ArticlePDF Available

Exploring the effect of media images on women's leadership self-perceptions and aspirations

Authors:

Abstract

a gender gap still pre-vails in top-level leadership positions. Women occupy less than 3% of Fortune 500 CEO seats, only 15.7% of the Fortune 500 board seats (Catalyst, 2011), and women currently hold only 90 of the 535 seats (16.8%) in the U.S. Congress (Center for American Women and Politics, 2012). Numerically, women make up half of our pop-ulation but are grossly underrepresented in powerful leadership positions—positions that make important decisions for the entire population. While research aimed towards increasing women's representation in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields has Article 1176 XXX10.1177/1368430212451176Simon and HoytGroup Processes & Intergroup Relations Abstract Across two experimental studies, the present research explores how media images depicting counterstereotypical roles for women, compared to those that depict stereotypical roles for women, affect women's gender role beliefs (Study 1) and responses to a leadership situation (Study 2). Study 1 predicted and found that women exposed to images depicting counterstereotypical roles subsequently reported stronger nontraditional gender role beliefs than women exposed to images depicting stereotypical roles. Study 2 then directly assessed the effect of media images of women on female participants' self-reported responses following a leadership task. Women exposed to media images of women in counterstereotypical roles reported less negative self-perceptions and greater leadership aspirations than women exposed to images of women in stereotypical roles. Moreover, negative self-perceptions mediated the relationship between media images and leadership aspirations. Implications for increasing women's representation in the leadership domain are discussed.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations
16(2) 232 –245
© The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission: sagepub.
co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1368430212451176
gpir.sagepub.com
Article
G
P
I
R
Group Processes &
Intergroup Relations
Despite the fact that women now make up nearly
half of the U.S. workforce and earn more than
half of higher-level degrees (U.S. Bureau of
Labor Statistics, 2009; U.S. National Center for
Education Statistics, 2009), a gender gap still pre-
vails in top-level leadership positions. Women
occupy less than 3% of Fortune 500 CEO seats,
only 15.7% of the Fortune 500 board seats
(Catalyst, 2011), and women currently hold only
90 of the 535 seats (16.8%) in the U.S. Congress
(Center for American Women and Politics, 2012).
Numerically, women make up half of our pop-
ulation but are grossly underrepresented in
powerful leadership positions—positions that
make important decisions for the entire population.
While research aimed towards increasing
womens representation in science, technology,
engineering, and mathematics (STEM) fields has
Article
451176
XXX10.1177/1368430212451176Simon and HoytGroup Processes & Intergroup Relations
1
Tulane University, USA
2
University of Richmond, USA
Corresponding author:
Stefanie Simon, Department of Psychology, Tulane
University, 2007 Percival Stern Hall, New Orleans, LA
70118, USA
Email: ssimon3@tulane.edu
Exploring the effect of media
images on women’s leadership
self-perceptions and aspirations
Stefanie Simon
1
and Crystal L. Hoyt
2
Abstract
Across two experimental studies, the present research explores how media images depicting
counterstereotypical roles for women, compared to those that depict stereotypical roles for women,
affect womens gender role beliefs (Study 1) and responses to a leadership situation (Study 2). Study 1
predicted and found that women exposed to images depicting counterstereotypical roles subsequently
reported stronger nontraditional gender role beliefs than women exposed to images depicting
stereotypical roles. Study 2 then directly assessed the effect of media images of women on female
participants’ self-reported responses following a leadership task. Women exposed to media images
of women in counterstereotypical roles reported less negative self-perceptions and greater leadership
aspirations than women exposed to images of women in stereotypical roles. Moreover, negative self-
perceptions mediated the relationship between media images and leadership aspirations. Implications
for increasing womens representation in the leadership domain are discussed.
Keywords
leadership, women, media images, gender roles, gender stereotypes
Paper received 24 October 2011; revised version accepted 14 May 2012.
Simon and Hoyt 233
gained a great deal of attention (STEMconnector,
2012), much research is still needed to understand
the underrepresentation of women in other
domains, such as leadership. Increasing womens
representation in top-level leadership positions is
important for giving women a voice across all
domains, from politics, to industry, to academia.
Moreover, empirical research suggests that gender
differences in leadership style and effectiveness
may offer women a slight advantage to men
(Eagly, Johannesen-Schmidt, & van Engen, 2003;
Hoyt, 2010a; 2010b). Thus, increasing womens repre-
sentation in leadership positions is also important for
improving the leadership of our society.
Gender leader stereotype
While many factors may help to explain womens
difficulty in attaining powerful leadership posi-
tions (e.g., household and family responsibilities),
the present research examines how gender stereo-
types prevalent in the media may negatively
impact womens responses in leadership situa-
tions. Negative stereotypes about women in lead-
ership positions represent one factor that
contributes to the large disparity that remains
between men and women in top-level leadership
positions (Eagly & Carli, 2007). These negative
stereotypes result in prejudice against women such
that they are perceived as less competent leaders
and less deserving of leadership roles (Heilman,
2001; Phelan & Rudman, 2010; Ridgeway, 2001;
Schein 2001).
Negative stereotypes about women in leader-
ship positions are closely tied to the gender role
stereotypes about men and women. Whereas,
women generally are thought to be communal,
possessing attributes such as affectionate, helpful,
kind, and sensitive, men are thought to be more
agentic, possessing attributes such as ambitious,
aggressive, and dominant (Eagly, 1987).
According to Eagly and Karau’s (2002) role
congruity theory, prejudice against female leaders
results from the incongruity between the take
charge, or agentic, stereotype linked with leader-
ship and the take care, or communal, stereotype
associated with women. That is, the stereotypical
image of a leader is someone who has agentic,
masculine traits. Thus, prejudice against female
leaders stems from the perceived lack of fit
between the agentic traits that are stereotypical of
leaders and the communal traits that are stereo-
typical of women. In their recent meta-analysis,
Koenig, Eagly, Mitchell, and Ristikari (2011)
found a strong and robust tendency for leader-
ship to be viewed as culturally masculine across
three different research paradigms that address
the masculinity of leader stereotypes.
Responses to stereotypes
This gender leader stereotype can have harmful
effects on womens self-perceptions, well-being,
and behavior in leadership situations (Hoyt &
Blascovich, 2007; Hoyt, Johnson, Murphy, &
Skinnell, 2010; Hoyt & Simon, 2011). For
example, after explicitly priming women with the
gender leader stereotype, Hoyt and Blascovich
(2010) found women with low levels of leader-
ship self-efficacy assimilated to the negative
stereotype by performing poorly and showing
deflated self-report responses. Hoyt and Simon
(2011) found that exposure to role models that
activated gender stereotypes resulted in lower
perceived performance and leadership aspira-
tions, and greater perceived task difficulty and
feelings of inferiority for women performing a
leadership task, compared to women who were
primed with stereotype-disconfirming leaders.
The research discussed above suggests that
exposure to negative stereotypes about women
and leadership results in an effect similar to
stereotype threat that often occurs when individ-
uals perceive themselves to be at risk of confirm-
ing a negative stereotype about their group
(Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999; Steele & Aronson,
1995). However, while stereotype threat research
has traditionally focused on and demonstrated
deficits in performance in a specific domain (e.g.,
womens poorer math performance), stereotypes
also impact the psychological well-being and self-
esteem of those targeted by the stereotype (Swim,
Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001). In fact, recent
research has demonstrated that the performance
234 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 16(2)
deficits that stereotype threat induces are due, in
part, to these negative self-appraisals during the
task (Schmader, Forbes, Zhang, & Mendes, 2009;
see Schmader & Croft, 2011; and Schmader,
Johns, & Forbes, 2008, for reviews of stereotype
threat mechanisms). For example, Cadinu, Maass,
Rosabianca, and Kiesner (2005) found that for
women completing a math task, stereotype threat
resulted in negative task-related thoughts, which
resulted in a decrease in performance (see also,
Beilock, Rydell, & McConnell, 2007).
These negative self-perceptions also result
in diminished participation of targeted individuals
in the domain in which they are negatively
stereotyped, including lower identification (Hoyt
& Blascovich, 2007), lower intention for future
participation (Davies, Spencer, & Steele, 2005),
and eventual disengagement from the given
domain altogether (Steele, 1997). Exploring this
idea that stereotype threat can lower womens
intentions or motivation to become leaders
because of negative self-perceptions, Hoyt and
Simon (2011, Study 2) found that women who
were exposed to highly successful female leaders
who served to activate negative stereotypes
subsequently reported reduced leadership aspira-
tions, an effect that was driven by negative self-
perceptions. Thus, exposure to negative
stereotypes has important implications for both
performance outcomes and self-perceptions.
Moreover, these deflated self-perceptions seem
to be directly related to performance outcomes
and future aspirations in the given domain.
Gender and media images
Habitual exposure to images of women and men
engaging in traditional gender role activities in the
media serves to perpetuate and strengthen gender
stereotypes. Media images portray men and
women almost exclusively in traditional gender
roles (Coltrane & Adams, 1997; Davis, 2003;
Ganahl, Prinsen, & Netzley, 2003; Goffman,
1976; Reichert, 2008; Reichert & Carpenter, 2004;
Rounder, Slater, & Domenech-Rodríguez, 2003),
which differentially ascribe men to breadwinner
and high-status roles and women to homemaker
and low-status roles.
These media images deliver both gender role
and leadership role beliefs and expectations, and
not surprisingly, these expectations and beliefs
can have profound effects on how women
perceive themselves. Geis, Brown, Walstedt, and
Porter (1984) found that women who viewed
traditional commercials mentioned more home-
making than achievement themes compared to
men in the same condition. On the other hand,
women who viewed commercials with female
and male actors in reverse roles had more achieve-
ment aspirations than women in the traditional
condition. Consistent with research exploring the
malleability of gender role beliefs and behavior
(Clément-Guillotin & Fontayne, 2011; Deaux &
Major, 1987; Miller, Lewy, & Peckham, 1997;
Smith, Noll, & Bryant, 1999; Uchronski, 2008),
this research demonstrates that seemingly rigid
gender role beliefs can be altered by exposure to
different media images.
In a more recent replication of the research by
Geis et al. (1984) previously described, Yoder,
Christopher, and Holmes (2008) found no difference
in womens achievement aspirations across the
traditional and nontraditional commercials;
however, the traditional commercials still served to
activate gender stereotypes. The fact that women
no longer differ in their aspirations to have a
career, an education, money, and possessions
accurately reflects the now equal gender break-
down in the workforce (U.S. Bureau of Labor
Statistics, 2009). What remains unclear is how
these gender-stereotypic portrayals of women in
the media continue to dampen womens level of
achievement aspirations (i.e., leadership aspira-
tions). Do media portrayals of women contribute
to the gender disparity in top-level leadership
positions by dampening womens achievement
aspirations?
Davies et al. (2005) addressed this question by
examining whether women experienced stereo-
type threat in the leadership domain after viewing
gender-stereotypic commercials or neutral
commercials which did not depict people
Simon and Hoyt 235
or feature any gender-stereotypic products or
companies. They found that women exposed to
gender-stereotypic commercials strongly preferred
a follower role compared to a leadership role in an
upcoming leadership task, while women exposed
to neutral commercials expressed no clear prefer-
ence for either role. These findings suggest that
the vulnerability women feel about confirming
negative stereotypes in a masculine domain (i.e.,
leadership) can deter women from leadership
roles in favor of stereotypically appropriate roles.
Although these gender stereotypes conveyed by
the media may no longer prevent women from
pursuing a career in general, they do seem to
dampen womens motivation to attain more tradi-
tionally masculine roles, such as leadership
positions.
Counterstereotypical women
as role models
To date, no research that we are aware of has
directly examined the effect of media images
depicting women in counterstereotypical, non-
traditional gender roles on womens responses in
leadership situations. However, the fact that expo-
sure to neutral advertisements (i.e., advertisements
displaying only products and not people) did not
result in stereotype threat effects in Davies et al.s
(2005) research suggests that advertisements
depicting counterstereotypical roles may also have
a buffering effect for women in leadership situa-
tions. Given that many advertisements are not
neutral, and instead they often depict gendered
products or women and men engaging in activi-
ties, exploring the effects of counterstereotypical
images is a worthy endeavor. If exposure to adver-
tisements with women undertaking counter-
stereotypical roles does have a positive effect on
womens leadership aspirations, increasing the
number of advertisements that picture women in
a variety of roles can provide a practical approach
to remedying an intractable problem.
Yet the possibility remains that women will not
respond positively to advertisements portraying
women in counterstereotypical roles. Research by
Rudman and Phelan (2010) found that exposure to
counterstereotypical roles (e.g., a female surgeon
or a female business executive) decreased womens
leadership self-concept and lowered their interest
in traditionally masculine occupations. Similarly,
research has also shown that exposure to highly
successful female leaders can have a self-deflating
effect on womens leadership self-perceptions
compared to exposure to less elite, up-and-coming
female leaders (Hoyt & Simon, 2011).
The research by Rudman and Phelan (2010)
and Hoyt and Simon (2011) described before both
suggest that the negative effect of exposure to
women in counterstereotypical roles is due to self-
deflating upward social comparisons. Because
women in advertisements are generally not highly
successful superstars, but rather depict “average”
people, we predict for the current research that
media images of counterstereotypical roles will be
a viable way to improve womens self-perceptions
and performance in the leadership domain. For
example, Hoyt and Simon (2011, Study 2) found
that exposure to female leaders whose success
seemed attainable had a positive effect on women’s
leadership aspirations, in part because these women
activated greater gender-counterstereotypic
thoughts. In this study, women did not seem to
experience upward social comparison threat because
their level of success was perceived as attainable (see
also Asgari, Dasgupta, & Stout, 2011).
Research by Dasgupta and Asgari (2004) pro-
vides further evidence that exposure to counter-
stereotypic women can have positive effects on
women. They found that women who attended an
all-womens college expressed less automatic
gender stereotypes than those who attended
a coeducational college (Dasgupta & Asgari,
2004). Moreover, this effect was mediated by the
frequency of exposure to female leaders. These
findings suggest that exposure to gender-
counterstereotypic women in everyday life (so
long as they do not induce upward social compari-
son threat) can decrease the perceived incongruity
between leadership roles and the female gender
role by altering womens gender stereotypes and
gender role beliefs. Consequently, exposure to
236 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 16(2)
images of women in counterstereotypical gender
roles may buffer women from stereotype threat
effects in the leadership domain.
While the negative impact of gender-stereotypic
media images has been well documented, there is
a lack of experimental research investigating the
impact of counterstereotypical media images,
with the exception of the research by Yoder et al.
(2008) previously described, which did not assess
womens leadership aspirations or self-perceptions.
No research to date has examined if or how
counterstereotypical portrayals of women in the
media might influence womens achievement
aspirations in the leadership domain. By merging
the literature on stereotyping, media images, and
counterstereotypic role models, the present
research takes a first look at whether media
images portraying women in counterstereotypical
roles can have beneficial effects for womens
responses in a leadership situation.
Present research
In the current research, we explored the effect of
counterstereotypical portrayals of women in the
media (i.e., magazine advertisements) on womens
gender role attitudes, self-perceptions, and leader-
ship aspirations. First, we pilot tested the adver-
tisements we selected to represent stereotypical
and counterstereotypical female gender roles
before use in Study 1 and Study 2 to ensure that
the two sets of media images were being perceived
as differentially stereotypic of the female gender
role and the leadership role. Next, in Study 1, we
explored the effect of advertisements of women
in counterstereotypical roles, compared to adver-
tisements of women in stereotypical roles, on
female participants’ gender role beliefs. Finally, in
Study 2, we examined the effect of media images
(counterstereotypical vs. stereotypical roles) on
female participants’ self-perceptions and leader-
ship aspirations after a leadership task.
We hypothesized that, compared to advertise-
ments with stereotypical roles, advertisements
with counterstereotypical roles would be (a) seen
as less stereotypic of the female gender role and
more congruent with leadership (pilot study),
(b) would strengthen womens nontraditional atti-
tudes toward womens roles in society (Study 1),
and (c) would result in more positive self-reported
responses from women after a leadership task
(Study 2). Furthermore, based on the harmful
effects of negative, intrusive thoughts shown in
stereotype threat research (Cadinu et al., 2005;
Hoyt & Simon, 2011; Schmader et al., 2009), we
predicted that greater leadership aspirations
would be driven by less negative self-perceptions
following a leadership task.
Pilot study
We pretested a set of advertisements displaying
women in counterstereotypical and stereotypical
roles to use in Study 1 and Study 2 with a separate
sample of 40 women recruited from Zoomerang
(an online survey site) who viewed one of two sets
of advertisements. The purpose of the pilot study
was to ensure that advertisements depicting women
in nontraditional, counterstereotypical gender roles
were perceived as less stereotypic of the female
gender role and more congruent with the leader-
ship role than a set of advertisements depicting
women in stereotypical (or traditional) gender roles.
The advertisements were selected from popu-
lar womens magazines in the U.S. In the counter-
stereotypical condition, magazine advertisements
pictured women in nontraditional female gender
roles, such as professional athletes, entrepreneurs,
and doctors. Importantly, we selected advertise-
ments where women were portraying agentic
traits associated with the leadership role or the
male gender role—not explicitly displaying
women in leadership roles. The stereotypical
condition consisted of magazine advertisements
picturing women in stereotypical female gender
roles, such as homemakers, mothers, and sex
symbols. For this set of advertisements, women
were portraying communal traits associated with
the female gender role. The same nine gender-
neutral advertisements were used in both sets to
avoid demand characteristics; these advertise-
ments displayed only the product (e.g., a tooth-
brush) and did not display men or women (see
Appendix for sample advertisements).
Simon and Hoyt 237
The results from a one-way MANOVA
revealed a multivariate main effect for images
(Wilks’ λ = .57, F(4, 35) = 6.66, p < .001, η
2
= .43;
see Table 1 for descriptive statistics for depend-
ent variables. Univariate ANOVA tests revealed
that, compared to the stereotypical images, the
counterstereotypical images were rated as lower
on gender stereotypicality (F[1, 38] = 25.25, p <
.001, η
2
= .40), higher on leader attributions (F[1,
38] = 10.89, p = .002, η
2
= .22), higher on agentic
traits (F[1, 38] = 4.76, p = .035, η
2
= .11), and
there was no impact on communal trait ratings,
F(1, 38) = .50, ns.
1
Study 1
While past research has demonstrated that
exposure to advertisements with stereotypical
roles impacts womens gender role beliefs by
limiting their leadership and career aspirations
to goals that are congruent with the female gender
role (Davies et al., 2005; Geis et al., 1984), the pre-
sent study explores whether media images with
gender-counterstereotypical roles could also impact
gender role beliefs, but in the opposite direction.
Thus, we predicted that, compared to exposure to
advertisements in the stereotypical condition,
women exposed to advertisements in the counter-
stereotypical condition would subsequently report
more nontraditional gender role beliefs.
Method
Participants and design. Twenty female
undergraduate students at a liberal arts college
participated in this brief study. As in the
pilot study, Study 1 also employed a two-group
between-subjects design (media images:
counterstereotypical, stereotypical).
Procedure and measures. Participants were
run individually and were randomly assigned to
view one of the two sets of advertisements. The
experimenter instructed participants to pay close
attention to each advertisement, as they would be
asked to recall information later.
Media images manipulation. Participants in the
counterstereotypical condition viewed a set of 10
advertisements with gender-counterstereotypical
roles, and participants in the stereotypical con-
dition viewed a set of magazine advertisements
consisting of 10 advertisements with gender-
stereotypical roles. The same advertisements
were used as in the pilot study.
Attitudes toward women. After viewing the adver-
tisements, participants responded to a shortened,
10-item version of Spence, Helmreich and Stapp’s
(1973) Attitudes Towards Women Scale (ATW;
α = .67) which measures attitudes regarding
appropriate responsibilities, rights, and roles for
women versus men in society (Eagly & Mladinic,
1989; Spence & Hahn, 1997). All 10 items indi-
cate egalitarian attitudes toward women such
as “Women should take increasing responsibil-
ity for leadership in solving the intellectual and
social problems of the day” and “Under modern
economic conditions with women being active
outside the home, men should share in house-
hold tasks such as washing dishes and doing the
laundry.” Participants indicated their agreement
to items on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging
from −3 (strongly disagree) to 3 (strongly agree).
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations for pilot study
Variable M
count
SD
count
M
st
SD
st
1 2 3 4
1. Leader attributes 4.57 .61 3.60 1.16 -.53*** .67*** .34*
2. Gender stereotypical 2.40 .88 3.80 .88 -.45** -.24
3. Agentic traits 4.95 .52 4.48 .81 .48**
4. Communal traits 4.62 .56 4.45 .90
Note: Means and standard deviations for both the counterstereotypical (count) and stereotypical (st) conditions are reported.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
238 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 16(2)
Higher scores on the ATW represent greater
endorsement of profeminist, egalitarian attitudes
toward womens roles.
Results and discussion
To test the prediction that women in the counter-
stereotypical condition would report more coun-
terstereotypical gender role beliefs, participants’
attitudes toward womens roles were analyzed
with a one-way analysis of variance with the
media images manipulation as the independent
variable. Results confirmed that participants in
the counterstereotypical condition endorsed a
more egalitarian attitude toward womens roles
(M = 1.79, SD = .47) than those in the stereotypi-
cal condition (M = 1.22, SD = .73), F(1, 18) =
4.51, p = .048, η
2
= .20.
These results suggest that advertisements that
disconfirm gender stereotypes can alter these
seemingly rigid gender role beliefs—resulting in
more counterstereotypical beliefs about the
female gender role. These findings add to the
body of research demonstrating that situational
cues can alter seemingly rigid gender role beliefs
(e.g., Uchronski, 2008). Moreover, if media images
displaying counterstereotypical roles can put into
effect less stereotypical gender role beliefs (i.e.,
beliefs that make the female gender role more
congruent with the leadership role), they should
also be able to buffer women from negative
stereotypes about women in leadership situations.
Therefore, the purpose of Study 2 is to explore
whether media images with women in counter-
stereotypical roles can also influence womens
responses in actual leadership situations. Because
these advertisements are seemingly able to make
the female gender role more congruent with the
leadership role, Study 2 examines whether they
can also serve to buffer women against negative
stereotype threat effects in a leadership situation.
Study 2
In Study 2, we test whether media images with
counterstereotypical roles have a more positive
effect than stereotypical images do on womens
self-perceptions and leadership aspirations after a
leadership task. Because women perceived these
advertisements as less stereotypic and more con-
gruent with the leadership role (pilot study) and
had more counterstereotypical attitudes toward
women (Study 1), we predicted that advertise-
ments with women in counterstereotypical roles
would buffer women from stereotype threat effects
in the leadership domain. Specifically, we predicted
that women in the counterstereotypical condition
would report less negative self-perceptions and
greater leadership aspirations than women in the
stereotypical condition. Moreover, based on pre-
vious work that highlights negative thoughts and
self-perceptions as the mechanism that causes
impaired performance (Hoyt & Simon, 2011;
Schmader et al., 2009), we tested the prediction
that negative self-perceptions would be the driving
force behind another important outcome of ste-
reotype threat: aspirations to participate in the ste-
reotype-relevant domain.
Method
Participants and design. Sixty undergraduate
women at a private liberal arts college were given
$10.00 for participation. The experiment employed
a two-group between-subjects design (media
images: counterstereotypical, stereotypical).
Procedure. Participants were informed that
they signed up to participate in two ostensibly
separate studies. The experimenter informed the
participants that the first study was examining the
relationship between personality characteristics
and people’s ratings of certain magazine advertise-
ments. For this first study, participants filled out a
set of filler questionnaires purportedly assessing
their personality. Next, participants viewed one
of the two sets of advertisements and were asked
to rank order their top six favorite advertisements
in order to bolster the cover study. Participants
then completed what they believed to be a second,
separate study with two other participants,
who were in reality two research confederates.
Simon and Hoyt 239
The “second study” served as the leadership task
in which the participant was always assigned to the
leadership role for a 10-minute group discussion
task. After the leadership task, participants
completed the dependent measures. Participants
were paid, debriefed, and thanked for their
participation.
2
Media images manipulation. After signing the consent
form, participants completed the first task, described
to them as the first study, which served to expose
them to the media images of women. The same
two sets of magazine advertisements were used in
Study 1, which again included the neutral advertise-
ments to avoid demand characteristics.
Group leadership task. After completing the
advertisement task, participants began what they
believed to be a separate three-person group
study. For the group task, two female research
confederates acted as two other participants,
and the real participant was always “randomly”
selected to be the leader. The goal of the task was
for participants to successfully lead the group to
a unanimous consensus about whether the uni-
versity should keep or eliminate its coordinate
system, a system that consists of separate all-
male and all-female colleges, with distinct tradi-
tions, student governments, and deaneries within
the same university. Confederates were trained to
speak according to a set of scripted statements
and responses so that their behavior remained
as consistent as possible for all experimental
sessions. In order to make the task difficult for
the participant, confederates were instructed to
disagree with the participant’s own opinion about
keeping or eliminating the coordinate system.
3
Measures. Participants responded to the follow-
ing measures using a 7-point Likert-type scale rang-
ing from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Negative self-perceptions. Because this study is
exploring how women feel about themselves
after completing a task in which they were inter-
acting with other people, we created an 11-item
scale assessing social aspects of their self-esteem,
affect, and concern about their performance. The
scale consisted of items adapted from the social
subscale of Heatherton and Polivy’s (1991) state
self-esteem measure (e.g., “I feel inferior to others
at this moment”), from Lubin, Zuckerman, and
Woodward’s (1985) Multiple Affect Adjective
Checklist (MAACL; e.g., “I feel like a failure”),
and from Hoyt and Simons (2011) perfor-
mance difficulty scale (“I felt a lot of pres-
sure during this task”). A factor analysis shows
that all items load highly onto one factor (all
loadings > .60) and the scale is highly reliable
(α = .89).
Leadership aspirations. Participants indicated their
agreement to the following two items assessing
their future leadership aspirations: “I will actively
pursue leadership positions in the future” and
“I would work hard to be selected as leader”
(r(58) = .67, p < .001).
Results
The self-report dependent variables (negative
self-perceptions and leadership aspirations) were
analyzed with a one-way (media images manipula-
tion) multivariate analysis of variance. The two
dependent variables were significantly correlated
(r = −.43, p < .001).
The overall MANOVA revealed a multivariate
main effect for images, Wilks’ λ = .89, F(1, 58) =
3.47, p = .038, η
2
= .11. The univariate ANOVA
with negative self-perceptions as the outcome
revealed a main effect for images such that
participants in the counterstereotypical condition
reported lower levels of negative self-perceptions
after the leadership task (M = 2.82, SD = .92)
than those in the stereotypical condition (M =
3.41, SD = 1.00; F[1, 58] = 5.65, p = .021, η
2
=
.09). The univariate ANOVA with leadership
aspirations as the outcome revealed a main effect
of images such that participants in the counter-
stereotypical condition reported higher levels of
leadership aspirations after the leadership task
(M = 5.18, SD = 1.14) than those in the
240 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 16(2)
stereotypical condition (M = 4.47, SD = 1.58;
F[1, 58] = 4.04, p = .049, η
2
= .07; see Figure 1).
Mediational analysis. We explored whether
negative self-perceptions mediate the impact of
media images on leadership aspirations by con-
ducting mediational analysis. To test for mediation
with this small sample, we used the bootstrapping
approach, instead of the low-power Sobel test, as
advocated by Shrout and Bolger (2002). We used
Preacher and Hayes’ (2008) macro to implement
the bootstrapping approach in SPSS. A bootstrap-
based confidence interval (95%) for the indirect
effect was generated by taking 5,000 samples
from the original data set (using sampling with
replacement) thus yielding 5,000 estimates of
each path coefficient. These estimates were used
to calculate estimates of conditional indirect
effects of media images (dummy coded as 0 =
stereotypical, 1 = counterstereotypical) on leader-
ship aspirations through negative self-perceptions.
The bias-corrected and accelerated confidence
interval for negative self-perceptions was [.054,
.876]. As indicated with the cutoff value in the
lower tail of the bootstrap distribution of condi-
tional indirect effects being above zero, the indi-
rect effect is statistically significant. The indirect
effect remains significant when using the more
stringent 99% confidence interval [.001, 1.10].
The direction of the paths indicate that partici-
pants in the counterstereotypical condition
experienced less negative self-perceptions than
those in the stereotypical condition resulting in
greater levels of leadership aspirations. In sum,
negative self-perceptions mediated the relation-
ship between media images and leadership
aspirations (see Figure 2).
4
Discussion
The results from Study 2 supported our predic-
tions: women in the counterstereotypical condi-
tion were buffered from the negative stereotype
threat effect that women in the stereotypical con-
dition experienced. After performing a leadership
task, women in the counterstereotypical condi-
tion reported lower negative self-perceptions and
greater leadership aspirations than women in the
stereotypical condition. Moreover, negative self-
perceptions mediated the relationship between
media images and leadership aspirations such that
the greater leadership aspirations reported by
women in the counterstereotypical condition
was due in large part to their less negative self-
perceptions after performing the leadership task.
General discussion
The present research sought to explore whether
media images of women in counterstereotypical
roles have a positive effect, compared to images
of women in stereotypical roles, on women in the
leadership domain. Past research has shown that
images of women in stereotypical roles activate
gender stereotypes (Davies, Spencer, Quinn, &
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Negative self-perceptions Leadership aspirations
Counterstereotypical
Stereotypical
Figure 1. Mean scores on self-report measures for
Study 2.
B = −.59*
B = −.55***
Total effect = .72*
Negative self-
perceptions
Indirect effect = .32** Direct effect
=
.39
Leadership
aspirations
Media
images
Figure 2. Negative self-perceptions as a mediator
of the impact of media images on participants’
leadership aspirations.
Note: Direct, indirect, and total effects are quantified with
unstandardized regression weights;
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
Simon and Hoyt 241
Gerhardstein, 2002; Yoder et al., 2008) and result
in negative self-report effects for women in the
leadership domain (Davies et al., 2002). However,
research showing the positive effects of counter-
stereotypical role models on womens leadership
aspirations and weakening of gender stereotypes
(Dasgupta & Asgari, 2004) led us to hypothesize
that counterstereotypical images might also be
able to buffer women from negative stereotype
threat effects in the leadership domain. The find-
ings from the present research demonstrate that
media images of women in counterstereotypical
roles can have a positive effect on womens
responses in the leadership domain.
After demonstrating that advertisements of
women in counterstereotypical roles were per-
ceived as less stereotypical of the female gender
role and more congruent with the leadership role
(pilot study), we then conducted two studies
using the same set of advertisements to explore
the effect of these images in relation to womens
gender role attitudes (Study 1) and their self-
perceptions and leadership aspirations following
a leadership task (Study 2). The findings from
Study 1 supported the prediction that women
who viewed media images of women in counter-
stereotypical roles subsequently reported more
nontraditional, egalitarian gender role beliefs
about women than women who viewed media
images of women in stereotypical roles.
From here, our reasoning followed that if the
images could alter womens gender role beliefs,
then viewing media images of women in counter-
stereotypical roles before performing in a non-
traditional female role in which women are
negatively stereotyped (i.e., leadership) would
buffer against negative stereotype threat effects.
The findings from Study 2 supported our predic-
tion. Female participants who viewed images of
women in counterstereotypical roles as a part of
an ostensibly separate study then reported lower
negative self-perceptions and greater leadership
aspirations after performing a leadership task than
participants who viewed images of women in
stereo typical roles.
The present research has important implica-
tions for helping to increase womens presence in
top-level leadership positions. The findings from
this research suggest that exposure to media
images of women in counterstereotypical roles can
help to break rigid gender stereotypes and gender
role beliefs. Consequently, these images can help to
diminish the perceived incongruency between
women and leadership and buffer women against
negative responses in leadership situations. This
research builds on prior research related to
counter stereotypical role models by demonstrating
that contact with a counterstereotypical or non-
traditional woman is not requisite to alter gender
stereotypes and gender role beliefs; mere exposure
to media images with counterstereotypical roles
can have a meaningful impact of womens leader-
ship self-perceptions. Considering the pervasive-
ness of media images, if more media images of
women in counterstereotypical roles were utilized,
women from a young age may develop more
flexible gender role beliefs that incorporate stereo-
typical masculine traits (or agentic traits) and roles
such as leadership.
Limitations and future
directions
While the current research shows the impact that
the media have on leadership aspirations, it will
be important for future research to also examine
if viewing advertisements of women in counter-
stereotypical roles helps to boost actual leader-
ship performance. In Study 2, participants did
complete a leadership task; however, this particu-
lar task was not designed to assess an objective
measure of performance. Thus, future research
would benefit from using an established leader-
ship task used to assess performance. Despite the
lack of performance data in Study 2, the present
research, nonetheless, adds to the literature by
expanding the type of dependent measures
assessed in stereotype threat research. As noted
by Davies et al. (2005), Steele (1997; Steele &
Aronson, 1995) originally described two impor-
tant consequences of stereotype threat: under-
performance and decreased aspirations. However,
the research that examines effects other than
242 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 16(2)
performance still remain in the minority (for
examples of exceptions, see Davies et al., 2002;
Davies et al., 2005; Murphy, Steele, & Gross,
2007).
Moreover, the present study extends stereotype
threat research that does examine leadership aspira-
tions by exploring the mediational role of negative
self-perceptions, another important consequence
to consider. While negative, intrusive thoughts
(Cadinu et al., 2005; Schmader et al., 2009) have
been explored as mediators of performance in
stereo type threat research, the present research
explored whether negative self-perceptions also
result in deflated aspirations. Having a nuanced
understanding of multiple self-report assessments
will not only help to better understand stigmatized
individuals’ experiences in targeted domains, but
will also help to develop effective interventions.
Furthermore, the present research expands our
understanding of self-reported consequences of
stereotype threat because, unlike past research
examining aspirations, participants in the present
research actually performed a task in the targeted
domain at hand prior to filling out dependent
variables.
Another limitation of this research is the use
of a two-group design (stereotypical vs. counter-
stereotypical). Exploring another comparison
that exposed women to neutral images would
help provide a more detailed examination of how
images of women in both stereotypical and coun-
terstereotypical roles affect women in leadership
situations. Nonetheless, we chose to utilize a two-
group design because advertisements that portray
women in stereotypical ways are the norm in the
U.S. Therefore, the stereotypical condition is an
apt comparison condition to compare how things
are to how things could be (i.e., our experimental
condition in which we manipulate the advertise-
ments to display women in a nontypical manner).
While a neutral condition would certainly be better
suited to demonstrate whether advertisements
with stereotypical roles are detrimental and/or
whether advertisements with counterstereotypical
roles are beneficial, past research speaks to these
points (Davies et al., 2005, the former; Dasgupta
& Asgari, 2004, the latter). Moreover, the
purpose of the present research was to examine
the media’s portrayal of women. Therefore, a
condition in which advertisements do not display
people is not an appropriate comparison for this
particular research. Lastly, despite this limitation,
this research does help to answer an important
question in its own right—by exploring whether
advertisements of women in counter stereotypical
roles would have the same detrimental effect that
media images of women in stereotypical roles have
(in which case a null effect would have emerged).
Thus, the present research does provide novel sup-
port for the positive effect of media images that
depict women in counterstereotypical roles for
women in leadership situations.
Conclusion
The present research explored the effect of
media images of women in counterstereotypical
versus stereotypical roles on female participants’
gender role beliefs (Study 1) and responses in a
leadership situation (Study 2). Overall, the find-
ings offer a positive perspective on the potential for
media images of women in counterstereotypical
roles to help increase womens leadership aspira-
tions, and eventually closing the gender gap that
currently exists between men and women in top-
level leadership positions. While women may in
fact hold a leadership advantage (Eagly et al.,
2003), they remain underrepresented in powerful
leadership positions. Thus, these findings not
only offer hope for achieving equal representa-
tion in leadership positions, but they also offer
insight for how to promote more successful
societal institutions, businesses, and governments.
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge and thank Audrey Innella, Yelena
Johnson, and Henrietta Matheson for their excellent
work as research assistants on this project. We would
also like to thank Laurie T. O’Brien for her helpful
comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Notes
1. That the increase in perceived agency is not
accompanied with a commensurate decrease in
Simon and Hoyt 243
perceived communality supports the arguments
that agency and communality are orthogonal
constructs, and it is perceptions of agency that are
directly related to images depicting traditionally
masculine roles. Past research has similarly
demons trated that participants’ implicit theories
about leadership attributes are malleable on agen-
tic traits, but not on communal traits (Hoyt, Simon,
& Innella, 2011).
2. None of the participants mentioned suspicion
about the confederates.
3. Although the goal of the present research was not to
assess performance differences, for exploratory
purposes, two independent coders coded the audio-
recordings from each session and rated participants’
on several items devised to assess performance,
including whether the group reached a consensus,
and general leadership skills, such as oral communica-
tion, motivational ability, and confidence. While the
coders had adequate reliability, we found that partici-
pants did not differ on their rated leadership perfor-
mance based on condition. Please see the Discussion
section for further discussion regarding performance.
4. Although not predicted from theory, we tested a
reverse mediational model, with leadership aspirations
as the mediator predicting negative self-perceptions,
which might also explain the data. The 99%
bootstrap-based confidence interval was not
significant. Thus, the reverse model is neither
theoretically nor empirically supported.
References
Asgari, S., Dasgupta, N., & Stout, J. G. (2012). When do
counterstereotypic ingroup members inspire ver-
sus deflate? The effect of successful professional
women on young women’s leadership self-concept.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 370–383.
doi:10.1177/0146167211431968
Beilock, S. L., Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R.
(2007). Stereotype threat and working memory:
Mechanisms, alleviation, and spillover. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 256–276. doi:
10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.256
Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Rosabianca, A., & Kiesner, J.
(2005). Why do women underperform under ste-
reotype threat? Evidence for the role of nega-
tive thinking. Psychological Science, 16, 572–578. doi:
10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01577.x
Catalyst. (2011). U.S. women in business. Retrieved from
http://www.catalyst.org/publication/132/us-
women-in-business
Center for American Women and Politics. (2012).
Women in elective office 2009. Retrieved from http://
www.cawp.rutgers.edu/fast_facts/levels_of_
office/Congress-CurrentFacts.php
Clément-Guillotin, C., & Fontayne, P. (2011). Situational
malleability of gender schema: The case of the
competitive sport context. Sex Roles, 64, 426–439.
doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9912-1
Coltrane, S., & Adams, M. (1997). Work–family
imagery and gender stereotypes: Television and
the reproduction of difference. Journal of Vocational
Behavior, 50, 323–347. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575
Dasgupta, N., & Asgari, S. (2004). Seeing is believing:
Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and
its effect on the malleability of automatic gender
stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
40, 642–658. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.02.003
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D. M., &
Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How
television commercials that elicit stereotype threat
can restrain women academically and professionally.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2, 1615–1628.
doi: 10.1177/014616702237644
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005).
Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates the
effects of stereotype threat on womens leadership
aspirations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
88, 276–287. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.276
Davis, S. N. (2003). Sex stereotypes in commercials
targeted toward children: A content analysis.
Sociological Spectrum, 23, 407–424. doi: 10.1080/
02732170309220
Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into
context: An interactive model of gender-related
behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389. doi:
10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.369
Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A
social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (2007). Through the labyrinth:
The truth about how women become leaders. Boston, MA:
Harvard Business School Press.
Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & van
Engen, M. (2003). Transformational, transactional,
and laissez-faire leadership styles: A meta-analysis
comparing women and men. Psychological Bulletin,
129, 569–591. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.569
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory
of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review,
109, 573–598. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573
Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1989). Gender ste-
reotypes and attitudes toward women and men.
244 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 16(2)
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 543–558.
doi: 10.1177/0146167289154008
Ganahl, D. J., Prinsen, T. J., & Netzley, S. (2003). A con-
tent analysis of prime time commercials: A contex-
tual framework of gender representation. Sex Roles,
49, 545–551. doi: 10.1023/A:1025893025658
Geis, F. L., Brown, V., Walstedt, J. J., & Porter, N. (1984).
TV commercials as achievement scripts for women.
Sex Roles, 10, 513–525. doi: 10.1007/BF00287260
Goffman, E. (1976). Gender advertisements. New York,
NY: Harper & Row.
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development
and validation of a scale for measuring state self-
esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
895–910. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.895
Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription:
How gender stereotypes prevent womens ascent
up the organizational ladder. Journal of Social Issues,
57, 657–674. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00234
Hoyt, C. L. (2010a). Women and leadership. In P. Northouse
(Ed.), Leadership: Theory and practice (5
th
ed., pp.
301–333). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hoyt, C. L. (2010b). Women, men, and leadership:
Exploring the gender gap at the top. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 484–498. doi: 10.1111/
j.17519004.2010.00274.x
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2007). Leadership effi-
cacy and women leaders’ responses to stereotype
activation. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10,
595–616. doi: 10.1177/1368430207084718
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2010). The role of leader-
ship self-efficacy and stereotype activation on car-
diovascular, behavioral and self-report responses in
the leadership domain. The Leadership Quarterly, 21,
89–103. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.10.007
Hoyt, C. L., Johnson, S., Murphy, S., & Skinnell, K.
(2010). The impact of blatant stereotype activa-
tion and group sex-composition on female leaders.
Leadership Quarterly, 21, 716–732. doi: 10.1016/j.lea-
qua.2010.07.003
Hoyt, C. L., & Simon, S. (2011). Female leaders:
Injurious or inspiring role models for women?
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 35, 143–157.doi:
10.1177/0361684310385216
Hoyt, C. L., Simon, S., & Innella, A. (2011). Taking a
turn toward the masculine: The impact of mortal-
ity salience on implicit leadership theories. Basic
and Applied Social Psychology, 33, 374–381. doi:
10.1080/01973533.2011.614173
Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., &
Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes mascu-
line? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms.
Psychological Bulletin, 137, 616–642. doi: 10.1037/
a0023557
Lubin, B., Zuckerman, M., & Woodward, L. (1985).
Bibliography for the Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist.
San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing
Service.
Miller, J., Lewy, J., & Peckham, E. (1997). Context
effects on self-perceptions of feminine and mascu-
line qualities. Sex Roles, 37, 723–751. doi: 10.1007/
BF02936337
Murphy, M. C., Steele, C., Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling
threat: How situational cues affect women in
math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological
Science, 18, 879–885. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.
01995.x
Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). Prejudice
toward female leaders: Backlash effects and
womens impression management dilemma. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 807–820. doi:
10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00306.x
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and
resampling strategies for assessing and compar-
ing indirect effects in multiple mediator models.
Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/
BRM.40.3.879
Reichert, T. (2008). Issues in American advertising: Media,
society, and a changing world (2
nd
ed.). Chicago, IL:
Copy Workshop.
Reichert, T., & Carpenter, C. (2004). An update on sex
in magazine advertising: 1983 to 2003. Journalism &
Mass Communication Quarterly, 81, 823–837.
Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status, and leader-
ship. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 637–655. doi:
10.1111/0022-4537.00233
Rounder, D., Slater, M. D., & Domenech-Rodríguez, M.
(2003). Adolescent evaluation of gender role and
sexual imagery in television advertisements. Journal
of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47, 435–454.
doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4703_7
Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2010). The effect of
priming gender roles on womens implicit gender
beliefs and career aspirations. Social Psychology, 41,
192–202. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000027
Schein, V. E. (2001). A global look at psychological barriers
to womens progress in management. Journal of Social
Issues, 57, 675–688. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00235
Schmader, T., & Croft, A. (2011). How stereotypes
stifle performance potential. Social and Personality
Psychology Compass, 5, 792–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-
9004.2011.00390.x
Simon and Hoyt 245
Schmader, T., Forbes, C. E., Zhang, S., & Mendes,
W. (2009). A metacognitive perspective on the
cognitive deficits experienced in intellectually
threatening environments. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 35, 584–596. doi: 10.1177/
0146167208330450
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An inte-
grated process model of stereotype threat effects
on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336–356.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experi-
mental and nonexperimental studies: New proce-
dures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7,
422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422
Smith, C. J., Noll, J. A., & Bryant, J. (1999). The effect
of social context on gender self-concept. Sex Roles,
40, 499–512. doi: 10.1023/A:1018879811991
Spence, J. T., & Hahn, E. D. (1997). The Attitudes
Toward Women Scale and attitude change in college
students. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 17–34. doi:
10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00098.x
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1973). A
short version of the Attitudes Toward Women
Scale (AWS). Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2,
219–220.
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999).
Stereotype threat and women’s math performance.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28. doi:
10.1006/jesp.1998.1373
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereo-
types shape intellectual identity and performance.
American Psychologist, 52, 613–629. doi: 10.1037/
0003-066X.52.6.613
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat
and the intellectual test performance of African
Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
69, 797–811. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
STEMconnector. (2012). Research on gender in science and
engineering (NSF). Retrieved from http://stemcon-
nector.org/genderresearchNSF
Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson,
M. J. (2001). Everyday sexism: Evidence for its inci-
dence, nature, and psychological impact from three
daily diary studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 31–53.
doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00200
Uchronski, M. (2008). Agency and communion in
spontaneous self-descriptions: Occurrence and
situational malleability. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 38, 1093–1102. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.563
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2011). Labor force sta-
tistics from the current population survey. Retrieved from
http://www.bls.gov/cps/cpsaat09.htm
U.S. National Center for Education Statistics. (2009).
Digest of education statistics [Bachelor’s, master’s, and
doctor’s degrees conferred by degree-granting insti-
tutions, by sex of student and discipline division:
2008–09, Table 286]. Retrieved from http://nces.
ed.gov/programs/digest/d10/tables/dt10_286.asp
Yoder, J. D., Christopher, J., & Holmes, J. D. (2008). Are
television commercials still achievement scripts for
women? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 303–311.
doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00438.x
... Another effect of the stereotype threat is related to the aspiration and intention for leadership roles. Experimental studies showed women who were exposed to stereotypic images stated lower preference for a leadership role relative to women who were exposed to neutral (Walsh et al., 2022) or counterstereotypical images (Simon & Hoyt, 2013). ...
... Finally, in both studies researchers measured affective reactions after rather than before performance, making it impossible to separate these affective reactions from performance effects. In another study, Simon and Hoyt (2013) found that women exposed to stereotypic gender roles (i.e., implicit threat) had lower social self-esteem, positive affect, and higher concerns about performance than those presented counterstereotypic images. However, providing counterstereotypic images instead of neutral images created an extreme comparison in that study. ...
... Manipulation Material. Print advertisements were used to manipulate stereotype threat in the current study, consistent with prior research (Prasad, 2011;Simon & Hoyt, 2013). Previous studies used only neutral advertisements in the control condition and both neutral and feminine advertisements in the experimental condition. ...
Article
This paper aims to investigate the role of stereotype threat and the moderating role of gender stigma consciousness on women’s leadership aspiration, leadership career goal, social self-esteem, and negative affect across two experimental studies in Türkiye. We expected the detrimental effects of streotype threat to be experienced by those with high gender stigma consciousness. The first study, involving 130 female undergraduates (M age = 20.7, SD = 4.4), presented implicit stereotype threat and showed that the threat increased the interest of team membership and women low in stigma consciousness reported higher leadership career goals than those high in stigma consciousness. The second study, conducted with 90 female undergraduates (M age = 20.6, SD = 1.6), presented explicit stereotype threat and showed that the explicit threat had negative effect on leadership aspiration, and women high in stigma consciousness felt more negative affect and less social self-esteem due to threat than those who were low. The present research contributes to the women’s leadership literature by identifying for the first time the role of stigma consciousness in the motivational and affective consequences of stereotype threat.
... Consequently, rather than simply mirroring existing social realities, discursive practises can contribute to creating new ones (Caputo, 2013). For instance, research indicates that exposure to media portraying women in leadership positions, thereby challenging conventional gender stereotypes, seems to result into individuals' internalisation of stronger nontraditional gender role beliefs (Sheerin & Garavan, 2022;Simon & Hoyt, 2013). Thus, by presenting an alternative narrative, the media can play a pivotal role in shifting cultural norms and fostering, in this case, a more egalitarian society (Sheerin & Garavan, 2022;Simon & Hoyt, 2013). ...
... For instance, research indicates that exposure to media portraying women in leadership positions, thereby challenging conventional gender stereotypes, seems to result into individuals' internalisation of stronger nontraditional gender role beliefs (Sheerin & Garavan, 2022;Simon & Hoyt, 2013). Thus, by presenting an alternative narrative, the media can play a pivotal role in shifting cultural norms and fostering, in this case, a more egalitarian society (Sheerin & Garavan, 2022;Simon & Hoyt, 2013). Considering the power of the press to influence the beliefs of the general population, researchers have started investigating the language that the press media use to portray specific topics and social groups (de Vreese et al., 2011;Silveirinha, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Media framing encompasses the intentional curation and arrangement of relevant information, revolving around a central theme, to fashion a unified storyline. This article aims to explore how the news media frame women who have experienced intimate partner violence (IPV). By understanding these perceptions, it is possible to gain insight into societal attitudes and biases, shaping how IPV is perceived, discussed, and addressed within our communities. Methods The review was pre-registered on the PROSPERO database of systematic reviews (registration number: CRD42022347911). Moreover, the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA statement) was followed. A total of 17 articles were selected from 8158 search results across four databases: PubMed, PsycINFO, Scopus, and Web of Science. Data collection was conducted in 2022. Results The 17 articles were divided into two groups: the media framing of women with an IPV experience and IPV-related femicides. Episodic framing was the most used frame in both groups. Moreover, a tendency to minimise the violence that occurred, to use inappropriate language, to blame the women, and to confound violence with love and sex resulted in both groups. Conclusions News media tend to depict women who experienced IPV in a stigmatising way, but the review highlighted that media could make a positive impact by increasing public awareness and promoting more positive portrayals. Policy Implications The review offers recommendations to develop policies and practises that can train media professionals to avoid perpetuating stereotypical images of women who experienced violence and report in a responsible and ethical ways.
... For example, clinicians working with Asian American men may consider fostering conversation related to the impacts of these factors if Asian American men report distress in regard to dating or romance. This study also highlights the necessity of structural interventions (e.g., diversification of representation in media, programs that prevent interpersonal forms of racism against Asian American men) that may help prevent internalized racism from manifesting within Asian American men in the first place (Appel & Weber, 2021;Boone Blanchard et al., 2021;Simon & Hoyt, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Past studies indicate that sociocultural factors may affect who Asian American adults wish to date. To expand upon this burgeoning field of research, the present study examined how internalized racism and resistance and empowerment against racism are associated with Asian American men’s preferences to date four different racial groups of women: Asian women, Black women, Latina women, and White women. The study included a sample of 244 Asian American men living in the United States who completed a cross-sectional online survey. Results showed that internalized racism was negatively associated with a dating preference toward Asian and Black women, whereas greater resistance and empowerment against racism was positively associated with a dating preference toward Asian, Black, Latina, and White women. Paired-samples t tests also revealed that participants were more likely to prefer dating Asian women compared to Black, Latina, and White women; more likely to prefer dating White women compared to Black and Latina women; and more likely to prefer dating Latina women compared to Black women. The present study’s results emphasize the importance of taking into account how sociocultural factors such as internalized racism and resistance and empowerment against racism are associated with Asian American men’s racial dating preferences. Implications for interventionists and practitioners working with Asian American men are discussed.
Chapter
To grasp the experiences of Lone Mother Entrepreneurs (LME), it’s crucial to identify the main factors that shape their entrepreneurial journeys. Traditionally, women were regarded as holding a lower status compared to men, often facing stereotyping and discrimination fuelled by biased attitudes, which in turn diminished their self-perception. This chapter explores and helps to understand two central questions: Why do lone mothers pursue entrepreneurship in the UK, and how do they leverage their unique circumstances to enhance their self-worth through their personal approaches to leadership?
Chapter
It examines persistent gender disparities in various employment sectors, highlighting need for equitable opportunities & representation. Despite societal advancements, women remain underrepresented in leadership roles & many industries, while overrepresented in undervalued & low-paying jobs. It explores how gender biases, stereotypes, & unsupportive workplace cultures limit women's career progression. These challenges are compounded by a lack of female role models & supportive networks, leading to limited upward mobility & continued marginalization in many sectors. It emphasizes collective impact of these disparities, where women are systematically controlled in influential positions. It discusses role of media in perpetuating gender stereotypes, that further threatens efforts to achieve true diversity & inclusion. It concludes by acknowledging ongoing advocacy & initiatives aimed at promoting gender diversity, equity, & inclusion while stressing the need for sustained efforts to dismantle structural barriers that prevent women from reaching their full potential across all sectors.
Article
This study investigated how situational negative stereotypical expectations about men or women affected followers’ evaluations of their male or female leaders representing their teams in an inter-team context. Undergraduate students enrolled in a leadership course participated in two experiments for experiential learning as part of non-graded in-class course activity. The study used a cross-sectional experimental design with gender stereotype condition (experiment 1: advantage or disadvantage; experiment 2: advantage or control) and leader gender (male or female) as independent variables and followers’ ratings for their team-leaders as the dependent variable. The teams were randomly assigned to the conditions. Prior to the leaders’ performances (experiment 1: giving an oral presentation; experiment 2: participating in a committee meeting), the followers read stereotype manipulations targeting the gender of their leader and then rated their leaders’ communication effectiveness (experiment 1) or representation effectiveness (experiment 2). Followers evaluated male leaders more favorably when males were expected to underperform females, relative to a male stereotype advantage (experiment 1, d = .90) or control condition (experiment 2, d = .87). The ratings for female leaders were not different between the experimental and the comparative conditions. Negative stereotyping seemed to mobilize followers to show solidarity with male leaders, but there was a lack of vigor to show support for female leaders. The study enhances the criterion validity of the stereotype threat construct in leadership by extending outcomes to leader–follower relations, using stereotype salience manipulations. To mitigate consequences of the situational salience of stereotypes, interventions should focus on enhancing follower support for female leaders.
Article
Purpose This study investigates the relationship between child gender and a CEO’s top management hiring decisions. Design/methodology/approach Hypotheses were tested using secondary data on 121 S&P 500 male CEOs, their children, and their top management teams. Findings Results indicate that child gender is associated with a male CEO’s TMT hiring decisions. Specifically, we find that male CEOs with only daughters were significantly more likely to hire women to their TMTs than male CEOs with only sons and those with both sons and daughters. Practical implications This study provides evidence for the roles of familiarity, learning, and empathy in reducing gender biases in selection decisions. Top management hiring decisions have wide implications for organizational settings in general and for the breaking of the glass ceiling in particular. Social implications Reducing gender bias in top manager hiring decisions directly relates to the United Nations’ Sustainable Development Goal 5 of achieving gender equality as women are consistently under-represented at the top of organizations across the world. Originality/value By focusing on the hiring of top managers, this study includes hiring decisions that directly impact firm operations. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the relationship between child gender and executive hiring decisions with a US S&P 500 sample.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study is to adapt the “Leadership Aspiration” scale developed by Fritz and Knippenberg (2018) into Turkish. The study consists of three stages. In the first stage, the scale was translated into Turkish by experts through a two-way translation process. In the second stage, to test language equivalence and reveal semantic, conceptual, and normative equivalence, the scale was applied to a primary study group of 43 employees in both English and Turkish at two-week intervals. It was concluded that the original form and the Turkish version were equivalent. In the third stage, the scale was applied to a secondary study group of 1011 white-collar employees. Validity and reliability of Turkish form was tested using Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analyses. The findings showed that Turkish form of the scale, consisting of two dimensions and 15 items, is an appropriate measure of Leadership Aspiration.
Article
Full-text available
A role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders proposes that perceived incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles leads to 2 forms of prejudice: (a) perceiving women less favorably than men as potential occupants of leadership roles and (b) evaluating behavior that fulfills the prescriptions of a leader role less favorably when it is enacted by a woman. One consequence is that attitudes are less positive toward female than male leaders and potential leaders. Other consequences are that it is more difficult for women to become leaders and to achieve success in leadership roles. Evidence from varied research paradigms substantiates that these consequences occur, especially in situations that heighten perceptions of incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles.
Article
Full-text available
To determine cohort changes in gender-role attitudes, responses to the 15-item form of the Attitudes Toward Women Scale (AWS; Spence & Helmreich, 1972a, 1978) were compared for students at the same university tested in 1972, 1976, 1980, and 1992. In both males and females, members of the 1992 cohort were the most egalitarian, and members of the 1972 cohort were the least egalitarian. In all groups, women were significantly less traditional in their attitudes than men. As has been found in previous studies, detailed analyses of the data from the 1992 cohort revealed that the scale was unifactorial, but that the score distributions were skewed. There was also some indication of ceiling effects at the egalitarian end of the scale, particularly in women. The implications of these latter results for the usefulness of the AWS in current research were explored.
Article
Full-text available
In academic and organizational domains, performance measures are often used to assess achievement or aptitude. When certain groups of people systematically underperform on such measures, a common interpretation is that the groups differ in inherent ability. However, social psychological research over the past 15 years has documented a phenomenon called stereotype threat whereby subtle situational reminders of negative stereotypes can stifle the performance of those who are targeted by them. In this article, we review research aimed at understanding the sequence of cognitive and affective processes that underlie these situationally-induced performance impairments. We review evidence that being the target of negative stereotypes cues self-uncertainty and a physiological stress response, engages more explicit monitoring of one’s performance, and efforts to regulate unwanted negative thoughts and feelings. Alone or in concert, these extra-task processes hijack cognitive resources needed for successful performance. Armed with the knowledge of these mediating mechanisms, we then review evidence from both field and laboratory based research demonstrating that gender and racial gaps in achievement can be alleviated if not eliminated through creative and often subtle interventions that diffuse the pernicious effects that stereotypes can have.
Article
This review article posits that the scarcity of women at the upper levels of organizations is a consequence of gender bias in evaluations. It is proposed that gender stereotypes and the expectations they produce about both what women are like (descriptive) and how they should behave (prescriptive) can result in devaluation of their performance, denial of credit to them for their successes, or their penalization for being competent. The processes giving rise to these outcomes are explored, and the procedures that are likely to encourage them are identified. Because of gender bias and the way in which it influences evaluations in work settings, it is argued that being competent does not ensure that a woman will advance to the same organizational level as an equivalently performing man.
Article
For this content analysis we recorded a sample of 1,337 prime time commercials from the 3 major networks (ABC, CBS, and NBC) in 1998. There were 5,473 primary and secondary characters identified. Each character was coded for gender, age, acting role, and product being advertised. The findings were then compared to Bretl and Cantor (1988), the U.S. Census Bureau's 2000 population statistics, and Mediamark Research and Simmons syndicated marketing services. The commercials' producers cast their female and male characters much the same way as was done in the 1980s. Although women make most purchases of goods and services, they are still underrepresented as primary characters during most prime time commercials except for health and beauty products. Women are still cast as younger, supportive counterparts to men, and older women are still the most underrepresented group. Television commercials perpetuate traditional stereotypes of women and men.
Article
The impact of female role models on women’s leadership aspirations and self-perceptions after a leadership task were assessed across two laboratory studies. These studies tested the prediction that upward social comparisons to high-level female leaders will have a relatively detrimental impact on women’s self-perceptions and leadership aspirations compared to male and less elite female leaders. In Study 1 (N = 60), women were presented with both female and male leaders before serving as leaders of ostensible three-person groups in an immersive virtual environment. This study established the relatively deflating impact of high-level female leaders, compared to high-level male leaders and the control condition, on participants' self-perceptions. Using a similar methodology, Study 2 (N = 57) further demonstrated that the impact of elite female leaders on participants' self-perceptions in turn adversely affected their leadership aspirations. This study also showed more positive responses to nonelite female leaders with whom participants more strongly identify and who increase counterstereotypic thinking. Taken together, these studies point to a potential dark side of elite female leaders as role models in a domain where individuals are possible targets of a negative stereotype. However, they also point to the relatively more beneficial impact of female role models who disconfirm the negative stereotype.
Article
The present research investigates the influence of subtle death-related thoughts (i.e., mortality salience) on people's images of effective leaders (i.e., their implicit leadership theories [ILTs]). We test the prediction that mortality salience will change the content of these implicit theories to be more gender stereotypical such that individuals will conceive of effective leaders in a significantly more masculine, or agentic, manner. To test this prediction, we assessed the communal and agentic components of participants' ILTs after they were presented with a mortality salience or control manipulation. Results show that priming individuals to think about their mortality with two open-ended questions resulted in a significant shift in their ILTs such that an effective leader is described in significantly more agentic terms compared to the control condition. This masculine shift in people's ILTs was demonstrated in both women and men, and mortality salience did not influence perceptions of effective leaders' communal traits. This work contributes to research on gender bias in leadership, ILTs, and terror management theory and has implications for female leaders.
Article
In this study we re-examine the role of genderwithin the self-concept and challenge the assumptionthat our gender self-concept is static and consistentacross contexts. We used the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) to measure masculinity and femininityacross six contexts. These six contexts were interactingwith same sex friends, interacting with other sexfriends, interacting at home, work, and school, and interacting in a social context where one doesnot know many people. Two hundred twenty-three femalesand 52 males from a large public university in theSoutheast participated in the study. A majority of the participants (76%) were Caucasian with 10%being Hispanic, 4% Asian, and 4% African American.Multivariate analyses of variance provided strongevidence for differences across contexts for both males and females. The results indicated that we aredynamic beings and those characteristics associated withgender are dynamic as well.