ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Acoustic noise has the potential to cause stress, to distract and to mask important sounds, and thus to affect behaviour. Human activities have added considerable noise to both terrestrial and aquatic habitats, and there is growing evidence that anthropogenic noise affects communication and movement patterns in a variety of species. However, there has been relatively little work considering the effect on behaviours that are fundamental to survival, and thus have direct fitness consequences. We conducted a series of controlled tank-based experiments to consider how playback of ship noise, the most common source of underwater noise, affects foraging and antipredator behaviour in the shore crab, Carcinus maenas. Ship noise playback was more likely than ambient-noise playback to disrupt feeding, although crabs experiencing the two sound treatments did not differ in their likelihood of, or speed at, finding a food source in the first place. While crabs exposed to ship noise playback were just as likely as ambient-noise controls to detect and respond to a simulated predatory attack, they were slower to retreat to shelter. Ship noise playback also resulted in crabs that had been turned on their backs righting themselves faster than those experiencing ambient-noise playback; remaining immobile may reduce the likelihood of further predatory attention. Our findings therefore suggest that anthropogenic noise has the potential to increase the risks of starvation and predation, and showcases that the behaviour of invertebrates, and not just vertebrates, is susceptible to the impact of this pervasive global pollutant.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Noise negatively affects foraging and antipredator behaviour in shore
crabs
Matthew A. Wale
a
, Stephen D. Simpson
b
, Andrew N. Radford
a
,
*
a
School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, U.K.
b
Biosciences, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter, U.K.
article info
Article history:
Received 25 January 2013
Initial acceptance 6 March 2013
Final acceptance 8 April 2013
Available online 10 June 2013
MS. number: 13-00078R
Keywords:
anthropogenic noise
Carcinus maenas
distraction
environmental change
invertebrate behaviour
pollution
predation risk
shore crab
starvation risk
stress
Acoustic noise has the potential to cause stress, to distract and to mask important sounds, and thus to
affect behaviour. Human activities have added considerable noise to both terrestrial and aquatic habitats,
and there is growing evidence that anthropogenic noise affects communication and movement patterns
in a variety of species. However, there has been relatively little work considering the effect on behaviours
that are fundamental to survival, and thus have direct tness consequences. We conducted a series of
controlled tank-based experiments to consider how playback of ship noise, the most common source of
underwater noise, affects foraging and antipredator behaviour in the shore crab, Carcinus maenas. Ship
noise playback was more likely than ambient-noise playback to disrupt feeding, although crabs expe-
riencing the two sound treatments did not differ in their likelihood of, or speed at, nding a food source
in the rst place. While crabs exposed to ship noise playback were just as likely as ambient-noise
controls to detect and respond to a simulated predatory attack, they were slower to retreat to shelter.
Ship noise playback also resulted in crabs that had been turned on their backs righting themselves faster
than those experiencing ambient-noise playback; remaining immobile may reduce the likelihood of
further predatory attention. Our ndings therefore suggest that anthropogenic noise has the potential to
increase the risks of starvation and predation, and showcases that the behaviour of invertebrates, and not
just vertebrates, is susceptible to the impact of this pervasive global pollutant.
Ó2013 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
To survive and reproduce successfully, animals must minimize
the risks of starvation and predation. Events that compromise
either the foraging or antipredator behaviour of an individual are
therefore likely to have detrimental consequences for its tness.
Stressful events, for example, can lead to changes in the intensity,
duration or frequency of particular activities as part of an allostatic
response (Broom & Johnson 1993;McEwen & Wingeld 2003;
Wingeld 2005). If this response includes a reduction or cessation
of normal locomotor activity (see Metcalfe et al. 1987), then the
likelihood of successful escape from a predator may be reduced
and, if less time is spent foraging, food intake may decline. Food
acquisition may also be negatively affected if stress results in un-
necessary and costly antipredator responses (Lima & Dill 1990).
Moreover, stress might cause a reduction in appetite, mediated by
peptides associated with the corticotrophin-releasing factor system
(Bernier 2006); animals might be less inclined to search for food
when stressed.
Since foraging and antipredator behaviour involve various
cognitive processes, including detection, classication and decision
making (Shettleworth 2010), events that impair attention could
also pose a problem. Effects on attention might arise as part of a
stress-related allostatic response (as above), but attention might
also be compromised if an animal is distracted (Dukas 2004;Chan &
Blumstein 2011). If attention is narrowed, with animals either
ignoring stimuli or focusing on a smaller spatial scale (Dukas 2002),
then food or predators may be less likely to be detected (Hockey
1970). Distracted animals may also be more likely to respond
inappropriately to an imminent threat and run the risk of losing
current food items, either because they escape or because they are
stolen by others (Dukas 2002). These attention-mediated effects
are driven by a limited capacity to attend simultaneously to mul-
tiple stimuli (Dukas 2004;Chan & Blumstein 2011).
Many animals are alerted to the presence of predators or prey by
auditory cues, such as acts of intraspecic communication or the
sounds inadvertently produced as a consequence of movement
(Barrera et al. 2011;Siemers & Schaub 2011). Moreover, alarm calls
have evolved in a wide range of mammals and birds to warn others
of impending danger (Hollén & Radford 2009), while other vocal-
izations can provide information on the current level of risk (Hollén
*Correspondence: A. N. Radford, School of Biological Sciences, University of
Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol BS8 1UG, U.K.
E-mail address: andy.radford@bristol.ac.uk (A. N. Radford).
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Animal Behaviour
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav
0003-3472/$38.00 Ó2013 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.001
Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118
et al. 2008;Bell et al. 2009). Thus, predation and starvation may be
more likely if such acoustic information is masked, that is, if the
threshold for detection or discrimination of one sound is increased
in the presence of another (Brumm & Slabbekoorn 2005). Masking
can be complete (energeticmasking), whereby the signal is not
detected at all, or partial (informationalmasking), whereby the
signal is detectable by the listener, but the content is hard to un-
derstand. Either way, there are likely to be tness consequences for
foragers and potential prey from a reduced ability to detect valu-
able auditory information (Brumm & Slabbekoorn 2005;Siemers &
Schaub 2011;Lowry et al. 2012).
Acoustic noise, especially if it is loud, persistent, unexpected or
novel, has the potential to cause stress (Wysocki et al. 2006;Wright
et al. 2007), to distract (Hockey 1970;Chan et al. 2010a) and to
mask important sounds (Brumm & Slabbekoorn 2005;Siemers &
Schaub 2011;Lowry et al. 2012). In recent decades, human activ-
ities such as urban development, expansion of transport networks
and resource extraction have added considerable noise to both
terrestrial and aquatic environments across the globe, and led to
major changes in the acoustic landscape (see McDonald et al. 2006;
Watts et al. 2007;Barber et al. 2009). Consequently, anthropogenic
noise is now recognized as a major pollutant of the 21st century,
appearing in national and international legislation (e.g. US National
Environment Policy Act and European Commission Marine Strategy
Framework Directive). A burgeoning research effort using both
natural sound sources and playback experiments has indicated that
anthropogenic noise can affect the behaviour of species in a variety
of taxonomic groups (reviewed by Tyack 2008;Barber et al. 2009;
Slabbekoorn et al. 2010). However, most of that behavioural work
has focused on vocal communication or movement patterns (see,
e.g. Radford et al. 2012); far less research attention has been paid to
foraging and antipredator behaviour, which are of fundamental
importance to survival (see Chan et al. 2010a,b;Purser & Radford
2011;Siemers & Schaub 2011;Bracciali et al. 2012 for exceptions).
In this study, we used a series of controlled tank-based experi-
ments to explore how additional noise affected foraging and anti-
predator behaviour of a common marine crustacean, the shore crab,
Carcinus maenas. As our experimental sound to be added, we used
playback of ship noise. While underwater anthropogenic noise ari-
ses from many sources, including seismic surveys, mining activity,
sonar, wind farms and acoustic deterrent devices (Tasker et al. 2010),
ship noise is the most common (Vasconcelos et al. 2007) and has
alone led to a 10e100-fold increase in low-frequency (20e200 Hz)
ambient aquatic noise over the past century (Tyack 2008). To date,
the vast majority of studies investigating potential impacts of un-
derwater anthropogenic noise have been conducted on vertebrates
(Nowacek et al. 2007;Popper & Hastings 2009;Slabbekoorn et al.
2010). However, crustaceans and other marine invertebrates are
capable of hearing (Salmon 1971;Goodall et al. 1990) and use sound
for a variety of reasons (e.g. Jeffs et al. 2003;Stanley et al. 2010;
Simpson et al. 2011); thus they are likely to be vulnerable to the
impact of anthropogenic noise (see Wale et al. 2013).
In two separate foraging experiments, we examined the likeli-
hood and speed with which individuals located a food source and
whether their feeding behaviour was disrupted. We predicted that
ship noise playback would reduce the likelihood of crabs nding a
food item or would result in their taking longer to do so, and that it
might lead to an interruption in feeding behaviour. In an additional
two experiments, we probed antipredator behaviour. We simulated
a predatory attack, and predicted that ship noise playback might
make individuals less likely to detect the attack, respond differently
or take longer to retreat to shelter. We also investigated the
response of crabs to being unrighted (turned on their backs);
immobility is a well-documented antipredator behaviour (OBrien
& Dunlap 1975), potentially reducing the likelihood of further
predatory attention, and thus the most appropriate response might
be to remain in that position. If ship noise playback impairs deci-
sion making, we predicted that crabs would right themselves faster
than during playback of ambient noise.
METHODS
Ethical Note
All experiments in this study were approved by the University of
Bristol Animal Services Ethical Committee (University Investigation
Number: UB/10/034). The research adhered to the legal re-
quirements of the country (U.K.) in which the work was carried out,
and all institutional guidelines. Crabs showed no signs of adverse
reactions to the test set-ups; all tested individuals appeared to
return to normal pretrial behaviour when inspected and fed at the
end of each test day. At the end of the experiments, animals were
either kept for further study or given to the Bristol Aquarium.
Study Animals and Husbandry
All crabs were collected from Newquay harbour, U.K. (50
25
0
N
5
5
0
W), using a seine net, on 9 and 10 February 2012 (rst cohort)
and on 2 and 3 May 2012 (second cohort). Inside Newquay harbour
itself, there is sporadic trafc noise from pleasure craft, shing and
angling boat trips, and speed boats; noise from larger ships further
aeld is also likely, although those vessels do not enter the harbour
itself. Crabs were held for a maximum of 48 h in salt-water tanks at
the Blue Reef Aquarium, Newquay before transfer to Bristol
Aquarium by courier. During the transfer (265 km; 3.5 h), crabs
were out of water, but covered in damp cloths and newspaper, and
were maintained at their usual 12e14
C by the use of surrounding
ice packs; this method was adapted from Ingle (1999). Holding
tanks (48 32 cm and 28 cm high) in Bristol were made of poly-
styrene, to reduce noise transmission, and received water from one
of the Aquarium display tanks, which were plumbed into advanced
ltration facilities. Holding tanks were tted with a subsurface
inow pipe to prevent noise from water falling or collision with the
tank oor; the ow was adjusted to allow complete tank ush-
through every 30 min and thus ensure the maintenance of high
water quality. Sound levels in holding tanks were kept as low as
possible and were comparable to those for ambient-noise playback
during experiments (holding tank: 116 dB RMS re 1
m
Pa peaking at
1 kHz; ambient noise recording during playback: 111 dB RMS re
1
m
Pa peaking at 1.8 kHz; Fig. 1a).
Holding tanks contained sand on the oor and two shelters
made with inverted plastic ower pots weighted down with a layer
of pea gravel secured around the base with Milliput epoxy putty
(The Milliput Company, Gwynedd, U.K.). Holding tank lids included
a mesh window to allow light to reach the animals; this light did
not, however, cover all areas of the tank or reach into shelters and
thus animals had a choice of light/dark conditions. Water temper-
ature was kept at 12e14
C, salinity at 32e35 ppt and water qual-
ities within safe parameters (NO
2
:<0.3 mg/litre; NO
3
: 0 mg/
litre; NH
3þ
:0.25 mg/litre; pH: 7.4e7.9). Crabs were fed every 48 h
(except when part of foraging experiments; see below) on a variety
of previously frozen meats (cockle, mussel, shrimp, krill, sand eel,
mackerel) or dry composite marine pellets (New Era Aquaculture
Ltd., Thorne, U.K.), with any excess cleared from the holding tank
during tank maintenance no more than 8 h after feeding. Although
there was a constant water change within the holding tank (see
above), 25% of water was removed by siphon with excess food and
waste; this water was replaced by normal tank ow-through.
Water changes and the ow-through system ensured
M. A. Wale et al. / Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118112
maintenance of constant oxygen levels and the removal of any
water that had become trappedin the tank corners.
Noise Treatments
The sounds used as playbacks were from three major U.K. ports
(Gravesend, Plymouth, Portsmouth). In each location, recordings
were made of both ambient noise and noise generated by a single
passing ship at ca. 200 m distance (Gravesend: Rio de la Plata,
a 286 m long, 64 730 tonne container ship; Plymouth: Bro
Distributor, a 14 m long, 14 500 tonne LPG tanker; Portsmouth:
Commodore Goodwill, a 126 m long, 5215 tonne ferry). Ships were
travelling at constant, relatively slow speeds (<10 knots), as
enforced by port authorities for vessels entering and leaving
estuarine areas. All recordings were made with a calibrated
omnidirectional hydrophone (HiTech HTI-96-MIN with inbuilt
preamplier, High Tech Inc., Gulfport, MS, U.S.A.) and an Edirol R09-
HR 24-Bit recorder (44.1 kHz sampling rate, Roland Systems Group,
Bellingham, WA, U.S.A.). The recording level was calibrated for the
R09-HR using pure sine wave signals, measured in line with an
oscilloscope, produced by a function generator. Intensity (root
mean square, RMS) and power spectral density (units normalized
to 1 Hz, Hann evaluation, 50% overlap, FFT size 1024; averaged from
2 min of recording) were calculated for each recording in Avisoft
SASLab Pro v4.5.2 (Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany). This
analysis found some variation in average noise levels between re-
cordings: ambient tracks between 92 and 106 dB RMS re 1
m
Pa, and
ship noise tracks between 126 and 136 dB RMS re 1
m
Pa.
Sound samples of 60e140 s, incorporating the highest ampli-
tude of the ship passes (in which amplitude did not vary by more
than one quarter in magnitude) and the most stable levels of
ambient noise (where there were no events such as boats passing
or sounds of objects striking each other under the water) were used
to create experimental tracks in Audacity 1.3.13 (http://audacity.
sourceforge.net/). Each track included a 30 s fade in, 6.5 min of
ambient or ship noise and a 30 s fade out; one sample of ambient or
ship noise was looped to create a given playback track. Experi-
mental tracks were played back as WAV les using a similar set-up
to Purser & Radford (2011) and Wale et al. (2013): WAV/MP3
Player (Ultradisk DVR2 560 h; frequency range 20e20 000 Hz);
amplier (Kemo Electronic GmbH; 18 W; frequency response: 40e
20 000 Hz); potentiometer (set to minimum resistance; Omeg Ltd;
10 K logarithmic); and Aqua 30 underwater speaker (DNH; effective
frequency range 80e20 000 Hz). To give consistency between the
three exemplars in each treatment, tracks were re-recorded (using
the same hydrophone set-up as above) in the centre of the
experimental tank and modied (uniform amplication or atten-
uation); received sound levels for that tank position were therefore
103 e108 dB RMS re 1
m
Pa for ambient noise (chosen to be higher
than the noise oor in the experimental tank at low frequencies)
and 148e155 dB RMS re 1
m
Pa for ship noise (Fig. 1b, c). Exact sound
levels will differ throughout the tank, but our aim in this study was
to consider the potential behavioural impact of additional noise in
the environment, not establish the precise links between given
sound levels and responses.
The noise playbacks presented a range of frequencies that are
likely to fall within the hearing range of shore crabs, inferred from
studies on ddler crabs, Uca rapax and U. pugilator (Salmon 1971),
but the ship noise playbacks peaked at lower frequencies than the
ambient-noise playbacks. It is likely that crabs are primarily sen-
sitive to particle motion (Salmon 1971;Goodall et al. 1990), but, for
technical reasons, sound levels are given throughout in terms of
sound pressure; we do not attempt to establish absolute values for
sensitivity, but rather provide an indication of the behaviours that
can potentially be affected by the addition of noise to the
environment.
General Experimental Design
We chose to conduct our experiments in tanks to control care-
fully the conditions and contexts of the study animals, and to allow
detailed data collection. Care must of course be taken when
extrapolating results from tank-based experiments to meaningful
implications for free-ranging animals in open water. From a bio-
logical perspective, captive animals are usually more constrained
than in the wild, although in some of our experiments the
60
70
80
90
100
110
120
130
140
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 2 2.2 2.4 2.6 2.8 3
60
70
80
90
100
110
120
140
150
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 2 2.2 2.4 2.6 2.8
60
70
80
90
100
110
120
130
140
150
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 2 2.2 2.4 2.6 2.8 3
Holding-tank noise
Ambient-noise playback
Ship noise playback
Ambient-noise playback
Experimental-tank noise
Ship noise playback
Ambient-noise playback
Experimental-tank noise
130
(a)
(b)
(c)
3
1
1
Fre
q
uenc
y
(kHz)
150
Power spectral density (dB re 1µPa2 Hz-1)
Figure 1. Variation in sound proles for (a) holding tanks and ambient-noise play-
backs, (b) playbacks in the tank used for both foraging experiments and the simulated
predatory attack experiment, and (c) playbacks in the tank used for the unrighted crab
experiment. Shown are illustrative power spectrographic examples from recordings
made in the centre of the relevant tank.
M. A. Wale et al. / Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118 113
behavioural response measured was localized. Moreover, captive
individualsfeeding regimes may not be replicated naturally. From
an acoustics perspective, playbacks cannot perfectly replicate nat-
ural sound sources and the sound eld in a tank is complex
(Parvulescu 1964,1967;Okumura et al. 2002). However, our pri-
mary aim was to determine for the rst time whether addition of
noise can potentially affect various key life history variables in
marine invertebrates; future work can then establish the precise
effects in natural conditions.
Both foraging experiments and the simulated predatory attack
experiment were conducted in a tank (40 30 cm and 30 cm high)
with walls 4 mm thick; the experiment in which the crab was
unrighted took place in a tank (60 30 cm and 40 cm high) with
walls 6 mm thick. In the former, the playback speaker was placed
behind a vertical partition situated 10 cm from one short side; in
the latter, the speaker was beneath a platform (Fig. 2). Both
experimental tanks were placed on 5 cm thick foam pipe lagging, to
minimize sound transfer, and had the sides covered with opaque
black paint to half the height, to minimize distraction from visual
reections.
All crabs used in experiments weighed between 40 and 60 g,
and were randomly allocated to one of the two sound treatments
(ambient or ship noise). Experiments used an independent-
samples design to avoid any potential carryover effects. Separate
groups of crabs were used for each experiment, to remove any
compounding effects from repetitive handling stress and/or expo-
sure to sound (see Wale et al. 2013). Two cohorts of animals were
tested within each experiment, the rst occurring 1e3 months
before the second; experiments in each cohort were conducted at
the same time of day to remove any bias caused by the circadian
rhythm of the crabs (see Bojsen & Witthøfft 1998). Each animal was
randomly assigned one of the three playback tracks in each treat-
ment. Thirty-six animals were tested in each experiment in coun-
terbalanced blocks of six (ambient and ship noise from each of
three harbours).
Foraging Experiments
Prior to both foraging experiments, crabs were food deprived for
96 h. After placement in the holding area of the experimental tank
(Fig. 2a), crabs in both experiments were given 30 s to acclimatize.
The food source in both cases was a mussel, Mytilus edulis, which
had been drilled through and attached to a stainless steel washer
on the tank oor (as per Jensen et al. 2002). In the food search
experiment, the playback track was rst started and the divider
between the holding area and the experimental area then removed.
Each crab was given a maximum of 7.5 min (equivalent to a single
ship pass in the playback tracks) to nd the food source, after which
individuals were noted as having been successful or not. For those
that were successful, the time taken to locate the food source was
recorded.
In the feeding-disruption experiment, the divider was rst
removed and the crab allowed to nd the food source without any
playback of noise. Once the food was found and the animal had
begun eating, the playback track was started and any reaction
recorded in real time by the observer. Feeding was considered
disrupted if the animal ceased eating, indicated by a cessation in
maxilliped movement (normally ca. 5 beats/s; cessation scored if at
least 5 s with no beats), complete freezing of the animal or move-
ment away from the food. After 1 min of noise, a lack of any such
response was recorded as no apparent distraction.
Antipredator Experiments
The simulated predator attack experiment began when an ani-
mal was taken from the pre-experimental holding tank and placed
into the shelter provided in the experimental tank (Fig. 2b). The
shelter was identical to that in the holding tanks (see above).
The playback was started when the crab rst ventured out of the
shelter. After the 30 s fade in on the playback track, a wooden dowel
was plunged by hand into the water and straight out again (only the
dowel touched the water), directly in front of the crab; this was
used to simulate the action of a bird attempting to catch the crab.
The initial reaction of the crab to the predator stimulus was
recorded as either running (movement away from the stimulus) or
freezing (cessation of all movement). After the dowel was removed,
the time taken for the crab to return to the shelter was recorded.
Crabs were classied as returning to shelter either when they
entered it or stopped in the gap between it and the back of the
experimental tank.
To examine the righting behaviour of crabs when unrighted
(placed on their back), the playback track was started and after 15 s
an animal was placed inverted on the sand tray in the experimental
tank (Fig. 2c). The crab was then pinned in this position using a
3 cm diameter stainless steel washer, mounted on a brass rod at 20
(a)
(b)
(c)
Insulation
Speaker
Speaker holder
Direction of sound
Food source Divider
Speaker holder
Speaker
Insulation
Shelter
Direction of sound
Speaker
Insulation
Direction of sound
Experimental platform
Figure 2. Tank set-ups for (a) both foraging experiments, (b) the simulated predatory
attack experiment, and (c) the unrighted crab experiment.
M. A. Wale et al. / Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118114
with Milliput (see OBrien & Dunlap 1975). The crab was held in this
position for 15 s, since pinning for 30 s or longer can trigger an
immobility response (OBrien & Dunlap 1975). The restraint was
then removed and the time until the crab had completely turned
over and its second pereiopod touched the sand was recorded.
Data Analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted in R version 2.14.2 (The R
Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, http://www.
r-project.org). Data tted the assumptions of normality and ho-
moscedasticity for parametric testing. In some cases, two-sample
ttests were adjusted accordingly for unequal variances, resulting
in altered degrees of freedom. Owing to external events or unusual
behaviour, data from six individuals across the four experiments
were not available for analysis; nal sample sizes were therefore 35
for the food search experiment, 36 for the feeding-disruption
experiment, 34 for the simulated predatory attack experiment,
and 33 for the experiment in which the crab was unrighted. We
conducted more than one test on each experimental data set.
However, since application of sequential Bonferroni corrections
(Rice 1989) does not qualitatively alter any of our ndings, and
since there is considerable debate about whether such corrections
should actually be made for multiple testing using the same data
set (see Nakagawa 2004), we present the uncorrected statistical
output in the Results.
RESULTS
Foraging Experiments
The two cohorts of crabs used for each of the foraging experi-
ments did not differ signicantly in their likelihood of nding the
food source (chi-square test:
c
2
1
¼0:01, P¼0.927), their search
times (two-sample ttest: t
33
¼0.21, P¼0.837) or their likelihood of
being disrupted from feeding (chi-square test:
c
2
1
¼0:11,
P¼0.739); cohorts were therefore combined for an assessment of
the effect of ship noise playback. The ability of crabs to nd the food
item was not impaired by the playback of ship noise compared to
ambient noise: crabs in the two sound treatments did not differ
signicantly in either their likelihood of locating the food in the
available time (chi-square test:
c
2
1
¼2:90, P¼0.089; Fig. 3a) or in
the time taken to nd the food source (two-sample ttest, only
individuals that found the food: t
25
¼0.75, P¼0.458; Fig. 3b).
Sound treatment did, however, have a signicant effect on the
behaviour of feeding crabs (chi-square test:
c
2
1
¼16:05, P<0.001).
Individuals exposed to ship noise playback were more likely to
suspend feeding than were those experiencing ambient-noise
playback (Fig. 3c).
Antipredator Experiments
All tested individuals in both sound treatments responded to the
simulated predatory attack by freezing or running; no individuals
failed to detect the attack. The two cohorts of crabs used did not
differ signicantly in the type of initial reaction (chi-square test:
c
2
1
¼2:03, P¼0.154) or in their time to nd shelter (two-sample
ttest: t
32
¼0.46, P¼0.650); samples were therefore combined for
an assessment of the effect of ship noise playback. There was no
signicant difference in the initial reaction to the predator stimulus
depending on whether ambient or ship noise was playing (chi-
square test:
c
2
1
¼0:15, P¼0.699; Fig. 4a). However, crabs exposed
to ship noise playback took signicantly longer to return to shelter
than those experiencing ambient-noise playback (two-sample
ttest: t
19
¼3.95, P¼0.001; Fig. 4b). This was true of both those
individuals that initially reacted by freezing (t
6
¼2.97, P¼0.030)
and those that ran in response to the simulated predatory attack
(t
15
¼4.12, P¼0.001).
In the experiment in which crabs were unrighted, there was no
signicant difference between the two cohorts in the time that
individuals took to right themselves (two-sample ttest: t
31
¼0.23,
P¼0.817); samples were therefore combined to assess the effect of
ship noise playback. Crabs experiencing ship noise playback turned
over signicantly faster than those played back ambient noise
(t
17
¼5.97, P<0.001; Fig. 4c).
DISCUSSION
Single exposure to the playback of noise from an individual ship
resulted in a variety of behavioural changes in shore crabs: their
feeding was disrupted, they were slower to return to shelter
following a simulated predatory attack, and they righted them-
selves faster when inverted. Previous work has shown that
anthropogenic noise, including that from ships, can affect the
behaviour of marine mammals (e.g. Parks et al. 2007;Castellote
et al. 2012). Of more direct relevance, given the shared ability to
detect particle motion, is the recent work demonstrating that such
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
20
Ship noise (N=18)
Found food
Did not find food
(a)
(b)
(c)
NS
0
50
100
150
200
250
Ship noise (N=16)
NS
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
18
20
22
Ship noise (N=18)
Number of individuals
Not distracted
Distracted
***
18
Number of individuals
Ambient noise (N=17)
Ambient noise (N=11)
Time (s)
Ambient noise (N=18)
Pla
y
back treatment
16
Figure 3. Results of the foraging experiments. (a) Number of crabs that did or did not
nd a food source, (b) mean SE time taken to nd the food source, and (c) number of
crabs that were or were not disrupted from their feeding during playback of ambient
or ship noise. Sample sizes are provided in parentheses. ***P<0.001.
M. A. Wale et al. / Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118 115
noise could also affect sh behaviour (e.g. Purser & Radford 2011;
Bracciali et al. 2012;Bruintjes & Radford 2013). However, our
ndings represent the rst indication that marine decapods can be
affected behaviourally by underwater anthropogenic noise (see
Wale et al. 2013 for a physiological response); studies have shown
this is also true of terrestrial decapods, with evidence for increased
distraction and decreased antipredator responses when exposed to
playback of anthropogenic noise (Chan et al. 2010a,b;Stahlman
et al. 2011). Future work needs to ascertain whether the demon-
strated responses occur in natural conditions, and the scale of likely
impact in response to real sources of anthropogenic noise.
Animals disturbed while foraging, especially if they move away
from the food source, run the risk of losing the item, either because
the prey escapes or because it is stolen; food acquisition is highly
competitive in decapod crustaceans (Smallegange et al. 2009). Lost
food would increase the amount of foraging time required to satisfy
energetic needs, at the cost to other essential behaviours
(Abrahams & Dill 1989) and potentially an increased predation risk;
animals often have to trade off vigilance with time spent foraging
(Lima & Dill 1990). Moreover, selection of more items to ensure the
same food intake increases the likelihood of foraging errors, such as
the consumption of toxic or harmful food, especially since perfor-
mance mistakes may be more likely in noisy conditions (see Purser
& Radford 2011). If individuals persistently or frequently had to
increase their foraging effort or compensation is not possible, then
there would be likely consequences for growth, reproductive suc-
cess and survival; individuals might also be at a competitive
disadvantage (Pintor & Sih 2008), although that depends on the
relative impact of noise on different species.
Noise also appears to have a negative impact on important
antipredator behaviours in the shore crab. Retreating into a shelter,
where the prey becomes inaccessible, is a well-known defence
mechanism (Guerra-Bobo & Brough 2010;Rossong et al. 2011), and
extra time spent in exposed conditions before reaching safety in-
creases the chances of predation. Of course, the vulnerability to
predation depends on how predators are affected by noise, since
not all species are affected similarly (see Francis et al. 2011). Crabs
placed on their backs and exposed to ship noise playback also
righted themselves faster than those experiencing ambient noise.
From a functional perspective, a faster righting could be perceived
as benecial, with the crabs able to escape predation quicker,
spending shorter periods of time on their back with their weak
undersides exposed. However, because remaining motionless may
reduce the likelihood of further predatory attack (OBrien & Dunlap
1975), ceasing this behaviour sooner could further increase the
predation likelihood, with ultimate consequence for survival.
It is possible thatanimals might habituateto continuous exposure
to the same noise, or become more tolerant of it (Smith et al. 2004;
Bejder et al. 2009). If so, the potential impacts discussed above would
be lessened over time. However, variable or unpredictable exposure
or the occurrence of novel noise might prevent this (for example,
various sh species produced increased cortisol in response to vari-
able boat noise but not to continuous Gaussian noise; Wysocki et al.
2006) and could even lead to a sensitized response to such distur-
bances. The processes of habituation and sensitization to noise
exposure are only just beginning to be explored (Masini et al. 2008;
Wale et al. 2013) and the implications are far from simple (Bejder
et al. 2009), so further research is certainly warranted.
Our study did not explicitly explore whether stress, distraction,
masking or a combination of these underpins the demonstrated
behavioural responses to noise playback. Masking is perhaps un-
likely as an explanation for our results, because there were no
obvious acoustic cues or signals available, certainly in the two
foraging experiments (which utilized pinned mussels) or the
experiment when the crab was unrighted. Previous work has sug-
gested that exposure to noise can reduce attention to a primary
task, such as foraging or the detection of predators (Chan et al.
2010a;Purser & Radford 2011), because attention capacity is
limited (Mendl 1999;Dukas 2002). However, crabs in our experi-
ments did not take longer to nd a food item and were no less likely
to respond to the simulated predator attack during ship noise
playback, which does not lend any support to the distraction hy-
pothesis (see Chan et al. 2010a;Chan & Blumstein 2011). It is
possible that our set-up was too simple to allow any such effects to
be determined: for instance, the limited connes of the tank may
have made nding the food item rather too easy. Future work needs
to test explicitly between potential underlying mechanisms for
noise-induced differences in behavioural responses (see Chan et al.
2010a;Siemers & Schaub 2011).
In general, studies of anthropogenic noise both on land and
underwater have tended to focus on vertebrates (Tyack 2008;
Barber et al. 2009;Slabbekoorn et al. 2010;Radford et al. 2012). The
0
2
4
6
8
12
Ship noise (N=16)
Number of individuals
Run
Freeze
(a) NS
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
Ship noise (N=16)
(b)
(c)
**
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
Ship noise (N=17)
Time (s)
***
10
Ambient noise (N=18)
Ambient noise (N=18)
Time (s)
Ambient noise (N=16)
Pla
y
back treatment
Figure 4. Results of the antipredator experiments. (a) Number of crabs that ran or
froze as an initial response to a simulated predatory attack, (b) mean SE time taken
to return to a shelter in the immediate aftermath of the simulated attack, and (c)
mean SE time taken to right themselves from an inverted position during playback
of ambient or ship noise. Sample sizes are provided in parentheses. **P<0.01;
***P<0.001.
M. A. Wale et al. / Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118116
paucity of attention on invertebrates is not commensurate with
their abundance and diversity (they make up 60% of marine species,
for example), their importance ecologically (as essential compo-
nents of food webs) and economically (especially in light of
changing sheries), or their value in terms of new natural products
(Cesar et al. 2003;Ausubel et al. 2010;Leal et al. 2012). Care is
clearly needed when interpreting our results from tank-based
playback experiments (chosen to ensure tight control of potential
confounding factors) in a real-world context. Tank playbacks
cannot replicate natural sound elds perfectly (Parvulescu 1964,
1967;Okumura et al. 2002) and crustaceans probably detect
sounds using particle motion (Goodall et al. 1990). However, given
the paucity of studies examining the impact of anthropogenic noise
on foraging and antipredator behaviour in any taxa, even pre-
liminary indications in this regard are potentially valuable. More-
over, our study highlights both that invertebrates are likely to be
susceptible to the impacts of anthropogenic noise and that they
provide a tractable option for detailed investigations into the im-
pacts of this pervasive global pollutant.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to the Bristol Aquarium for housing the study
animals and to members of the Bioacoustics and Behavioural
Ecology Research Group at the University of Bristol for discussions
about the impacts of anthropogenic noise. We thank Irene Voellmy
and Sophie Holles for making the original sound recordings and
Nick Roberts, Vincent Janik, Dan Blumstein and two anonymous
referees for comments on the manuscript. Funding for this project
was provided by Defra.
References
Abrahams, M. & Dill, L. 1989. A determination of the energetic equivalence of the
risk of predation. Ecology,70, 999e1007.
Ausubel, J., Crist, D. & Waggoner, P. 2010. First Census of Marine Life 2010: High-
lights of a Decade of Discovery. Washington DC: Census of Marine Life.
Barber, J. R., Crooks, K. R. & Fristrup, K. M. 2009. The costs of chronic noise
exposure for terrestrial organisms. Trends in Ecology & Evolution,25,180e18 9.
Barrera, J. P., Chong, L., Judy, K. N. & Blumstein, D. T. 2011. Reliability of public
information: predators provide more information about risk than conspecics.
Animal Behaviour,81, 779e787.
Bejder, L., Samuels, A., Whitehead, H., Finn, H. & Allen, S. 2009. Impact assess-
ment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in
describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Marine Ecology Progress
Series,395,177e185.
Bell, M. B. V., Radford, A. N., Rose, R., Wade, H. M. & Ridley, A. R. 20 09. The value
of constant surveillance in a risky environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society
B,276, 2997e3005.
Bernier, N. J. 2006. The corticotropin-releasing factor system as a mediator of the
appetite-suppressing effects of stress in sh. General and Comparative Endo-
crinology,146,45e55.
Bojsen, B. & Witthøfft, H. 1998. In situ studies on heart rate and locomotor activity
in the freshwater craysh, Astacus astacus (L.) in relation to natural uctuations
in temperature and light intensity. Freshwater Biology,39, 455e465.
Bracciali, C., Campobello, D., Giacoma, C. & Sara, G. 2012. Effects of nautical trafc
and noise on foraging patterns of Mediterranean damselsh (Chromis chromis).
PLoS One,7,e40582.
Broom, D. M. & Johnson, K. G. 1993. Stress and Animal Welfare. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Bruintjes, R. & Radford, A. N. 2013. Context-dependent impacts of anthropogenic
noise on individual and social behaviour in a cooperatively breeding sh. Ani-
mal Behaviour,85, 1343e1349.
Brumm, H. & Slabbekoorn, H. 2005. Acoustic communication in noise. Advances in
the Study of Behavior,35, 151e209.
Castellote, M., Clark, C. W. & Lammers, M. O. 2012. Acoustic and behavioral
changes by n whales (Balaenoptera physalus) in response to shipping and
airgun noise. Biological Conservation,147,115e122.
Cesar, H., Burke, L. & Pet-Soede, L. 2003. The Economics of Worldwide Coral Reef
Degradation. Arnheim: Cesar Environmental Economics Consulting (CEEC).
Chan, A. A. Y.-H.& Blumstein, D. T. 2011. Attention, noise,and implications for wildlife
conservation and management. Applied Animal Behaviour Science,131,1e7.
Chan, A. A. Y.-H., Giraldo-Perez, P., Smith, S. & Blumstein, D. T. 2010a. Anthro-
pogenic noise affects risk assessment and attention: the distracted prey hy-
pothesis. Biology Letters,6, 458e461.
Chan, A. A. Y.-H., Stahlman, D. W., Garlick, D., Fast, C. D., Blumstein, D. T. &
Blaisdell, A. P. 2010b. Increased amplitude and duration of acoustic stimuli
enhance distraction. Animal Behaviour,80,1075e1079.
Dukas, R. 2002. Behavioural and ecological consequences of limited attention.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B,357, 1539e1547.
Dukas, R. 2004. Causes and consequences of limited attention. Brain Behaviour and
Evolution,63,197e
210.
Francis, C. D., Ortega, C. P. & Cruz, A. 2011. Different behavioural responses to
anthropogenic noise by two closely related passerine birds. Biology Letters,7,
850e852.
Goodall, C., Chapman, C. & Neil, D. 1990. The acoustic response threshold of the
Norway lobster, Nephrops norvegicus. In: Frontiers in Crustacean Neurobiology
(Ed. by K. Wiese, W. D. Krenz, J. Tautz, H. Reichert & B. Mulloney), pp. 106e113.
Basel: Birkhäuser.
Guerra-Bobo, M. & Brough, T. E. 2010. Neighbour density, body size and anti-
predator hiding time in the New Zealand mud-crab Austrohelice crassa.Jour-
nal of the Marine Biology Association,91,691e694.
Hockey, G. R. J. 1970. Effect of loud noise on attentional selectivity. Quarterly Journal
of Experimental Psychology,22,28e36.
Hollén, L. I. & Radford, A. N. 2009. The development of alarm call behaviour in
mammals and birds. Animal Behaviour,78,791e800.
Hollén, L. I., Bell, M. B. V. & Radford, A. N. 2008. Cooperative sentinel calling?
Foragers gain increased biomass intake. Current Biology,18,576e57 9.
Ingle, R. W. 1999. Decapod crustaceans. In: UFAW Handbook on the Care and
Management of Laboratory Animals, Vol. 2: Amphibious and Aquatic Vertebrates
and Advanced Invertebrates (Ed. by T. Poole). 7th edn. Oxford: Blackwell Science,
page 168.
Jeffs, A., Tolimieri, N. & Montgomery, J. C. 2003. Crabs on cue for the coast: the use
of underwater sound for orientation by pelagic crab stages. Marine and Fresh-
water Research,54,841e845.
Jensen, G., McDonald, P. & Armstrong, D. 2002. East meets west: competitive
interactions between green crab Carcinus maenas, and native and introduced
shore crab Hemigrapsus spp. Marine Ecology Progress Series,225,251e262.
Leal, M. C., Puga, J., Serôdio, J., Gomes, N. C. M. & Calado, R. 2012. Trends in
the discovery of new marine natural products from invertebrates over the
last two decades ewhere and what are we bioprospecting? PLoS One,7,
e30580.
Lima, S. L. & Dill, L. M. 1990. Behavioural decisions made under the risk of pre-
dation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology,68,619e640.
Lowry, H., Lill, A. & Wong, B. B. M. 2012. How noisy does a noisy miner have to be?
Amplitude adjustments of alarm calls in an avian urban adaptor.PLoS One,7,
e29960.
McDonald, M. A., Hildebrand, J. A. & Wiggins, S. M. 2006. Increases in deep ocean
ambient noise in the Northeast Pacic west of San Nicholas Island, California.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,12 0,711e718.
McEwen, B. S. & Wingeld, J. C. 2003. The concept of allostasis in biology and
biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior,43,2e15.
Masini, C. V., Day, H. E. W. & Campeau, S. 2008. Long-term habituation to repeated
loud noise is impaired by relatively short inter-stressor intervals in rats.
Behavioral Neuroscience,122,210e223.
Mendl, M. 1999. Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive function. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science,65,221e244.
Metcalfe, N. B., Huntingford, F. A. & Thorpe, J. E. 1987. The inuence of predation
risk on the feeding motivation and foraging strategy of juvenile Atlantic salmon.
Animal Behaviour,35,901e911.
Nakagawa, S. 2004. A farewell to Bonferroni: the problems of low statistical power
and publication bias. Behavioral Ecology,15, 1044e1045.
Nowacek, D. P., Thorne, L. H., Johnston, D. W. & Tyack, P. L. 2007. Responses of
cetaceans to anthropogenic noise. Mammal Review,37,8
1e115.
OBrien, T. J. & Dunlap, W. P. 1975. Tonic immobility in the blue crab (Callinectes
sapidus, Rathbun) its relation to threat of predation. Journal of Comparative
Physiology and Psychology,89,86e94.
Okumura, T., Akamatsu, T. & Yan, H. Y. 2002. Analyses of small tank acoustics:
empirical and theoretical approaches. Bioacoustics,12,330e332.
Parks, S. E., Clark, C. W. & Tyack, P. L. 2007. Short- and long-term changes in right
whale calling behaviour: the potential effects of noise on acoustic communi-
cation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,122,3725e3731.
Parvulescu, A. 1964. Problems of propagation and processing. In: Marine
Bioacoustics (Ed. by W. N. Tavolga), pp. 87e10 0. Oxford: Pergamon.
Parvulescu, A. 1967. The acoustics of small tanks. In: Marine Bioacoustics, Vol. 2 (Ed.
by W. N. Tavolga), pp. 7e13. Oxford: Pergamon.
Pintor, L. M. & Sih, A. 2008. Differences in growth and foraging behavior of native
and introduced populations of an invasive craysh. Biological Invasions,11,
1895e1902.
Popper, A. N. & Hastings, M. C. 2009. The effects of anthropogenic sources of sound
on shes. Journal of Fish Biology,75, 455e489.
Purser, J. & Radford, A. N. 2011. Acoustic noise induces attention shifts and reduces
foraging performance in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus).
PLoS One,6,e17478.
Radford, A. N., Morley, E. L. & Jones, G. 2012. The effects of noise on biodiversity.
Defra Report NO0235. http://randd.defra.gov.uk/Default.aspx?Menu¼Menu&
Module¼More&Location¼None&Completed¼O&ProjectID¼1813 6.
Rice, W. R. 1989. Analysing tables of statistical tests. Evolution,43, 223e225.
Rossong, M. A., Quijon, P. A., Williams, P. J. & Snelgrove, P. V. R. 2011. Foraging and
shelter behavior of juvenile American lobster (Homarus americanus): the
M. A. Wale et al. / Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118 117
inuence of a non-indigenous crab. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and
Ecology,403,75e80.
Salmon, M. 1971. Signal characteristics and acoustic detection by the ddler crabs,
Uca rapax and Uca pugilator.Physiological Zoology,44,210e224.
Shettleworth, S. J. 2010. Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Siemers, B. M. & Schaub, A. 2011. Hunting at the highway: trafc noise reduces
foraging efciency in acoustic predators. Proceedings of the Royal Society B,278,
164 6e1652.
Simpson, S. D., Radford, A. N., Tickle, E. J., Meekan, M. G. & Jeffs, A. G. 2011.
Adaptive avoidance of reef noise. PLoS One,6, e16625.
Slabbekoorn, H., Bouton, N., van Opzeeland, I., Coers, A., ten Cate, C. &
Popper, A. N. 2010. A noisy spring: the impact of globally rising underwater
sound levels on sh. Trends in Ecology & Evolution,25,419e427.
Smallegange, I. M., van Noordwijk, C. G. E., van der Meer, J. & van der Veer, H. W.
2009. Spatial distribution of shore crabs Carcinus maenas in an intertidal
environment in relation to their morphology, prey availability and competition.
Marine Ecology Progress Series,392,143e155.
Smith, M. E., Kane, A. S. & Popper, A. N. 2004. Noise-induced stress response and
hearing loss in goldsh (Carassius auratus). Journal of Experimental Biology,207,
427e435.
Stahlman, W. D., Chan, A. A. Y.-H., Blumstien, D. T., Fast, C. D. & Blaisdell, A. P.
2011. Auditory stimulation dishabituates anti-predator escape behavior in
hermit crabs (Coenobita clypeatus). Behavioral Processes,88,7e11.
Stanley, J. A., Radford, C. A. & Jeffs, A. G. 2010. Induction of settlement in crab
megalopae by ambient underwater reef sound. Behavioral Ecology,21,113e120.
Tasker, M. L., Amundin, M., Andre, M., Hawkins, A., Lang, W., Merck, T., Scholik-
Sclomer, A., Teilmann, J., Thomsen, F., Werner, S., et al. Marine Strategy
Framework Directive, Task Group 11 Report: Underwater Noise and Other Forms
of Energy. Prepared under the Administrative Arrangement between the Joint
Research Centre (JRC) and the Directorate-General for the Environment (DG ENV)
(no. 31210 - 2009/2010). http://ec.europa.eu/environment/marine/pdf/10-Task-
Group-11.pdf.
Tyack, P. L. 2008. Implications for marine mammals of large-scale changes in the
marine acoustic environment. Journal of Mammalogy,89, 549e558.
Vasconcelos, R. O., Amorim, M. C. P. & Ladich, F. 2007. Effects of ship noise on the
detectability of communication signals in the Lusitanian toadsh. Journal of
Experimental Biology,210,2104e2112.
Wale, M. A., Simpson, S. D. & Radford, A. N. 2013. Size-dependent physiological
responses of shore crabs to single and repeated playback of ship noise. Biology
Letters,9, 20121103.
Watts, R. D., Compton, R. W., McCammon, J. H., Rich, C. L., Right, S. M., Owens, T.
& Ouren, D. D. S. 2007. Roadless space of the conterminous United States.
Science,316,736e738.
Wingeld, J. C. 2005. The concept of allostasis: coping with a capricious environ-
ment. Journal of Mammalogy,86, 248e254.
Wright, A. J., Soto, N. A., Baldwin, A. L., Bateson, M., Beale, C. M., Clark, C.,
Deak, T., Edwards, E. F., Fernández, A., Godinho, A., et al. 2007. Anthropogenic
noise as a stressor in animals: a multidisciplinary perspective. International
Journal of Comparative Psychology,20, 250e273.
Wysocki, L. E., Dittami, J. P. & Ladich, F. 20 06. Ship noise and cortisol secretion in
European freshwater shes. Biological Conservation,128,501e
508.
M. A. Wale et al. / Animal Behaviour 86 (2013) 111e118118
... To understand the consequences of stress transmission, it is essential to identify how stress-exposed individuals can trigger outcomes for other group members. There is widespread evidence that behaviour is affected by physiological state [25,26], that the behaviour of individuals shapes collective processes [27] and that these processes feed back to alter individual states [1]. For example, the composition of the group [28], their collective physiological profile [29] and their physiological synchrony [30] can affect collective outcomes [31], such as group cohesion [30] or group movements [32]. ...
... We defined three days before the onset of the treatment (days 4-6 in colonies) as the pre-phase and the first three days after the onset of the treatment (days 8-10) as the treatment phase. We did not include the period during which the nest boxes were installed on the cages (days [11][12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25][26][27][28] in the tracking-based analyses because birds engaging in nest building and incubating were less visible to the cameras. Nevertheless, we evaluated the reproductive effort during this time. ...
Article
Full-text available
The stress response helps individuals cope with challenges, yet how individual stress levels shape group-level processes and the behaviour of other group members has rarely been explored. In social groups, stress responses can be buffered by others or transmitted to members that have not even experienced the stressor first-hand. Stress transmission, in particular, can have profound consequences for the dynamics of social groups and the fitness of individuals therein. We experimentally induced chronic stress within replicated colonies of zebra finches and used fine-scale tracking to observe the consequences of stress-exposed colony members for the behaviour and reproduction of non-manipulated colony members. Non-manipulated individuals in colonies containing stress-exposed individuals exhibited reduced activity, and fewer—but more differentiated—social bonds. These effects were stronger in colonies with a greater proportion of stress-treated individuals, demonstrating that the impact of stressors can reach beyond directly exposed individuals by also affecting their group mates. We found no evidence that socially transmitted stress affected reproduction or long-term physiological measurement in unmanipulated birds, even though the stress-exposed demonstrators laid slightly fewer eggs and showed stressor-dependent changes in feather corticosterone. Social transmission of these effects, if occurring at all, might be more subtle.
... Anthropogenic noise has been found to induce complex biological responses and influence wildlife in many ways (Shannon et al. 2016). Noise pollution can directly affect the behavior of many animals, such as mating (Bent et al. 2018;Senzaki et al. 2018;Duarte et al. 2019), foraging (Wale et al. 2013;Luo et al. 2015;Mason et al. 2016;Song et al. 2020), vigilance (Wale et al. 2013;Zhou et al. 2019), and territorial defence (Luther and Derryberry 2012). Human-generated noise can also influence multiple physiological responses that correlate with the stress response, immune regulation, hearing damage, cardiovascular health, DNA integrity, and gene expression (Kight and Swaddle 2011;Shannon et al. 2016). ...
... Anthropogenic noise has been found to induce complex biological responses and influence wildlife in many ways (Shannon et al. 2016). Noise pollution can directly affect the behavior of many animals, such as mating (Bent et al. 2018;Senzaki et al. 2018;Duarte et al. 2019), foraging (Wale et al. 2013;Luo et al. 2015;Mason et al. 2016;Song et al. 2020), vigilance (Wale et al. 2013;Zhou et al. 2019), and territorial defence (Luther and Derryberry 2012). Human-generated noise can also influence multiple physiological responses that correlate with the stress response, immune regulation, hearing damage, cardiovascular health, DNA integrity, and gene expression (Kight and Swaddle 2011;Shannon et al. 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Noise pollution has been shown to affect wild animals in various ways, but little is known about its consequences at the community level. Investigating animals’ overall vocal responses to noise across multiple sympatric species can reveal the complex nature of noise impacts but is challenging. In this study, we employed social network analysis (SNA) to evaluate how anuran communities and populations vary their calling behaviors in response to aircraft noise. SNA of anuran communities revealed that conspecific individuals increase the aggregation of their spectral (i.e., minimum frequency, maximum frequency, and dominant frequency), temporal (call duration, call rate, and call effort), and overall spectral–temporal features as an airplane passes through. SNA of populations also revealed that anurans could increase the interindividual similarity of multiple call characteristics in response to airplane noise. Furthermore, our network analysis of multiple species and multiple call traits revealed an effect of noise in species whose calling behavior did not change in previous separate analyses of each species and single traits. This study suggests that noise pollution may change the pattern of combined acoustic properties at the community level. Our findings highlight the importance of integrated methods and theories for understanding the ecological consequences of noise pollution in future studies.
... Habituation is a common phenomenon observed across diverse animal groups, including birds, mammals, crustaceans, and reptiles (e.g. Ellenberg et al. 2009, Raderschall et al. 2011, Wale et al. 2013, Schakner 2016, where repeated exposure to a stimulus leads to a gradual reduction in responsiveness (NRC 2003, Hemmi 2005, Coleman et al. 2008, Hemmi and Merkle 2009 ). This allows animals to adapt to a constantly changing environment and avoid unnecessary responses (Domjan and Burkhard 1986, Raderschall et al. 2011, Blumstein 2016. ...
Article
Full-text available
A rise in pinniped-fisheries interactions has been observed due to the global decline in fish populations primarily attributed to overfishing, and/or due to the recent population recovery of several pinniped species. Although many studies have quantified these interactions, a limited number delve into their behavioral aspects. We consulted 374 studies on pinniped-fisheries interactions from 1980 to 2020 and analyzed which of them addressed the interaction from a behavioral perspective. Additionally, we examined the role of learning in pinnipeds and their ability to capture prey from fishing gear. The stimulus-reward relationship in operational interactions was addressed in 51 (14%) studies, involving 10 (19%) of the world’s 54 pinniped species or subspecies. Depredation behavior (43%) and attack behavior (35%) were frequently reported during fishing interactions. Understanding associative and non-associative learning mechanisms has the potential to reduce economic losses from fishing activity, advance efficient mitigation measures to minimize conflicts, and establish pragmatic conservation priorities based on a better understanding of the behavioral aspects of these interactions. More research is needed to explore the effectiveness of deterrents in different species, improve methods of reducing habituation, and evaluate long-term behavioral effects, fostering effective strategies for minimizing conflicts and promoting coexistence between humans and pinnipeds.
... Picciulin et al., 2010;Purser & Radford, 2011;Wale et al., 2013b) and there is increasing evidence that this stressor can impact freshwater environments as well (Bolgan et al., 2016;Holt & Johnston, 2015;Mickle & Higgs, 2017;Pieniazek et al., 2020;Rountree et al., 2020). When a fish encounters a stressor in its environment, its initial response is often behavioral (Tuomainen & Candolin, 2011), and behavioral changes have commonly been documented when fish are exposed to acoustic stimuli in both laboratory and natural settings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Due to the increasing prevalence and variety of underwater anthropogenic noise sources, and the growing human population, anthropogenic noise has the potential to negatively impact aquatic organisms. With this growing threat, the question of how fishes respond to this stressor in their natural environments becomes more urgent. The current study used behavioral trials with bluegill sunfish Lepomis macrochirus, pumpkinseed sunfish Lepomis gibbosus, and rock bass Ambloplites rupestris, both in isolation and in a heterospecific trial, to determine how behaviors indicative of stress were influenced by interspecific interactions when exposed to recordings of pure tones and boat motors. Regardless of social context, all three species experienced an increase in fin beats per second, an increase in time spent at the bottom of the pen, and a decrease in time spent swimming when exposed to boat noise. Fishes in heterospecific trials experienced more fin beats per second and spent less time swimming, but there was no significant difference when comparing time spent at the bottom of pen with fish in individual trials. Our findings of behavioral changes when exposed to acoustic stimuli, in two social contexts, allow for a deeper understanding of interspecific effects and provide insight into how varied field studies can be useful in studying fish behavior when encountering acoustic stressors.
... reduced locomotion in N. granulata, when exposed to different anthropogenic sound sources, was previously described Snitman et al., 2022). Similarly, several studies conducted on decapods found reduced locomotion, a resting time increased or a response behavior (antipredator) diminished, in the presence of motorboat noise Wale et al., 2013;Filiciotto et al., 2016;Nousek-McGregor and Mei, 2016;Solan et al., 2016). Considering the effect of biological sounds from predators, a study conducted in crabs demonstrated that reduced the feeding behavior (Hughes et al., 2014), in cetaceans diminished the foraging behavior (Miller et al., 2022), and in fishes affected the mating choruses and calling rates (Luczkovich et al., 2000;Remage-Healey et al., 2006). ...
... Noise has been shown to negatively affect fishes and invertebrates, in terms of physical damage, physiology, and behavioral changes (André et al. 2011;Casper et al. 2013;Wale et al. 2013Wale et al. , 2019Hawkins et al. 2014;Solé et al. 2016Solé et al. , 2022Day et al. 2019). Given the prevalence of seabed vibration-producing activities, such as pile driving, many benthic species are exposed to a variety of stimuli that have the potential to impact benthic invertebrates in their vicinity. ...
Article
Full-text available
The diversity of biotic and abiotic sounds that fill underwater ecosystems has become polluted by anthropogenic noise in recent decades. Yet, there is still great uncertainty surrounding how different acoustic stimuli influence marine and freshwater (i.e., aquatic) communities. Despite capabilities to detect and produce sounds, aquatic invertebrates are among the most understud-ied taxa within the field of soundscape ecology. We conducted a meta-analysis to understand how sounds from various sources influence the behavior and physiology of aquatic invertebrates. We extracted 835 data points from 46 studies conducted in 15 countries. The resulting data included 50 species, a range of experimental conditions, and four sound categories: anthropo-genic, environmental, synthetic, and music. We used meta-analytic multivariate mixed-effect models to determine how each sound category influenced aquatic invertebrates and if responses were homogeneous across taxa. Our analyses illustrate that anthropogenic noise and synthetic sounds have detrimental impacts on aquatic invertebrate behavior and physiology, and that environmental sounds have slightly beneficial effects on their behavior. Defence responses were the most impacted behaviors, while the most prominent physiological responses were related to biochemistry, genetics, and morphology. Additionally, arthro-pods and molluscs exhibited the most pronounced physiological responses to anthropogenic and synthetic noise. These findings support the conclusion that many invertebrate species are sensitive to changes in aquatic soundscapes, which can cause adverse or favorable consequences to individuals and populations, dependent on the sound source. This quantitative synthesis highlights the necessity of including marine and freshwater invertebrates in acoustic exposure studies, aquatic ecosystem assessments, and emerging noise pollution policies.
Article
The marine environment is increasingly subject to changes driven by anthropogenic stressors which may alter species’ key behaviours and impact phenotypic plasticity. Such stressors rarely occur in isolation, yet our understanding of how simultaneous stresses affect marine organisms is limited. Here we study the combined impacts of a major global stressor, temperature increase, and a local stressor, anthropogenic noise, upon key defensive traits of the shore crab, Carcinus maenas. We tested the colour change and behavioural responses of crabs in relatively colder and warmer water, and in the presence of natural ambient or ship noise. Using image analysis and a model of predator vision, we demonstrate that crabs change colour, and improve camouflage, fastest in warmer water in the absence of anthropogenic noise. When anthropogenic noise was present it adversely impacted crab colour change and camouflage, to the extent that the accelerated change due to temperature was negated. Additionally, anthropogenic noise affected C. maenas’ behaviour, reducing the likelihood and increasing the latency of antipredator response to stimuli. This reveals an interaction between the two stressors, with the combination of temperature and noise eliciting different biological responses compared to the effects of each stressor in isolation. Our study demonstrates how such interactions between anthropogenic stressors may impact marine life.
Article
Full-text available
The influence of predation risk on patch choice was measured by examining the spatial distribution of 10 guppies (Poecilia reticulata) between two feeders, at one of which there was a risk of predation. The distribution was assumed to be ideal free. Nine unique situations were examined using all possible combinations of three risk levels and three diet levels, for each sex of guppy separately. Both sex and diet level influenced the effect of predation risk on patch choice. For the females the effect of risk was highest at the intermediate diet level. However, the males exhibited the opposite response: the effect of risk of predation was lowest at the intermediate diet level. A simple equation was then used to predict how much extra food (representing the energetic equivalent of risk) must be added to the risky patch for the guppies to become indifferent to the risk differences between the two types of patches. This manipulation caused a similar number of guppies to use both the risky and safe feeders, reducing or offsetting the influence of risk of predation. However, the male guppies were less influenced by this manipulation than were the females. The different results for the two sexes are consistent with known differences in their life histories, indicating that a knowledge of an animal's life history will often be necessary to understand how it makes trade-offs when choosing were to forage.
Article
Full-text available
Several studies have suggested that underwater sound may provide an orientation cue for the pelagic stages of coastal crustacea, such as crabs and lobsters, to find their way from the open ocean to the coast where they can settle. Yet, there has been no field evidence to support this phenomenon and it is unclear whether pelagic crustacean stages even have the ability to orient towards sources of underwater sound, such as that which emanates from reefs. Artificial sources of natural underwater sound were deployed offshore in conjunction with light traps to determine if the larval and post-larval stages of coastal crabs were attracted to coastal reef sound. The results demonstrated that the pelagic stages of crabs respond to underwater sounds and that they may use underwater sound to orient towards the coast. The orientation behaviour was modulated by lunar phase, being evident only during first- and last-quarter moon phases, at the time of neap tides. Active orientation during neap tides may take advantage of these incoming night-time tides for predator avoidance or may permit more effective directed swimming activity than is possible during new and full moon spring tides.
Article
The behavioural responses of Nephrops noruegicus (L.) to acoustic stimuli have been investigated under laboratory and free field conditions. In the laboratory a distinct set of postural motor responses elicited, by sound frequencies of 20–180Hz, was identified. This set of responses was then exploited during free field experiments conducted in a Scottish sea loch to determine the acoustic response threshold. Nephrops were found to be displacement rather than pressure sensitive with a response threshold of 0.888μm which was independent of frequency within the range 20–200Hz.
Article
Non-lethal behavioural effects of underwater noise in marine mammals are difficult to measure. Here we report acoustic and behavioural changes by fin whales in response to two different types of anthropogenic noise: shipping and airgun noise. Acoustic features of fin whale 20-Hz song notes recorded in the Mediterranean Sea and Northeast Atlantic Ocean were compared for areas with different shipping noise levels, different traffic intensities in the Strait of Gibraltar and during a seismic airgun array survey. In high noise conditions 20-Hz note duration shortened, bandwidth decreased, centre frequency decreased and peak frequency decreased. Similar results were obtained in 20-Hz song notes recorded during a 10-day seismic survey. During the first 72 h of the survey, a steady decrease in song received levels and bearings to singers indicated that whales moved away from the airgun array source and out of our detection area, and this displacement persisted for a time period well beyond the 10-day duration of seismic airgun activity. This study provides evidence that male fin whales from two different subpopulations modify song characteristics under increased background noise conditions, and that under seismic airgun activity conditions they leave an area for an extended period. We hypothesize that fin whale acoustic communication is modified to compensate for increased background noise and that a sensitization process may play a role in the observed temporary displacement. The observed acoustic and behavioural changes of this endangered species are discussed in the context of reproduction success and population survival.