex (areas PFop, PFt,
PF, PFm, PFcm, PGa, PGp; Caspers, Geyer, Schleicher, Mohlberg, Amunts, & Zilles, 2006;
Caspers, Eickhoff, Geyer, Scheperjans, Mohlberg, Zilles, & Amunts, 2008), as well as the
intraparietal sulcus (areas hIP1, hIP2, hIP3; Choi, Zilles, Mohlberg, Schleicher, Fink,
Armstrong, & Amunts, 2006; Scheperjans et al., 2008a; Scheperjans et al., 2008b).
This anatomical mask, based on regions previously involved in numerical cognition
(Cohen Kadosh, Lammertyn, & Izard, 2008; Wu, Chang, Majid, Caspers, Eickhoff, &
Menon, 2009), was combined with a functional mask including posterior parietal voxels
activated during either one of the experimental tasks (see fMRI analysis below).
The preprocessed images entered a multiple regression general linear model (GLM) with
SNARC and FoNARC effect size estimates as regressors of interest. Two additional
covariates were added to the GLM: median overall reaction times, aggregated over both
tasks, to control for general performance differences between participants, and total
intracranial volume, to control for general overall size differences of grey matter, white
matter and cerebro spinal fluid (Good, Johnsrude, Ashburner, Henson, Friston & Frackowiak,
2001).
The statistical threshold was p < 0.05 at voxel level, corrected for multiple comparisons
by means of the family-wise error (FWE).
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 10
fMRI analysis
Functional image preprocessing and statistical testing was done using SPM8
(http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm).
For each volume, the five multi-echo images were combined into a T2*-weighted
average image (Poser, Versluis, Hoogduin, & Norris, 2006). All weighted average images
were spatially realigned to the first image and corrected for differences in slice-time
acquisition. The T1-weighted anatomical image was co-registered with the mean functional
image, segmented and normalized to the MNI standard space, and resampled to a 2x2x2 mm
resolution. The resulting normalization parameters were applied to the functional images,
which were subsequently spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm.
The preprocessed images entered a GLM with 4 sessions, describing two response
mappings for each of the two experimental tasks. For each session, task effects were modeled
using a combination of compatible and incompatible trials for four groups of numerical
stimuli (1, 2; 3, 4; 6, 7; 8, 9), resulting in 8 regressors, each describing the onset of the
response to the stimulus. An additional regressor was used to model erroneous responses. All
task-related regressors were convolved with a hemodynamic response function. Three
translational and three rotational head motion parameters and their first and second
derivative, resulting in 18 regressors, were added as covariates.
To capture posterior parietal voxels activated during the experimental tasks, the t-contrast
of each task compared to implicit baseline was evaluated for the whole brain on the group
level (thresholded at 0.05, uncorrected). The union of the results of both contrasts, restricted
to the entire posterior parietal cortex (i.e., areas 5L, 5M, 5Ci, 7A, 7PC, 7M, 7P, PFop, PFt,
PF, PFm, PFcm, PGa, PGp, hIP1, hIP2, hIP3), served as a functional mask (see VBM
analysis above).
We also assessed whether the regions showing structural variations as a function of
SNARC/FoNARC performance were also functionally engaged in performance of those
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 11
tasks. That is we tested whether the fMRI data showed increased BOLD signal (p<0.01,
Family-wise error corrected for search volume) within a search volume defined by two
spherical VOIs centered on the two local maxima of the VBM analyses, with a radius
matched to the FWHM of the VBM results (10 mm).
Results
Behavioural results
On average, participants made 5 % errors in the spatial task and 10 % errors in the
non-spatial task. The average reaction times were 539 ms (SD=60) and 585 ms (SD=62) for
the spatial task and non-spatial task, respectively. SNARC effect sizes of all participants
differed significantly from zero, t(32) = 6.70, p < .001, as did FoNARC effect sizes, t(32) =
8.61, p < .001. There was a weak, but non-significant positive correlation between the
individual SNARC and FoNARC effect sizes , r = 0.31, p = .07. Median overall reaction
times correlated with the size of the SNARC effect , r = 0.36, p < .05, and were therefore
included as an additional covariate in the GLM for the VBM analysis (see Method).
Importantly, there were no correlations between age and SNARC effect sizes, r = -0.10, p = .
57, age and FoNARC effect sizes, r = 0.03, p = .88, or gender and SNARC effect sizes, r =
0.11, p = .55, and gender and FoNARC effect sizes, r = 0.16, p = .37. Therefore, and since
any shared variance with age and gender is common to both regressors of interest in the
GLM, age and gender were not added as explicit covariates into the VBM analysis.
VBM results
Figure 1 shows the main findings of the VBM analysis (thresholded at 0.001,
uncorrected, for illustrative purposes). The multiple regression analysis on the posterior
parietal cortex revealed that SNARC effect size predicted local relative grey matter volume in
the right precuneus (area 5M; peak at MNI coordinates x = 7.5, y = -49.5, z = 52.5; t(28) =
4.97, Z = 4.17, pFWE < 0.05). The stronger the individual SNARC effect (i.e. the disposition to
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 12
associate numbers with a spatial response), the more relative grey matter was present in this
particular region. Furthermore, FoNARC effect size predicted local relative grey matter
volume in the left angular gyrus (area PGa; peak at MNI coordinates x = -45, y = -57, z =
37.5 ), t(28) = 5.37, Z = 4.42, pFWE < 0.05). The stronger the FoNARC effect (i.e. the
disposition to link numbers to force production), the more relative grey matter in this region
of the individual's brain. Figure 2 illustrates the differential correlations between grey matter
volume in each of the regions and number-response interference effects, corrected for average
reaction time and total intracranial volume. Importantly, grey matter volume in right
precuneus correlated significantly more with the spatial than with the non-spatial
number-response interference effect, Z = 3.55, p < 0.01, while grey matter volume in left
angular gyrus correlated significantly less with the spatial than with the non-spatial
number-response interference effect, Z = -3.89, p < 0.01, demonstrating a double-dissociation
between the behavioural and the structural cerebral effects.
fMRI results
SVC analysis on the regions identified in the VBM analysis revealed a significant
activation of right precuneus during both the spatial task, t(32) = 5.31, Z = 4.47, pFWE < 0.01,
and the non-spatial task, t(32) = 4.57, Z = 3.98, pFWE < 0.01. Likewise, left angular gyrus was
significantly activated during the spatial task, t(32) = 5.05, Z = 4.30, pFWE < 0.01, as well as
during the non-spatial task, t(32) = 6.97, Z = 5.40, pFWE < 0.01.
Discussion
The present study provides evidence for a contribution of both spatial and non-spatial
representations of numerical size when processing Arabic digits and demonstrates that the
weights of this contribution rely on stable individual traits. We show that inter-individual
differences in the dispositions to link numbers to either space or non-spatial sensorimotor
magnitude can be directly related to structural variance in two distinct regions in the superior
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 13
and inferior posterior parietal lobes.
Structural bases of spatial and non-spatial representations of numerical
size
There was a relation between the strength of the SNARC effect and the structure of a
parietal region (area 5m) in the right precuneus. Increased grey matter in this region predicted
stronger interference of numerical size with spatial responses, but not with the force of a
response. Although little is known about the specific functionality of area 5m in humans, its
cytoarchitecture suggests that it is comparable with area PE in the macaque brain
(Scheperjans, Grefkes, Palomero-Gallagher, Schleicher, & Zilles, 2005). Macaque PE has
been involved in somatosensory integration and in creating a spatial representation of limbs
during movement (Bakola, Passarelli, Gamberini, Fattori, & Galletti, 2013; Lacquaniti,
Guigon, Bianchi, Ferraina, & Caminiti, 1995; Jones, Coulter, & Hendry, 1978; Mountcastle,
Lynch, Georgopoulos, Sakata, & Acuna, 1975). In humans, the right precuneus has
repeatedly been shown to be important for spatial processing, such as shifting attention in
visual space or visual imagery (for a review see Cavanna & Trimble, 2006), but not in the
processing of numerical magnitude information (for a review see Dehaene et al. 2003). In
fact, the present findings indicate that this parietal region is involved in the spatial
representation of numbers, suggesting that the association between numbers and space is
closer to general cognitive-spatial processing than to numerical magnitude per se.
The strength of mapping numbers to motor force ( FoNARC effect) correlated with the
structure of the left angular gyrus. Increased grey matter in this region predicted stronger
interference of numerical size with the force of a response, but not with the laterality of a
response. In contrast to the precuneus, the angular gyrus – the left side in particular – has
been consistently related to the processing of numerical information (Arsalidou et al., 2011).
For instance, a lesion in the left angular gyrus can lead to arithmetical deficits (Gerstman
Syndrome; Gerstmann, 1940). Dehaene et al. (2003) concluded that the left angular gyrus is
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 14
involved in the retrieval of linguistic arithmetic facts from memory. Evidence for this notion
comes, for instance, from neuroimaging studies showing that activity in this region is
modulated by arithmetic training (Ischebeck, Zamarian, Siedentopf, Koppelstätter, Benke,
Felber & Delazer, 2006; Delazer, Domahs, Bartha, Brenneis, Lochy, Trieb & Benke, 2003).
Further support for memory-based processes is provided by the observation that neuronal
activity in angular gyrus is higher when arithmetic problems are solved by fact retrieval,
compared to calculations (Grabner, Ansari, Koschutnig, Reishofer, Ebner & Neuper, 2009).
In contrast, it has been argued by several authors that the angular gyrus is involved in the
processing of number symbols and their numerical magnitude information (Rusconi, Walsh &
Butterworth, 2005; Göbel, Walsh & Rushworth, 2001). For instance, transcranial magnetic
stimulation (TMS) over angular gyrus is known to disrupt parity as well as magnitude
comparisons (Rusconi et al., 2005). An involvement of the left angular gyrus has been
furthermore demonstrated while processing numerical symbols in the absence of any
arithmetic demands (Price & Ansari, 2011; Holloway, Price & Ansari, 2010). Ansari (2008)
hypothesized therefore that the left angular gyrus mediates the mapping of numerical symbols
onto magnitude representations. The present finding now adds further empirical evidence for
this idea and demonstrates an involvement of the left angular gyrus in a non-linguistic and
non-spatial analogue representation of numerical size. We therefore interpret our findings as
support for the notion that this region is involved in the mapping of number symbols onto
magnitude information.
Spatial and non-spatial number-response interference effects
The current findings shed new light on the nature of spatial and non-spatial
number-response interferences effects and might also have practical implications for
investigating numerical representations. The present study is one of the first to demonstrate a
direct brain correlate of the well known association between numbers and spatial responses
reflected by the SNARC effect. The results of a recent functional near-infrared spectroscopy
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 15
study show a functional activation of bilateral intraparietal sulcus and left angular gyrus when
participants are engaged in a spatial number-response interference task (Cutini, Scarpa,
Scatturin, Dell'acqua, & Zorzi, 2012). The present study now extends these findings and
shows that the existing individual preferences in the association between numbers and space
(e.g. Fischer, 2006) relate to structural variance in right precuneus. Importantly, the precuneus
is known to be involved in the processing of various types of spatial information in different
domains and modalities, but the brain region is typically not assumed to play any specific
crucial role in the processing of numerical magnitude information.
Taking into account the current finding and the general function of the precuneus for
spatial processing, one might question whether spatial number-response interference effects
are actually informing us about core mechanisms of number processing, rather than about the
use of more general cognitive coding strategies. For instance, associations with space have
also been observed for a variety of ordinal/sequential information like letters of the alphabet,
months or days of the week (Gevers, Reynvoet & Fias, 2003; 2004), suggesting that spatial
associations are driven by any type of ordinal information and not specifically related to the
representation of numerical magnitude. Our current data are in line with this view, as they
suggest that the nature of the number-space mapping is not magnitude related, but related to a
brain structure involved in general spatial processing. On the other hand, differences in the
strength of the mapping between numbers and force production (FoNARC effect) were
shown to be related to structural variance in a brain region known to be crucial for processing
magnitude-related aspects of numbers. This emphasizes the importance of non-spatial
magnitude representations and the suitability of using non-spatial magnitude-related
number-response interference paradigms to investigate the mechanisms of numerical
processing.
Both behavioural and VBM results of the present study showed that the inter-individual
differences in the size of a SNARC and FoNARC effect are uncorrelated and independent of
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 16
each other, which strongly suggests that the two effects reflect different aspects of the
cognitive processing and representation of numerical information. The dissociation of spatial
and non-spatial representation of numerical size might have relevant implications for
education. While a number line mapping seems to be a suitable tool for children to visualize
numerical information (Fischer, Moeller, Bientzle, Cress & Nuerk, 2011), the success of this
method might vary tremendously from child to child. If numerical representations vary
strongly between individuals, as the current results suggest, identifying and supporting these
differences might be educationally beneficial. However, at this point in time, the relation
between differences in number representation and differences in number competence remain
largely unclear. While a relation between left angular gyrus activity and mathematical
competence has recently been reported (Grabner, Ansari, Resihofer, Stern, Ebner & Neuper,
2007), future research with a strong focus on inter-individual differences in numerical skills
will be needed to address this open question further.
Interpretational limitations of the current study
The exact nature of the grey matter volumetric differences identified with VBM is still
poorly understood, as they could be related to changes in neuropil, neuronal size, dendritic or
axonal arborisation, as well as cortical folding (Michelli, Price, Friston, & Ashburner, 2005).
This complicates the interpretation of any VBM study with respect to linking structural
variability to its underlying functionality. In the current study, this interpretational issue is
somehow reduced by the observation that the task-related structural variability occurs in
parietal regions functionally engaged during performance of those tasks. Although it is
generally assumed that larger grey matter volume reflects enhanced neuronal processing
(Kanai, Feilden, Firth & Rees, 2011; Mechelli, Crinion, Noppeney, O'Doherty, Ashburner,
Frackowiak & Price, 2004; Maguire, Gadian, Johnsrude, Good, Ashburner, Frackowiak &
Frith, 2000), future studies will need to detail the microstructural and computational
mechanisms associated with the present findings.
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 17
The structural findings emerged from an analysis focused on the parietal lobe. The
rationale of this choice was to include in the analysis posterior parietal areas previously
shown to be involved in number processing (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2008; Wu et al., 2009), and
active during performance of the tasks used in the current study. However, the structural
changes reported here could also be observed in a whole-brain analysis (right precuneus: Z =
4.40; left angular gyrus: Z = 4.42).
It is unclear in how far the brain regions identified in the current study are bound to
specific demands of the numerical tasks used. This is especially important for understanding
the role of left angular gyrus during number processing. Here, force production was used as
an instance of non-spatial sensorimotor magnitude. It remains to be seen whether the current
observations generalize from the domain of force production to other non-spatial
sensorimotor magnitudes.
Conclusion
Taken together, the current findings suggest that numerical cognition relies on multiple
mental representations of analogue magnitude using distinct neural implementations that are
linked to individual traits. We showed that the way we represent numerical size is not only
dependent on situational requirements of a given task, but also subject to inter-individual
differences. Importantly, these differences appear to be stable traits as they can be linked to
distinct structural variance in the posterior parietal cortex. Our finding of individual traits
stimulates new research to investigate whether these traits are innate (“nature”) or the result
of external factors and emerge only later during development (“nurture”; see also Dehaene,
1997) – a question whose answer will have wide implications for math education.
DIFFERENT BRAINS PROCESS NUMBERS DIFFERENTLY 18
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Pascal de Water and Paul Gaalman for technical support and
assistance during data acquisition, as well as Roi Cohen Kadosh for his valuable comments
during an early data analysis stage.
References
Adams, J. W. (2007). Individual differences in mathematical ability: genetic, cognitive and behavioural
factors. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 7(2), 97-103.
Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 278-291.
Arsalidou, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and
calculations. NeuroImage, 54(3), 2382-2393.
Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage, 38(1), 95-113.
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry--the methods. NeuroImage, 11(6 Pt
1), 805-821.
Badets, A., Andres, M., Di Luca, S., & Pesenti, M. (2007). Number magnitude potentiates action
judgements. Experimental Brain Research, 180(3), 525-534.
Bakola, S., Passarelli, L., Gamberini, M., Fattori, P., & Galletti, C. (2013). Cortical Connectivity
Suggests a Role in Limb Coordination for Macaque Area PE of the Superior Parietal Cortex. The
Journal of Neuroscience, 33(15), 6648-6658.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645.
Butterworth, B. (2010). Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 534-541.
Butterworth, B. (1999). The mathematical brain. Macmillan.
Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human
inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage,
33(2), 430-448.
Caspers, S., Eickhoff, S., Geyer, S., Scheperjans, F., Mohlberg, H., Zilles, K., & Amunts, K. (2008).
The human inferior parietal lobule in stereotaxic space. Brain Structure and Function, 212(6),
481-495.
Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and
behavioural correlates. Brain, 129(Pt 3), 564-583.
Choi, H.-J., Zilles, K., Mohlberg, H., Schleicher, A., Fink, G. R., Armstrong, E., & Amunts, K. (2006).
Cytoarchitectonic identification and probabilistic mapping of two distinct areas within the anterior
ventral bank of the human intraparietal sulcus. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 495(1), 53-69.
Cohen Kadosh, R., Lammertyn, J., & Izard, V. (2008). Are numbers special? An overview of
chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental and comparative studies of magnitude representation.
Progress in Neurobiology, 84(2), 132-147.
Cohen Kadosh, R. & Walsh, V. (2009). Numerical representation in the parietal lobes: abstract or not
abstract? The Behavioral and brain sciences, 32(3-4), 313-373.
Cutini, S., Scarpa, F., Scatturin, P., Dell'acqua, R., & Zorzi, M. (2012). Number-Space Interactions in
the Human Parietal Cortex: Enlightening the SNARC Effect with Functional Near-Infrared
Spectroscopy. Cerebral Cortex.
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42.
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number
magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396.
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. How the Mind Creates Mathematics.
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three Parietal Circuits for Number Processing.
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3-6), 487-506.
Dehaene, S. (2009). Origins of mathematical intuitions: the case of arithmetic. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 1156, 232-259.
de Hevia, M. D., Vallar, G., & Girelli, L. (2008). Visualizing numbers in the mind’s eye: the role of
visuo-spatial processes in numerical abilities. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(8),
1361-1372.
Delazer, M., Domahs, F., Bartha, L., Brenneis, C., Lochy, A., Trieb, T., & Benke, T. (2003). Learning
complex arithmetic--an fMRI study. Brain research. Cognitive Brain Research, 18(1), 76-88.
Eickhoff, S., Stephan, K., Mohlberg, H., & Grefkes, C. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining
probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage, 25(4), 1325-1335.
Fias, W., Brysbaert, F., Geypens, F., Ydewalle, G. (1996). The Importance of Magnitude Information in
Numerical Processing : Evidence from the SNARC Effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2(1), 95-110.
Fischer, M. (2006). The Future for Snarc Could Be Stark…. Cortex, 42(8), 1066-1068.
Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Sensori-motor spatial training
of number magnitude representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 177-183.
Gerstmann, J. (1940). Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left, agraphia and
acalculia—Local diagnostic value. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry, 44(2), 398-408.
Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is
spatially organized. Cognition, 87(3), B87-95.
Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2004). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is
spatially organized: evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 40(1), 171-172.
Göbel, S., Walsh, V., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2001). The mental number line and the human angular
gyrus. NeuroImage, 14(6), 1278-1289.
Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I. S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J., & Frackowiak, R. S. (2001).
A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage,
14(1 Pt 1), 21-36.
Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Reishofer, G., Stern, E., Ebner, F., & Neuper, C. (2007). Individual
differences in mathematical competence predict parietal brain activation during mental calculation.
NeuroImage, 38(2), 346-356.
Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neuper, C. (2009). To retrieve
or to calculate? Left angular gyrus mediates the retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem
solving. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 604-608.
Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in
parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(6), 435-448.
Holloway, I. D., Price, G. R., & Ansari, D. (2010). Common and segregated neural pathways for the
processing of symbolic and nonsymbolic numerical magnitude: an fMRI study. NeuroImage,
49(1), 1006-1017.
Ischebeck, A., Zamarian, L., Siedentopf, C., Koppelstätter, F., Benke, T., Felber, S., & Delazer, M.
(2006). How specifically do we learn? Imaging the learning of multiplication and subtraction.
NeuroImage, 30(4), 1365-1375.
Jones, E. G., Coulter, J. D., Hendry, S. H. (1987). Intracortical connectivity of architectonic fields in the
somatic sensory, motor and parietal cortex of monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 181.
291-347.
Kanai, R., Feilden, T., Firth, C., & Rees, G. (2011). Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain
Structure in Young Adults. Current Biology, 21(8), 677-680.
Krause, F., Bekkering, H. & Lindemann, O. (2013). A feeling for numbers: shared metric for symbolic
and tactile numerosities. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(January), 7.
Lacquaniti, F., Guigon, E., Bianchi, L., Ferraina, S., Caminiti, R. (1995). Representing spatial
information for limb movement: role of area 5 in the monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 5, 391-409.
Lindemann, O., Abolafia, J. M., Girardi, G., & Bekkering, H. (2007). Getting a grip on numbers:
numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human
Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1400-1409.
Lindemann, O., Rueschemeyer, S.-A., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Symbols in Numbers: From Numerals
to Magnitude Information. Commentary. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 32(3-4), 341-342.
Maguire, E. a, Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith,
C. D. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(8), 4398-4403.
Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C.
J. (2004). Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature, 431(7010), 757.
Mechelli, A., Price, C. J., Friston, K. J., & Ashburner, J. (2005). Voxel-Based Morphometry of the
Human Brain: Methods and Applications. Current Medical Imaging Reviews, 1, 105-113.
Morecraft, R., Cipolloni, P., Stilwell-Morecraft, K., Gedney, M., & Pandya, D. (2004). Cytoarchitecture
and cortical connections of the posterior cingulate and adjacent somatosensory fields in the rhesus
monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 469(1), 37–69.
Mountcastle, V. B., Lynch, J. C., Georgopoulos, A., Sakata, H., & Acuna, C. (1975). Posterior parietal
association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 38. 871-908.
Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality.
Nature, 215(5109), 1519-1520.
Oliveri, M., Vicario, C. M., Salerno, S., Koch, G., Turriziani, P., Mangano, R., Chillemi, G., et al.
(2008). Perceiving numbers alters time perception. Neuroscience Letters, 438(3), 308-311.
Petrides, M., & Pandya, D. N. (1984). Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal
region in the rhesus monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 228(1), 105–116.
Poser, B. A., Versluis, M. J., Hoogduin, J. M., & Norris, D. G. (2006). BOLD contrast sensitivity
enhancement and artifact reduction with multiecho EPI: Parallel-acquired
inhomogeneity-desensitized fMRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 55, 1227-1235.
Price, G. R., & Ansari, D. (2011). Symbol processing in the left angular gyrus: Evidence from passive
perception of digits. NeuroImage, 57(3), 1205-1211.
Rusconi, E., Walsh, V., & Butterworth, B. (2005). Dexterity with numbers: rTMS over left angular
gyrus disrupts finger gnosis and number processing. Neuropsychologia, 43(11), 1609-1624.
Santens, S., & Gevers, W. (2008). The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line. Cognition,
108(1), 263–270.
Scheperjans, F., Grefkes, C., Palomero-Gallagher, N., Schleicher, A., & Zilles, K. (2005). Subdivisions
of human parietal area 5 revealed by quantitative receptor autoradiography: a parietal region
between motor, somatosensory, and cingulate cortical areas. NeuroImage, 25(3), 975-992.
Scheperjans, F., Hermann, K., Eickhoff, S. B., Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., & Zilles, K. (2008a).
Observer-independent cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human superior parietal cortex. Cerebral
Cortex, 18(4), 846-867.
Scheperjans, F., Eickhoff, S. B., Hömke, L., Mohlberg, H., Hermann, K., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K.
(2008b). Probabilistic maps, morphometry, and variability of cytoarchitectonic areas in the human
superior parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18(9), 2141-2157.
Tzelgov, J., Meyer, J., & Henik, A. (1992). Automatic and intentional processing of numerical
information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(1), 166.
Vierck, E., & Kiesel, A. (2010). Congruency effects between number magnitude and response force.
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(1), 204-209.
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483-488.
Wood, G., Nuerk, H., & Willmes, K. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A
meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science, 50(4), 489-525.
Wu, S. S., Chang, T. T., Majid, A., Caspers, S., Eickhoff, S. B., & Menon, V. (2009). Functional
Heterogeneity of Inferior Parietal Cortex during Mathematical Cognition Assessed with
Cytoarchitectonic Probability Maps. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2930-2945.
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Relative changes in grey matter volume in the posterior parietal cortex, related to spatial and non-spatial
representations of numerical size. Individuals' disposition to associate numbers with spatial responses predicts local grey
matter volume in right precuneus (blue). The disposition to link numbers to force production predicts grey matter volume in
left angular gyrus (red). Thresholded at p < 0.001, uncorrected, for illustrative purposes.
Figure 2. Correlations between grey matter volume and number-response interference effects, corrected for average
reaction time and total intracranial volume, demonstrating a double-dissociation between brain regions and representations
of numerical size. Significant correlations (as revealed by the multiple regression analysis) are plotted with a continuous
regression line.