ChapterPDF Available

An Introduction to Design-Based Research with an Example From Statistics Education

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Reference: Bakker, A., & Van Eerde, H. A. A. (in press). An introduction to design-based research with an example from statistics education. In A. Bikner-Ahsbahs, C. Knipping, & N. Presmeg (Eds.), Doing qualitative research: methodology and methods in mathematics education. New York: Spring-er. Abstract This chapter arose from the need to introduce researchers, including Master and PhD students, to design-based research (DBR). In Part 1 we address key features of DBR and differences from other research ap-proaches. We also describe the meaning of validity and reliability in DBR and discuss how they can be improved. Part 2 illustrates DBR with an ex-ample from statistics education.
Content may be subject to copyright.
An introduction to design-based research with an
example from statistics education
Arthur Bakker (a.bakker4@uu.nl) and Dolly van Eerde
(h.a.a.vaneerde@uu.nl)
Address: Freudenthal Institute for Science and Mathematics Education, Utrecht University,
Princetonplein 5, 3584 CC Utrecht, the Netherlands
17 September 2013
Reference:
Bakker, A., & Van Eerde, H. A. A. (in press). An introduction to design-
based research with an example from statistics education. In A. Bikner-
Ahsbahs, C. Knipping, & N. Presmeg (Eds.), Doing qualitative research:
methodology and methods in mathematics education. New York: Spring-
er.
Abstract
This chapter arose from the need to introduce researchers, including
Master and PhD students, to design-based research (DBR). In Part 1 we
address key features of DBR and differences from other research ap-
proaches. We also describe the meaning of validity and reliability in DBR
and discuss how they can be improved. Part 2 illustrates DBR with an ex-
ample from statistics education.
2
Part 1: Theory of design-based research
1.1 Purpose of the chapter
The purpose of this chapter is to introduce researchers, including Master
and PhD students, to design-based research. In our research methods
courses for this audience and in our supervision of PhD students, we no-
ticed that students considered key publications in this field unsuitable as
introductions. These publications have mostly been written to inform or
convince established researchers who already have considerable experi-
ence with educational research. We therefore see the need to write for
an audience that does not have that level of experience, but may want to
know about design-based research. We do assume a basic knowledge of
the main research approaches (e.g., survey, experiment, case study) and
methods (e.g., interview, questionnaire, observation).
Compared to other research approaches, educational design-
based research (DBR) is relatively new (Anderson & Shattuck, 2012). This
is probably the reason that it is not discussed in most books on qualitative
research approaches. For example, Creswell (2007) distinguishes five
qualitative approaches, but these do not include DBR (see also
Denscombe, 2007). Yet DBR is worth knowing about, especially for stu-
dents who will become teachers or researchers in education: Design-
based research is claimed to have the potential to bridge the gap be-
tween educational practice and theory, because it aims both at develop-
ing theories about domain-specific learning and the means that are de-
signed to support that learning. DBR thus produces both useful products
(e.g., educational materials) and accompanying scientific insights into
how these products can be used in education (McKenney & Reeves, 2012;
Van den Akker, Gravemeijer, McKenney, & Nieveen, 2006). It is also said
to be suitable for addressing complex educational problems that should
be dealt with in a holistic way (Plomp & Nieveen, 2007).
3
In line with the other chapters in this book, Part 1 provides a gen-
eral theory of the research approach under discussion and Part 2 gives an
example from statistics education on how the approach can be used.
1.2 Characterizing design-based research
In this section we outline some characteristics of DBR, compare it with
other research approaches, go over terminology and history, and finally
summarize DBR’s key characteristics.
1.2.1 Integration of design and research
Educational design-based research (DBR) can be characterized as research
in which the design of educational materials (e.g., computer tools, learn-
ing activities, or a professional development program) is a crucial part of
the research. That is, the design of learning environments is interwoven
with the testing or developing of theory. The theoretical yield distin-
guishes DBR from studies that aim solely at designing educational materi-
als through iterative cycles of testing and improving prototypes.
A key characteristic of DBR is that educational ideas for student or
teacher learning are formulated in the design, but can be adjusted during
the empirical testing of these ideas, for example if a design idea does not
quite work as anticipated. In most other interventionist research ap-
proaches design and testing are cleanly separated. See further the com-
parison with a randomized control trial in Section 1.2.5.
1.2.2 Predictive and advisory nature of DBR
To further characterize DBR it is helpful to classify research aims in gen-
eral (cf. Plomp & Nieveen, 2007):
4
To describe (e.g., What conceptions of sampling do 7th-grade students
have?)
To compare (e.g., Does instructional strategy A lead to better test
scores than instructional strategy B?)
To evaluate (e.g., How well do students develop an understanding of
distribution in an instructional sequence?)
To explain or to predict (e.g., Why do so few students choose a bache-
lor in mathematics or science? What will students do when using a par-
ticular software package?)
To advise (e.g., How can secondary school students be supported to
learn about correlation and regression?)
Many research approaches such as surveys, correlational studies, and
case studies, typically have descriptive aims. Experiments often have a
comparative aim, even though they should in Cook’s (2002) view “be de-
signed to explain the consequences of interventions and not just to de-
scribe them” (p. 181, emphasis original). DBR typically has an explanatory
and advisory aim, namely to give theoretical insights into how particular
ways of teaching and learning can be promoted. The type of theory de-
veloped can also be of a predictive nature: Under conditions X using edu-
cational approach Y, students are likely to learn Z (Van den Akker et al.,
2006).
Research projects usually have one overall aim, but several stages
of the project can have other aims. For example, if the main aim of a re-
search project is to advise how a particular topic (e.g., sampling) should
be taught, the project most likely has parts in which phenomena are de-
scribed or evaluated (e.g., students’ prior knowledge, current teaching
practices). It will also have a part in which an innovative learning envi-
ronment has to be designed and evaluated before empirically grounded
advice can be given. This implies that research projects are layered. De-
sign-based research (DBR) has an overall predictive or advisory aim but
often includes research stages with a descriptive, comparative, or evalua-
tive aim.
5
1.2.3 The role of hypotheses and the engineering nature of DBR
In characterizing DBR as different from other research approaches, we al-
so need to address the role of hypotheses in theory development. Put
simply, a scientific theory can explain particular phenomena and predict
what will happen under particular conditions. When developing or testing
a theory, scientists typically use hypotheses—conjectures that follow
from some emergent theory that still needs to be tested empirically. This
means that hypotheses should be formulated in a form in which they can
be verified or falsified. The testing of hypotheses is typically done in an
experiment: Reality is manipulated according to a theory-driven plan. If
hypotheses are confirmed, this is support for the theory under construc-
tion.
Just as in the natural sciences, it is not always possible to test hy-
potheses empirically within a short period of time. As a starting point de-
sign researchers, just like many scientists in other disciplines, use thought
experiments—thinking through the consequences of particular ideas.
When preparing an empirical teaching experiment, design researchers
typically do a thought experiment on how teachers or students will re-
spond to particular tools or tasks based on their practical and theoretical
knowledge of the domain (Freudenthal, 1991).
In empirical experiments, a hypothesis is formulated beforehand.
A theoretical idea is operationalized by designing a particular setting in
which only this particular feature is isolated and manipulated. To stay ob-
jective experimental researchers are often not present during the inter-
ventions. In typical cases, they collect only pre- and posttest scores. In de-
sign-based research, however, researchers continuously take their best
bets (Lehrer & Schauble, 2001), even if this means that some aspect of
the learning environment during or after a lesson has to be changed. In
many examples, researchers are involved in the teaching or work closely
with teachers or trainers to optimize the learning environment (McClain
& Cobb, 2001; Smit & Van Eerde, 2011; Hoyles, Noss, Kent, & Bakker,
2010). In the process of designing and improving educational materials
(which we take as a prototypical case in this chapter), it does not make
6
sense to wait until the end of the teaching experiment before changes
can be made. This would be inefficient.
DBR is therefore sometimes characterized as a form of what
Freudenthal (1978) called didactical engineering: Something has to be
made with whatever theories and resources are available. The products
of DBR are judged on innovativeness and usefulness, not just on the rigor
of the research process that is more prominent in evaluating true experi-
ments (Plomp, 2007).
In many research approaches, changing and understanding a situ-
ation are separated. However, in design-based research these are inter-
twined in line with the following adage that is also common in sociocul-
tural traditions: If you want to understand something you have to change
it, and if you want to change something you have to understand it (Bak-
ker, 2004a, p. 37).
1.2.4 Open and interventionist nature of DBR
Another way to characterize DBR is to contrast it with other approaches
on the following two dimensions: naturalistic vs. interventionist and open
vs. closed. Naturalistic studies analyze how learning takes place without
interference by a researcher. Examples of naturalistic research approach-
es are ethnography and surveys. As the term suggests, interventionist
studies intervene in what naturally happens: Researchers deliberately
manipulate a condition or teach according to particular theoretical ideas
(e.g., inquiry-based or problem-based learning). Such studies are neces-
sary if the type of learning that researchers want to investigate is not pre-
sent in naturalistic settings. Examples of interventionist approaches are
experimental research, action research, and design-based research.
7
Table 1 Naturalistic vs. interventionist and open vs. closed research approaches.
Naturalistic Interventionist
Closed Survey: questionnaires with closed questions Experiment (randomized control trial)
Open Survey: interviews with open questions
Ethnography
Action research
Design-based research
Research approaches can also be more open or closed. The term open
here refers to little control of the situation or data whereas closed refers
to a high degree of control or a limited number of options (e.g., multiple
choice questions). For example, surveys by means of questionnaires with
closed questions or responses on a Likert scale are more closed than sur-
veys by means of semi-structured interviews. Likewise, an experiment
comparing two conditions is more closed than a DBR project in which the
educational materials or ways of teaching are emergent and adjustable.
Different research approaches can thus be positioned in a two-by-two ta-
ble as in Table 1. DBR thus shares an interventionist nature with experi-
ments and action research. We therefore continue by comparing DBR
with experiments (1.2.5) and with action research (1.2.6).
1.2.5 Comparison of DBR and randomized control trials (RCT)
A randomized control trial (RCT) is sometimes referred to as “true” exper-
iment. Assume we want to know whether a new teaching strategy for a
particular topic in a particular grade is better than the traditionally used
one. To investigate this question one could randomly assign students to
the experimental (new teaching strategy) or control condition (traditional
strategy), measure performances on pre- and posttests, and use statistical
methods to test the null hypothesis that there is no significant difference
between the two conditions. The researchers’ hope is that this hypothesis
can be rejected so that the new type of intervention (informed by a par-
ticular theory) proves to be better. The underlying rationale is: If we know
8
“what works” we can implement this method and have better learning re-
sults (see Figure 1).
Fig. 1. A pre-posttest experimental design (randomized control trial)
This so-called experimental approach of randomized control trials (Cre-
swell, 2005) is sometimes considered the highest standard of research
(Slavin, 2002). It has a clear logic and is a convincing way to make causal
and general claims about what works. It is based on a research approach
that has proven extremely helpful in the natural sciences.
However, its limitations for education are discussed extensively in
the literature (Engeström, 2011; Olsen, 2004). Here we mention two re-
lated arguments. First, if we know what works, we still do not know why
and when it works. Even if the new strategy is implemented, it might not
work as expected because teachers use it in less than optimal ways.
An example can clarify this. When doing research in an American
school, we heard teachers complain about their managers’ decision that
every teacher had to start every lesson with a warm-up activity (e.g., a
puzzle). Apparently it had been proven by means of an RCT that student
scores were significantly higher in the experimental condition in which
lessons started with a warm-up activity. The negative effect in teaching
practice, however, was that teachers ran out of good ideas for warm-up
activities, and that these often had nothing to do with the topic of the les-
son. Effectively, teachers therefore lost five minutes of every lesson. Bet-
ter insight into how and why warm-up activities work under particular
9
conditions could have improved the situation, but the comparative nature
of RCT had not provided this information because only the variable of
starting the lesson with or without warm-up activity had been manipulat-
ed.
A second argument why RCT has its limitations is that a new
strategy has to be designed before it can be tested, just like a Boeing air-
plane cannot be compared with an Airbus without a long tradition of en-
gineering and producing such airplanes. In many cases, considerable re-
search is needed to design innovative approaches. Design-based research
emerged as a way to address this need of developing new strategies that
could solve long-standing or complex problems in education.
Two discussion points in the comparison of DBR and RCT are the
issues of generalization and causality. The use of random samples in RCT
allows generalization to populations, but in most educational research
random samples cannot be used. In response to this point, researchers
have argued that theory development is not just about populations, but
rather about propensities and processes (Frick, 1998). Hence rather than
generalizing from a random sample to a population (statistical generaliza-
tion), many (mainly qualitative) research approaches aim for generaliza-
tion to a theory, model or concept (theoretical or analytic generalization)
by presenting findings as particular cases of a more general model or con-
cept (Yin, 2009).
Where the use of RCTs can indicate the intervention or treatment
being the cause of better learning, DBR cannot claim causality with the
same convincing rigor. This is not unique to DBR: All qualitative research
approaches face this challenge of drawing causal claims. In this regard it is
helpful to distinguish two views on causality: a regularity, variance-
oriented understanding of causality versus a realist, process-oriented un-
derstanding of causality (Maxwell, 2004). People adopting the first view
think that causality can only be proven on the basis of regularities in larg-
er data sets. People adopting the second view make it plausible on the
basis of circumstantial evidence of observed processes that what hap-
pened is most likely caused by the intervention (e.g., Nathan & Kim,
10
2009). The first view is underlying the logic of RCT: If we randomly assign
subjects to an experimental and control condition, treat only the experi-
mental group and find a significant difference between the two groups,
then it can only be attributed to the difference in condition (the treat-
ment). However, if we were to adopt the same regularity view on causali-
ty we would never be able to identify the cause of singular events, for ex-
ample why a driver hit a tree. From the second, process-oriented view, if
a drunk driver hits a tree we can judge the circumstances and judge it
plausible that his drunkenness was an important explanation because we
know that alcohol can cause less control, slower reaction time etcetera.
Similarly, explanations for what happens in classrooms should be possible
according to a process-oriented position based on what happens in re-
sponse to particular interventions. For example, particular student utter-
ances are very unlikely if not deliberately fostered by a teacher (Nathan &
Kim, 2009). Table 2 summarizes the main points of the comparison of RCT
and DBR.
Table 2. Comparison of experimental versus design-based research
Experiment (RCT) Design-based research (DBR)
Testing theory
Comparison of existing teaching methods by
means of experimental and control groups
Proof of what works
Research interest is isolated by manipulating
variables separately
Statistical generalization long white word
long
Causal claims based on a regularity view on
causality are possible
Developing and testing theory simultaneously
Design of an innovative learning environment
long
Insight into how and why something works
Holistic approach long white word long white
word
Analytic or theoretical generalization, transfera-
bility to other situations
Causality should be handled with great care and
be based on a realist, process-oriented view on
causality
11
1.2.6 Comparison of DBR with action research
Like action research, DBR typically is interventionist and open, involves a
reflective and often cyclic process, and aims to bridge theory and practice
(Opie, 2004). In both approaches the teacher can be also researcher. In
action research, the researcher is not an observer (Anderson & Shattuck,
2012), whereas in DBR s/he can be observer. Furthermore, in DBR design
is a crucial part of the research, whereas in action research the focus is on
action and change, which can but need not involve the design of a new
learning environment. DBR also more explicitly aims for instructional the-
ories than does action research. These points are summarized in Table 3.
Table 3. Commonalities and differences between DBR and action research
DBR Action research
Commonalities
Open, interventionist, researcher can be participant, reflective cyclic process
Differences Researcher can be observer
Design is necessary
Focus on instructional theory
Researcher can only be participant
Design is possible
Focus on action and improvement of a situa-
tion
1.2.7 Names and history of DBR
In its relatively brief history, DBR has been presented under different
names. Design-based research is the name used by the Design-Based Re-
search Collective (see special issues in Educational Researcher, 2003; Edu-
cational Psychologist, 2004; Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2004). Other
terms for similar approaches are:
developmental or development research (Freudenthal, 1988; Grave-
meijer, 1994; Lijnse, 1995; Romberg, 1973; Van den Akker, 1999)
12
design experiments or design experimentation (Brown, 1992; Cobb,
Confrey, diSessa, Lehrer, & Schauble, 2003; Collins, 1992)
educational design research (Van den Akker et al., 2006)
The reasons for these different terms are mainly historical and rhetorical.
In the 1970s Romberg (1973) used the term development research for re-
search accompanying the development of curriculum. Discussions on the
relation between research and design in mathematics education, espe-
cially on didactics, mainly took place in Western Europe in the 1980s and
the 1990s, particularly in the Netherlands (e.g., Freudenthal, 1988; Gof-
free, 1979), France (e.g., Artigue, 1988) and Germany (e.g., Wittmann,
1992). The term developmental research is a translation of the Dutch on-
twikkelingsonderzoek, which Freudenthal introduced in the 1970s to justi-
fy the development of curricular materials as belonging to a university in-
stitute (what is now called the Freudenthal Institute) because it was
informed by and leading to research on students’ learning processes
(Freudenthal, 1978; Gravemeijer & Koster, 1988; De Jong & Wijers, 1993).
The core idea was that development of learning environments and the
development of theory were intertwined. As Goffree (1979, p. 347) put it:
“Developmental research in education as presented here, shows the
characteristics of both developmental and fundamental research, which
means aiming at new knowledge that can be put into service in continued
development.” At another Dutch university (Twente University), the term
ontwerpgericht (design-oriented) research was more common, but there
the focus was more on the curriculum than on theory development (Van
den Akker, 1999). One disadvantage of the terms ‘development’ and ‘de-
velopmental’ is their connotations to developmental psychology and re-
search on children’s development of concepts. This might be one reason
that this term is hardly used anymore.
In the United States, the terms design experiment and design re-
search were more common (Brown, 1992; Cobb, Confrey, et al., 2003;
Collins, 1992; Edelson, 2002). One advantage of these terms is that design
is more specific than development. One possible disadvantage of the
term design experiment can be explained by reference to a critical paper
by Paas (2005) titled Design experiment: Neither a design nor an experi-
13
ment. The confusion that his pun refers to is two-fold. First, in many edu-
cational research communities the term design is reserved for research
design (e.g., comparing an experimental with a control group), whereas
the term in design research refers to the design of learning environments
(Sandoval & Bell, 2004). Second, for many researchers, also outside the
learning sciences, the term experiment is reserved for “true” experiments
or RCTs. In design experiments, hypotheses certainly play an important
role, but they are not fixed and tested once. Instead they may be emer-
gent, multiple, and temporary. In line with the Design-Based Research
Collective, we use the term design-based research because this suggests
that it is predominantly research (hence leading to a knowledge claim)
that is based on a design process.
1.2.8 Theory development in design-based research
We have already stated that theory typically has a more central role in
DBR than in action research. To address the role of theory in DBR, it is
helpful to summarize diSessa and Cobb’s (2004) categorization of differ-
ent types of theories involved in educational research. They distinguish:
Grand theories (e.g., Piaget’s phases of intellectual development; Skin-
ner’s behaviorism)
Orienting frameworks (e.g., constructivism, semiotics, sociocultural
theories)
Frameworks for action (e.g., designing for learning, Realistic Mathe-
matics Education)
Domain-specific theories (e.g., how to teach density or sampling)
Hypothetical Learning Trajectories (Simon, 1995) or didactical scenarios
(Lijnse, 1995) formulated for specific teaching experiments (explained
in Section 1.3).
As can be seen from this categorization, there is a hierarchy in the gener-
ality of theories. Because theories developed in DBR are typically tied to
specific learning environments and learning goals, they are humble and
hard to generalize. Similarly, it is very rare that a theoretical contribution
to aerodynamics will be made in the design of an airplane; yet innova-
14
tions in airplane design occur regularly. The use of grand theoretical
frameworks and frameworks for action is recommended, but researchers
should be careful to manage the gap between the different types of theo-
ry on the one hand and design on the other (diSessa & Cobb, 2004). If
handled with care, DBR can then provide the basis for refining or develop-
ing theoretical concepts such as meta-representational competence, so-
ciomathematical norms (diSessa & Cobb), or whole-class scaffolding
(Smit, Van Eerde, & Bakker, in press).
1.2.9 Summary of key characteristics of design-based research
So far we have characterized DBR in terms of its predictive and advisory
aim, particular way of handling hypotheses, its engineering nature and
differences from other research methods. Here we summarize five key
characteristics of DBR as identified by Cobb, Confrey, diSessa, Lehrer, and
Schauble (2003):
1. The first characteristic is that its purpose is to develop theories about
learning and the means that are designed to support that learning. In
the example provided in Part 2 of in this chapter, Bakker (2004a) de-
veloped an instruction theory for early statistics education and instruc-
tional means (e.g. computer tools and accompanying learning activi-
ties) that support the learning of a multifaceted notion of statistical
distribution.
2. The second characteristic of DBR is its interventionist nature. One dif-
ference with RCTs is that interventions in the DBR tradition often have
better ecological validity—meaning that learning already takes place in
learning ecologies as they occur in schools and thus methods measure
better what researchers want to measure, that is learning in natural
situations. Findings from experiments do not have to be translated as
much from controlled laboratory situations to the less controlled ecol-
ogy of schools or courses. In technical terms, theoretical products of
DBR “have the potential for rapid pay-off because they are filtered in
advance for instrumental effect” (Cobb, Confrey, et al., 2003, p. 11).
15
3. The third characteristic is that DBR has prospective and reflective com-
ponents that need not be separated by a teaching experiment. In im-
plementing hypothesized learning (the prospective part) the research-
ers confront conjectures with actual learning that they observe
(reflective part). Reflection can be done after each lesson, even if the
teaching experiment is longer than one lesson. Such reflective analysis
can lead to changes to the original plan for the next lesson. Kanselaar
(1993) argued that any good educational research has prospective and
reflective components. As explained before, however, what distin-
guishes DBR from other experimental approaches is that in DBR these
components are not separated into the formulation of hypotheses be-
fore and after a teaching experiment.
4. The fourth characteristic is the cyclic nature of DBR: Invention and revi-
sion form an iterative process. Multiple conjectures on learning are
sometimes refuted and alternative conjectures can be generated and
tested. The cycles typically consist of the following phases: preparation
and design phase, teaching experiment, and retrospective analysis.
These phases are worked out in more detail later in this chapter. The
results of such a retrospective analysis mostly feed a new design phase.
Other types of educational research ideally also build upon prior exper-
iments and researchers iteratively improve materials and theoretical
ideas in between experiments but in DBR changes can take place dur-
ing a teaching experiment or series of teaching experiments.
5. The fifth characteristic of DBR is that the theory under development
has to do real work. As Lewin (1951, p. 169) wrote: “There is nothing so
practical as a good theory.” Theory generated from DBR is typically
humble in the sense that it is developed for a specific domain, for in-
stance statistics education. Yet it must be general enough to be appli-
cable in different contexts such as classrooms in other schools in other
countries. In such cases we can speak of transferability.
16
1.3 Hypothetical learning trajectory (HLT)
DBR typically consists of cycles of three phases each: preparation and de-
sign, teaching experiment, and retrospective analysis. One might argue
that the term ‘retrospective analysis’ is pleonastic: All analysis is in retro-
spect, after a teaching experiment. However, we use it here to distinguish
it from analysis on the fly, which takes place during a teaching experi-
ment, often between lessons.
A design and research instrument that proves useful during all
phases of DBR is the hypothetical learning trajectory (HLT), which we re-
gard as an elaboration of Freudenthal’s thought experiment. Simon
(1995) defined the HLT as follows:
The hypothetical learning trajectory is made up of three components: the learning goal
that defines the direction, the learning activities, and the hypothetical learning process—
a prediction of how the students’ thinking and understanding will evolve in the context
of the learning activities. (p. 136)
Simon used the HLT for one or two lessons. Series of HLTs can be used for
longer sequences of instruction (also see the literature on didactical sce-
narios in Lijnse, 1995). The HLT is a useful research instrument to manage
the gap between an instruction theory and a concrete teaching experi-
ment. It is informed by general domain-specific and conjectured instruc-
tion theories (Gravemeijer, 1994), and it informs researchers and teach-
ers how to carry out a particular teaching experiment. After the teaching
experiment, it guides the retrospective analysis, and the interplay be-
tween the HLT and empirical results forms the basis for theory develop-
ment. This means that an HLT, after it has been mapped out, has different
functions depending on the phase of the DBR and continually develops
through the different phases. It can even change during a teaching exper-
iment.
17
HLT in the design phase
The development of an HLT starts with an analysis of how the mathemati-
cal topic of the design study is elaborated in the curriculum and the
mathematical textbooks, an analysis of the difficulties students encounter
with this topic, and a reflection on what they should learn about it. These
analyses result in the formulation of provisional mathematical learning
goals that form the orientation point for the design and redesign of activi-
ties in several rounds. While designing mathematical activities the learn-
ing goals may become better defined. During these design processes the
researcher also starts formulating hypotheses about students’ potential
learning and about how the teacher would support students’ learning
processes. The confrontation of a general rationale with concrete tasks
often leads to a more specific HLT, which means that the HLT gradually
develops during the design phase (Drijvers, 2003).
An elaborated HLT thus includes mathematical learning goals,
students’ starting points with information on relevant pre-knowledge,
mathematical problems and assumptions about students’ potential learn-
ing processes and about how the teacher could support these processes.
HLT in teaching experiment
During the teaching experiment, the HLT functions as a guideline for the
teacher and researcher for what to focus on in teaching, interviewing, and
observing. It may happen that the teacher or researcher feels the need to
adjust the HLT or instructional activity for the next lesson. As Freudenthal
wrote (1991, p. 159), the cyclic alternation of research and development
can be more efficient the shorter the cycle is. Minor changes in the HLT
are usually made because of incidents in the classroom such as student
strategies that were not foreseen, activities that were too difficult, and so
on. Such adjustments are generally not accepted in comparative experi-
mental research, but in DBR, changes in the HLT are made to create opti-
mal conditions and are regarded as elements of the data corpus. This
18
means that these changes have to be reported well and the information is
stronger when changes are supported by theoretical considerations. The
HLT can thus also change during the teaching experiment phase.
HLT in the retrospective analysis
During the retrospective analysis, the HLT functions as a guideline deter-
mining what the researcher should focus on in the analysis. Because pre-
dictions are made about students’ learning, the researcher can contrast
those conjectures with the observations made during the teaching exper-
iment. Such an analysis of the interplay between the evolving HLT and
empirical observations forms the basis for developing an instruction theo-
ry. After the retrospective analysis, the HLT can be reformulated, often
more drastically than during the teaching experiment, and the new HLT
can guide a subsequent design phase.
An HLT can be seen as a concretization of an evolving domain-
specific instruction theory. Conversely, the instruction theory is informed
by evolving HLTs. For example, if patterns of an HLT stabilize after a few
cycles, these generalized patterns in learning or instruction and the in-
sights of how these patterns are supported by instructional means can
become part of the emerging instruction theory.
Overall, the idea behind developing an HLT is not to design the
perfect instructional sequence, which in our view does not exist, but to
provide empirically grounded results that others can adjust to their local
circumstances. The HLT remains hypothetical because each situation,
each teacher, and each class is different. Yet patterns can be found in
students’ learning that are similar across different teaching experiments.
Those patterns and the insights of how particular educational activities
support students in particular kinds of reasoning can be the basis for a
more general instructional theory of how a particular domain can be
taught. Bakker (2004a), for example, noted that when estimating the
number of elephants in a picture, students typically used one of four
19
strategies, and these four strategies reoccurred in all of the five class-
rooms in which he used the same task. Having observed such a pattern in
strategy use, the design researcher can assume the pattern to be an ele-
ment of the instruction theory.
For some readers, the term ‘trajectory’ might have a linear con-
notation. Although we aim for a certain direction, like the course of a
ship, Bakker’s (2004a) HLTs were non-linear in the sense that he did not
make a linear sequence of activities in advance that he strictly adhered to
(cf. Fosnot & Dolk, 2001). Moreover, two subtrajectories came together
later on in the sequence. In the following sections we give a more de-
tailed description of the three phases of a DBR cycle and discuss relevant
methodological issues. Further details about hypothetical learning
trajectories can be found in a special issue of Mathematical Thinking and
Learning (Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 2004, volume 6, issue 2)
devoted to HLTs.
The term HLT stems from research in which the teacher was a researcher
or a member of the research team (Simon, 1995). However, if the teacher
is not so familiar with the research team’s intentions it may be necessary
to pay extra attention to what the teacher can or should do to realize the
potential of the learning activities. In such cases, the terms hypothetical
teaching and learning trajectory (HTLT) or teaching and learning strategy
(Dierdorp, Bakker, Eijkelhof, & Van Maanen, 2011) may be more appro-
priate.
1.4 Phases in DBR
1.4.1 Phase 1: Preparation and design
It is evident that the relevant present knowledge about a topic should be
studied first. Gravemeijer (1994) characterizes the design researcher as a
20
tinkerer or, in French, a bricoleur, who uses all the material that is at
hand, including theoretical insights and practical experience with teaching
and designing.
In the first design phase, it is recommended to collect and invent
a set of tasks that could be useful and discuss these with colleagues who
are experienced in designing for mathematics education. An important
criterion for selecting a task is its potential role in the HLT towards the
mathematical end goal. Could it possibly lead to types of reasoning that
students could build upon towards that end goal? Would it be challeng-
ing? Would it be a meaningful context for students?
There are several design heuristics, principles, and guidelines. In
Part 2 we explain heuristics from the theory of Realistic Mathematics Ed-
ucation.
1.4.2 Phase 2: Teaching experiment
The notion of a teaching experiment arose in the 1970s. Its primary pur-
pose was to experience students’ learning and reasoning first-hand, and it
thus served the purpose of eliminating the separation between the prac-
tice of research and the practice of teaching (Steffe & Thompson, 2000).
Over time, teaching experiments proved useful for a broader purpose,
namely as part of DBR. During a teaching experiment, researchers and
teachers use activities and types of instruction that according to the HLT
seem most appropriate at that moment. Observations in one lesson and
theoretical arguments from multiple sources can influence what is done
in the next lesson. Observations may include student or teacher devia-
tions from the HLT.
Hence, this type of research is different from experimental re-
search designs in which a limited number of variables are manipulated
and effects on other variables are measured. The situation investigated
here, the learning of students in a new context with new tools and new
end goals, is too complicated for such a set-up. Besides that, a different
21
type of knowledge is looked for, as pointed out earlier in this chapter: We
do not want to assess innovative material or a theory, but we need proto-
typical educational materials that could be tested and revised by teachers
and researchers, and a domain-specific instruction theory that can be
used by others to formulate their own HLTs suiting local contingencies.
During a teaching experiment, data collection typically includes
student work, tests before and after instruction, field notes, audio record-
ings of whole-class discussions , and video recordings of every lesson and
of the final interviews with students and teachers. We further find ‘mini-
interviews’ with students, lasting from about twenty seconds to four
minutes, very useful provided that they are carried out systematically
(Bakker, 2004a).
1.4.3 Retrospective analysis
We describe two types of analysis useful in DBR, a task oriented analysis
and a more overall, longitudinal, cyclic approach. The first is to compare
data on students’ actual learning during the different tasks with the HLT.
To this end we find the data analysis matrix (Table 4) described in
Dierdorp et al. (2011) useful. The left part of the matrix summarizes the
HLT and the right part is filled with excerpts from relevant transcripts,
clarifying notes from the researcher as well as a quantitative impression
of how well the match was between the assumed leaning as formulated
in the HLT and the observed learning. With such analysis it is possible to
give an overview, as in Table 5, which can help to identify problematic
sections in the educational materials. Insights into why particular learning
takes place or does not take place help to improve the HLTs in subse-
quent cycles of DBR. This iterative process allows the researcher to im-
prove the predictive power of HLTs across subsequent teaching experi-
ments.
22
Table 4. Data analysis matrix for comparing HLT and actual learning trajectory (ALT).
Hypothetical Learning Trajectory Actual Learning Trajectory
Task
number
Formulation
of the task
Conjecture of
how students
would respond
Transcript
excerpt
Clarification Match between HLT and
ALT: Quantitative impres-
sion of how well the con-
jecture and actual learning
matched (e.g., -, 0, +)
Table 5. ALT Result Compared with HLT Conjectures for the Tasks Involving a particular type
of reasoning.
+ x x x x x x x x x x x x x
± x x x
- x x x
Task:
5d 5f 6a 6c 7 8 9c 9e 10b
11c 15 17 23b
23c 24a
24c 25d
34a 42
Note: a x means how well the conjecture accompanying that task matched the observed
learning (- refers to confirmation for up to 1/3 of the students, and + to at least 2/3 of the
students)
An elaborated HLT would include assumptions about students’ potential
learning and about how the teacher would support students’ learning
processes. In this task-oriented analysis above no information is included
about the role of the teacher. If there are crucial differences between
students’ assumed and observed learning processes or if the teaching has
been observed to diverge radically form what the researcher had intend-
ed, the role of the teacher should be included into the analysis in search
of explanations for these discrepancies.
A comparison of HLTs and observed learning is very useful in the
redesign process, and allows answers to research questions that ask how
particular learning goals could be reached. However, in our experience
additional analyses are often needed to gain more theoretical insights in-
to learning process. An example of such additional analysis is a method
inspired by the constant comparative method (Glaser & Strauss, 1967;
23
Strauss & Corbin, 1998) and Cobb and Whitenack’s (1996) method of lon-
gitudinal analyses. Bakker (2004a) used this type of analysis in his study in
the following way. First, all transcripts were read and the videotapes were
watched chronologically episode-by-episode. With the HLT and research
questions as guidelines, conjectures about students’ learning and views
were generated and documented, and then tested against the other epi-
sodes and other data material (student work, field notes, tests). This test-
ing meant looking for confirmation and counter-examples. The process of
conjecture generating and testing was repeated. Seemingly crucial epi-
sodes were discussed with colleagues to test whether they agreed with
our interpretation or perhaps could think of alternative interpretations.
This process is called peer examination.
For the analysis of transcripts or videos it is worth considering
computer software such as Atlas.ti (Van Nes & Doorman, 2010) for coding
the transcripts and other data sources. As in all qualitative research, data
triangulation (Denscombe, 2007) is commonly used in design-based re-
search.
1.5 Validity and reliability
Researchers want to analyze data in a reliable way and draw conclusions
that are valid. Therefore, validity and reliability are important concerns. In
brief, validity concerns whether we really measure what we intend to
measure. Reliability is about independence of the researcher. A brief ex-
ample may clarify the distinction. Assume a researcher wants to measure
students’ mathematical ability. He gives everyone 7 out of 10. Is this a val-
id way of measuring? Is this a reliable way?
It is a very reliable way because the instruction “give all students
a 7” can be reliably carried out, independently of the researcher. Howev-
er, it is not valid, because there is most likely variation between students’
mathematical ability, which is not taken into account with this way of
measuring.
24
We should emphasize that validity and reliability are complex
concepts with multiple meanings in different types of research. In qualita-
tive research the meanings of validity and reliability are slightly different
than in quantitative research. Moreover, there are so many types of valid-
ity and reliability that we cannot address them all. In this chapter we have
focused on those types that seemed most relevant to us in the context of
DBR. The issues discussed in this section are inspired by guidelines of
Maso and Smaling (1998) and Miles and Huberman (1994), who distin-
guish between internal and external validity and reliability.
1.5.1 Internal validity
Internal validity refers to the quality of the data and the soundness of the
reasoning that has led to the conclusions. In qualitative research, this
soundness is also labeled as credibility (Guba, 1981). In DBR, several tech-
niques can be used to improve the internal validity of a study.
During the retrospective analysis conjectures generated and tested for
specific episodes are tested for other episodes or by data triangulation
with other data material, such as field notes, tests, and other student
work. During this testing stage there is a search for counterexamples to
the conjectures.
The succession of different teaching experiments makes it possible to
test the conjectures developed in earlier experiments in later experi-
ments.
Theoretical claims are substantiated where possible with transcripts to
provide a rich and meaningful context. Reports about DBR tend to be long
due to the thick descriptions (Geertz, 1973) required. For example, the
paper by Cobb, McClain, and Gravemeijer (2003) is 78 pages long!
1.5.2 External validity
25
External validity is mostly interpreted as the generalizability of the results.
The question is how we can generalize the results from these specific con-
texts to be useful for other contexts. An important way to do so is by
framing issues as instances of something more general (Cobb, Confrey, et
al., 2003; Gravemeijer & Cobb, 2006). The challenge is to present the re-
sults (instruction theory, HLT, educational activities) in such a way that
others can adjust them to their local contingencies.
In addition to generalizability as a criterion for external validity we
mention transferability (Maso & Smaling, 1998). If lessons learned in one
experiment are successfully applied in other experiments, this is a sign of
successful generalization. At the end of Part 2 we give an example of how
a new type of learning activity was successfully enacted in a new research
project in another country.
1.5.3 Internal reliability
Internal reliability refers to the degree of how independently of the re-
searcher the data are collected and analyzed. It can be improved with
several methods. Data collection by objective devices such as audio- and
video registrations contribute to the internal reliability. During his retro-
spective analysis Bakker (2004a) ensured reliability by discussing the criti-
cal episodes, including those discussed in Part 2, with colleagues for peer
examination. For measuring interrater reliability, the agreement among
independent researchers, it is advised to calculate not only the percent-
age of agreement but also use Cohen’s kappa or another measure that
takes into account the probability of agreement by chance (e.g., Krippen-
dorff’s alpha). It is not necessary for a second coder to code all episodes,
but ensure that a random sample should be of sufficient size: The larger
the number of possible codes, the larger the sample required (Bakkenes,
Vermunt, & Wubbels, 2010; Cicchetti, 1976). Note that the term internal
reliability can also refer to the consistency of responses on a question-
naire or test, often measured with help of Cronbach’s alpha.
26
1.5.4 External reliability
External reliability usually denotes replicability, meaning that the conclu-
sions of the study should depend on the subjects and conditions, and not
on the researcher. In qualitative research, replicability is mostly interpret-
ed as virtual replicability. The research must be documented in such a
way that it is clear how the research has been carried out and how con-
clusions have been drawn from the data. A criterion for virtual replicabil-
ity is ‘trackability’ (Gravemeijer & Cobb, 2006), ‘traceability’ (Maso &
Smaling, 1998), or transparency (Akkerman, Admiraal, Brekelmans, &
Oost, 2008). This means that the reader must be able to track or trace the
learning process of the researchers and to reconstruct their study: failures
and successes, procedures followed, the conceptual framework used, and
the reasons for certain choices must all be reported. In Freudenthal’s
words:
Developmental research means: experiencing the cyclic process of development and
research so consciously, and reporting on it so candidly that it justifies itself, and that
this experience can be transmitted to others to become like their own experience. (1991,
p. 161)
We illustrate the general characterization and description of DBR of Part 1
by an example of a design study on statistics education in Part 2.
27
Part 2 Example of design-based research
In this second part we illustrate the theory of design-based research
(DBR) as outlined in Part 1 with an example from Bakker’s (2004a) PhD
thesis on DBR in statistics education. We briefly describe the aim and the-
oretical background of this DBR project and then focus on one design
idea, that of growing samples, to illustrate how it is related to different
layers of theory and how it was analyzed. Finally we discuss the issue of
generalizability. In the appendix we provide a structure of DBR project
with examples from this Part 2.
2.1 Relevance and aim
The background problem addressed in Bakker’s (2004a) research on sta-
tistics education was that many stakeholders were dissatisfied with what
and how students learned about statistics. For example, in many curricula
there was a focus on computing arithmetic means and making bar charts
(Friel, Curcio, & Bright, 2001). Moreover, there was very little knowledge
about how to use innovative educational statistics software (cf. Biehler,
Ben-Zvi, Bakker, & Makar, 2012, for an historical overview).
To solve these practical problems, Bakker’s (2004a) aim was to
contribute to an empirically grounded instruction theory for early statis-
tics education with new computer tools for the age group from 11 to 14.
Such a theory should specify patterns in students’ learning as well as the
means supporting that learning in the domain of statistics education. Like
Cobb, McClain, and Gravemeijer (2003), Bakker (2004a) focused his re-
search on the concept of distribution as a key concept in statistics. One
problem is that students tend to see isolated data points instead of a data
set as a whole (Bakker & Gravemeijer, 2004; Konold & Higgins, 2003). Yet
statistics is about features of data sets, in particular distributions of sam-
ples. The selected learning goal was therefore that distribution had to be-
28
come an object-like entity with which students could see data sets as an
entity with characteristics.
2.2 Research question
Bakker’s initial research question was: How can students with little statis-
tical background develop a notion of distribution? In trying to answer this
question in grade 7, however, Bakker came to include a focus on other
statistical key concept such as data, center, and sampling because these
are so intricately connected to that of distribution (Bakker & Derry, 2011).
The concept of distribution also proved hard for seventh-grade students.
The initial research question was therefore reformulated for grade 8 as
follows: How can we promote coherent reasoning about distribution in
relation to data, variability, and sampling in a way that is meaningful for
students with little statistical background?
Our point here is that research questions can change during a re-
search project. Indeed, the better and sharper your research question is
in the beginning of the project, the better and more focused your data
collection will be. However, our experience is that most DBR researchers,
due to progressive insight, end up with slightly different research ques-
tions than they started with.
As pointed out in Part 1, DBR typically draws on several types of
theories. Given the importance of graphical representations in statistics
education, it made sense for Bakker to draw on semiotics as an orienting
framework. He came to focus on Peirce’s semiotics, in particular his ideas
on diagrammatic reasoning. The domain-specific theory of Realistic
Mathematics Education proved a useful framework for action in the de-
sign process even though it had hardly been applied in statistics educa-
tion.
29
2.3 Orienting framework: diagrammatic reasoning
The learning goal was that distribution would become an object-like enti-
ty. Theories on reification of concepts (Sfard & Linchevski, 1992) and the
relation between process and concept (cf. Tall, Thomas, Davis, Gray, &
Simpson, 2000, on procept) were drawn upon. One theoretical question
unanswered in the literature was what the process nature of a distribu-
tion could be. It is impossible to make sense of graphs without having ap-
propriate conceptual structures, and it is impossible to communicate
about concepts without any representations. Thus, to develop an instruc-
tion theory it is necessary to investigate the relation between the devel-
opment of meaning of graphs and concepts. After studying several theo-
ries in this area, Bakker deployed Peirce’s semiotic theory on
diagrammatic reasoning (Bakker, 2007; Bakker & Hoffmann, 2005). For
Peirce, a diagram is a sign that is meant to represent relations. Diagram-
matic reasoning involves three steps:
1. The first step is to construct a diagram (or diagrams) by means of a rep-
resentational system such as Euclidean geometry, but we can also
think of diagrams in computer software or of an informal student
sketch of statistical distribution. Such a construction of diagrams is
supported by the need to represent the relations that students consid-
er significant in a problem. This first step may be called diagrammati-
zation.
2. The second step of diagrammatic reasoning is to experiment with the
diagram (or diagrams). Any experimenting with a diagram is executed
within a not necessarily perfect representational system and is a rule or
habit-driven activity. Contemporary researchers would stress that this
activity is situated within a practice. What makes experimenting with
diagrams important is the rationality immanent in them (Hoffmann,
2002). The rules define the possible transformations and actions, but
also the constraints of operations on diagrams. Statistical diagrams
such as dot plots are also bound by certain rules: a dot has to be put
above its value on the x axis and this remains true even if for instance
30
the scale is changed. Peirce stresses the importance of doing some-
thing when thinking or reasoning with diagrams:
Thinking in general terms is not enough. It is necessary that something should be DONE.
In geometry, subsidiary lines are drawn. In algebra, permissible transformations are
made. Thereupon the faculty of observation is called into play. (CP 4.233—CP refers to
Peirce’s collected papers, volume 4, section 233)
In the software used in this research, students can do something with
the data points such as organizing them into equal intervals or four
equal groups.
3. The third step is to observe the results of experimenting. We refer to
this as the reflection step. As Peirce wrote, the mathematician observ-
ing a diagram “puts before him an icon by the observation of which he
detects relations between the parts of the diagram other than those
which were used in its construction” (Peirce, NEM III, p. 749). In this
way he can “discover unnoticed and hidden relations among the parts”
(Peirce, CP 3.363; see also CP 1.383). The power of diagrammatic rea-
soning is that “we are continually bumping up against hard fact. We
expected one thing, or passively took it for granted, and had the image
of it in our minds, but experience forces that idea into the background,
and compels us to think quite differently” (Peirce, CP 1.324).
Diagrammatic reasoning, in particular the reflection step, is what
can introduce the ‘new’. New implications within a given representational
system can be found, but possibly the need is felt to construct a new dia-
gram that better serves its purpose.
2.4 Domain-specific framework for action: Realistic Mathematics
Education (RME)
As pointed out by diSessa and Cobb (2004), grand theories and orienting
frameworks do not tell the design researcher how to design learning envi-
ronments. For this purpose, frameworks for action can be useful. Here we
discuss Realistic Mathematics Education (RME).
31
Our research took place in the tradition of RME as developed over the last
40 years at the Freudenthal Institute (Freudenthal, 1991; Gravemeijer,
1994; Treffers, 1987; van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 1996). RME is a theory
of mathematics education that offers a pedagogical and didactical philos-
ophy on mathematical learning and teaching as well as on designing edu-
cational materials for mathematics education. RME emerged from re-
search and development in mathematics education in the Netherlands in
the 1970s and it has since been used and extended, also in other coun-
tries.
The central principle of RME is that mathematics should always
be meaningful to students. For Freudenthal, mathematics was an exten-
sion of common sense, a system of concepts and techniques that human
beings had developed in response to phenomena they encountered. For
this reason, he advised a so-called historical phenomenology of concepts
to be taught, a study of how concepts had been developed in relation to
particular phenomena. The insights from such a study can be input for the
design process (Bakker & Gravemeijer, 2006).
The term ‘realistic’ stresses that problem situations should be
‘experientially real’ for students (Cobb, Yackel, & Wood, 1992). This does
not necessarily mean that the problem situations are always encountered
in daily life. Students can experience an abstract mathematical problem
as real when the mathematics of that problem is meaningful to them.
Freudenthal’s (1991) ideal was that mathematical learning should be an
enhancement of common sense. Students should be allowed and encour-
aged to invent their own strategies and ideas, and they should learn
mathematics on their own authority. At the same time, this process
should lead to particular end goals. This process is called guided reinven-
tion—one of the design heuristics of RME. This heuristic points to the
question that underlies much of the RME-based research, namely that of
how to support this process of engaging students in meaningful mathe-
matical and statistical problem solving, and using students’ contributions
to reach certain end goals.
32
The theory of RME is especially tailored to mathematics educa-
tion, because it includes specific tenets on and design heuristics for math-
ematics education. For a description of these tenets we refer to Treffers
(1987) and for the design heuristics to Gravemeijer (1994) or Bakker and
Gravemeijer (2006).
2.5 Methods
The absence of the type of learning aimed for is a common reason to car-
ry out design research. For Bakker’s study in statistics education, descrip-
tive, comparative, or evaluative research did not make sense because the
type of learning aimed for could not be readily observed in classrooms.
Considerable design and research effort first had to be taken to foster
specific innovative types of learning. Bakker therefore had to design HLTs
with accompanying educational materials that supported the desired type
of learning about distribution. Design-based research offers a systematic
approach to doing that while simultaneously developing domain-specific
theories about how to support such learning for example here on the
domain of statistics. In general, DBR researchers first need to create the
conditions in which they can develop and test an instruction theory, but
to create those conditions they also need research.
Teaching experiment. Bakker designed educational materials with
accompanying HLTs in several cycles. Here we focus on the last cycle, in-
volving a teaching experiment in grade 8. Half of the lessons were carried
out in a computer lab and as part of them students used two minitools
(Cobb, Gravemeijer, Bowers, & McClain, 1997), simple Java applets with
which they analyzed data sets on, for instance, battery life span, car
colours, and salaries (Figure 3). The researcher was responsible for the
educational materials and the teacher was responsible for the teaching,
though we discussed in advance on a weekly basis both the materials and
appropriate teaching style. Three preservice teachers served as assistants
33
and helped with videotaping and interviewing students and with
analyzing the data.
In the example that we elaborate we focus on the fourth of a
series of ten lessons, each 50 minutes long. In this specific lesson,
students reasoned about larger and larger samples and about the shape
of distributions.
Subjects. The teaching experiment was carried out in an eighth-
grade class with 30 students in a state school in the center of a Dutch city.
The students in this study were being prepared for pre-university (vwo) or
higher professional education (havo). The students in the class reported
on here were not used to whole-class discussions, but rather to be “taken
by the hand” as the teacher called it; they were characterized by the
three research assistants as “passive but willing to coorporate.” These
students had no prior instruction in statistics; they were acquainted with
bar and line graphs, but not with dot plots, histograms, or box plots.
Students already knew the mean from calculating their report grades, but
mode and median were not introduced until the second half of the
educational sequence after variability, data, sampling, and shape had
been topics of discussion.
Data collection. The collected data on which the results presented
in this chapter are based include student work, field notes, and the audio
and video recordings of class activities that the three assistants and
researcher made in the classroom. An essential part of the data corpus
was the set of mini-interviews we held during the lessons; they varied
from about twenty seconds to four minutes, and were meant to find out
what concepts and graphs meant for students, or how the minitools were
used. These mini-interviews influenced students' learning because they
often stimulated reflection. However, we think that the validity of the
research was not put in danger by this, since the aim was to find out how
students learned to reason with shape or distribution, not whether
teaching the sequence in other eighth-grade classes would lead to the
same results in the same number of lessons. Furthermore, the interview
34
questions were planned in advance as part of the HLT, and discussed with
the assistants.
Retrospective analysis. In this example we do not illustrate how
HLTs can be compared with observed learning (see Dierdorp et al., 2011).
Here we highlight one type of analysis that in Bakker’s case yielded more
theoretical insights: a method resembling Glaser and Strauss’s constant
comparative method (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). For the analysis, Bakker
watched the videotapes, read the transcripts, and formulated conjectures
on students’ learning on the basis of transcript episodes. Numbering the
conjectures served as useful codes to work with during the analysis.
Examples of such codes and conjectures were:
C1. Students divide imaginary data sets into three groups of low,
‘average’, and high values.
C2. Students either characterize spread as range or look very lo-
cally at spread
C3. Students are inclined to think of small samples when first
asked about how one could test something (batteries, weight).
C5. What-if questions work well for letting students think of ag-
gregate features of a graph or a situation. What would a weight
graph of older students look like? What would the graph look like
if a larger sample was taken? What would a larger sample of a
good battery brand look like?
C7. Students’ notions of spread, distribution, and density are not
yet distinguished. When explaining how data are spread out, they
often describe the distribution or the density in some area.
C9. Even when students see a large sample of a particular distri-
bution, they often do not see the shape we see in it.
The generated conjectures were tested against other episodes and the
rest of the collected data (student work, field observations, and tests) in
the next round of analysis by data triangulation. Conjectures that were
35
confirmed remained in the list; conjectures that were refuted were
removed from the list. Then the whole generating and testing process
was repeated. The aforementioned examples were all confirmed
throughout this analysis.
To get a sense of the interrater reliability of the analysis, about one
quarter of the episodes including those discussed in this chapter and the
conjectures belonging to these episodes were judged by the three
assistants who attended the teaching experiment. The amount of
agreement among judges was very high: all four judges agreed about 33
out of 35 codes. A code was only accepted if all judges agreed after
discussion. We give an example of a code that was finally rejected and
one that was accepted. This example stems from the seventh lesson in
which two students used the four equal groups option in Minitool 2 for a
revised version of the jeans activity. Their task was to advise a jeans fac-
tory about frequencies of jeans sizes to be produced.
Fig. 2. Jeans data with four equal groups option in Minitool 2.
Sofie: Because then you can best see the spread, how it is distributed.
Int.: How it is distributed. And how do you see that here [in this graph]?
What do you look at then? (...)
Sofie: Well, you can see that, for example, if you put a [vertical] line here,
here a line, and here a line. Then you see here [two lines at the right]
that there is a very large spread in that part, so to speak.
In the first line, Sofie seems to use the terms spread and distributed as
almost synonymous. This line was therefore coded with C7, which states
36
that “students’ notions of spread, distribution, and density are not yet
distinguished. When explaining how data are spread out, they often de-
scribe the distribution or the density in some area.” In the second line, So-
fie appears to look at spread very locally, hence it was coded with C2,
which states that “students either characterize spread as range or look
very locally at spread.”
We also give an example of a code assignment that was dismissed
in relation to the same diagram.
Int.: What does this tell you? Four equal groups?
Melle: Well, I think that most jeans are between 32 and 34 [inches].
We had originally assigned the code C1 to the this episode (students talk
about data sets as consisting of three groups of low, ‘average’, and high
values), because “most jeans are between 32 and 34” implies that below
32 and above 34 the frequencies are relatively low. In the episode, how-
ever, this student did not talk about three groups of low, average, and
high values or anything equivalent. We therefore removed the code from
this episode.
2.6 HLT and retrospective analysis
To illustrate relationships between theory, method, and results, this sec-
tion presents the analysis of students’ reasoning during one educational
activity which was carried out in the fourth lesson. Its goal was to stimu-
late students to reason about larger and larger samples. We summarize
the HLT of that lesson: the learning goal, the activity of growing a sample
and the assumptions about students’ potential learning processes and
about how the teacher could support these processes. We then present
the retrospective analysis of three successive phases in growing a sample.
The overall goal of the growing samples activity as formulated in
the hypothetical learning trajectory for this fourth lesson was to stimulate
37
students’ diagrammatic reasononing about shape in relation to sampling
and distribution aspects in the context of weight. This implied that
students should first make diagrams, then experiment with them and
reflect on them. The idea was to start with ideas invented by the students
and guide them toward more conventional notions and representations.
This process of guiding students toward these culturally accepted
concepts and graphs while building on their own inventions is called
guided reinvention. We had noted in previous teaching experiments that
students were inclined to choose very small samples initially. It proved
necessary to stimulate reflection on the disadvantages of such small
samples and have them predict what larger samples would look like. Such
insights from the analyses of previous teaching experiments helped to
better formulate the HLT of a new teaching experiment. More
particularly, Bakker assumed that starting with students’ initial ideas
about small samples and asking for predictions about larger samples
would make students aware of various features of distributions.
The activity of growing a sample consisted of three phases of
making sketches of a hypothetical situation and comparing those sketches
with graphs displaying real data sets. In the first phase students had to
make a graph of their own choice of a predicted weight data set with
sample size 10. The results were discussed by the teacher to challenge
this small sample size, and in the subsequent phases students had to
predict larger data sets,one class and three classes in the second phase,
and all students in the province in the third phase.. Thus, three such
phases took place as described and analyzed below. Aiming for guided
reinvention, the teacher and researcher tried to strike a balance between
engaging students in statistical reasoning and allowing their own
terminology on the one hand, and guiding them in using conventional and
more precise notions and graphical representations on the other. Figure
3b is the result of focusing only on the endpoints of the value bars in
Figure 3a. Figure 3c is the result of these endpoints falling down vertically
on the x-axis. In this way, students can learn to understand the
relationship between value-bar graphs and dot plots, and what
38
distribution features in different representations look like (Bakker &
Hoffmann, 2005).
Fig 3a
Fig. 3b
39
Fig. 3c
Fig. 3. a) Minitool 1 showing a value-bar graph of battery life spans in hours of two brands.
b) Minitool 1, but with bars hidden. c) Minitool 2 showing a dot plot of the same data sets.
2.6.1 Analysis of the first phase of growing a sample
The text of the student activity sheet for the fourth lesson contained a
number of tasks that we cite in the following subsections. The sheet
started as follows:
Last week you made graphs of predicted data for a balloon pilot.
During this lesson you will get to see real weight data of students
from another school. We are going to investigate the influence of
the sample size on the shape of the graph.
Task a) Predict a graph of ten data values, for example with the dots
of minitool 2.
The sample size of ten was chosen because the students had found that
size reasonable after the first lesson in the context of testing the life span
of batteries. Figure 4a shows examples for three different types of
diagrams the students made to show their predictions: there were three
value-bar graphs (such as in minitool 1—e.g., Ruud’s diagram), eight with
40
only the endpoints (such as with the option of minitool 1 to “hide bars”—
e.g., Chris’s diagram) and the remaining nineteen plots were dot plots
(such as in minitool 2—e.g., Sandra’s diagram). For the remainder of this
section, the figures and written explanations of these three students are
demonstrated, because their work gives an impression of the variety of
the whole class. Those three students were chosen because their
diagrams represent all types of diagrams made in this class, also for other
phases of growing a sample.
Chris
Ruud
41
Fig. 4a. Student predictions (Ruud, Chris, and Sandra) for ten data points (weight in kg) (Bakker,
2004a, p. 219).
Fig. 4b. Three real data sets in minitool 2 (Bakker, 2004a, p. 219)
To stimulate the reflection on the graphs, the teacher showed three
samples of ten data points on the blackboard and students had to
compare their own graphs (Figure 4a) with the graphs of the real data
sets (Figure 4b).
Task b) You get to see three different samples of size 10. Are they
different from your own prediction? Describe the differences.
The reason for showing three small samples was to show the variation
among these samples. There were no clear indications, though, that
students conceived this variation as a sign that the sample size was too
small for drawing conclusions, but they generally agreed that larger
samples were more reliable. The point relevant to the analysis is that
students started using predicates to describe aggregate features of the
graphs. The written answers of the three students were the following:
Ruud: Mine looks very much like what is on the blackboard.
42
Chris: The middle-most [diagram on the blackboard] best resembles mine
because the weights are close together and that is also the case in my
graph. It lies between 35 and 75 [kg].
Sandra: The other [real data] are more weights together and mine are further
apart.
Ruud’s answer is not very specific, like most of the written answers in the
first phase of growing samples. Chris used the predicate “close together”
and added numbers to indicate the range, probably as an indication of
spread. Sandra used such terms as “together” and “further apart,” which
address spread. The students in the class used common predicates such
as “together,” “spread out” and “further apart” to describe features of
the data set or the graph. For the analysis it is important to note that the
students used predicates (together, apart) and no nouns (spread,
average) in this first phase of growing samples. Spread can only become
an object-like concept, something that can be talked about and reasoned
with, if it is a noun. In the semiotic theory of Peirce, such transitions from
the predicate “the dots are spread out” to “the spread is large” are
important steps in the formation of concepts (see Bakker & Derry, 2011,
for our view on concept formation).
2.6.2 Analysis of the second phase of growing a sample
The students generally understood that larger samples would be more
reliable. With the feedback students had received after discussing the
samples of ten data points in dot plots, students had to predict the weight
graph of a whole class of 27 students and of three classes with 67
students (27 and 67 were the sample sizes of the real data sets of eighth
graders of another school).
Task c. We will now have a look how the graph changes with larger
samples. Predict a sample of 27 students (one class) and of 67
students (three classes).
Task d. You now get to see real samples of those sizes. Describe the
differences. You can use words such as majority, outliers, spread,
average.
43
During this second phase, all of the students made dot plots, probably
because the teacher had shown dot plots on the blackboard, and because
dot plots are less laborious to draw than value bars (only one student
started with a value-bar graph for the sample of 27, but switched to a dot
plot for the sample of 67). The hint on statistical terms was added to
make sure that students’ answers would not be too superficial as often
happened before) and to stimulate them to use such notions in their
reasoning. It was also important for the research to know what these
terms meant to them. When the teacher showed the two graphs with real
data, once again there was a short class discussion in which the teacher
capitalized on the question of why most student predictions now looked
pretty much like what was on the blackboard, whereas with the earlier
predictions there was much more variation. No student had a reasonable
explanation, which indicates that this was an advanced question. The
figures of the same three students are presented in Figure 5 and their
written explanations were:
Ruud: My spread is different.
Chris: Mine resembles the sample, but I have more people around a certain
weight and I do not really have outliers, because I have 10 about the 70
and 80 and the real sample has only 6 around the 70 and 80.
Sandra: With the 27 there are outliers and there is spread; with the 67 there are
more together and more around the average.
Ruud
44
Chris
Sandra
Fig. 5a. Predicted graphs for one class (n = 27, top plot) and three classes (n = 67, bottom plot)
by Ruud, Chris, and Sandra (Bakker, 2004a, p. 222)
45
Fig. 5b. Real data sets of size 27 and 67 of students from another school (Bakker, 2004a, p.
222).
Here, Ruud addressed the issue of spread (“my spread is different”). Chris
was more explicit about a particular area in her graph, the category of
high values. She also correctly used the term “sample,” which was newly
introduced in the second lesson. Sandra used the term “outliers” at this
stage, by which students meant “extreme values,” which did not
necessarily mean exceptional or suspect values. She also seemed to
locate the average somewhere and to understand that many students are
about average. These examples illustrate that students used statistical
notions for describing properties of the data and diagrams.
In contrast to the first phase of growing a sample, students used
nouns instead of just predicates for comparing the diagrams. Like others
Ruud used the noun “spread” (“my spread is different”) whereas students
earlier used only predicates such as “spread out” or “further apart” (e.g.,
Sandra). Of course, this does not always imply that if students use these
nouns that they are thinking of the right concept. Statistically, however, it
makes a difference whether we say, “the dots are spread out” or “the
spread is large.” In the latter case, spread is an object-like entity that can
have particular aggregate characteristics that can be measured, for
instance by the range, the interquartile range, or the standard deviation.
Other notions such as outliers, sample, and average, are now used as
nouns, that is as conceptual objects that can be talked about and
reasoned with.
2.6.3 Analysis of the third phase of growing a sample
The aim of the hypothetical learning trajectory was that students would
come to draw continuous shapes and reason about them using statistical
terms. During teaching experiments in the seventh-grade experiments
(Bakker & Gravemeijer, 2004), reasoning with continuous shapes turned
46
out to be difficult to accomplish, even if it was asked for. It often seemed
impossible to nudge students toward drawing the general, continuous
shape of data sets represented in dot plots. At best, students drew spiky
lines just above the dots. This underlines that students have to construct
something new (a notion of signal, shape, or distribution) with which they
can look differently at the data or the variable phenomenon.
In this last phase of growing the sample, the task was to make a
graph showing data of all students in the city, not necessarily with dots.
The intention of asking this was to stimulate students to use continuous
shapes and dynamically relate samples to populations, without making
this distinction between sample and population explicit yet. The
conjecture was that this transition from a discrete plurality of data values
to a continuous entity of a distribution is important to foster a notion of
distribution as an object-like entity with which students could model data
and describe aggregate properties of data sets. The task proceeded as
follows:
Task e. Make a weight graph of a sample of all eighth graders in the city. You need not
draw dots. It is the shape of the graph that is important.
Task f. Describe the shape of your graph and explain why you have drawn that shape.
Ruud
47
Chris
Sandra
Fig. 6. Predicted graphs for all students in the city by Ruud, Chris, and Sandra (Bakker, 2004a, p.
224)
The figures of the same three students are presented in Figure 6 and their
written explanations were:
Ruud: Because the average [values are] roughly between 50 and 60 kg.
Chris: I think it is a pyramid shape. I have drawn my graph like that because I
found it easy to make and easy to read.
Sandra: Because most are around the average and there are outliers at 30 and
80 [kg].
Ruud’s answer focused on the average group. During an interview after
the fourth lesson, Ruud like three other students literally called his graph
a “bell shape,” though he had probably not encountered that term in a
school situation before. This is probably a case of reinvention. Chris’s
graph was probably inspired by line graphs that the students made during
48
mathematics lessons. She introduced the vertical axis with frequency,
though such graphs had not been used before in the statistics course.
Sandra may have started with the dots and then drawn the continuous
shape.
In this third phase of growing a sample, 23 students drew a bump
shape. The words they used for the shapes were pyramid (three
students), semicircle (one), and bell shape (four). Although many students
draw continuous shapes but they were all symmetrical. Since weight
distributions are not symmetrical and because skewness is an important
concept, a subsequent lesson addressed asymmetrical shapes in relation
to the weight data (see Bakker, 2004b).
2.7 Reflection on the example
The research question we addressed in the example is: How can we pro-
mote coherent reasoning about distribution in relation to data, variability,
and sampling in a way that is meaningful for students with little statistical
background? We now discuss those key elements for the educational
activity and speculate about what can be learned from the analysis
presented here.
The activity of growing a sample involved short phases of con-
structing diagrams of new hypothetical situations, and comparing these
with other diagrams of a real sample of the same size. The activity has a
broader empirical basis than just the teaching experiment reported in this
chapter, because it emerged from a previous teaching experiment (Bak-
ker & Gravemeijer, 2004) as a way to address shape as a pattern in varia-
bility.
To theoretically generalize the results, Bakker analyzed students’
reasoning as an instance of diagrammatic reasoning, which typically in-
volves constructing diagrams, experimenting with them, and reflecting on
the results of the previous two steps. In this growing samples activity, the
49
quick alternation between prediction and reflection during diagrammatic
reasoning appears to create ample opportunities for concept formation,
for instance of spread.
In the first phase involving the prediction of a small data set, stu-
dents noted that the data were more spread out, but in subsequent
phases, students wrote or said that the spread was large. From the terms
used in this fourth lesson, we conclude that many statistical concepts
such as center (average, majority), spread (range and range of subsets of
data), and shape had become topics of discussion (object-like entities)
during the growing samples activity. Some of these words were used in a
rather unconventional way, which implies that students needed more
guidance at this point. Shape became a topic of discussion as students
predicted that the shape of the graph would be a semicircle, a pyramid, or
a bell shape, and this was exactly what the HLT targeted. Given the stu-
dents’ minimal background in statistics and the fact that this was only the
fourth lesson of the sequence, the results were promising. Note, howev-
er, that such activities cannot simply be repeated in other contexts; they
need to be adjusted to local circumstances if they are to be applied in
other situations.
The instructional activity of growing samples later became a con-
necting thread in Ben-Zvi’s research in Israel, where it also worked to help
students develop statistical concepts in relation to each other (Ben-Zvi,
Aridor, Makar, & Bakker, 2012). This implies that this instructional idea
was transferable to other contexts. The transferability of instructional
ideas from the USA to the Netherlands to Israel, even to higher levels of
education, illustrates that generalization in DBR can take place across
contexts, cultures and age group.
2.8 Final remarks
The example presented in Part 2 was intended to substantiate the
issues discussed in Part 1, and we hope that readers will have a sense of
what DBR could look like and feel invited to read more about it. It should
be noted that there are many variants of DBR. Some have are more fo-
50
cused on theory, some more on empirically grounded products. Some
start with predetermined learning outcomes, others have more open-
ended goals (cf. Engeström, 2011). DBR may be a challenging research
approach but it is in our experience also a very rewarding one given the
products and insights that can be gained.
Acknowledgments The research was funded by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Re-
search under grant number 575-36-003B. The writing of this chapter was made possible with a
grant from the Educational and Learning Sciences Utrecht awarded to Arthur Bakker. We thank
our Master students in our Research Methodology courses for their feedback on earlier ver-
sions of this manuscript. Angelika Bikner-Ahsbahs’s and reviewers’ careful reading has also
helped us tremendously. We also acknowledge PhD students Adri Dierdorp, Al Jupri, and Victor
Antwi, and our colleague Frans van Galen for their helpful comments, and Nathalie Kuijpers and
Norma Presmeg for correcting this manuscript.
2.9 References
Akkerman, S. F., Admiraal, W., Brekelmans, M., & Oost, H. (2008). Auditing quality of research
in social sciences, Quality & Quantity, 42, 257–274.
Anderson, T., & Shattuck, J. (2012). Design-based research: A decade of progress in education
research? Educational Researcher, 41, 16-25.
Artigue, M. (1988). Ingénierie didactique [Didactical engineering]. In Artigue, M., Brousseau, G.,
Brun J., Chevallard Y., Conne F. & Vergnaud, G. (Eds.). Didactique des mathematiques [Di-
dactics of mathematcs]. Paris Delachaux et Niestlé.
Bakkenes, I., Vermunt, J.D., & Wubbels, T. (2010). Teachers learning in the context of educa-
tional innovation: Learning activities and learning outcomes of experienced teachers. Learn-
ing and Instruction, 20(6), 533-548.
Bakker, A. (2004a). Design research in statistics education: On symbolizing and computer tools.
Utrecht, the Netherlands: CD-Bèta Press.
Bakker, A. (2004b). Reasoning about shape as a pattern in variability. Statistics Education Re-
search Journal, 3(2), 64-83. Online
http://www.stat.auckland.ac.nz/~iase/serj/SERJ3(2)_Bakker.pdf
Bakker, A. (2007). Diagrammatic reasoning and hypostatic abstraction in statistics education.
Semiotica, 164, 9-29.
Bakker, A. & Derry, J. (2011). Lessons from inferentialism for statistics education. Mathematical
Thinking and Learning, 13, 5-26.
Bakker, A., & Gravemeijer, K.P.E. (2004). Learning to reason about distribution. In D. Ben-Zvi &
J. Garfield (Eds.), The challenge of developing statistical literacy, reasoning, and thinking (pp.
147-168). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Bakker. A., & Gravemeijer, K.P.E. (2006). An historical phenomenology of mean and median.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 62(2), 149-168.
Bakker, A., & Hoffmann, M. (2005). Diagrammatic reasoning as the basis for developing con-
cepts: A semiotic analysis of students’ learning about statistical distribution. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 60, 333-358.
51
Ben-Zvi, D., Aridor, K., Makar, K., & Bakker, A. (2012). ZDM The International Journal on Math-
ematics Education, 44, 913-925.
Biehler, R., Ben-Zvi, D., Bakker, A., & Makar, K. (2013). Technological advances in developing
statistical reasoning at the school level. In Bishop, A., Clement, K., Keitel, C., Kilpatrick, J., &
Leung, A. Y. L. (Eds.). Third international handbook on mathematics education, (pp. 643-
689). New York: Springer.
Brown, A. (1992). Design experiments: Theoretical and methodological challenges in creating
complex interventions in classroom settings. Journal of the learning sciences, 2, 141-178.
Cicchetti, D. V. (1976). Assessing inter-rater reliability for rating scales: Resolving some basic is-
sues. British Journal of Psychiatry, 129, 452-456.
Cobb, P., Gravemeijer, K.P.E., Bowers, J., & McClain, K. (1997). Statistical Minitools. Designed
for Vanderbilt University, TN, USA. Programmed and revised (2001) at the Freudenthal Insti-
tute, Utrecht University, the Netherlands.
Cobb, P., Confrey, J., diSessa, A., Lehrer, R., & Schauble L. (2003). Design experiments in educa-
tional research. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 9–13.
Cobb, P., McClain, K., & Gravemeijer, K. P. E. (2003). Learning about statistical covariation.
Cognition and Instruction, 21, 1-78.
Cobb, P., & Whitenack, J. W. (1996). A method for conducting longitudinal analyses of class-
room videorecordings and transcripts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 30,(3), 213-228.
Cobb, P., Yackel, E., & Wood, T. (1992). A constructivist alternative to the representational view
of mind in mathematics education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23, 2-
33.1.
Collins, A. (1992). Toward a design science of education. In E. Scanlon & T. O'Shea (Eds.), New
directions in educational technology (pp. 15-22). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Cook, T. (2002). Randomized experiments in education: A critical examination of the reasons
the educational evaluation community has offered for not doing them. Educational Evalua-
tion and Policy Analysis, 24(3), 175–199.
Creswell, J. W. (2005). Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative
and qualitative research (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.
Creswell, J. W. (2007). Qualitative inquiry and research design. Choosing among five traditions
(2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
De Jong, R., & Wijers, M. (1993). Ontwikkelingsonderzoek: Theorie en praktijk [Developmental
research: Theory and practice]. Utrecht, the Netherlands: NVORWO.
Denscombe, M. (2007, 3rd ed). The good research guide. Maidenhead, UK: Open University
Press.
Dierdorp, A., Bakker, A., Eijkelhof, H. M. C., & Van Maanen, J. A. (2011). Authentic practices as
contexts for learning to draw inferences beyond correlated data. Mathematical Thinking
and Learning, 13, 132-151.
diSessa, A. A. & Cobb, P. (2004). Ontological innovation and the role of theory in design experi-
ments. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 77-103.
Drijvers, P. H. M. (2003). Learning algebra in a computer algebra environment: Design research
on the understanding of the concept of parameter. Utrecht, the Netherlands: CD-Beta Press.
Edelson, D. C. (2002). Design research: What we learn when we engage in design. Journal of the
Learning Sciences, 11, 105-121.
Educational Researcher (2003). Special issue on design-based research collective.
Educational Psychologist (2004). Special issue design-based research methods for studying
learning in context.
Engeström, Y. (2011). From design experiments to formative interventions. Theory and Psychol-
ogy, 21(5), 598-628.
Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young mathematicians at work. Constructing number sense,
addition, and subtraction. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
52
Freudenthal, H. (1978). Weeding and sowing: Preface to a Science of Mathematical Education.
Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Reidel.
Freudenthal, H. (1988). Ontwikkelingsonderzoek [Developmental research]. In K. Gravemeijer &
K. Koster (Eds.), Onderzoek, ontwikkeling en ontwikkelingsonderzoek [Research, development
and developmental research]. Universiteit Utrecht, the Netherlands: OW&OC.
Freudenthal, H. (1991). Revisiting mathematics education: China lectures. Dordrecht, The
Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Frick, R. W. (1998). Interpreting statistical testing: Process and propensity, not population and
random sampling. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30(3), 527-535.
Friel, S., N., Curcio, F. R., & Bright, G. W. (2001) Making sense of graphs: Critical factors influenc-
ing comprehension and instructional implications. Journal of Research in Mathematics Edu-
cation. 32(2),124-158.
Geertz, C. (1973). Thick description: Toward an interpretive theory of culture. In Geertz, C., The
interpretation of cultures: Selected essays, (pp. 3-30). New York: Basic Books.
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative
research. Chicago: Aldine.
Goffree, F. (1979). Leren onderwijzen met Wiskobas. Onderwijsontwikkelingsonderzoek
'Wiskunde en Didaktiek' op de pedagogische akademie. [Learning to teach Wiskobas. Educa-
tional development research.] Rijksuniversiteit Utrecht, the Netherlands.
Gravemeijer, K.P.E. (1994). Educational development and developmental research in mathe-
matics education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 25(5), 443-471.
Gravemeijer, K.P.E., & Cobb, P. (2006). Design research from a learning design perspective. In J.
Van den Akker, K. P. E. Gravemeijer, S. McKenney, & N. Nieveen (Eds.), Educational design
research (pp. 17-51). London: Routledge.
Gravemeijer, K. P. E., & Koster, K. (Eds.) (1988). Onderzoek, ontwikkeling en
ontwikkelingsonderzoek. [Research, development, and developmental research]. Utrecht, the
Netherlands: OW&OC.
Guba, E. G. (1981). Criteria for assessing trustworthiness of naturalistic inquiries. ECTJ, 29(2), 75-
91.
Hoffmann, M. H. G. (2002). Peirce’s “diagrammatic reasoning” as a solution of the learning par-
adox. In G. Debrock (Ed.), Process pragmatism: Essays on a quiet philosophical revolution
(pp. 147-174). Amsterdam: Rodopi Press.
Hoyles, C., Noss, R., Kent, P., & Bakker, A. (2010). Improving mathematics at work: The need for
techno-mathematical literacies. Abingdon: Routledge.
Journal of the Learning Sciences (2004). Special issue on design-based research, 13(1), guest-
edited by S. Barab and K. Squire.
Kanselaar, G. (1993). Ontwikkelingsonderzoek bezien vanuit de rol van de advocaat van de
duivel [Design research: Taking the position of the devil’s advocate]. In R. de Jong & M.
Wijers (Red.), Ontwikkelingsonderzoek, theorie en praktijk. Utrecht, the Netherlands:
NVORWO.
Konold, C. & Higgins, T. L. (2003). Reasoning about data, In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin, & D.
Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to Principles and Standards for School Mathematics,
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, pp.193-215.
Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2001). Accounting for contingency in design experiments. Paper
presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Education Research Association, Seattle,
WA.
Lewin, K. (1951) Field theory in social science; selected theoretical papers. In D. Cartwright (Ed.).
New York: Harper & Row.
Lijnse, P. L. (1995). “Developmental Research” as a way to an empirically based “didactical
structure” of science. Science Education, 29(2), 189-199.
Lijnse, P. L., & Klaassen, K. (2004). Didactical structures as an outcome of research on teaching-
learning sequences? International Journal of Science Education, 26(5), 537-554.
53
Maso, I., & Smaling, A. (1998). Kwalitatief onderzoek: praktijk en theorie. [Qualitative research:
Practice and theory]. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Boom.
Mathematical Thinking and Learning (2004). Special issue on learning trajectories in mathemat-
ics education, guest-edited by D. H. Clements and J. Sarama, 6(2).
Maxwell, J.A. (2004). Causal explanation, qualitative research and scientific inquiry in education.
Educational Researcher, 33(2), 3-11.
McClain, K., & Cobb, P. (2001). Supporting students’ ability to reason about data. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 45, 103-129.
McKenney, S., & Reeves, T. (2012). Conducting educational design research. London:
Routledge.
Miles, M. B. & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: A sourcebook of new meth-
ods. Beverly Hills CA: Sage.
Nathan, M. J., & Kim, S. (2009). Regulation of teacher elicitations in the mathematics classroom.
Cognition and Instruction, 27(2), 91–120.
Olsen, D. R. (2004). The triumph of hope over experience in the search for “what works”: A re-
sponse to Slavin. Educational Researcher, 33(1), 24-26.
Opie, C (2004). Doing educational research. London: Sage.
Paas, F. (2005). Design experiments: Neither a design nor an experiment. In C.P. Constantinou,
D. Demetriou, A. Evagorou, M. Evagorou, A. Kofteros, M. Michael, Chr. Nicolaou, D. Pa-
pademetriou, & N. Papadouris (Eds.), Integrating multiple perspectives on effective learning
environments. Proceedings of 11th biennial meeting of the European Association for Re-
search on Learning and Instruction (pp. 901-902). Nicosia, Cyprus: University of Cyprus.
Peirce, C. S. (NEM). The new elements of mathematics (C. Eisele, Ed.). The Hague, the Nether-
lands: Mouton.
Peirce, C. S. (CP). Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce 1931–1958. C. Hartshorne and P.
Weiss (Eds). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Plomp, T. (2007). Educational design research: An introduction. In N. Nieveen & T. Plomp (Eds.),
An introduction to educational design research (pp. 9-35). Enschede, the Netherlands: SLO.
Plomp, T., & Nieveen, N. (Eds.) (2007). An introduction to educational design research. Ensche-
de, the Netherlands: SLO.
Romberg, T. A. (1973). Development research. Overview of how development-based research
works in practice. Wisconsin Research and Development Center for Cognitive Learning, Uni-
versity of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI.
Sandoval, W. A., & Bell, P (2004). Design-dased research methods for studying learning in con-
text: Introduction. Educational Psychologist, 39(4), 199-201.
Sfard, A., & Linchevski, L. (1992). The gains and the pitfalls of reification — The case of algebra.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26(2-3), 191-228.
Simon, M. (1995). Reconstructing mathematics pedagogy from a constructivistic perspective.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(2), 114-145.
Slavin, R. E. (2002) Evidence-based educational policies: Transforming educational practice and
research. Educational Researcher, 31, 15–21.
Smit, J., & Van Eerde, H. A. A. (2011). A teacher’s learning process in dual design research:
Learning to scaffold language in a multilingual mathematics classroom. ZDM The Interna-
tional Journal on Mathematics Education, 43(6-7), 889-900.
Smit, J., Van Eerde, H. A. A., Bakker, A. (in press). A conceptualisation of whole-class scaffolding.
British Educational Research Journal. doi: 10.1002/berj.3007
Steffe, L. P., & Thompson, P. W. (2000). Teaching experiments methodology: Underlying princi-
ples and essential elements. In R. Lesh & A.E. Kelly (Eds.), Research design in mathematics
and science education. (pp. 267-307). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Strauss, A. & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research techniques and procedures for de-
veloping grounded theory (2nd edition). London: Sage.
54
Tall, D., Thomas, M., Davis, G., Gray, E., & Simpson, A. (2000). What is the object of the encap-
sulation of a process? Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 18, 223-241.
Treffers, A. (1987). Three dimensions. A model of goal and theory description in mathematics in-
struction. The Wiskobas project. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.
Van den Akker, J. (1999). Principles and methods of development research. In J. van den Akker,
R.M. Branch, K. Gustafson, N. Nieveen, & T. Plomp (Eds), Design approaches and tools in ed-
ucation and training (pp 1-14). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Van den Akker, J., Gravemeijer, K., McKenney, S. & Nieveen, N. (Eds.) (2006). Educational design
research. London: Routledge.
Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (1996). Assessment and realistic mathematics education.
Utrecht, the Netherlands: CD-Bèta Press.
Van Nes, F, & Doorman, L. M. (2010). The interaction between multimedia data analysis and
theory development in design research. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 22(1), 6-
30.
Yin, R.K. (2009). Case study research: Design and methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Wittmann, E. C. (1992). Didaktik der Mathematik als Ingenieurwissenschaft. [Didactics of
mathematics as an engineering science.] Zentralblatt fur Didaktik der Mathematik, 3, 119-
121.
Appendix: structure of a DBR project with illustrations
In line with Oost and Markenhof (2010), we formulate the following gen-
eral criteria any research project:
1. The research should be anchored in the literature.
2. The research aim should be relevant, both in theoretical and practical
terms.
3. The formulation of aim and questions should be precise, i.e. using
concepts and definitions in the correct way.
4. The method used should be functional in answering the research
question(s).
5. The overall structure of the research project should be consistent, i.e.
title, aim, theory, question, method and results should form a coher-
ent chain of reasoning.
In this appendix we present a structure of general points of attention dur-
ing DBR and specifications for our statistics education example, including
references to relevant sections in the chapter. In this structure these cri-
teria are bolded. This structure could function as the blueprint of a book
or article on a DBR project.
55
General points Examples
Introduction: 1. Choose a topic
2. Identify common
problems and
3. Identify knowledge gap
and relevance
4. Choose mathematical
learning goals
1. Statistics education at the middle
school level
2. Statistics as a set of unrelated
concepts and techniques
3. How middle school students can
be supported to develop a con-
cept of distribution and related
statistical concepts
4. Understanding of distribution
(2.1)
Literature review forms the basis for formulating the research aim
(the research has to be anchored and relevant)
Research aim:
It has to be
clear whether an
aim is descriptive, explana-
tory, evaluative, advisory
etc.
(1.2.2)
Contribute to an empirically and the
o-
retically grounded instruction theory
for statistics education at the middle
school level (advisory aim)
(2.1)
Research aim has to be narrowed down to a research question and possibly subquestions
with the help of different theories
Literature
review
(theoretical
background):
-
Orienting frameworks
- Frameworks for action
- Domain-specific
learning theories (1.2.8)
-
Semiotics (2.3)
- Theories on learning with com-
puter tools
- Realistic Mathematics Education
(2.4)
With the help of theoretical constructs the research question(s) can be formulated
(the formulation has to be precise)
Research
question:
Zoom in what knowledge is
required to achieve the re-
search aim
How can students with little statistical
background develop a notion of distri-
bution?
It should be underpinned why this research question requires DBR
(the method should be functional)
Research
approach:
The lack of the type of learn-
ing aimed for is a common
reason to carry out DBR: It
has to be enacted so it can
be studied
Dutch statistics education was atomis-
tic: Textbooks addressed mean, medi-
an, mode, and different graphical rep-
resentations one by one. Software was
hardly used. Hence the type of learning
aimed for had to be enacted.
56
Using a research method involves several research instruments and techniques
Research in-
struments and
techniques
Research instrument that
connects different theories
and concrete experiences in
the form of testable hypoth-
eses.
1. Identify students’ prior
knowledge
2. Professional
development of
teacher
3. Interview schemes and
planning
4. Intermediate feedback
and reflection with
teacher
5. Determine learning
yield
(1.4.2)
Series of hypothetical learning trajec-
tories (HLTs)
1. Prior interviews and pretest
2. Preparatory meetings with
teacher
3. Mini-interviews, observation
scheme
4. Debrief sessions with teacher
5. Posttest
(2.5)
Design Design guidelines Guided reinvention; Historical and di-
dactical phenomenology (2.4)
Data analysis
Hypotheses have to be tes
t-
ed by comparison of hypo-
thetical and observed learn-
ing. Additional analyses may
be necessary (1.4.3)
Comparison of hypothetical and o
b-
served learning
Constant comparative method of gen-
erating conjectures and testing them
on the remaining data sources (2.6)
Results Insights into patterns in
learning and means of sup-
porting such learning
Series of HLTs as progressive diagram-
matic reasoning about growing sam-
ples (2.6)
Discussion Theoretical and practical
yield
- Concrete example of an historical
and didactical phenomenology in
statistics education
- Application of semiotics in an ed-
ucational domain
- Insights into computer use in the
mathematics classroom
- Series of learning activities
- Improved computer tools
The aim, theory, question, method and results should be aligned
(the research has to be consistent)
... Esta propuesta didáctica se fundamenta en la Investigación Basada en Diseño (IBD), cuyo propósito es relacionar la práctica educativa y la teoría. Esta metodología se centra en desarrollar teorías a través del diseño de métodos, materiales educativos o pruebas empíricas para apoyar el aprendizaje de un dominio específico (Bakker & van Eerde, 2015). El objetivo principal es observar las ventajas y desventajas del diseño, permitiendo realizar ajustes durante su aplicación para facilitar el aprendizaje significativo en los estudiantes. ...
... De esta manera, el diseño se evalúa y valida a través de los resultados de su implementación y el logro de los objetivos previamente establecidos. La IBD consta de tres fases (Bakker & van Eerde, 2015): ...
Article
Full-text available
Para hacer frente a los desafíos del aprendizaje del álgebra, se ha desarrollado un movimiento denominado Álgebra Temprana. Desde este movimiento se promueve el desarrollo del álgebra desde edades tempranas mediante la generalización de patrones. Sin embargo, algunos estudios han mostrado las dificultades que enfrentan los estudiantes para lograr estos procesos de generalización. El objetivo de esta propuesta didáctica es generar un diseño que permita a estudiantes de 9 y 10 años desarrollar el pensamiento algebraico. Esta propuesta se sustentó en la perspectiva didáctica del álgebra como instrumento de modelización matemática, particularmente para generalizar patrones. La metodología para la elaboración, implementación y validación de las actividades tuvo en cuenta el enfoque de investigación basada en diseño y el Método Singapur (MS). La propuesta se aplicó en una escuela privada en México y el análisis de las respuestas de los estudiantes se basó en los niveles del álgebra temprana. Los resultados muestran que transitar por las etapas del MS (concreta, pictórica y abstracta) favorece que los estudiantes puedan identificar las regularidades de los patrones y plantear sus reglas generales, empleando elementos simbólicos y alcanzando el nivel 2 de algebrización.
... Educational Design-Based Research includes designing teaching materials as part of the research process. In contrast to other research methods that primarily focus on creating educational materials, Design-Based Research also employs iterative tests and refinement of materials by putting the design of learning materials in context with theory development (Bakker & van Eerde, 2015). In line with Baumgartner et al. (2003), Design-Based Research represents a crucial approach to explore how educational innovations, such as the FLINK project in the case of the underlying study, function in practice and to uncover conditions in the learning materials that are successful. ...
... Thereby, the DLE can be refined based on real-time feedback by the students. Moreover, Design-Based Research permits the refinement of educational resources during practical testing, which provides the researchers with a more adaptable approach compared to other research methods, where the separation between design and its analysis are treated as independent processes (Bakker & van Eerde, 2015). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Digital technologies in education in Austria are rapidly advancing, particularly since the Austrian Federal Ministry of Education, Science, and Research initiated the distribution of digital devices in Austrian classrooms. Consequently, there is a growing demand for high-quality digital learning materials. However, empirical research on the design of these materials remains limited. This master's thesis aims to explore design aspects of digital learning environments (DLE) for geometry learning that hold significance for students at the beginning of lower secondary level. Through a qualitative study employing the Think-Aloud method, 17 students in the fifth grade of lower secondary level were observed working with digital learning materials for geometry learning provided by the project FLINK in Maths. The observations identified three key design aspects of DLEs: 1) navigation, 2) contextual framing of tasks, and 3) fostering self-effective experiences. The findings underscore the necessity for dynamic and interactive exercises along with the importance of teacher guidance and mediation.
... Hasil analisis digunakan untuk melakukan penyempurnaan HLT. Analisis yang disajikan dalam artikel ini adalah kesesuaian antara HLT dan ALT secara kuantitatif serta serta analisis kualitatif yang penting dan menjadi perhatian utama tentang bagaimana konjektur pada HLT berjalan dari kondisi aktual pembelajaran berdasarkan(Bakker & van Eerde, 2015).Selain itu, dideskripsikan juga bagaimana serangkaian tugas berbasis sejarah masalah segitiga dari Jiuzhang Shuansu dapat mendukung siswa dalam mengkontruksi pemahaman Pythagoras.HASIL DAN PEMBAHASANPada bagian awal, akan dijelaskan secara singkat HLT dan konjektur pemikiran siswa yang kami susun untuk mendukung pemahaman siswa dalam Teorema Pythagoras.Selanjutnya, kami akan membahas secara mendalam tentang bagaimana rangkaian tugas yang kami kembangkan dapat mendukung kemampuan penalaran visual dan literasi matematika siswa. Berikut penjelasan rangkaian tugas pembelajaran yang kami kembangkan dan analisis perbandingan antara HLT dan Actual Learning Trajectories (ALT). ...
Article
Full-text available
Studi ini mengkaji bagaimana Sejarah teorema Pythagoras, khususnya dari Cina kuno, dapat berfungsi sebagai sumber inspirasi untuk desain instruksional. Berdasarkan contoh sejarah dan pemecahan masalah dari sejarah matematika di Jiuzhang Shuanshu, dikembangkan serangkaian tugas instruksional untuk siswa sekolah menengah kelas tujuh dan bagaimana perannya dalam meningkatkan pengetahuan siswa tentang teorema Pythagoras. Dalam artikel ini dibahas preliminary investigations and teaching experiments dari tiga fase utama penelitian design-based research. Konsep dasar dari desain yang dikembangkan adalah untuk memperkenalkan manipulasi geometris historis dan memecahkan masalah segitiga siku-siku menurut metode geometris Liu Hui (abad ke-3 M). Tujuan pengajaran manipulasi geometris adalah untuk menggabungkan penalaran aljabar simbolis siswa dengan pemahaman visual mereka tentang Teorema Pythagoras. Dalam penelitian ini, kami membandingkan Hypothetical Learning Trajectory (HLT) dengan Actual Learning Trajectory (ALT) untuk mengetahui bagaimana peranan desain yang dikembangkan. Temuan penelitian ini menunjukkan bahwa masalah berbasis sejarah sebagai konteks dalam desain pembelajaran yang dikembangkan memberikan kesempatan kepada siswa untuk mempelajari teorema Pythagoras secara bermakna.
... Brown (1992) and Collins (1992) first introduced design experiments as an interventionalist methodology in which the goal was to engineer and simultaneously investigate educational innovations and further develop design theories for future innovations. Accordingly, the design-based research process implies designing and testing innovations and identifying elements that work (Bakker & van Eerde, 2015). Designing ENSA and evaluating the validity evidence of the tool is, in this way, connected to design-based research through the development of an assessment tool and the identification of how this tool can contribute to the development of further assessments. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Children enter school with several valuable mathematical experiences, but many teachers lack the methods to formatively assess young children’s mathematical knowledge. Consequently, children may not be met with sufficient challenges to facilitate their further mathematical development. Moreover, current assessment tools used for the youngest children are either too time-demanding to carry out, lack focus on specific mathematical domains or do not consider various aspects of assessment validity. In this PhD thesis, I investigate how technology can enhance the formative assessment of children’s early number sense by developing a digital assessment tool—early number sense assessment (ENSA)—focusing specifically on early number sense and assessment validity. The development and further investigation of ENSA in use reveals that technology can supplement the assessment of number sense in a time-efficient way without being dependent on reading and writing skills or the competence of the individual instructor. ENSA also describes previously unknown aspects of Norwegian children’s early mathematical knowledge. Furthermore, the thesis shows how interactive items have the potential to describe more of the process, leading to an answer to mathematical problems while displaying evidence of assessment validity. Additionally, the use of technology brings some specific design elements that can affect children’s results and strategies when finding answers to mathematical problems. Finally, teachers’ interpretations of children’s assessment results situated in a wider social practice are limited and shaped by external factors that ultimately affect the formative value of assessments for teachers.
... KCT needs to be a continuously evolving aspect of PCK, with educational representations and methods steadily improving as new research-based insights are gained about their effectiveness. Educational researchers are positioned to engage in this type of research and development with methods such as designbased research (Bakker & van Eerde 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) traditionally has been used in research concerning prospective and practicing classroom teachers. This essay argues that PCK is also relevant to other professions including those advancing quantitative reasoning (QR). To illustrate, the case of PCK for teaching QR is considered. Those in fields such as public health, journalism, meteorology, and government increasingly find themselves responsible for helping the public understand an ever-growing amount of quantitative information that has a bearing on societal well-being. Several examples illustrate how such professionals’ responsibilities require knowing prevalent QR patterns in society, strategies for fostering sound reasoning, and the general nature of school curricula. Professional organizations in education, colleges of education, and educational researchers would benefit from expanding PCK research to encompass multiple professions. These expanded research and development efforts would simultaneously address urgent societal needs.
... Similar results were also found in Şanal and Elmali's (2023) research; their findings revealed that RME had a significant and positive influence on students' mathematical problem-solving performance. In addition, a study by Bakker and van Eerde (2015) showed that LIT is effective in developing specific and relevant teaching strategies, which ultimately improve student learning outcomes. This suggests that local teaching theories can help teachers by developing viable local teaching theories that can be used by classroom teachers to interpret hypothetical learning trajectories that suit students' actual characteristics and situations. ...
Article
Full-text available
[English]: This research examines the improvement of students' mathematical proficiency in linear equation material using local instruction theory (LIT) with a realistic mathematics education (RME) approach. This research used a one-group pretest-posttest design. The sample was 37 secondary school students at SMP Negeri 1 South Halmahera, class VIII. The instrument used is a mathematical proficiency test consisting of 4 essay questions. Data were analyzed using the prerequisite test, Mann-Whitney test, Cohen's (d) effect sizes test, and the gain value was determined. As a result, there are differences in students' mathematical proficiency before and after implementing LIT through the RME approach. The influence of LIT with the RME approach on mathematics proficiency is very high. The students are able to define a linear equation, determine the slope of the line, construct a linear equation, integrate the linear graphs, and determine solutions for the linear equation problems. [Bahasa]: Penelitian ini mengkaji peningkatan kemampuan matematis siswa pada materi persamaan garis lurus menggunakan local instruction theory (LIT) melalui pendekatan realistic mathematics education (RME). Penelitian ini menggunakan desain one-group pretest-posttest. Sampel penelitian adalah siswa SMP Negeri 1 Halmahera Selatan kelas VIII sebanyak 37 orang. Instrumen yang digunakan berupa tes kemampuan matematis yang terdiri atas 4 soal essai. Data dianalisis menggunakan uji prasyarat, uji Mann-Whitney, uji Cohen's (d) effect sizes, dan juga dilakukan perhitungan nilai gain. Hasil penelitian menunjukkan bahwa terdapat perbedaan kemampuan matematis antara sebelum dan setelah penerapan LIT dengan pendekatan RME. Pengaruh LIT dengan pendekatan RME terhadap kemampuan matematis siswa berkategori sangat tinggi. Siswa mampu mendefinisikan persamaan garis lurus, mampu menentukan kemiringan suatu garis lurus, dapat menyusun persamaan garis lurus, mengintegrasikan suatu grafik garis lurus, dan menentukan solusi dari masalah yang berkaitan dengan persamaan garis lurus.
Article
Full-text available
In the frame of a design-based research project, this paper presents analysis of Danish grade 8 students working together to prove conjectures, which they formulated based on guided explorations in a dynamic geometry environment. A systematic account of the rationale and hypothesis behind the task design is described in the form of objectives, hypotheses, and choices, which are then evaluated in light of the analysis of data. The case indicates that the designed task can bridge a connection between conjecturing activities in dynamic geometry environments and deductive reasoning. The students manage to explain theoretically, what is initially empirically evident for them in their exploration in the dynamic geometry environment. The proving activity seems to make sense for the students, as a way of explaining ”why” the conjecture is true. The Theory of Semiotic Mediation frames the design of the study and the data analysis.
Article
Full-text available
Situados en la investigación cuyo interés es la creación de estrategias que favorezcan formas de pensamiento propias del conocimiento estocástico, este artículo expone la primera fase de una investigación de diseño en el marco del campo disciplinar, resaltando la problemática particular de la inferencia bayesiana. La propuesta está fundamentada, principalmente, en un modelo epistemológico basado en prácticas, desde la socioepistemología, y guiada metodológicamente por la construcción de Trayectorias de Aprendizaje. Como resultado, se propone una trayectoria de desarrollo de prácticas para explorar la actividad estocástica de estudiantes de bachillerato sobre la inferencia binomial bayesiana. A manera de reflexión, planteamos que las tareas del diseño fomentan formas de razonamiento que subyacen a la visión bayesiana de la inferencia y se alinean con elementos de alfabetización estadística y probabilística para formar ciudadanos críticos. Una futura fase de implementación nos permitirá validar el diseño, más aún, robustecer el modelo epistemológico de partida.
Thesis
Full-text available
Microlearning is a buzzword in eLearning, referring to learning content that is delivered in short bursts or small bites. With no universally accepted definition, it means different things to different people, which further adds to its nebulous nature. This thesis study uses learning theory principles in a graduate education context to explore how students see the benefits or issues with microlearning activities integrated into an online course. Despite its popularity, much is still unknown and unclear about its educational potential especially within the context of formal higher education and how it can be integrated within university-level online courses. By means of a design-based study spanning across three iterations, I explored the role of microlearning in a graduate-level online course from a course design perspective. I achieved this by developing a model called midweek microlesson, under which graduate students enrolled in an online course in Education at a Canadian university were exposed to microlessons on a weekly basis. These microlessons were carefully designed based on theories and evidence-informed practices within the science of learning. The model intended to: a) offer students opportunities for distributed practice; and b) further augment and enrich students’ understanding of the course’s weekly theme. Qualitative and quantitative data was collected and analyzed to understand students’ interaction and experience with the microlessons. The main purpose of this exploratory study is to understand how a specific form of microlearning (i.e., microlessons) can be integrated within a graduate-level online course with the goal to enhance learners’ interaction with the content. Thematic analysis of student commentary led to the emergence of six design themes: content, purpose, timing, length & structure, format and interactivity. I discuss these in light of cognitive learning theories and what they mean for practice. Overall, this study is an attempt to lay a foundation to explore potential educational affordances of microlearning within graduate-level online courses. The study also raises awareness about the lack of theory in current discussions surrounding microlearning and presents an example of exploring and integrating microlearning in course design. Limitations of the study and areas for future exploration are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Extracts available on Google Books (see link below). For integral book, go to publisher's website : http://www.routledge.com/books/details/9780415480086/
Book
A title that sounds like poetry, and a subtitle that seems to contradict the title! But the subtitle is right, and originally it was just the title. A strange subtitle, isn’t it? Preface to a Science of Mathematical Education. All sciences – in their prenatal stage – have known this kind of literature: only the term used was not ‘Preface’, but, for instance, ‘Prolegomena’, which * means the same though it sounds less provisional. In fact such works were thicker than the present one, by up to ten times. There is much more that can be said about a science before it comes into being than after; with the first results comes modesty. This is the preface to a book that will never be written: not by me, nor by anybody else. Once a science of mathematical education exists, it will get the preface it deserves. Nevertheless this preface – or what for honesty’s sake I have labelled so – must fulfil a function: the function of accelerating the birth of a science of mathematical education, which is seriously impeded by the unfounded view that such already exists. Against this view I have to argue: it rests on a wrong estimation – both over and under estimation at the same time – of what is to be considered as science.
Book
This book is a product of love and respect. If that sounds rather odd I initially apologise, but let me explain why I use those words. The original manuscript was of course Freudenthal’s, but his colleagues have carried the project through to its conclusion with love for the man, and his ideas, and with a respect developed over years of communal effort. Their invitation to me to write this Preface e- bles me to pay my respects to the great man, although I am probably incurring his wrath for writing a Preface for his book without his permission! I just hope he understands the feelings of all colleagues engaged in this particular project. Hans Freudenthal died on October 13th, 1990 when this book project was well in hand. In fact he wrote to me in April 1988, saying “I am thinking about a new book. I have got the sub-title (China Lectures) though I still lack a title”. I was astonished. He had retired in 1975, but of course he kept working. Then in 1985 we had been helping him celebrate his 80th birthday, and although I said in an Editorial Statement in Educational Studies in Mathematics (ESM) at the time “we look forward to him enjoying many more years of non-retirement” I did not expect to see another lengthy manuscript.
Chapter
There is no science of mathematical education. Not yet. Again, there are many marvellous activities — educational engineering in mathematics — sources from which a science of mathematical education may spring. But not yet, not in the present chapter, which is a mere collection of suggestions, supported and illustrated by experience. Team work, in particular in curriculum development, may be such a source. In order to communicate, a team must create a working language. A language that covers some content can be not only a carrier, but even a source of science. Learning situations, and in particular open ones, learning processes, their levels, and their discontinuities are worth observing and analysing, in order to build them into theories. I try to show some features of language as a vehicle of research and of motivation as a motor in the learning process — motivation by discontinuites in the learning process, motivation by goals, motivation by make-up. In a number of sections the author pursues the origin of general ideas, concepts, judgments and attitudes in the learning process, whether they are attained in a continuous process, by comprehension, that is by generalising from numerous examples, as is the common opinion, or by apprehension, that is, by grasping directly the general situation, which is my thesis. One way of apprehending creation of general mental objects is the paradigm: rather than a multitude — one example, which evokes the general idea. A series of examples of apprehending by paradigms is shown, and abortive quests for paradigms and discontinuities in learning processes are revealed. This, in particular, concerns the number concept. Another kind of apprehension is direct grasp of generality, illustrated by an apprehending approach to algebra, not unlike that of the school of Davydov, but on a higher level of learning. I then turn to levels of language, illustrated by several learning sequences: the ostensive level, the level of relative language, the level of conventional variables, the functional level. Another theme, extending over a number of sections, is change of perspective, again illustrated by many examples related to grasping the context, logical conversion, the switch from global to local perspective, and the converse, from qualitative to quantitative perspective, and the converse. Grasping the context is resumed in the section on probability, and many examples of change of perspective appear in the section on geometry entitled by the phrase “I see it so” by which young children justify their geometric statements. The book closes with an example of what has been postulated on several occasions as a precondition of educational research in mathematics: a piece of didactical phenomenology of mathematical concepts.