ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

We studied the ability of domestic cats to distinguish familiarity based on faecal odours. This was evaluated by comparing the sniffing duration of cats’ own, familiar, and unfamiliar faeces. We found that (1) sniffing durations differed between unfamiliar faeces and the other types of faeces, (2) sniffing durations of faeces of the same unfamiliar individuals decreased over time, and (3) sniffing durations toward unfamiliar faeces increased after change of donors. These results indicate that domestic cats can distinguish the faecal odours based on familiarity. This ability could be adaptive for domestic cats to maintain their social relationships.
Content may be subject to copyright.
SHORT COMMUNICATION
Do faecal odours enable domestic cats (Felis catus) to distinguish
familiarity of the donors?
Miyabi Nakabayashi Ryohei Yamaoka
Yoshihiro Nakashima
Received: 3 April 2011 / Accepted: 28 November 2011 / Published online: 3 January 2012
ÓJapan Ethological Society and Springer 2011
Abstract We studied the ability of domestic cats to dis-
tinguish familiarity based on faecal odours. This was
evaluated by comparing the sniffing duration of cats’ own,
familiar, and unfamiliar faeces. We found that (1) sniffing
durations differed between unfamiliar faeces and the other
types of faeces, (2) sniffing durations of faeces of the same
unfamiliar individuals decreased over time, and (3) sniffing
durations toward unfamiliar faeces increased after change
of donors. These results indicate that domestic cats can
distinguish the faecal odours based on familiarity. This
ability could be adaptive for domestic cats to maintain their
social relationships.
Keywords Felis catus Faecal odours Familiarity
Sniffing durations Olfactory communication
Introduction
Olfaction plays key roles in communication in various
mammalian species. Many species of Carnivora use olfac-
tion for intra-specific communication and inter-specific
communication among individuals and groups (reviewed by
Dielenberg and McGregor 2001; Gorman and Trowbridge
1989; Macdonald 1980). They rely on various odour sour-
ces such as urine for their scent marking (Gorman and
Trowbridge 1989; Macdonald 1980). Faeces are one such
odour source and provide information about the sex, rela-
tive health conditions, and reproductive status of individu-
als (Gorman and Trowbridge 1989). Faeces also provide
spatial and temporal records of an individual’s and group’s
movements and behaviours, even in the absence of the
producer (reviewed by Brown and Macdonald 1985).
For domestic cats, familiarity may be important for the
relationship with the other individuals. The domestic cat is
known to form groups when the food resources are suffi-
cient (Kerby and Macdonald 1988; Liberg and Sandell
1988). Among group members, they exhibit amicable
relationships (Izawa et al. 1982; Macdonald et al. 1987). In
contrast, unfamiliar cats are prone to antagonistic behav-
iours from the other cats (Crowell-Davis et al. 2004)
because an unfamiliar individual would cause a greater
threat than well-known individuals (Temeles 1994).
Some scents may be important tools for adjusting the
social interactions of domestic cats (Natoli 1985, Natoli
et al. 2001). They occasionally leave some faeces exposed
when they are out hunting or on the periphery of their
territories, but they usually bury them after defecation near
their nesting sites or within their home range for hygienic
reasons or avoidance of informing others about their
physical conditions (Bradshaw and Cameron-Beaumont
2000; Case 2003; Liberg 1980). Thus, faeces are likely to
play a role in olfactory communication in domestic cats.
While urinary odours are often presumed to be olfactory
communication tools, faecal odours have rarely been
studied despite their potential importance.
If faecal odours play an important role in their society,
cats might have the ability to distinguish other cats by their
M. Nakabayashi (&)
Wildlife Research Center of Kyoto University,
2-24 Tanaka-Sekiden-cho, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8203, Japan
e-mail: bqdbd255@yahoo.co.jp
R. Yamaoka
Kyoto Institute of Technology, Matsugasaki,
Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8585, Japan
Y. Nakashima
Graduate School of Science, Kyoto University, Oiwake-cho,
Kitashirakawa, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8502, Japan
123
J Ethol (2012) 30:325–329
DOI 10.1007/s10164-011-0321-x
faecal odours. This ability might be very important for
perception of the existence of unfamiliar individuals, pos-
sible competitors for limited resources. In this study, the
possibility of domestic cats’ distinguishing familiarity based
on the faecal odours was studied by comparing the sniffing
durations of their own, familiar, and unfamiliar faeces.
Materials and methods
Collection of samples
The faeces of domestic cats were obtained from five dif-
ferent groups: Hyogo group, Kyoto 1 group, Kyoto 2 group,
Osaka 1 group, and Osaka 2 group. The Kyoto 1 group
consisted of four individuals that were kept in a cat cafe
´,a
theme cafe
´where customers can watch and play with cats.
In contrast, all the other cats were kept by private citizens;
the Hyogo group consisted of three individuals, the Kyoto 2
and Osaka 1 group consisted of two individuals, and the
Osaka 2 group consisted of only one individual.
We chose sexually mature individuals as sample donors
and recipients. For all these individuals, we checked sex,
age, breed, and the history of castration. All the cats were
fed the same food (Adult Fit 32; Royal Canin Japon, Tokyo,
Japan) from 3 days before sample collection until the end of
the experiment. Faeces were collected in resealable plastic
bags and stored in styrol boxes with cooling elements until
experiments, for a maximum of 3 days.
Behavioural experiment
To test the ability of domestic cats to distinguish between
faeces, we observed behavioural responses when presenting
the following three types of faeces simultaneously; (1) own
faeces, (2) familiar faeces, which were excreted by cats
belonging to the same group as the recipient cat, and (3)
unfamiliar faeces, which were excreted by cats belonging to
a different group than the recipient cat’s group.
All the observations were carried out in the rooms of the
cat owners, which typically measured about 10 m
2
, in the
Hyogo, Osaka, and Kyoto areas. In the centre of the rooms,
3 plastic plates (15 cm 915 cm) were placed on the
ground in a straight line at 15-cm intervals, and three types
of fresh faeces (collected within 3 days) were randomly
placed on the plates (Fig. 1). The order of the plates was
changed randomly before each experiment in order to
prevent the cats from remembering the order of the faecal
samples. The plastic plates were not reused after each
experiment in order to avoid cats sniffing the odours left on
the plates from previous trials.
We determined a point on the circumference of the
circle of 100-cm radius around the central plate where we
placed the recipient cat at the start of each experiment. The
circles were not written in the experimental fields. That
point was located at 100 cm from the central plate, and the
same distance from the other plates. Sniffing durations of
recipient cats for each faecal sample were recorded for ten
minutes after the cats entered the area within a 100-cm
radius around the central plate. This experiment was
repeated three times, hereafter referred to as 3-day suc-
cessive bouts. In all experiments, sniffing duration was
recorded with a precision of 0.01 s as the time during
which a cat’s nose came to within about 5 cm of the faeces.
Observations were made from a point situated at least 3 m
from the plates.
In this study, habituation to sniffing of cat faeces was
said to occur when sniffing duration decreased across
experiments, indicating that the recipient cats perceived the
stimulus as the same, as described in Halpin (1974) and
Johnston et al. (1993). To assess this paradigm, we used a
new batch of faeces for all three types of faeces at every
new bout. At the same time, the donors of unfamiliar faeces
were changed. If there were more than two familiar cats,
the donors of familiar faeces were also changed. When
they failed to discriminate a novel faecal odours under this
procedure, it may be caused either by a lack of interest in
the faecal odours or by an inability to distinguish between
faecal odours. The experiments were performed only once
a day per recipient cat.
In each place, we conducted a different experiment,
hereafter referred to as test 1, test 2, and test 3, respec-
tively. In test 1, experiments were conducted in Hyogo.
The bout of experiments was measured four times each on
three recipient cats (#1 and 3, $2, all in the Hyogo group)
for 12 consecutive days. In test 2, experiments were
Fig. 1 Schematic illustration of the experimental room and apparatus
used. The dashed circle was not drawn in the room itself
326 J Ethol (2012) 30:325–329
123
conducted in Osaka. The bout of experiments was mea-
sured twice on a recipient cat (#10 in the Osaka 1 group)
for 6 consecutive days. In test 3, experiments were con-
tinued for 3 consecutive days in Kyoto using different
faeces of the same individuals on 2 recipient cats ($8 and
#9 in the Kyoto 2 group). After each observation, the
faeces were collected and stored to avoid faecal desiccation
and loss of odours.
Statistical analyses
To test whether cats distinguish between different types of
faecal odours, differences in the sniffing durations of each
type of faeces were compared using repeated one-way
analysis of variance (ANOVA). The Bonferroni post hoc
procedure (with an adjusted alpha of 0.017) was applied to
assess differences in the sniffing durations of each type of
faeces within each experimental day, although these post-
priori tests may be statistically risky.
Habituation to repeated exposures to faecal odours from
the same donors was tested by comparing time spent
sniffing each type of faeces on the first and third experi-
mental day with paired ttests.
To test whether cats distinguish between novel faecal
odours and that of less novel which they have experienced
in sniffing during this experiment, differences in the
sniffing durations of faeces just before and after presenta-
tions of a new batch of faeces for all 3 types of faeces were
compared using paired ttests.
All analyses were conducted using the statistical pro-
gramme R 2.10.1 (R Development Core Team 2009) with
the critical value set to 0.05 unless otherwise indicated.
Results
Collection of samples
In total, 46 faecal samples were collected from seven
domestic cats during May to December 2009 and May
2011. Totals of 24 samples were collected from the Hyogo
group, 14 from the Kyoto 1 group, 4 from the Kyoto 2
group, 2 from the Osaka 1 group, and 2 from the Osaka 2
group. The profiles of the tested cats are shown in Table 1
and the donor sources of the faeces used are shown in
Table 2.
Behavioural experiment
With respect to time spent sniffing three types of faecal
odours, the ANOVA reveals that there was significant
difference among types (F
2,141
=28.2, P\0.001). On all
experimental days, the sniffing duration of unfamiliar
faeces was significantly longer than that of their own faeces
(first day: t=4.26, P\0.001; second day: t=3.70,
P\0.001; third day: t=3.43, P\0.001) and that of the
familiar faeces (first day: t=3.92, P\0.001; second day:
t=3.82, P\0.001; third day: t=4.50, P\0.001).
However, the sniffing durations showed no significant
differences between the familiar faeces and their own
faeces on all experimental days (first day: t=0.34,
P[0.1; second day: t=0.12, P=0.90; third day:
t=1.07, P=0.29).
The sniffing durations of unfamiliar faeces on the first
experimental day were significantly longer than those on
the third day (t=5.37, P\0.001). However, the sniffing
durations of familiar and own faeces showed no significant
differences between the first and third experimental days
(familiar faeces: t=1.31, P=0.21; own faeces: t=0.79,
P=0.44) (Fig. 2).
Table 1 Profiles of all the tested domestic cats
ID Sex Age Breed Castration
a
Group
b
1#15 Mix Y Hyogo
2$7 Mix Y Hyogo
3#5 Mix Y Hyogo
4#2 Mix Y Kyoto 1
5$2 Mix Y Kyoto 1
6#2 Mix Kyoto 1
7$2 Mix Y Kyoto 1
8$0.75 Mix Kyoto 2
9#0.75 Mix Kyoto 2
10 #1 Mix Y Osaka 1
11 $Unknown Mix Y Osaka 1
12 $2 Mix Y Osaka 2
The number after the name of the area indicates the place within that
area
a
Yindicates the cat was castrated
b
Group shows the names of the area where the cats lived
Table 2 The donor sources of the faeces presented to the recipient
cats
ID of recipient
cats
ID of familiar
donor
ID of unfamiliar
donor
1 2(1,3), 3(2,4) 4(4), 5(1), 6(3), 7(2)
2 1(1,3), 3(2,4) 4(2), 5(4), 6(1), 7(3)
3 1(1,3), 2(2,4) 4(1), 5(3), 6(2), 7(4)
8912
9812
10 11 1(1), 3(2)
ID numbers are equivalent to those in Table 1
Numbers in parentheses indicate the number of the 3-day successive
trial bouts, while their absence means the same donors were used in
all the experiments
J Ethol (2012) 30:325–329 327
123
In test 1 and test 2, when the faeces were replaced with a
new batch of faeces (on the fourth, seventh, and tenth
experimental day in test 1, and on the fourth experimental
day in test 2), the sniffing durations of the novel batch of
unfamiliar faeces were significantly longer (t=2.39,
P=0.04) than those that the recipient cats had experi-
enced sniffing on the previous day (Fig. 3). Meanwhile,
sniffing durations of the familiar (t=0.24, P=0.82) and
their own (t=1.92, P=0.09) faeces showed no signifi-
cant differences (Table 2).
Discussion
In this study, we obtained three important findings. First,
the domestic cats spent a significantly longer time sniffing
the faeces of unfamiliar conspecifics than sniffing those of
familiar conspecifics and themselves. Second, sniffing
duration of faeces of the same unfamiliar individuals
decreased over time. Third, the time spent sniffing unfa-
miliar faeces was longer when they had not experienced it
before compared to when they had experienced it before.
These points suggest that domestic cats have the ability to
distinguish the familiarity based on faecal odours. Natoli
(1985) reported that domestic cats sniffed sprayed urine of
unfamiliar males longer than that of familiar males. We
obtained quite similar results here using faeces. In addition,
our results also found that cats habituate to the faecal
odours of unfamiliar individuals.
The ability to distinguish familiarity based on faecal
odours using their olfactory memories could be adaptive in
the social system of domestic cats. For example, during the
oestrous season, male cats extend the moving range,
thereby increasing the probability of encountering several
male cats within their ranges (Izawa et al. 1982; Liberg and
Sandell 1988). These abilities allow roaming cats to per-
ceive the presences of unfamiliar males in the area they
range and to evade unnecessary costs of fighting. The
faeces deposited within their territory could also be used as
territorial markers. Investigating familiarity by the faecal
odours would be efficient, because they can detect
unknown individuals without actual encounter.
Our results do not suggest that domestic cats distinguish
their own faeces from that of familiar individuals. With
regard to adaptation, it seems preferable to have ability to
distinguish their own faecal odours from familiar faecal
odours, because they exhibit antagonistic behaviour among
group members in the oestrous season since they become
competitors for mates as unfamiliar individuals especially
within males (Natoli and De Vito 1991; Yamane et al.
1996). However, our results do not always suggest that they
have no ability to distinguish chemical substances on their
faeces. Except in the oestrous season, they probably do not
have to be cautious about the presence of the other indi-
viduals under variable conditions of nutrition and season.
Further studies of their behavioural changes in sniffing
conspecific faeces along with seasonal change are never-
theless needed.
Acknowledgments We are deeply grateful to the cat keepers for
their warm assistance in sample collection and conducting behavioral
experiments. Their understandings and enthusiasms made this work
possible. We sincerely thank Assoc. Prof. Dr. T. Akino in Kyoto
Institute of Technology for his valuable supports and guidance. We
are also grateful to Dr. H. Samejima and Dr. R. Ito for their kind
advices and comments on earlier drafts of this paper. We thank two
Fig. 2 Sniffing duration of unfamiliar (n=6), own (n=6), and
familiar (n=6) faeces. Values are given as the mean ?SE. Asterisks
indicate significant differences of the sniffing duration between and
within types of faeces (P\0.001)
Fig. 3 Sniffing duration of unfamiliar, own, and familiar faeces on
the days previous and subsequent to the exchange the faeces for a new
batch of faeces. Values are given as the mean ?SE. Asterisk
indicates a significant difference between the duration of sniffing
unfamiliar faeces on the previous days and that on the subsequent
days after exchange of faeces (P\0.001)
328 J Ethol (2012) 30:325–329
123
anonymous reviewers for critical readings and helpful comments that
greatly improved previous versions of the manuscript.
References
Bradshaw J, Cameron-Beaumont C (2000) The signaling repertoire of
the domestic cat and its undomesticated relatives. In: Turner DC,
Bateson P (eds) The domestic cat: the biology of its behaviour.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 67–93
Brown RE, Macdonald DW (1985) Social odors in mammals. Oxford
University Press, Oxford
Case LP (2003) The cat: its behavior, nutrition and health. In: Case
LP, Helms K, MacAllister B (eds) The cat: its behavior, nutrition
and health. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, pp 131–150
Crowell-Davis SL, Curtis TM, Knowles RJ (2004) Social organiza-
tion in the cat: a modern understanding. J Feline Med Surg
6:19–28
Dielenberg RA, McGregor IS (2001) Defensive behavior in rats
towards predatory odors: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:
597–609
Gorman ML, Trowbridge BJ (1989) The role of odor in the social
lives of carnivores. In: Gittleman JL (ed) Carnivore behavior,
ecology and evolution. Chapman and Hall, New York, pp 57–88
Halpin ZT (1974) Individual differences in the biological odors of the
Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). Behav Biol 11:
253–259
Izawa M, Doi T, Ono Y (1982) Grouping patterns of feral cats (Felis
catus) living on a small island in Japan. Jpn J Ecol 32:373–382
Johnston RE, Derzie A, Chiang G, Jernigan P (1993) Individual scent
signatures in golden hamsters: evidence for specialization of
function. Anim Behav 45:1061–1070
Kerby G, Macdonald DW (1988) Cat society and the consequences of
colony size. In: Turner DC, Bateson P (eds) The domestic cat:
the biology of its behaviour. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge, pp 67–81
Liberg O (1980) Spacing patterns in a population of rural free
roaming domestic cats. Oikos 35:336–349
Liberg O, Sandell M (1988) Spatial organisation and reproductive
tactics in the domestic cat and other felids. In: Turner DC,
Bateson P (eds) The domestic cat: the biology of its behaviour.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 83–98
Macdonald DW (1980) Patterns of scent marking with urine and feces
amongst carnivore communities. In: Stoddart DM (ed) Olfaction
in mammals, vol 45. Academic, London, pp 107–139
Macdonald DW, Apps PJ, Carr GM, Kerby G (1987) Social
dynamics, nursing coalitions and infanticide among farm cats,
Felis catus. Adv Ethol 28:1–66
Natoli E (1985) Behavioural responses of urban feral cats to different
types of urine marks. Behaviour 94:234–243
Natoli E, De Vito E (1991) Agonistic behaviour, dominance rank and
copulatory success in a large multi-male feral cat, Felis catus L.,
colony in central Rome. Anim Behav 42:227–241
Natoli E, Baggio A, Pontier D (2001) Male and female agonistic and
affiliative relationships in a social group of farm cats (Felis catus
L.). Behav Process 53:137–143
R Development Core Team (2009) R: language and environment for
statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical computing,
Vienna, Austria. http://cran.r-project.org/
Temeles EJ (1994) The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when
are they ‘dear enemies’? Anim Behav 47:339–350
Yamane A, Doi T, Ono Y (1996) Mating behaviors, courtship rank
and mating success of male feral cat (Felis catus). J Ethol
14:35–44
J Ethol (2012) 30:325–329 329
123
... These secretions contain volatile and non-volatile compounds, such as fatty acids, aldehydes, and phenols (Miyazaki et al., 20032006;Uetake et al., 2018), being crucial for obtaining information about the environment and other animals, serving as important vehicles for chemical communication (Kasbaoui et al., 2022a). There is also evidence that domestic cats can use olfactory communication in feces to establish social relationships, identifying individuals based on familiarity (Nakabayashi et al., 2012). In urine production, cats generate felinine, a cauxin-dependent substance, resulting in specific volatile compounds identified as species-specific odorants (Miyazaki et al., 2008). ...
... For cats, the information conveyed by their waste matter is crucial for enabling encounters or even avoiding conflicts (Natoli and De Vito, 1991;Yamaguchi et al., 2019). Furthermore, cats can distinguish the odors of excreta based on familiarity (Nakabayashi et al., 2012). In this sense, the elimination behavior of non-receptive female cats observed in our study agrees with literature reports (Hendriks et al., 1995). ...
... Feline excretions contain various volatile and non-volatile organic compounds, some of which play important roles in communication among felines of the same species, as reported by Nakabayashi et al. (2012). Additionally, these compounds contribute to inter-species communication between cats and other prey, such as small rodents; for example, the house mouse (Mus musculus) and Brandt's voles (Lasiopodomys brandtii), as indicated by Hegab et al. (2014), Voznessenskaya et al., 2016, andGrau et al. (2021). ...
Article
Olfactory communication is an important means of chemocommunication among animals, particularly in relation to reproduction. This study investigated the elimination behavior patterns of defecation and urination in female cats (Felis catus) from different age and reproductive status groups, including kittens, young adults, non-receptive adults, and nursing adults. Special attention was given to the number of movements (using the forelimbs) made during excreta concealment and the time (in seconds) spent on this process. Comparisons were conducted separately for feces and urine between the age groups. Significant differences (P <0.05) were observed in urine processing among non-receptive and nursing females, as well as kittens. Non-receptive females consistently made fewer movements to cover their urine, and less time was dedicated to this process. Comparisons between feces and urine within the same age group also revealed significant behavioral differences (P <0.05). Feces required a greater number of movements and more time compared to urine. These findings emphasize how different phases of a cat’s life cycle influence the behavior of burying excretions (feces and urine), underscoring the importance of these excreta in olfactory communication.
... Some animals investigate odour from novel or unfamiliar conspecifics for longer than they do odour from a familiar one (e.g. Carr et al., 1976;Horowitz, 2017;Nakabayashi et al., 2012). The shorter investigation duration of odour from familiar conspecifics is interpreted as an indicator of memory (i.e. ...
Article
Full-text available
Odour serves as a long-lasting cue or signal that animals with developed olfactory systems rely on heavily for communication. The ability to discriminate a particular odour modulates the social behaviours of a receiver. Animals investigate odours from unfamiliar conspecifics more than they do those from familiar ones, either as a means of choosing a mate or to avoid competition. The African painted dog, Lycaon pictus, which is known for its sophisticated sociality and extremely strong body odour, appears to rely heavily on odour communication. However, empirical research on odour-related behaviour in this species is limited. We investigated whether captive African painted dogs discriminated between their own odours and those of conspecifics, between familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics, and between the sexes. Our behavioural experiments showed that subjects investigated other conspecifics' odours longer than they did their own odour. Detailed analysis showed that they spent more time investigating unfamiliar conspecifics than familiar ones. The effect of donor sex on the investigation duration was not significant when comparing odours from males and females. However, the investigation time of odours from females was longer than that of their own, with no such difference observed between odours from males and their own. This study is the first to demonstrate that the African painted dog is capable of discriminating its own odour from the odours of conspecifics, between familiar and unfamiliar animals and between the sexes. This research provides fundamental knowledge of odour communication in this species.
... Natoli found that cats investigated urine sprayed from an unfamiliar male tom longer than urine sprayed from a familiar individual. This indicates that FRCs discriminated between familiar and unfamiliar odors [54], similar to results seen with pet house cats [56]. Given the variability of cat sociability toward humans, with some cats being highly social [57], the fact FRCs display social behavior to people, the prevalence of FRCs in human spaces, and the success of researchers currently studying socio-cognitive abilities in FRDs [58,59], it appears research into FRC-human social cognition is an important area of future research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite the diversity of social situations in which cats live, the degree to which free-ranging cats (FRCs) are social is still debated. The aim of this review is to explore the literature on the social behavior of FRCs. A search of two major databases revealed that observations of intraspecies and interspecies social interactions have been conducted. The intraspecific social dynamics of FRCs differ based on group of cats surveyed. Some groups display strong social bonds and preferential affiliations, while other groups are more loosely associated and display little to no social interaction. Factors impacting FRC conspecific interactions include cat body size, cat social rank, cat individuality, cat age, relationship to conspecific (kin/familiar), cat sex, level of human caretaking, presence of food, the health of the individual, or sexual status of conspecifics. Interspecies interactions also occur with humans and wildlife. The human’s sex and the weather conditions on the day of interaction have been shown to impact FRC social behavior. Interactions with wildlife were strongly linked to the timing of cat feeding events. These findings support the idea that FRCs are “social generalists” who display flexibility in their social behavior. The social lives of FRCs exist, are complex, and deserve further study.
... Although woolly monkeys have never been observed performing coprophagy to our knowledge, the benefits associated with feeding on others' feces may also have precluded the evolution of the ability to discriminate between own and conspecifics' feces. Alternatively, while such a discrimination could rely on olfactory cues (Johnston et al., 1993;Nakabayashi et al., 2012;Poirotte et al., 2017;Sarabian et al., 2020), the experimental context of the study may have inhibited their ability to distinguish the two types of fecal substrate. This study is the first to our knowledge to compare hosts' avoidance behaviors towards their own and conspecifics' feces and additional studies, particularly in the wild, are required to understand whether animals modulate fecal avoidance depending on fecal origin. ...
Article
Objectives Parasite selection pressures have driven the evolution of numerous behavioral defenses in host species, but recent studies revealed individual variation in their expression. As little is known about the factors causing heterogeneity among individuals in infection-avoidance behaviors, we investigated in woolly monkeys (Lagothrix lagotricha poeppigii) the influence of several environmental and individual characteristics on the tendency to avoid food contaminated by soil and by their own and conspecifics' feces. Materials and methods We conducted feeding tests on 40 semi-free ranging individuals rescued from the pet trade. Using generalized linear mixed models, we investigated the effect of season, sex, age, dominance rank, and exposure to non-natural living conditions on feeding decisions. Results Woolly monkeys did not avoid soil-contaminated food and equally avoided food contaminated by their own and conspecifics' feces. Individuals varied greatly in their level of fecal avoidance. Only females exhibited strong avoidance of fecally contaminated food, but adapted their behavior to food availability, highlighting the trade-off between nutritional intake and parasite avoidance. Additionally, low-ranking females, less competitive over food resources, exhibited lower avoidance than dominant ones. Juveniles were more cautious than adults, possibly to compensate for a higher parasite susceptibility. Finally, we reported an unknown effect of exposure to non-natural living conditions on behavioral defenses, as animals kept as household pets for an extended period apparently lost their ability to avoid fecally contaminated food. Conclusion We argue that striving to understand variation in infection-avoidance behaviors in natural populations is crucial to predict disease spread and inform conservation policies.
Article
Olfactory enrichment is a strategy that can improve welfare among animals managed in captivity, such as household domestic cats. Catnip (Nepeta cataria) and silver vine (Actinidia polygama) that produce iridoids are used as olfactory enrichments for cats, but little is known about the safety or the best plant resources to use that maximize positive cat responses. We report physiological effects and suitable harvest and drying methods for using silver vine as olfactory enrichment. Continuous exposure of cats to silver vine showed no hallmarks of addictive behavior, while blood indicators of stress and hepatic or renal injury showed no increase in cats stimulated with it. Drying the leaves changed the iridoid profile, enhancing the feline response. In conclusion, dried silver vine leaves are the most suitable resource for developing olfactory enrichment that maximizes feline typical response, which would not result in dependence, stress, or toxicity to the liver or kidneys in cats.
Article
Feces littered on the ground by free-roaming cats contain bacteria, viruses, and parasites and pose a significant health risk to humans. The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of communal litter box provision on the defecation behavior of a free-roaming cat population. The study was conducted at H temple and its graveyard in the uptown area of old-town Onomichi, Japan. Cat feces were collected and weighed once a week for 4 weeks, at five popular defecation sites in the temple precincts and graveyard, to assess the quantity of feces left by the cats. A commercial cat repellent was then applied to the ground at 11 sites, including the five popular defecation sites, and six communal litter boxes, created by filling repurposed plastic planters with cat litter, were provided at different sites. The feces in the six litter boxes and on the ground at the five defecation sites were collected and weighed once a week for 14 weeks. The behavior of the cats around the litter boxes and defecation sites was captured using trail cameras. The total weight of the feces collected from the ground before the application of the litter boxes and cat repellent was 939 g. Three adult cats were mainly responsible for the feces on the ground. The amount of feces found on the ground around the temple decreased gradually and significantly after the provision application of the litter boxes and repellent, and reached 0 g in the final week of the study. In contrast, the average weight of the feces in the six litter boxes increased gradually and significantly, and reached 65.7 g/litter box/week in the 14th week. The results showed that the provision of litter boxes and the use of repellent is effective in changing the defecation behavior of ownerless free-roaming cats. We recommend promoting the provision of litter boxes to free-roaming cats to reduce fecal pollution in Onomichi and engaging with local cat feeders to participate in the management of the litter boxes, such as cleaning and changing the litter.
Article
Discusses variation in Felis catus social behaviour, distinguishing between: 1) populations in which adult females are generally solitary and those where they are gregarious; 2) variation in group size and social structure, and 3) individual variation within and between age and sex classes in terms of the nature of social relationships. Specifically, note is taken of the significance of spacing, encounters, use of social odours, group size and structure, and social dynamics. Attention is paid to the nature and consequences of the social hierarchy that is established in farm cat societies, which varies considerably between populations. -S.J.Yates
Article
The range was composed of 3 characteristic components such as a feeding site, resting sites and paths. Each cat used only one feeding site and did not switch it seasonally. The cats utilizing the same feeding site organized 'feeding group'. Synchronization of feeding activity and overlapping ranges of the members of the same feeding group were observed. These features of feeding group show the amicable relationship among the members. This probably results from adaptation to a clumped distribution of abundant food resource. -from Authors
Chapter
Discusses patterns in density and home range size, there being great variability in each for both sexes in Felis catus. Group living is usually associated with females and kittens, though adult males are sometimes included. Roaming behaviour and overlap in male home ranges are noted. Mating tactics are discussed: there is no active mate choice in females, who mate with the most dominant male present; and although close kin matings are not uncommon, inbreeding is often avoided by females in oestrus temporarily leaving groups which contained related males. Comparison is made with spatial behaviour in other felids. -S.J.Yates