ArticlePDF Available

‘Nudging’ hotel guests to reduce food waste as a win–win environmental measure

Authors:
  • CICERO Center for International Climate Research

Abstract and Figures

We show that two simple and nonintrusive ‘nudges’–reducing plate size and providing social cues–reduce the amount of food waste in hotel restaurants by around 20%. The results are statistically significant. They are also environmentally substantial as food waste is a major contributor to climate change and other forms of environmental degradation. Given the magnitude of the contribution of food waste to global environmental change, it is surprising that this issue has not received greater attention. The measures reduce the amount of food the restaurants need to purchase, and there is no change in guest satisfaction, making it likely that profits will increase. The measures thus constitute potential win–win opportunities.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/authorsrights
Author's personal copy
Economics Letters 119 (2013) 325–327
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Economics Letters
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ecolet
‘Nudging’ hotel guests to reduce food waste as a win–win
environmental measure
Steffen Kallbekken 1, Håkon Sælen
CICERO Center for International Climate and Environmental Research Oslo, P.O. Box 1129 Blindern, 0318, Oslo, Norway
highlights
Wasted food embodies large environmental impacts.
We conduct a field experiment to test two ‘nudges’ to reduce food waste.
Both measures reduce food waste in hotel restaurants by around 20%.
The measures also reduce private costs, making a win–win outcome likely.
article info
Article history:
Received 20 December 2012
Received in revised form
23 February 2013
Accepted 8 March 2013
Available online 18 March 2013
JEL classification:
Q2
Q3
Keywords:
Field experiment
Food waste
Behavioral economics
Climate policy
Choice architecture
Nudge
abstract
We show that two simple and nonintrusive ‘nudges’ reducing plate size and providing social cues
reduce the amount of food waste in hotel restaurants by around 20%. The results are statistically
significant. They are also environmentally substantial as food waste is a major contributor to climate
change and other forms of environmental degradation. Given the magnitude of the contribution of food
waste to global environmental change, it is surprising that this issue has not received greater attention.
The measures reduce the amount of food the restaurants need to purchase, and there is no change in guest
satisfaction, making it likely that profits will increase. The measures thus constitute potential win–win
opportunities.
©2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
The environmental impact of food provision is well-known.
Food accounts for 20% of global greenhouse gas emissions
(Hertwich and Peters, 2009), and 92% of the global water footprint
is related to agriculture (UNEP, 2012). In addition are issues such as
land degradation, overfishing and local air and water pollution. The
fact that roughly one-third of all food is lost or wasted (Gustavsson
et al., 2011) has received less attention. It therefore appears that
food waste is a substantial, but largely neglected contributor to
environmental change. The IPCC (Metz et al., 2007), for instance,
Corresponding author. Tel.: +47 22 85 85 63.
E-mail addresses: steffen.kallbekken@cicero.uio.no (S. Kallbekken),
hakon.salen@cicero.uio.no,correctomundo@gmail.com (H. Sælen).
1Tel.: +47 22 85 87 58.
does not list any measures to reduce food waste among the more
than 50 mitigation policies highlighted in its summary for policy
makers.
Choice architecture can be used to alter people’s behavior in
predictable ways (Thaler and Sunstein, 2008). From obesity and
nutritional research we know that ‘‘the eating situation often (but
not always) provides clues allowing us to infer how much we can
eat without eating an inappropriately large amount’’ (Herman and
Polivy, 2005). These clues form a part of the choice architecture,
and can be used to alter behavior. Our focus is on reducing food
waste, and thereby also reducing emissions of greenhouse gases.
We take as our starting point the observation that some of the
measures aimed at reducing food intake have been reported to also
reduce food waste (Freedman and Brochado, 2010).
Our study includes a field experiment (Harrison and List,
2004) where we intervene to change two variables of interest,
and a complementary observational study, based on pre-existing
variation in one of the variables of interest. In the field experiment
0165-1765/$ see front matter ©2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2013.03.019
Author's personal copy
326 S. Kallbekken, H. Sælen / Economics Letters 119 (2013) 325–327
we test the effectiveness of two treatments in reducing the amount
of food waste generated. Both treatments rely on influencing
consumption norms through external cues.
The first treatment concerns the effect of plate size on the
amount of food waste. It has been argued that ‘‘plate shape and size
delineate norms for appropriate amounts of food to eat at a meal
(Sobal and Wansink, 2007) and it has been shown that’’. .. big bowls
lead to overserving, small bowls lead to underserving. .. ? (Ittersum
and Wansink, 2012). In addition to the social cue it provides, larger
plates might also contribute to people serving and consuming
more food due to visual illusions that lead to biased perceptions
of how much food is served or consumed (Ittersum and Wansink,
2012). Combining this with the finding that increased portion
size leads to both increased food intake and increased food waste
(Freedman and Brochado, 2010), it seems a reasonable hypothesis
that decreasing plate size will decrease the amount of food waste.
In the field experiment the typical plate size reduction was from
24 to 21 cm (the plates were used for breakfast buffets, and, if
applicable, also at lunch and dinner buffets). In the observational
study the average plate size ranged from 15 to 28 cm (average
24.0 cm).
The second treatment is to provide a more direct social cue by
displaying a sign at the buffet that encourages restaurant guests
to help themselves more than once. The text reads, in seven
different languages: ‘‘Welcome back! Again! And again! Visit our
buffet many times. That’s better than taking a lot once’’. The sign
is intended to make it salient that it is socially acceptable to
help yourself more than once from the buffet, which might affect
behavior as ‘‘just as people often look to portion size for guidance
in eating situations ... so they may rely on the example of others
for guidance, when such examples are salient’’ (Herman and Polivy,
2005). The hypothesis is that the sign will encourage guests to load
less food on their plates each time they serve themselves, in turn
reducing the amount left over.
2. Method
The two treatments are tested in an experiment conducted
in collaboration with hotel restaurants. From a hotel chain we
recruited individual hotels to implement the two treatments, and
the remaining hotels made up the control group. A total of 52 hotels
delivered data which we could use in the final analysis. There
were 7 in each treatment group. The experiment was implemented
between June 1st and August 15th 2012. All hotel restaurants in
the study recorded and reported the amount of food waste daily
during the whole period. The treatment hotels implemented their
respective treatments from July 1st until August 15th.
We estimate a difference-in-difference model (Card and
Krueger, 1994) using a fixed effects panel regression to analyze the
treatment effects. The difference-in-difference method controls for
pre-treatment differences between the hotels and for trends over
time that are unrelated to the intervention. In addition, we control
for the number of guests staying at the hotel (expected to be a
good predictor of the number of breakfasts served), and food sales
revenue (in 1000s of NOK, a useful proxy for the number of meals
served, excluding breakfast as this is commonly included in the
price). Food waste from hotel iat time tis modeled as follows:
Wasteit =β11D1i+β12 D2i+ · · · + β1nDni +β2Guestsit
+β3Food salesit +γti+δ(Ti×ti)+eit
where D1i. . . Dni are hotel specific intercept dummy variables
defined so that
D1i=1i=1
0 otherwise,D12 =1i=2
0 otherwise, . . . ,
D1n=1i=n
0 otherwise.
Table 1
Average amount of food waste (kg) per hotel in the control group (38 hotels) and
test groups (7 hotels in each group), before and after the treatment was introduced.
Standard deviations in brackets.
Group Pre-treatment food
waste (kg, average per
hotel)
Post-treatment food
waste (kg, average per
hotel)
Control 35.07 32.98
(34.63) (30.77)
Reduced plate size 36.88 25.84
(51.06) (27.15)
Salient sign 47.76 34.25
(38.88) (25.84)
These control for average permanent differences between hotels,
while
ti=0 before treatment begins
1 after treatment begins
controls for time trend common to control and treatment groups.
Tiis defined so that
Ti=0 for hotels in control group
1 for hotels in treatment group.
The random error term eit is assumed to be i.i.d. normal. It is the
coefficient of the interaction between tiand Tithat measures the
effect of the treatment. A separate regression is estimated for each
of the treatments.
In the observational study, we utilize pre-existing differences
in plate size across hotels in the control group. We use a panel
regression also here. Food sales revenue and guests are still
controlled for. As the fixed effect model cannot handle variables
that are constant within hotels over time, which plate size is in this
case, we specify a random effects model:
Wasteit =β1+β2Plate sizeit +β3Guestsit
+β4Food salesit +eit +vi
where β1is the average intercept and viare random hotel-specific
deviations from the average.
3. Results
The experiment indicates that reducing the plate size reduces
food waste by 19.5% (p<0.001), and that introducing the sign
pointing out that guests can help themselves more than once
reduces food waste by 20.5% (p<0.001). Descriptive statistics by
time period and treatment group are reported in Table 1, while
the regression results are shown in Table 2. All the estimated
regression coefficients are significant at 0.1% level or lower. The
percentage treatment effects are found by dividing the coefficient
by the mean pre-treatment level from Table 1. The hotel-specific
coefficients are not reported. Random effect estimations were also
run, giving essentially the same results, however Hausman tests
conclude that these estimators are inconsistent and we hence
report the fixed effect estimations. The R2values are 0.36 and 0.39
respectively.
The plate size treatment in the field experiment is supported
by the observational study, where we measure the strength
of association between plate size and food waste among the
untreated hotels. The results from the latter are reported in Table 3.
The R2value is 0.42. The percentage effect is approximated by
dividing the coefficient by the overall mean waste level in the
control group (33.82 kg). The results suggest that a 1 cm reduction
in plate size reduces food waste by 2.5 kg (p<0.01), which is
7.4% of the overall mean in the control group. This implies that
a 3 cm reduction reduces waste by approximately 22%, which is
Author's personal copy
S. Kallbekken, H. Sælen / Economics Letters 119 (2013) 325–327 327
Table 2
Estimated coefficients (and the associated standard errors) from the difference-in-
difference analysis.
Plate size Salient sign
Guests 0.033 0.038
0.004 0.005
Food sales 0.138 0.171
0.015 0.016
Time trend 4.317 4.428
0.754 0.749
Treatment effect 7.179 9.772
1.825 1.848
well within the 95% confidence interval for the treatment effect.
While lacking the within-subject design of the experiment, the
observational study benefits from a larger number of observations
than contained in the treatment groups.
4. Discussion
We have identified and quantified two no-regrets measures
that can substantially reduce the amount of food waste from
hotel restaurants. The findings, at least for the effect of reduced
plate size, are likely to be transferable to other contexts such as
food services at institutions (schools, hospitals, retirement homes,
prisons, workplace canteens, etc.) where buffet meals are served.
The results for the effect of plate size are the strongest, since the
experimental results are supported by an observational study.
The cost of the plate size measure is negative. All restaurants
regularly have to replace plates, and as smaller plates are cheaper
to purchase than larger one, the cost of the measure will be
negative as long as the rate of replacement remains the same.
The cost of the second measure is minimal (printing 10–30 small
posters per hotel). Reducing food waste represents a financial
saving to the hotel, estimated at around NOK 50/kg (USD 9/kg) by
one of the hotels. As 1 kg of food waste is responsible for lifecycle
emissions of around 1.9 kg of CO2e (European Commission, 2010),
the negative cost of each measure is perhaps as large as USD 4700
per ton of CO2.
One potential concern is that customer satisfaction could be
negatively affected, for instance because customers have to return
to the buffet more often to fill the smaller plates, or because
larger plates produce a more luxurious feeling. The hotels use
an online survey tool to record customer satisfaction with the
restaurants. Customer satisfaction with the buffet breakfast within
each treatment group was essentially constant from the pre-
treatment period to the treatment period. A simple difference-
in-difference with more than 45,000 observations shows no
significant change for the treatment hotels relative to the hotels
in the control group.
Our study leaves many important questions unaddressed, e.g.
the effect of implementing the two measures jointly or the optimal
plate size in terms of minimizing food waste, but it provides a
strong indication that using simple nudges to reduce food waste
might represent a very fruitful approach to achieving significant
greenhouse gas emission reductions. There is reason to believe
that the measures are also privately profitable as they reduce
food expenditure while having no effect on customer satisfaction
Table 3
Estimated coefficients, standard errors and p-values for the observational analysis.
Plate size
2.515
0.917
0.006
Guests
0.033
0.005
0.000
Food sales
0.172
0.016
0.000
Intercept
35.188
22.045
0.110
and requiring minimal costs of implementation. As such, they
represent an example of a strategy that makes both environmental
and business sense, giving anecdotal support to the controversial
Porter-hypothesis (Porter and van der Linde, 1995).
Acknowledgments
We thank Cathrine Dehli at Nordic Choice Hotels for substantial
help with planning and implementing the experiment, as well
as the hotel employees who recorded data on food waste, and
helped us implement the treatments. We are indebted to Bård
Romstad for helpful support with data management. Thanks to
Hege Westskog, Kristin Linnerud, Torben Mideksa and Todd Cherry
for valuable comments. The research was funded by the not-for-
profit organization GreeNudge.
References
Card, D., Krueger, A.B., 1994. Time-series minimum-wage studies: a meta-analysis.
American Economic Review 85, 238–243.
European Commission, 2010. Preparatory study on food waste across EU 27.
Technical Report 2010-054. European Commission, Brussels.
Freedman, M.R., Brochado, C., 2010. Reducing portion size reduces food intake and
plate waste. Obesity 18, 1864–1866.
Gustavsson, J., Cederberg, C., Sonesson, U., van Otterdijk, R., Meybeck, A., 2011.
Global food losses and food waste: extent, causes and prevention, In: FAO (Ed.),
FAO, Rome.
Harrison, G.W., List, J.A., 2004. Field experiments. Journal of Economic Literature 42,
1009–1055.
Herman, C.P., Polivy, J., 2005. Normative influences on food intake. Physiology &
Behavior 86, 762–772.
Hertwich, E.G., Peters, G.P., 2009. Carbon footprint of nations: a global, trade-linked
analysis. Environmental Science & Technology 43, 6414–6420.
Ittersum, K.V., Wansink, B., 2012. Plate size and color suggestibility: the Delboeuf
illusion’s bias on serving and eating behavior. Journal of Consumer Research
39, 215–228.
Metz, B., Davidson, O.R., Bosch, P.R., Dave, R., Meyer, L.A. (Eds.), 2007. Contribution
of Working Group III to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental
Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, United
Kingdom.
Porter, M., van der Linde, C., 1995. Green and competitive. Harvard Business Review
119–134.
Sobal, J., Wansink, B., 2007. Kitchenscapes, tablescapes, platescapes, and foodscapes.
Environment and Behavior 39, 124–142.
Thaler, R., Sunstein, C., 2008. Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth and
Happiness. Yale University Press, New Haven, USA.
UNEP, 2012. Global environment outlook 5—summary for policy makers. UNEP,
Nairobi.
... This not only has a detrimental impact on the environment, but on social and economic drivers as well (Gustavsson et al., 2011). Research has revealed that food waste can be reduced by creating cooperative strategies for staff (Oliveira et al., 2016;Pirani & Arafat, 2016), educating customers (Sakaguchi et al., 2018), and selecting managerial decisions regarding food planning operations, such as actively measuring food waste (Sakaguchi et al., 2018), selecting serving styles which are better for reducing food waste -e.g., avoiding buffets and opting for à la carte services (Papargyropoulou et al., 2019) or providing 'nudging' strategies for customers (Kallbekken & Saelen, 2013). ...
... Furthermore, the participation phase provided an opportunity for reflection and enquiry between the students, teachers, and business professionals. This was in line with CPS strategies identified by the research, e.g., Oliveira et al. (2016), Sakaguchi et al. (2018), Papargyropoulou et al. (2019), and Kallbekken and Saelen (2013). In our study, we noted the importance of structured feedback and teacher additional scaffolding in facilitating purposeful collaboration patterns between students. ...
Article
Full-text available
Collaborative problem solving (CPS) is a widely used pedagogical approach in work-based learning. To facilitate the complex process of situated learning, researchers have emphasized the need for scaffolding to enable learning of skills while engaged in problem-solving. While CPS as a pedagogical practice has mainly been examined in classroom situations, a research gap exists in studies of CPS in real-world contexts. In this study, we contribute to the understanding of CPS by examining the contextual characteristics that shape students' and teachers' experiences in situated learning. Consequently, we present a multi-case study to investigate involvement of a business professional as a source for scaffolding on site, in a hotel business environment. We employed a qualitative, multi-case methodology in the study. An ill-structured, real-world problem of food waste in the hotel service sector was presented to students (N = 72) and their accompanying teachers (N = 9) from second and tertiary education. They were provided with access to expert knowledge and opinion by industry professionals (N=5) on site. We collected data via observations, interviews, and questions from the involved stakeholders in three physical locations in Denmark and in Finland. Additionally, we documented their experiences using an online collaboration tool in each case. Despite the scaffolding provided by the business professionals, students underused the resources available for their learning in the extended learning environment. Students benefited from guided exploration of the problem space, structured feedback, and teacher interventions, resulting in improved perspective taking, participation, social regulation, task regulation and knowledge building.
... For instance, Dolnicar et al. (2020) reduced plate waste in sun-and-beach hotel restaurants with a game-based intervention. Removing trays and reducing plate size have also been shown to reduce plate waste (Kallbekken and Saelen, 2013;Thiagarajah and Getty, 2013;Obersteiner et al., 2021). Some studies suggest that the shape of a plate, specifically transitioning from round to oval, can reduce plate waste (Richardson et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Automated tools for waste quantification hold promise in providing preciser understanding of food waste. This study evaluated a tool to quantify plate waste in primary school canteens. It encompassed data from 421,015 instances of food wastage. The evaluation revealed high accuracy, with the tool's plate waste detection falling within ±10% of manual recordings. However, the tool estimated 40% fewer individual guests compared to manual entry due to not all students wasting food. As a result, the automatically collected data indicated a 35% higher waste-to-guest ratio. The findings showed that a minority of students (20%) accounted for a majority (60%) of plate waste. Halving the waste generated by this group would reduce overall plate waste by 31%, emphasizing the importance of tailored interventions for high-profile wasters rather than applying general measures to all students. Targeting areas with the greatest potential can contribute to a more sustainable food system with reduced waste.
... Previous studies showed that measures can reduce food waste, which may result from conscious consumer decisions or systemic issues. For instance, reducing plate size can lower food waste by 19.5% (Kallbekken and Saelen, 2013), providing different menu options can reduce it by 55%-70% (Berkowitz et al., 2016), and displaying informative and warning posters can reduce it by 15%-19% (Dölekoğlu and Var, 2016). Filimonou et al. (2021) also demonstrated that implementing improved demand forecasting methods, purchasing smaller amounts of food with greater frequency, and practising portion control can prevent up to 28% of food waste. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to identify what innovative technological solutions companies offer to reduce avoidable and unavoidable food waste and how successful these solutions are. The data in the study were obtained through document analysis, a qualitative research method. Within the study's scope, case studies and information from the websites of sixteen firms providing technological solutions for minimising avoidable food waste and nine firms providing technical solutions for unavoidable food waste disposal, alongside news articles and academic research about these firms, were analysed. Companies offer technological solutions to prevent food waste by tracking, analysing, and reporting it directly. Companies also have indirect technologica l solutions to prevent food waste, such as accurate sales forecasting, production planning, menu analysis, inventory management, production enhancement, temperature monitoring, and risk analysis. The study showed that the innovative solutions offered by technology companies for hotels, restaurants and catering companies directly and indirectly successfully reduce food waste. The study also revealed that companies can improve sales forecasting, production planning, and operational processes and reduce food and labour costs using technology solutions. The study also underlined that technology companies have different ways to deal with unavoidable food waste, such as turning it into energy, making new products, finding new ways to sell it, and using new composting methods.
... Dette er vist å gi mer gjenbruk av håndklaer, noe som reduserer antall klesvasker (Gold stein et al., 2008). På samme måte kan grønn dulting bli brukt som en demarketing eller antimarketingstrategi, for eksempel ved å benytte mindre tallerkenstørrelse i buffe ter for å legge til rette for lavere matkonsum og matavfall i restauranter (Kallbekken & Saelen, 2013;Soule & Reich, 2015). Et nøkkelspørsmål er hvorvidt bedrifter med slike enkle grep kan få kunder til å ta miljøvennlige valg som blir til mer vedvarende atferd også utover enkelthandlin gen. ...
Article
Både små og store bedrifter må i stadig større grad innovere for å lykkes med mer bærekraftige forretningsmodeller. Det hevdes imidlertid at en betydelig andel av bærekraftsproblemet kan tilskrives uansvarlige forbrukervalg, og da er spørsmålet hvordan bedrifter kan legge til rette for bærekraftig kundeatferd. Eksisterende litteratur viser at ved hjelp av «dulting» (nudging) kan bedrifter legge til rette for at kundene opptrer mer bærekraftig. For bedrifter, myndigheter og beslutningstakere vil det imidlertid være mer effektivt dersom slik dulting kan påvirke flere ulike atferder og ikke bare ha innvirkning på én valgsituasjon. Formålet med denne artikkelen er å gi innsikt i denne utfordringen. Basert på et online-eksperiment gjennomført med norske forbrukere (n = 405) tar artikkelen for seg indirekte effekter (smitteeffekter) av å dulte forbrukere i retning av bærekraftig atferd på påfølgende atferder. Artikkelen er et bidrag til den voksende mengden litteratur om smitteeffekter av grønn dulting. Ved å gi innsikt i hvordan grønn dulting kan påvirke forbrukeratferden, viser artikkelen hvordan ledere kan utforme og vurdere de direkte effektene og smitteeffektene av dulting for å fremme bærekraftig kundeatferd.
... Recent research highlights the potential of changing the physical environment (viz. the immediate decision situation) to encourage sustainable actions, such as promoting recycling (Linder et al., 2021;Rosenthal and Linder, 2019), limiting food waste (Kallbekken and Saelen, 2013), or promoting biking over car use (Kaaronen and Strelkovskii, 2020a). Such approaches could complement the more conventionally used strategies of communication, education, and normative information that are often implemented to foster sustainable behavior changes (Sörqvist, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Littering is a problem in many human societies. The current study aimed to explore if changing the appearance of street bins, thereby manipulating the different types of social norms they signal, can increase the amount of trash they collect and thereby mitigate littering. A field experiment tested the effectiveness of two alternatives to the conventional grey street bin; one bin foliated with pictures drawn by school children containing a normative anti-littering message (explicit norm), and one bright orange salient bin (implicit norm). Observed behavioral data was collected, and both the weight and volume of trash in the bins were measured each day for a period of one month. The results showed a tendency for the salient orange bin to increase trash collection compared to other bins; an effect most tangible towards the end of the weeks. The biggest effect was, however, that the explicitly normative bin reduced trash collection overall. These results provide lessons on how the appearance of bins can influence trash collection, potentially resulting in both desirable and undesirable outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Pro-sustainability changes are slow and incremental at best in the tourism sector. Research on the topic can take the form of secondary data (e.g., content analysis of strategic documents, social media posts), survey-based intent studies (e.g., willingness to pay), survey-based studies of self-reported behaviors, observation of actual behaviors (e.g., benchmarking studies), lab-based experimental manipulations of measurable behaviors, and, finally, in situ , or field-based, experimental manipulations of measurable behaviors. The latter are some of the rarest studies and are held up as the gold standard for changing behaviors by providing evidence-based, measurable, and actionable sustainability interventions for tourism businesses. This study draws inspiration from a 4-year program of action research into pro-sustainability changes in tourist accommodations. It questions whether any of these approaches are sufficient for changing sustainability-oriented behaviors. This questioning extends to whether the theoretical approaches that underpin even “gold standard experiments” capture the operational contexts of accommodation businesses. It proposes instead that a scaffolded approach, built from a systems map of the theories, tools, experimental findings, interviews with stakeholders and operational context is necessary to create sustainability transformations in tourism businesses. This is a radical departure from the dependent/independent variable approach adopted in traditional scientific methods and that requires a different ontological approach to the science of sustainability. The study has implications for contextualizing intervention-based experimental studies within a wider system of influential factors within tourist accommodations.
Article
Purpose This study seeks to examine motivations, challenges and strategies implemented to sustain environmentally friendly practices in Macau hotels and the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on such practices. Design/methodology/approach Executives in sustainability departments of hotels in Macau were purposefully targeted for the in-depth interviews. Ten executives were interviewed either face-to-face or via video call. Findings The findings revealed that hotels in Macau implement sustainable practices. The main motivation for their implementation was the need to protect the environment, compliance and business-driven reasons. The major challenge involved the implementation costs. The pandemic had a limited impact on the implementation of these practices. Practical implications The study recommends that the Macau government should establish clear environmental policies and regulations for hotels with guidelines and support systems. Hotels can also benchmark their performance and collaborate on sustainable practices, develop innovative financing mechanisms and cost-efficient waste recycling practices, organize customer education and engagement programs to promote environmentally friendly practices and incorporate crisis management and resilience strategies into environmental strategies. Originality/value The current study denotes that the “sustainable development” concept is crucial in Macau because the largely gaming-driven hospitality industry has been developed over the last decade, significantly impacting environmental degradation. This study examined how hotels respond to these environmental challenges and how their responses can be sustained in the long term. The authors suggest the environmental practice indicators that can be helpful for the hospitality industries in Macau. Social and economic implications are discussed based on the findings.
Article
Full-text available
Built environments at many scales influence the type and amount of food consumed. Macroscale food systems and food landscapes influence food choices, and microscale rooms, furniture, containers, and objects influence food intake. The authors review literature about how four ubiquitous microscale built environments are persistent but often unrecognized influences on food intake. Kitchenscapes influence food intake through availability, diversity, and visibility of foods; tablescapes through variety, abundance, and accessibility; platescapes through portion and/or package size, arrangement, and utensil type; and food-scapes through food-item forms and landmarks. Microgeographies of built environments provide a subtle, pervasive, and often unconscious influence on food choices, food intake, obesity, and health. Reengineering built environments may offer opportunities to shape food intake.
Book
Full-text available
Every day, we make decisions on topics ranging from personal investments to schools for our children to the meals we eat to the causes we champion. Unfortunately, we often choose poorly. The reason, the authors explain, is that, being human, we all are susceptible to various biases that can lead us to blunder. Our mistakes make us poorer and less healthy; we often make bad decisions involving education, personal finance, health care, mortgages and credit cards, the family, and even the planet itself. Thaler and Sunstein invite us to enter an alternative world, one that takes our humanness as a given. They show that by knowing how people think, we can design choice environments that make it easier for people to choose what is best for themselves, their families, and their society. Using colorful examples from the most important aspects of life, Thaler and Sunstein demonstrate how thoughtful "choice architecture" can be established to nudge us in beneficial directions without restricting freedom of choice. Nudge offers a unique new take-from neither the left nor the right-on many hot-button issues, for individuals and governments alike. This is one of the most engaging and provocative books to come along in many years. © 2008 by Richard H. Thaler and Cass R. Sunstein. All rights reserved.
Article
Despite the challenged contention that consumers serve more onto larger dinnerware, it remains unclear what would cause this and who might be most at risk. The results of five studies suggest that the neglected Delboeuf illusion may explain how the size of dinnerware creates two opposing biases that lead people to overserve on larger plates and bowls and underserve on smaller ones. A countercyclical sinus-shaped relationship is shown to exist between these serving biases and the relative gap between the edge of the food and the edge of the dinnerware. Although these serving biases are difficult to eliminate with attention and education, changing the color of one’s dinnerware or tablecloth may help attenuate them. By showing that the Delboeuf illusion offers a mechanistic explanation for how dinnerware size can bias serving and intake, we open new theoretical opportunities for linking illusions to eating behavior and suggest how simple changes in design can improve consumer welfare.
Article
As portion size (PS) increases, so does food intake. The effect of decreasing PS on food intake in a nonlaboratory setting is unknown. This 5-week study sought to determine whether decreasing PS resulted in decreased intake of the same food, and if so, at what point further PS reductions might lack benefit. It also assessed effects of PS reduction on food production and waste in a university all-you-can-eat dining facility (DF). Subjects were primarily freshmen who regularly ate lunch at the DF, and self-selected French fries (FF) presented in individual paper bags, portioned originally at 88 g, and decreased approximately 15 g/week for 3 weeks. Diners were covertly observed choosing one or more bags. Total FF production and plate waste (PW) were determined daily. Decreasing PS resulted in significant decreases in consumption per diner (P < 0.05) and PW (P < 0.05), and nonsignificant decreases in total FF consumption and production. PS was positively correlated with consumption per diner (r = 0.897, P = 0.001) and PW (r = 0.852, P = 0.001), but inversely correlated with number of diners choosing >or=2 bags (r = -0.809, P = 0.003). Total FF production was positively correlated with PW (r = 0.728, P = 0.011). This study shows that reducing PS of a particular item in an all-you-can-eat environment results in reduced intake of that food for most individuals, and that reducing PS reduces PW and food production.