ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract

In this paper, I will review why psychotherapy is relevant to the question of how consciousness relates to brain plasticity. A great deal of the research and theorizing on consciousness and the brain, including my own on hallucinations for example (Collerton and Perry, 2011) has focused upon specific changes in conscious content which can be related to temporal changes in restricted brain systems. I will argue that psychotherapy, in contrast, allows only a focus on holistic aspects of consciousness; an emphasis which may usefully complement what can be learnt from more specific methodologies.
MINI REVIEW ARTICLE
published: 06 September 2013
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00548
Psychotherapy and brain plasticity
Daniel Collerton1,2*
1Clinical Psychology, Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Foundation Trust, Gateshead, UK
2Newcastle upon Tyne, Newcastle University, UK
Edited by:
Elaine K. Perry, Newcastle
University, UK
Reviewed by:
Ursula Voss, Rheinische
Friedrich-Wilhelms University Bonn,
Germany
Elaine K. Perry, Newcastle
University, UK
*Correspondence:
Daniel Collerton, Clinical Psychology,
Northumberland, Tyne and Wear
NHS Foundation Trust, Bensham
Hospital, Saltwell Road, Gateshead,
NE8 4YL, UK
e-mail: daniel.collerton@ncl.ac.uk
In this paper, I will review why psychotherapy is relevant to the question of how
consciousness relates to brain plasticity. A great deal of the research and theorizing on
consciousness and the brain, including my own on hallucinations for example (Collerton
and Perry, 2011) has focused upon specific changes in conscious content which can be
related to temporal changes in restricted brain systems. I will argue that psychotherapy,
in contrast, allows only a focus on holistic aspects of consciousness; an emphasis which
may usefully complement what can be learnt from more specific methodologies.
Keywords: plasticity, psychotherapy, functional magnetic resonance imaging, multivoxel pattern analysis,
emotion, consciousness
INTRODUCTION
For the last century or so, psychotherapy has aimed to change
the mind. And if it has been effective in doing so, it must have
lead to lasting changes in conscious content, and potentially in
the process of consciousness itself. Few other human endeavors
seek so systematically to produce predictable enduring variation
in emotion, cognition, behavior and somatic perceptions; changes
which can persist for many years beyond the end of therapy. Over
the last few decades, as methods have become available for mea-
suring brain structure and function, it has provided a potential
real-life method by which meaningful changes in consciousness
can be related to measures of brain function. Admittedly, thus
far interest has been on understanding what brain function can
tell us about how psychotherapy works (for example, Joki´
c-Begi´
c,
2010) rather than the aim of this paper—what psychotherapy can
tell us about how the brain and mind are linked. However, in
theory, it should be possible to assess consciousness, or at least
some aspects of it, before and after psychotherapy, and to relate
these to brain changes. And indeed, as described later, such stud-
ies have been done. The evidence is still sporadic and somewhat
contradictory, but there is more potential now than ever before
to correlate psychotherapy-related changes in mind to changes in
brain.
In this paper, I will survey the current situation, outlining the
formidable conceptual and practical difficulties which still need to
be overcome, and suggest a potential strength of psychotherapy
as a tool for understanding consciousness which, in combina-
tion with advances in functional imaging analysis, may give a way
forwards.
CHALLENGES OF DEFINITION AND MEASUREMENT
There are a number of intractable definitional and measurement
issues in this field which have been only partially solved.
Consciousness itself is a fuzzy concept—and its components
are no clearer, as illustrated by the continuing discussions on
whether it even exists as a meaningful phenomenon. James explic-
itly posed the question in the first, 1904, volume of the Journal of
Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods and over a cen-
tury later the answer is still not clear. As exemplified by Newell
and Shanks (in press), the nature and role of consciousness and
its relationship to behavior are still under discussion.
The term psychotherapy equally lacks definition. It has been
applied to an exceptionally wide range of approaches with dif-
fering models, techniques, goals, and outcomes. Feltham and
Horton’s (2012) Handbook, for example, surveys some 23 major
approaches from Dialectical Behavior Therapy to Psychodrama.
Finally, though methods of investigating in vivo brain function
are incomparably better than even a decade ago, they are still lim-
ited in cognitive, temporal, and spatial resolution, while the large
and intrusive technology involved limits applications to settings
which bear little relationship to everyday life.
In the face of fuzzy concepts and methods, there is a scientific
temptation to retreat into investigating relatively specific, easily
measureable, aspects of consciousness and the brain. However,
looked at in another way, this very lack of focus may be a strength
rather than a weakness in that it forces attention to holistic
changes in consciousness which can then be related to systemic
changes in neural networks.
THE EFFECTS OF PSYCHOTHERAPY
Within the vast variety of psychotherapies, Cognitive Behavioral
Therapy (CBT) has become the most widely accepted approach,
and has amassed a strong body of evidence of effectiveness (Butler
et al., 2006). It leads to long lasting, reproducible changes in emo-
tion, cognition, behavior, and somatic symptoms across a range
of mood and other psychological disorders. This strongly sug-
gests that CBT is a powerful means of changing consciousness.
CBT has always had a marked emphasis on measurement and it
is also the therapy whose effects have most often been related to
brain changes. For these reasons, I will take it as the exemplar
www.frontiersin.org September 2013 | Volume 4 | Article 548 |1
Collerton Brain plasticity and psychotherapy
psychotherapy for the purpose of this paper, while acknowledging
that other approaches may be equally valid.
If we want to relate the effects of psychotherapy on conscious-
ness to brain changes, it would seem necessary to know the
changes it produces and how it does so. Consciousness itself is
not directly accessible of course, so, as summarized in Tab l e 1 ,
researchers have taken a number of routes to inferring what is
in the consciousness of a patient. Self-report, observer report,
behavioral measures, and experimental tasks can be used to more
or less directly infer changes in the content of patient’s con-
scious thought. Taken together, these methodologies have pro-
duced good evidence that the complex combinations of emotions,
cognitions, behaviors and somatic symptoms which characterize
mood disorders do shift as a result of CBT.
WHAT CHANGES AS A CONSEQUENCE OF
PSYCHOTHERAPY?
However, evidence is lacking as to what specifically changes as a
consequence of psychotherapy (see, for example, Murphy et al.,
2009). Despite the range of different ways of measuring the effects
of psychotherapy noted above, it is striking how closely these are
related. Thus, change in one symptom area, for example cogni-
tion, is accompanied by changes in other symptom areas such as
emotion or behavior; at least as averaged over the timescales and
group numbers common in treatment trials.
Similarly, though there is evidence that different modalities of
therapy may have different levels of effectiveness (see Tolin, 2010
for a meta-analytic comparison of CBT with other therapies),
where this does occur this appears to be more a quantitative than
a qualitative difference. The outcomes of psychodynamic, person-
centred, and behavioral psychotherapy are broadly equivalent
despite their varieties of approaches and targets for therapeu-
tic change (Stiles et al., 2008; Budd and Hughes, 2009)perhaps
because they work via common final paths (Mansell, 2011).
Even treatments as different as CBT and pharmacotherapy
appear to have broadly similar outcomes in, for example, depres-
sion (DeRubeis et al., 2008)withnoreliableevidenceofthe
differential effects of CBT on negative cognitions or medication
on somatic symptoms as might have been expected from their
mechanisms of action. Attempts to predict which patients might
benefit from any specific intervention have not been successful.
Taking all of these comparisons together, there is very lit-
tle evidence that specific areas of consciousness can change in a
meaningful way without these effects rippling through the rest of
consciousness. This suggests that trying to fractionate the effects
of psychotherapy on consciousness into components might not
be possible. It is the totality of consciousness which changes as a
result of CBT rather than one specific aspect of it.
IMPLICATIONS FOR LINKING THE EFFECTS OF
PSYCHOTHERAPY TO BRAIN PLASTICITY
This has implications for relating the effects of CBT to brain
plasticity. Traditionally, neuroscience has adopted a reductionist
approach to the brain; relating specific psychological functions
to specific brain areas. This has been enormously successful with
many psychological functions. Amongst many other pairings,
neuropsychological localization has linked learning to the hip-
pocampus and other structures in the medial temporal lobe,
object perception to the ventral visual stream, and language to
the left temporal cortex. However, the localization paradigm has
left in its wake the binding problem—how the functions of dis-
parate brain areas are tied together to produce the usual subjective
sense of a single coherent consciousness. In this context, perhaps
the holistic effects of CBT and other psychotherapies on con-
sciousness become a means of side stepping the binding problem.
If significant changes in bound consciousness can be related to
restricted changes in brain function, that might narrow down the
candidate brain areas which may underlie consciousness.
EFFECTS OF CBT ON MEASURES OF BRAIN FUNCTION
The plasticity in human brain which underlies individual,
idiosyncratic and instantaneous elements of consciousness—
specific words, thoughts, images, feelings, and memories—comes
from tiny, subtle, and dynamic changes which are embedded
within and across networks of microscopic cells. In comparison,
our ways of measuring plasticity in the human brain have to trade
off cognitive (the ability of scans to resolve precise psychological
states, particular memories for instance), spatial, and tempo-
ral resolutions with even the best resolution vastly greater than
the fundamental mechanisms of plasticity. The spatial resolution
limit of a Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scan, our best cur-
rent means of directly assessing brain structure and function,
contains somewhere around 9 million brain cells (deCharms,
2008).
However, this has not prevented functional imaging, partic-
ularly functional MRI (fMRI) to identify which areas of the
brain change following psychotherapy. (There have been a rather
small number of structural imaging studies of the effects of psy-
chotherapy, mainly in eating disorders, but many of these have
been confounded by the effects of weight gain and loss on brain
structure (Lobera, 2011) making it difficult to interpret their
results).
The dominant paradigm has been to compare levels of brain
activity pre and post CBT to see what changes. This approach
has mainly been used in depression and has identified that
changes are localized to specific frontal, cingulate, and limbic
areas. There is decreased activity in the limbic system, especially
Table 1 | Indicators of consciousness used in CBT outcome studies.
Type of measure Example measures Illustrative study
Self-report Structured questionnaires e.g., Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1988)Elkin et al., 1995
Observer report Structured observer ratings e.g., Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Bagby et al., 2004)Teasdale et al., 2000
Behavioral measures Observable behavioral change e.g., return to employment Della-Posta and Drummond, 2006
Experimental tasks Measures of perceptual distortion e.g., body morphing (Benson et al., 1999)Cornelissen et al., 2013
Frontiers in Psychology | Consciousness Research September 2013 | Volume 4 | Article 548 |2
Collerton Brain plasticity and psychotherapy
the amygdala, with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex becoming rela-
tively more active and orbitomedial and cingulate cortex less so; a
move toward normality from patterns observed before treatment
(Ochsner et al., 2002; Goldapple et al., 2004; Malhi et al., 2004;
Ritchey et al., 2011; Höflich et al., 2012) and consistent with what
is know of the processing of emotional stimuli (Simpson et al.,
2000; Northoff et al., 2004; Leppänen, 2006; Beck, 2008). Pre-
treatment levels of cingulate activity can even predict response
to CBT with some reliability (Konarski et al., 2009; Ritchey et al.,
2011; Siegle et al., 2012).
There have not been comparisons between the effects of differ-
ent types of psychotherapy on the brain (potentially interesting
in view of their equivalent effects on consciousness), but there
are conflicting reports of the effects of pharmacotherapy; similar
changes in brain activity to those following psychotherapy were
not seen after antidepressant treatment by Goldapple et al. (2004)
though they were identified by Furmark et al. (2002); opening up,
but not confirming, the possibility that different brain changes
might have similar effects on consciousness.
Taken as a whole, this evidence would suggest that the holistic
changes in consciousness seen after psychotherapy for depression
are associated with changes in a relatively restricted number of
brain areas; mainly frontal, cingulate, and limbic cortex, with the
implication that plasticity in those areas is particularly associated
with persistent variations in consciousness.
However, a one to one correspondence between change in
depression and change in specific brain areas may be over stated
(Linden, 2006; Frewen et al., 2008; Dichter et al., 2012). For
example, very similar changes in those brain areas are seen after
CBT and other psychological treatments for anxiety (Furmark
et al., 2002; Paquette et al., 2003; Straube et al., 2006; Porto
et al., 2009; Freyer et al., 2011), schizophrenia (Wykes et al.,
2002), eating disorders (Vocks et al., 2011) and Irritable Bowel
Syndrome (Lackner et al., 2006). This is consistent with a con-
sciousness network which depends upon these brain areas (and
no doubt others) but it also suggests that there is a large over-
lap in the brain changes associated with different holistic states
of consciousness. At present, fMRI data would suggest that, sim-
ply put, CBT is associated with a decrease in emotionality (less
limbic activity) and an increase in thoughtfulness (increased dor-
solateral frontal activity), as would be expected from its aims
and methods (Clark and Beck, 2010). Though we may be able to
link consciousness to a subset of anatomical structures using psy-
chotherapy as an investigative tool, we appear to lack specificity in
our account of how different states of consciousness could arise. Is
it that in using psychotherapy to localize holistic changes in con-
sciousness to a restricted set of brain structures, we have lost the
ability to account for why consciousness is so idiosyncratic and so
changeable?
APOTENTIAL WAY FORWARD
An analogous challenge has arisen in fMRI studies of visual per-
ception. Early attempts to localize specific perceptions to specific
brain areas worked only for grossly different stimuli—visualizing
navigating a house compared to imagining playing tennis (Owen
et al., 2006)—or for simple stimuli in early, highly specialized,
visual areas (Kay et al., 2008). More latterly, however, multivoxel
pattern analysis (MVPA), in which patterns of activity across wide
areas of the brain are analyzed, has produced a significant increase
in cognitive resolution. Fairly similar stimuli, for example chairs
and shoes (Norman et al., 2006; deCharms, 2008; Poldrack, 2011),
or over-riding categories of images such as living or non-living
(Naselaris et al., 2012) can now be recognized from pattern infor-
mation fMRI (Formisano and Kriegeskorte, 2012) data. Not only
perceptions, but also images and memories (Chadwick et al.,
2012; Rissman and Wagner, 2012) are starting to be distinguished.
Beginnings are starting to be made in reproducing data across
as well as within subjects (Accamma and Suma, 2012; Raizada
and Connolly, 2012). Significantly, cognitive resolution appears
to increase as the focus of the analysis is widened to include more
brain areas.
MVPA might therefore lead to the ability to map holis-
tic changes in consciousness to patterns within and across the
regions that classic fMRI has identified as responsive to psy-
chotherapy (Siegle et al., 2007). Thus it may provide the mech-
anism to bridge holistic and specific variations in consciousness
and brain.
CONCLUSIONS
It is clear that CBT, and probably other psychotherapies, alters
consciousness in personally important, lasting, and measurable
ways. Brain function and brain structure are different after CBT.
Looking at functional changes in the brain suggests that con-
sciousness changes in response to plasticity in the linked systems
of the frontal, cingulate, and limbic cortices. However, we do not
know how modulations in those areas link to different states of
consciousness. Using the most recent imaging analysis to map
activity simultaneously across these regions might give the miss-
ing specificity; allowing whole brain changes to be mapped to
holistic changes in consciousness.
In order to do this, our next challenge will be to develop ways
of capturing the experience of consciousness as a whole rather
than, as we have tried to do in the past, the individual thoughts,
images, and emotions which are bound together to produce it.
REFERENCES
Accamma,I.V.,andSuma,H.N.
(2012). Feature selection for decod-
ing of cognitive states in multiple-
subject functional magnetic reso-
nance imaging data. Adv. Intell. Sy st.
Comput. 174 2012, 997–1004. doi:
10.1007/978-81-322-0740-5_121
Bagby, R. M., Ryder, A. G., Schuller,
D. R., and Marshall, M. B. (2004).
The hamilton depression rat-
ing Scale: has the gold standard
become a lead weight? Am. J.
Psychiatry 161, 2163–2177. doi:
10.1176/appi.ajp.161.12.2163
Beck, A. (2008). The evolution of
thecognitivemodelofdepression
and its neurobiological correlates.
Am.J.Psychiatry165, 969–977. doi:
10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Carbin,
M. G. (1988). Psychometric prop-
erties of the Beck depression inven-
tory: twenty-five years of evaluation.
Clin. Psychol. Rev. 8, 77–100. doi:
10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5
Benson, P. J., Emery, J. L., Cohen-Tovée,
E. M., and Tovée, M. J. (1999).
A computergraphic technique for
the study of body size perception
and body types. Behav. Res. Methods
Instrum. Comput. 31, 446–454. doi:
10.3758/BF03200725
Budd, R., and Hughes, I. (2009). The
Dodo Bird Verdict—controversial,
inevitable and important: a
commentaryon30yearsof
metaanalyses. Clin. Psychol.
Psychother. 16, 510–522. doi:
10.1002/cpp.648
www.frontiersin.org September 2013 | Volume 4 | Article 548 |3
Collerton Brain plasticity and psychotherapy
Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman,
E. M., and Beck, A. T. (2006).
The empirical status of cognitive-
behavioral therapy: a review of
meta-analyses. Clin. Psychol. Rev.
26, 17–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.
07.003
Chadwick,M.J.,Bonnici,H.M.,and
Maguire, E. A. (2012). Decoding
information in the human
hippocampus: a user’s guide.
Neuro psy cho log ia 50, 3107–3121.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2012.07.007
deCharms, R. C. (2008). Applications
of real-time fMRI. Nat. Rev.
Neuro sci. 9, 720–729. doi: 10.1038/
nrn2414
Clark, D. A., and Beck, A. T. (2010).
Cognitive theory and therapy of
anxiety and depression: conver-
gence with neurobiological findings.
Tre n d s Cog n . S c i . 14, 418–424. doi:
10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.007
Collerton, D., and Perry, E. (2011).
Dreaming and hallucinations
– continuity or discontinuity.
Perspectives from dementia with
Lewy bodies. Conscious. Cogn. 20,
1016–1020. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.
2011.03.024
Cornelissen, P. L., Johns, A., and
Martin, J. T. (2013). Body size over-
estimation in women with anorexia
nervosa is not qualitatively dif-
ferent from female controls origi-
nal research article Body Image 10,
103–111.
Della-Posta, C., and Drummond, P.
D. (2006). Cognitive behavioural
therapy increases re-employment
of job seeking worker’s compensa-
tion clients. J. Occup. Rehabil. 16,
217–224. doi: 10.1007/s10926-006-
9024-5
DeRubeis, R. J., Siegle, G. J., and
Hollon, S. D. (2008). Cognitivether-
apy versus medication for depres-
sion: treatment outcomes and neu-
ral mechanisms. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
9, 788–796. doi: 10.1038/nrn2345
Dichter, G. S., Sikich, L., Song, A.,
Voyvodic, J., and Bodfish, J. W.
(2012). Functional neuroimaging
of treatment effects in psychiatry:
methodological challenges and rec-
ommendations. Int. J. Neurosci. 122,
483–493. doi: 10.3109/00207454.
2012.678446
Elkin, I., Gibbons, R. D., Shea, M.
T., Sotsky, S. M., Watkins, J. T.,
Pilkonis, P. A., et al. (1995). Initial
severity and differential treatment
outcome in the national institute
of mental health treatment of
depression collaborative research
program. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
63, 841. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.63.
5.841
Feltham, C., and Horton, I. (eds.).
(2012). The SAGE Handbook of
Counselling and Psychotherapy.
London: SAGE Publications.
Formisano, E., and Kriegeskorte, N.
(2012). Seeing patterns through
the hemodynamic veil—the future
of pattern-information fMRI.
Neuro ima ge 62, 1249–1265. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.078
Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J., and
Lanius, R. A. (2008). Neuroimaging
studies of psychological interven-
tions for mood and anxiety disor-
ders: empirical and methodologi-
cal review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 28,
228–246. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.
05.002
Freyer, T., Klöppel, S., Tüscher, O.,
Kordon,A.,Zurowski,B.,Kuelz,A.
K. et al. (2011). Frontostriatal
activation in patients with
obsessive–compulsive disorder
before and after cognitive behav-
ioral therapy. Psychol. Med. 41,
207–216. doi: 10.1017/S003329
1710000309
Furmark, T., Tillfors, M.,
Marteinsdottir, I., Fischer, H.,
Pissiota, A., Langstrom, B., et al.
(2002). Common changes in
cerebralbloodflowinpatients
with social phobia treated with
citalopram or cognitive-behavioral
therapy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
59:425. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.
5.425
Goldapple, K., Segal, Z., Garson, C.,
Lau, M., Bieling, P., Kennedy,
S., et al. (2004). Modulation of
cortical-limbic pathways in major
depression: treatment-specific
effects of cognitive behavior ther-
apy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 34.
doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.34
Höflich, A., Baldinger, P., Savli, M.,
Lanzenberger, R., and Kasper, S.
(2012). Imaging treatment effects
in depression. Rev. Neurosci. 23,
227–252. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-
2012-0038
James, W. (1904). Does ‘Consciousness’
exist. J. Philos.Psychol. Sci. Methods
1, 477–491. doi: 10.2307/2011942
Joki´
c-Begi´
c, N. (2010). Cognitive-
behavioral therapy and
neuroscience: towards closer
integration psychological topics. 19,
235–254.
Kay, K. N., Naselaris, T., Prenger, R. J.,
and Gallant, J. L. (2008). Identifying
natural images from human brain
activity. Nature 452, 352–355. doi:
10.1038/nature06713
Konarski, J. Z., Kennedy, S. H., Segal,
Z.V.,Lau,M.A.,Bieling,P.J.,
McIntyre, R. S., et al. (2009).
Predictors of nonresponse to
cognitive behavioural therapy
or venlafaxine using glucose
metabolism in major depressive
disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 34,
175.
Lackner, J. M., Lou Coad, M., Mertz, H.
R., Wack, D. S., Katz, L. A., Krasner,
S. S., et al. (2006). Cognitive therapy
for irritable bowel syndrome is asso-
ciated with reduced limbic activity,
GI symptoms, and anxiety. Behav.
Res. Ther. 44, 621–638. doi: 10.1016/
j.brat.2005.05.002
Leppänen, J. M. (2006). Emotional
information processing in mood
disorders: a review of behav-
ioral and neuroimaging findings.
Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 34.
doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000191500.
46411.00
Linden, D. E. J. (2006). How psy-
chotherapy changes the brain–the
contribution of functional neu-
roimaging. Mol. Psychiatry 11,
528–538. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.
4001816
Lobera, I. J. (2011). Neuroimaging
in eating disorders. Neuropsychiatr.
Dis. Treat. 82, 45.
Malhi, G. S., Lagopoulos, J., Ward, P. B.,
Kumari, V., Mitchell, P. B., Parker,
G. B. et al. (2004). Cognitive gener-
ation of affect in bipolar depression:
an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19,
741–754. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.
2003.03159.x
Mansell, W. (2011). Core processes
of psychopathology and recovery:
“Does the Dodo bird effect have
wings?” Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31,
189–192. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.
06.009
Murphy,R.,Cooper,Z.,Hollon,S.D.,
and Fairburn, C. G. (2009). How
do psychological treatments work.
investigating mediators of change.
Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 1. doi: 10.1016/
j.brat.2008.10.001
Naselaris, T., Stansbury, D. E., and
Gallant, J. L. (2012). Cortical rep-
resentation of animate and inan-
imate objects in complex natu-
ral scenes. J. Physiol. Paris 106,
239–249. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.
2012.02.001
Newell, B. R., and Shanks, D. R.
(in press). Unconscious influ-
ences on decision making. Behav.
Brain Sci.
Norman,K.A.,Polyn,S.M.,Detre,G.
J., and Haxby, J. V. (2006). Beyond
mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern
analysis of fMRI data. Tre n d s Cog n .
Sci. 10, 424–430. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.
2006.07.005
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., Bermpohl,
F., Niese, R., Pfennig, A.,
PascualLeone, A., et al. (2004).
Reciprocal modulation and atten-
uation in the prefrontal cortex: an
fMRI study on emotional–cognitive
interaction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 21,
202–212. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20002
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross,
J. J., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002).
Rethinking feelings: an fMRI
study of the cognitive regulation
of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
14, 1215–1229. doi: 10.1162/
089892902760807212
Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly,
M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., and
Pickard, J. D. (2006). Detecting
awareness in the vegetative state.
Science 313, 1402–1402. doi:
10.1126/science.1130197
Paquette, V., Lévesque, J., Mensour,
B., Leroux, J. M., Beaudoin,
G., Bourgouin, P., et al. (2003).
“Change the mind and you
change the brain”: effects of
cognitive-behavioral therapy
on the neural correlates of spi-
der phobia. Neuro ima ge 18,
401–409. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119
(02)00030-7
Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring
mental states from neuroimaging
data: from reverse inference to
large-scale decoding. Neuro n 72,
692–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.
2011.11.001
Porto, P., Oliveira, L., Mari, J.,
Volchan, E., Figueira, I., and
Ventura, P. (2009). Does cogni-
tive behavioral therapy change
the brain. a systematic review
of neuroimaging in anxiety
disorders. J. Neuropsychiatry.
Clin. Neurosci. 21, 114–125.
doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.21.
2.114
Raizada, R. D., and Connolly, A. C.
(2012). What makes different peo-
ple’s representations alike: neural
similarity space solves the prob-
lem of across-subject fmri decoding.
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 868–877. doi:
10.1162/jocn_a_00189
Rissman, J., and Wagner, A. D. (2012).
Distributed representations in
memory: insights from functional
brain imaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol.
63, 101–128. doi: 10.1146/annurev-
psych-120710-100344
Ritchey, M., Dolcos, F., Eddington, K.
M., Strauman, T. J., and Cabeza,
R. (2011). Neural correlates of
emotional processing in depression:
changes with cognitive behavioral
therapy and predictors of treat-
ment response. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45,
577–587. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.
2010.09.007
Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., and Thase,
M. E. (2007). Neurobehavioral
therapies in the 21st century:
summary of an emerging field and
an extended example of cognitive
Frontiers in Psychology | Consciousness Research September 2013 | Volume 4 | Article 548 |4
Collerton Brain plasticity and psychotherapy
control training for depression.
Cogn. Ther. Res. 31, 235–262. doi:
10.1007/s10608-006-9118-6
Siegle, G. J., Thompson, W. K., Collier,
A., Berman, S. R., Feldmiller, J.,
Thase, M. E., et al. (2012). Toward
clinically useful neuroimaging in
depression treatment prognostic
utility of subgenual cingulate
activity for determining depres-
sion outcome in cognitive therapy
across studies, scanners, and
patient characteristic ssgacc and
cognitive therapy outcome. Arch.
Gen. Psychiatry 69, 913–924. doi:
10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.65
Simpson, J. R., Öngür, D., Akbudak,
E., Conturo, T. E., Ollinger, J. M.,
Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2000). The
emotional modulation of cognitive
processing: an fMRI study. J. Cogn.
Neuro sci. 12(Suppl. 2), 157–170.
doi: 10.1162/089892900564019
Stiles, W. B., Barkham, M.,
Mellor-Clark, J., and Connell,
J. (2008). Effectiveness of
cognitive-behavioural, person-
centred, and psychodynamic
therapies in UK primary-care
routine practice: replication in a
larger sample. Psychol. Med. 38,
677–688. doi: 10.1017/S0033291
707001511
Straube, T., Glauer, M., Dilger, S.,
Mentzel, H. J., and Miltner, W.
H. (2006). Effects of cognitive-
behavioral therapy on brain
activation in specific phobia.
Neuro ima ge 29, 125–135. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.007
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams,
J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby,
J. M., and Lau, M. A. (2000).
Prevention of relapse/recurrence in
major depression by mindfulness-
based cognitive therapy. J. Consult.
Clin. Psychol. 68, 615. doi: 10.1037/
0022-006X.68.4.615
Tolin, D. F. (2010). Is cognitive–
behavioral therapy more effective
than other therapies?: a meta-
analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev.
30, 710–720. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.
2010.05.003
Vocks, S., Schulte, D., Busch, M.,
Grönemeyer, D., Herpertz, S., and
Suchan, B. (2011). Changes in neu-
ronal correlates of body image
processing by means of cognitive-
behavioural body image therapy for
eating disorders: a randomized con-
trolledfMRIstudy.Psychol. Med. 41,
1651–1663. doi: 10.1017/S00332917
10002382
Wykes,T.,Brammer,M.,Mellers,
J.,Bray,P.,Reeder,C.,Williams,
C., et al. (2002). Effects on the
brain of a psychological treatment:
cognitive remediation therapy
functional magnetic resonance
imaging in schizophrenia. Br. J.
Psychiatry 181, 144–152.
Conflict of Interest Statement: The
author declares that the research
was conducted in the absence of any
commercial or financial relationships
that could be construed as a potential
conflict of interest.
Received: 31 January 2013; accepted:
02 August 2013; published online: 06
September 2013.
Citation: Collerton D (2013)
Psychotherapy and brain plasticity.
Front. Psychol. 4:548. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.
2013.00548
This article was submitted to
Consciousness Research, a section of
the journal Frontiers in Psychology.
Copyright © 2013 Collerton. This is
an open-access article distributed under
the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (CC BY). The use,
distribution or reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the origi-
nal author(s) or licensor are credited and
that the original publication in this jour-
nal is cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution
or reproduction is permitted which does
not comply with these terms.
www.frontiersin.org September 2013 | Volume 4 | Article 548 |5
... It is also relevant for working with patients with autism, as it stimulates the brain to search for new neural connections and their active functioning. Psychotherapy helps to reconsider one's beliefs and life principles, views and stereotypes, and attitudes towards oneself and others, which also contributes to neuroplasticity activation [13,14]. E. Hertenstein et al. [15] highlighted the importance of mindfulness techniques, effectively used in psychotherapy to stimulate brain neuroplasticity through traditional meditation, including attentive listening and accepting the present moment without value judgments. ...
Article
Full-text available
The research aims to study the mechanisms and factors contributing to brain neuroplasticity. To achieve this goal, the following methods were used: analysis and synthesis, hermeneutic method, psychological testing, and comparative and generalization methods. The research results revealed the nature of the concept of brain neuroplasticity and types of neuroplasticity, analyzed the process of redistribution of brain functions, determined the role of compensatory plasticity, revealed methods of studying brain neuroplasticity, investigated the influence of brain processes on the course of learning, memory development, awareness, concentration, speech; identified factors that can affect brain neuroplasticity revealed the role of genetic factors, analyzed stimulation and rehabilitation methods to promote neuroplasticity. The findings may aid in developing novel rehabilitation techniques, specifically for stroke patients, by utilizing the brain’s compensatory abilities through physical activity, pharmacological interventions, and stimulation techniques. The practical significance of the research is determined by the current disclosure of the features of brain neuroplasticity to understand its ability to reorganize the sensory and perceptual systems.
... This approach has been used mainly in cases of depression, comparing brain activity levels in patients before and after therapy using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The differences have been observed, identifying localized changes in specific areas: a decrease in the activity of the amygdala, which plays a key role in attributing emotional meanings to memories, and an increase in the activity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which is responsible for planning and regulating behaviour [30]. The thus delineated picture of the brain process of unlocking emotional learning, which allows for the unlearning, rewriting and deletion of new learning during the period of lability, is of paramount importance for psychotherapy, as it allows for consciously explicating the stages of consolidation, significantly increasing the effectiveness with which a therapist can dissipate and/or delete a wide range of dysfunctional emotional responses [31,32]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Much data is now available on the effectiveness of psychotherapies on the stability of the change achieved and on the therapeutic relationship, which, in its various conceptual declinations, constitutes the most incisive predictor of outcome. Moreover, the principle that psychotherapy is more effective when it is more specifically oriented towards the psychopathological factors underlying the symptoms is becoming increasingly clear. Many studies have confirmed that the most effective psychotherapists can create deep alliances with a diverse typology of patients, regardless of the relational skills and attachment patterns the patient possessed at the beginning of therapy. An aspect not always considered is ‘memory reconsolidation,’ which allows modifying or replacing dysfunctional emotional responses with more functional ones.
... Mindfulness increases the brain grey matter in the right orbitofrontal area and the lower brain stem while reducing the hyperactivation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and frontal-limbic system involved in emotion regulation and modulates the midline prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortex by increasing self-awareness [85][86][87][88][89][90] . DBT modulates the brain's cortico-limbic and PFC and reduces amygdala hyperactivity 91 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a complex mental health condition with an altered image of self, impulsive acts, suicidal ideation, and self-harm requiring intensive care in outpatient and inpatient settings. The biopsychosocial (BPS) model adopted in the current study extracted the outcomes of a research about the diagnosis, causes and treatment of BPD. A network model helped link these results in a unitary model with applications in clinical practice for assessment and intervention. Methods: We conducted a literature review of current studies on the BPS causes of BPD and merged them through meta-synthesis. The results were then elaborated with a psychopathological network analysis for linking the extracted factors with higher degree of centrality in the network and merged in a final comprehensive model. Results: The theoretical modelisation suggests that BPS causes merged with the diathesis-stress model persistently activate the cortico-limbic system and prefrontal cortex, induce neuroinflammation, and stimulate suicidal and parasuicidal ideation and behaviours modulated by psychological and pharmacological treatment. Conclusions: Using a network model in psychopathology allowed the merging of data about BPD into a unitary and dynamic pattern which can be helpful to direct assessments and interventions in clinical practice.
... Neuroplastizität beschreibt die Fähigkeit des Gehirns, sich auf eine sich verändernde Umwelt flexibel anzupassen und sich und seine Arbeitsweise entsprechend neu zu organisieren (Pittenger und Duman 2008). Psychologisch-psychotherapeutische Interventionen selbst können die Neuroplastizität steigern (Collerton 2013). Zudem beeinflusst wiederum das vorhandene Funktionslevel an neurobiologischer Regulationsfähigkeit die Effektivität von Psychotherapie (Wendt et al. 2018). ...
Chapter
Die biologisch informierte Psychotherapie integriert das Wissen der klinischen Neurowissenschaften in die Weiterentwicklung potenzieller Psychotherapieverfahren für therapieresistente Depressionen (TRD). Eine spezifische Berücksichtigung findet dabei der gezielte Einsatz von biologischen Methoden zur Augmentation psychotherapeutischer Interventionen. Die Indikation wird aus einem mechanistischen biopsychologischen Prozessmodell von Psychotherapie abgeleitet. Vorläufige Ergebnisse zeigen, dass die Steigerung der Neuroplastizität die Effektivität sich anschließender psychologisch-psychotherapeutischer Interventionen erhöhen kann. Dabei werden pharmakologische Substanzen (z. B. Ketamin) und Methoden der nichtinvasiven Hirnstimulation eingesetzt (z. B. Elektrokonvulsive Therapie und transkranielle Gleichstromstimulation). Aktuell noch in den Kinderschuhen, hat die biologisch informierte Psychotherapie das Potenzial, neue Behandlungsmöglichkeiten für die TRD zu entwickeln.
... (Fu y Zuo, 2011;Skaliora, 2002). Existe gran cantidad de estudios comparados en humanos que han abordado los cambios neuroestructurales, neurofisiológicos y conductuales producidos por experiencias específicas; por ejemplo, el entrenamiento de habilidades motoras manuales puede incrementar el grosor cortical de las áreas motoras primarias, responsables de estos movimientos; (Herholz y Zatorre, 2012; Mawase, Uehara, Bastian y Celnik, 2017) el entrenamiento en ciertas profesiones como los taxistas de grandes ciudades, causa un agrandamiento del volumen del hipocampo; (Maguire et al., 2000) el aprendizaje de otra lengua conlleva a la reorganización de las cortezas temporales bilaterales; (Maguire et al., 2000) la psicoterapia suele causar la reorganización en la conectividad entre múltiples áreas corticales y límbicas; (Carroll, 2009;Collerton, 2013) la enseñanza de la lectoescritura causa reorganizaciones múltiples en el funcionamiento local y global del cerebro; (Dehaene et al., 2015); y la rehabilitación neuropsicológica produce cambios correlacionados con el desarrollo de habilidades cognitivas. (Prosperini y Filippo, 2019) Sobre la relación entre la plasticidad cerebral con los múltiples componentes corporales no-neurales (microbioma, sistema inmune, sistema cardiovascular y sistema endocrino) la mayor parte de las evidencias provienen de estudios con animales. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
En este capítulo el lector podrá encontrar una aproximación teó-rica que integra los múltiples eventos y mecanismos implicados en el desarrollo del funcionamiento neuropsicológico. En primer lugar, se realiza la descripción de algunos principios fundamenta-les para comprender el desarrollo humano. Luego se presenta la aproximación bioecológica y biocultural que descompone los de-terminantes del desarrollo en factores biológicos y ambientales. Dentro de los determinantes biológicos se describen procesos a nivel molecular, mecanismos del neurodesarrollo y la interacción entre sistemas corporales. Como factores ecológicos, se destaca el papel del ambiente y el entretejido entre biología y cultura. Por último, se describen los procesos de plasticidad cerebral y la forma como la postura sistémica y biocultural puede facilitar la interpretación de la plasticidad cerebral, asociada al funcionamiento neuropsicológico, a través del ciclo vital.
... Daniel Collerton in his review describe Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT), and probably other psychotherapies, altering the consciousness in personally important, lasting, and measurable ways. 3 Looking at functional changes in the brain suggests that consciousness changes in response to plasticity in the linked systems of the frontal, cingulate, and limbic cortices. However, we do not know how modulations in those areas link to different states of consciousness. ...
Article
Full-text available
We write to highlight an emerging non-invasive therapeutic option that can be potentially used for various medical ailments. RTT is Rapid Transformational Therapy, a hybrid therapy developed by Marisa Peer in England, UK.1 It combines the best principles of hypnotherapy, cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), Neuro- Linguistic Programming that offers fast effective results for a range of psychological, psychiatric and physical conditions. As opposed to traditional therapies, RTT is a solution-focussed treatment offering rapid, effective and long-lasting change usually within one (or within three maximum) session. RTT embraces the positive aspects of all the above techniques that are known to produce a transformative effect on the clients. Hypnotherapy uses trance, regression and hypnotic conditioning. RTT goes beyond this process, Marisa identified that regardless of their issue, in sessions clients benefitted from her applying a particular set of techniques over and over again. These insights provided the foundations for RTT to emerge as a distinctive approach.
... The complexity of the brain still baffles science such that basic questions as to how structure and function are related, as well as where and how information is stored, retrieved, and processed, remain unresolved. Neuroscientific contributions to construct validity in neuropsychology include a better understanding of memory systems (Budson & Price, 2005), new concepts such as salience (Menon, 2011), and the neurophysiological impact associated with successful psychotherapy (Collerton, 2013). The article demonstrates the potential value of the Rorschach in this developing field of study. ...
Chapter
This interdisciplinary work offers a comprehensive analysis of paradoxes and paradoxical thinking, exploring their manifestations in philosophy, societal dynamics, personality, and neuroscience. Demonstrating various methods for the augmentation of creativity and improved performance, this book uniquely integrates theoretical perspective with case studies and practical applications. As such it elucidates the theory and mechanisms of transforming the apparently impossible into the possible, illustrated by cases of social innovators successfully addressing insurmountable challenges. Aimed at graduate and postgraduate social science students and scholars, with over 500 bibliographical references, the text remains accessible to a broader audience due to its engaging language. Emphasizing the significance of paradoxes and paradoxical thinking in both professional and everyday contexts, it provides a nuanced exploration of paradoxical phenomena, making it a valuable resource for academic and general readers alike.
Article
Full-text available
Neither psychological nor neuroscientific investigations have been able to fully explain the paradox that placebo is designed to be inert in randomized controlled trials (RCTs), yet appears to be effective in evaluations of clinical interventions in all fields of medicine and alternative medicine. This article develops the Neuroplasticity Placebo Theory, which posits that neuroplasticity in fronto-limbic areas is the unifying factor in placebo response (seen in RCTs) and placebo effect (seen in clinical interventions) where it is not intended to be inert. Depression is the disorder that has the highest placebo response of any medical condition and has the greatest potential for understanding how placebos work: recent developments in understanding of the pathophysiology of depression suggest that fronto-limbic areas are sensitized in depression which is associated with a particularly strong placebo phenomenon. An innovative linkage is made between diverse areas of the psychology and the translational psychiatry literature to provide supportive evidence for the Neuroplasticity Placebo Theory. This is underpinned by neuro-radiological evidence of fronto-limbic change in the placebo arm of antidepressant trials. If placebo stimulates neuroplasticity in fronto-limbic areas in conditions other than depression - and results in a partially active treatment in other areas of medicine - there are far reaching consequences for the day-to-day use of placebo in clinical practice, the future design of RCTs in all clinical conditions, and existing unwarranted assertions about the efficacy of antidepressant medications. If fronto-limbic neuroplasticity is the common denominator in designating placebo as a partially active treatment, the terms placebo effect and placebo response should be replaced by the single term “placebo treatment.”
Article
Full-text available
Random regression models (RRMs) were used to investigate the role of initial severity in the outcome of 4 treatments (cognitive–behavior therapy [CBT], interpersonal psychotherapy [IPT], imipramine plus clinical management [IMI-CM], and placebo plus clinical management [PLA-CM]) for outpatients with major depressive disorder seen in the National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. Initial severity of depression and impairment of functioning significantly predicted differential treatment effects. A larger number of differences than previously reported were found among the active treatments for the more severely ill patients; this was due, in large part, to the greater power of the present statistical analyses.
Article
Full-text available
This review summarizes the current meta-analysis literature on treatment outcomes of CBT for a wide range of psychiatric disorders. A search of the literature resulted in a total of 16 methodologically rigorous meta-analyses. Our review focuses on effect sizes that contrast outcomes for CBT with outcomes for various control groups for each disorder, which provides an overview of the effectiveness of cognitive therapy as quantified by meta-analysis. Large effect sizes were found for CBT for unipolar depression, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder with or without agoraphobia, social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder, and childhood depressive and anxiety disorders. Effect sizes for CBT of marital distress, anger, childhood somatic disorders, and chronic pain were in the moderate range. CBT was somewhat superior to antidepressants in the treatment of adult depression. CBT was equally effective as behavior therapy in the treatment of adult depression and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Large uncontrolled effect sizes were found for bulimia nervosa and schizophrenia. The 16 meta-analyses we reviewed support the efficacy of CBT for many disorders. While limitations of the meta-analytic approach need to be considered in interpreting the results of this review, our findings are consistent with other review methodologies that also provide support for the efficacy CBT.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this review article is to provide an integrative perspective by combining basic assumptions of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) with neuroscience research results. In recent years, interdisciplinary research in the field of neuroscience has expanded our knowledge about neurobiological correlates of mental processes and changes occurring in the brain due to therapeutic interventions. The studies are largely based on non-invasive brain imaging techniques, such as functional neuroimaging technologies of positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The neuroscientific investigations of basic CBT hypotheses have shown that (i) functional and non-functional behavior and experiences may be learned through lifelong learning, due to brain neuroplasticity that continues across the entire lifespan; (ii) cognitive activity contributes to dysfunctional behavior and emotional experience through focusing, selective perception, memory and recall, and characteristic cognitive distortion; on a neurobiological level, there is a relationship between top-down and bottom-up regulation of unpleasant emotional states; and (iii) cognitive activity may be changed, as shown by therapeutic success achieved by metacognitive and mindfulness techniques, which also have their neurobiological correlates in the changes occurring in the cortical and subcortical structures and endocrine and immune systems. The empirical research also shows that neurobiological changes occur after CBT in patients with arachnophobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, social phobia, major depressive disorder and chronic fatigue syndrome.
Article
Full-text available
Multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA), or 'decoding', of fMRI activity has gained popularity in the neuroimaging community in recent years. MVPA differs from standard fMRI analyses by focusing on whether information relating to specific stimuli is encoded in patterns of activity across multiple voxels. If a stimulus can be predicted, or decoded, solely from the pattern of fMRI activity, it must mean there is information about that stimulus represented in the brain region where the pattern across voxels was identified. This ability to examine the representation of information relating to specific stimuli (e.g., memories) in particular brain areas makes MVPA an especially suitable method for investigating memory representations in brain structures such as the hippocampus. This approach could open up new opportunities to examine hippocampal representations in terms of their content, and how they might change over time, with aging, and pathology. Here we consider published MVPA studies that specifically focused on the hippocampus, and use them to illustrate the kinds of novel questions that can be addressed using MVPA. We then discuss some of the conceptual and methodological challenges that can arise when implementing MVPA in this context. Overall, we hope to highlight the potential utility of MVPA, when appropriately deployed, and provide some initial guidance to those considering MVPA as a means to investigate the hippocampus.
Article
Background The results of one randomised control trial testing a psychological rehabilitation programme aimed at information processing strategies showed improvements in cognition post-treatment.
Article
The last two decades have seen a surge in the interest in research based on Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) data. Decoding of cognitive states based on fMRI activation profiles has become a very active topic in this area. fMRI data is very high dimensional and noisy. However, there is a dearth of datasets to work on. Sharing of learning by analyzing datasets drawn across multiple subjects in an experiment can help in increasing the amount of data we have for analysis. Decoding of cognitive states using classifiers trained across multiple subjects is a challenging task because of differences in anatomy and cognition. Selecting features to analyze from the dataset is a key step in the analysis of fMRI data. In this paper we apply PCA, ICA and five non-linear dimensionality reduction techniques to the fMRI data. The aim of this work is to analyze which technique can provide the best feature selection to capture the commonality across multiple subjects. The reduced datasets are then used to train classifiers to solve a multiple-subject decoding problem.
Article
To what extent do we know our own minds when making decisions? Variants of this question have preoccupied researchers in a wide range of domains, from mainstream experimental psychology (cognition, perception, social behavior) to cognitive neuroscience and behavioral economics. A pervasive view places a heavy explanatory burden on an intelligent cognitive unconscious, with many theories assigning causally effective roles to unconscious influences. This article presents a novel framework for evaluating these claims and reviews evidence from three major bodies of research in which unconscious factors have been studied: multiple-cue judgment, deliberation without attention, and decisions under uncertainty. Studies of priming (subliminal and primes-to-behavior) and the role of awareness in movement and perception (e.g., timing of willed actions, blindsight) are also given brief consideration. The review highlights that inadequate procedures for assessing awareness, failures to consider artifactual explanations of "landmark" results, and a tendency to uncritically accept conclusions that fit with our intuitions have all contributed to unconscious influences being ascribed inflated and erroneous explanatory power in theories of decision making. The review concludes by recommending that future research should focus on tasks in which participants' attention is diverted away from the experimenter's hypothesis, rather than the highly reflective tasks that are currently often employed.
Article
Context Among depressed individuals not receiving medication in controlled trials, 40% to 60% respond to cognitive therapy (CT). Multiple previous studies suggest that activity in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC; Brodmann area 25) predicts outcome in CT for depression, but these results have not been prospectively replicated. Objective To examine whether sgACC activity is a reliable and robust prognostic outcome marker of CT for depression and whether sgACC activity changes in treatment. Design Two inception cohorts underwent assessment with functional magnetic resonance imaging using different scanners on a task sensitive to sustained emotional information processing before and after 16 to 20 sessions of CT, along with a sample of control participants who underwent testing at comparable intervals. Setting A hospital outpatient clinic. Patients Forty-nine unmedicated depressed adults and 35 healthy controls. Main Outcome Measures Pretreatment sgACC activity in an a priori region in response to negative words was correlated with residual severity and used to classify response and remission. Results As expected, in both samples, participants with the lowest pretreatment sustained sgACC reactivity in response to negative words displayed the most improvement after CT (R2 = 0.29, >75% correct classification of response, >70% correct classification of remission). Other a priori regions explained additional variance. Response/remission in cohort 2 was predicted based on thresholds from cohort 1. Subgenual anterior cingulate activity remained low for patients in remission after treatment. Conclusions Neuroimaging provides a quick, valid, and clinically applicable way of assessing neural systems associated with treatment response/remission. Subgenual anterior cingulate activity, in particular, may reflect processes that interfere with treatment (eg, emotion generation) in addition to its putative regulatory role; alternately, its absence may facilitate treatment response.