ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Research relating procrastination and mental health suggests that poor adjustment may be explained directly by the stress resulted from worry/anxiety over the delay, and indirectly by the tendency to put off important coping behaviors. In the present study, participants (80 women, 24 men; age range = 18-33 years, M age = 21.12 years old, SD = 2.03) completed Spanish versions of a measure of coping behaviours related to mental health and a procrastination inventory evaluating the tendency to avoid starting or completing tasks across a variety of everyday situations. Results indicated that procrastinators compared to non-procrastinators reported significantly lower positive actions and expression feelings/needs. Significant predictors of procrastination by students were low positive actions, expressing feelings, and assertiveness. Implications are considered relevant to student personality and development.
Individual Differences Research www.idr-journal.com
201x, Vol. x, No. x , pp. xx-xx ISSN: 1541-745X
© 201x Individual Differences Association, Inc.
1
Procrastination and Mental Health Coping:
A Brief Report Related to Students
Joseph Ferrari*
DePaul University
Juan Francisco Díaz-Morales
University of Madrid
*Joseph Ferrari; DePaul University; Psychology Department, 2219 North Kenmore Ave;
Chicago IL 60619. jferrari@depaul.edu (email).
ABSTRACT - Research relating procrastination and mental health suggests that poor adjustment
may be explained directly by the stress resulted from worry/anxiety over the delay, and indirectly
by the tendency to put off important coping behaviors. In the present study, participants (80
women, 24 men; age range = 18-33 years, M age = 21.12 years old, SD = 2.03) completed
Spanish versions of a measure of coping behaviors related to mental health and a procrastination
inventory evaluating the tendency to avoid starting or completing tasks across a variety of
everyday situations. Results indicated that procrastinators compared to non-procrastinators
reported significantly lower positive actions and expression feelings/needs. Significant predictors
of procrastination by students were low positive actions, expressing feelings, and assertiveness.
Implications are considered relevant to student personality and development.
Chronic procrastination, the purposive and frequent delay in beginning or completing
a task to the point of experiencing subjective discomfort (Ferrari, 2010), is estimated at
20-25% among adult men and women living around the world (cf. Ferrari, Díaz-Morales,
O’Callaghan, Díaz, & Argumedo, 2007). It is associated with higher levels of stress and
anxiety, weak impulse control, lack of persistence, lack of work discipline, lack of time
management skill, and the inability to work methodically (see Díaz-Morales, Ferrari,
Díaz, & Argumedo, 2006; Ferrari & Diaz-Morales, 2007a, 2007b; Schouwenburg, Lay,
Pychyl, & Ferrari, 2004; Sirois, 2006 ). Examining effective coping behaviors, several
studies supported links between procrastination and an inability to regulate negative
emotions, ego-depletion (Ferrari & Pychyl, 2007), speed-accuracy tradeoffs (Ferrari,
2001), or health-related goals (Tice & Bratslavsky, 2000). A variety of studies also
suggest that procrastination is linked to negative mental health outcomes with greater
stress and many symptoms of physical illness (i.e. number of symptoms, visits health-
care professionals, and amount of stress in the past week; see Ferrari, 2010; Ferrari,
Johnson, & McCown, 1995).
Sirois (2006) proposed that procrastination is a behavioral style affecting physical and
mental health, mediated through a behavioral pathway: poor health of procrastinators
may be explained directly by the stress resulting from procrastination, failing to cope
Ferrari... / Individual Differences Research, 201x, Vol. x, No. x, pp. xx-xx
2
effectively, and indirectly by the tendency to put off important health behaviors. The
current brief research extended the known relationships between coping and chronic
procrastination by exploring the underlying structure of frequent delays related to mental
health behaviours among emerging adults (i.e., undergraduates). We expected
procrastination tendencies to be related to mental health coping techniques such as
positive actions, controllability, expression of feelings and needs assertiveness, and well-
being seeking.
Method
Participants
Participants were 104 psychology students (80 women, 24 men; M age= 21.12 years
old, SD = 2.03) at a large, public, urban university in Spain.
Psychometric Scales and Procedure
All participants volunteered to complete, in counterbalanced order, within 20
minutes, both reliable and valid psychometric measures. One measure was McCown and
Johnson’s (1989) 15-item, uni-dimensional Adult Inventory of Procrastination (AIP; see
Ferrari et al., 1995, for psychometrics), assessing the tendency to avoid starting or
completing tasks across a variety of everyday situations. Sample items include I don’t
get things done on time.” The Spanish version-AIP (Díaz-Morales et al., 2006) had good
internal consistency (original authors
α
= 0.83; current
α
= 0.82). This inventory was
back-translated for accuracy from English to Spanish by Diaz-Morales et al. (2006) and
used effectively in previous studies.
The other measure translated effectively in Spanish by Grossarth-Maticek and
Eysenck (1995) to assess health-related coping behaviors was the 25-item Self-Regulation
Inventory-Short (SRI-S) by Marqués, Ibáñez, Ruipérez, Moya, and Ortet (2005). This
self-report coping measure has five factors that examine effective coping skills for life
problems, namely: positive actions, solving problems and facilitating happiness (original
author’s
α
= 0.79), controllability, internal control attributions (original author’s
α
=
0.68), expression of feelings and needs, identification and expression of needs, wishes
and feelings (original authors
α
= 0.72), assertiveness, autonomy and self-confidence
(original author’s
α
= 0.69), and well-being seeking, satisfaction with oneself and others
(original author’s
α
= 0.70). A total SRI-S score, across all 25-items, also was calculated
for analysis (original author’s
α
= 0.84).
Results and Discussion
Table 1 presents the mean sum score for each subscale. As noted from the table,
zero-order correlates found chronic procrastination was negatively related to positive
actions, expression feelings/needs factors, and total SRI-S scores. No significant
differences, however, emerged on well-being seeking, assertiveness, and controllability.
Also, in order to analyze the relative contribution of SRI-S’s subscales in predicting
procrastination, a multiple regression analysis was performed. Consistent with the bi-
variate analysis noted in Table 1, results indicated (standardized betas) that lower score
on positive actions (β = - 0.25, t = -1.98, p < 0.05) and expression of feeling and needs (β
= -0.23, t = -2.21, p < 0.05) predicted procrastination scores.
Ferrari... / Individual Differences Research, 201x, Vol. x, No. x, pp. xx-xx
3
Table 1
Mean Sum Scores and Correlation Coefficients Across Self-reported Measures
M
(SD)
Procras-
tination
Positive
Actions
Control-
ability
Asser-
tiveness
Well-being
seeking
TOTAL
SRI-S
Procrastination
036.84
(10.17)
[.82]
SR/Coping Style
-positive actions
022.40
(3.25)
-.25**
[.68]
-controllability
019.52
(4.46)
-.19
.48***
[.75]
-expression of feelings
019.89
(5.00)
-.27**
.39***
.26**
-assertiveness
021.07
(4.06)
-.04
.47***
.23*
[.69]
-well-being seeking
024.61
(2.93)
-.12
.43***
.13
.17
[.69]
TOTAL SRI-S score
107.50
(13.29)
-.24**
.79***
.65***
.65***
.54***
[.82]
Note. Value in brackets along the diagonal is coefficient alpha with the current sample. SRI-S =
Self-Regulation Inventory. n = 104; * p < .05, ** p < .01, ***p < .001
Results from this brief survey study suggest that chronic procrastinators engage in less
positive, constructive behaviors that might regulate their adjustment and mental health
coping styles. Chronic procrastinators also are less likely to express their needs and
concerns, necessary for effective coping. Consistent with popular literature about the
effectiveness of coping styles by procrastinators (see Ferrari, 2010), the present study
supports the claim that chronic procrastination may lead to greater perceived stress plus the
delay or omission of important adjustment behaviors, resulting in poor mental health. The
present study, then, adds to current research on the procrastination and coping, and
connects the negative consequences of procrastination with increased vulnerability for
negative coping styles impacting, for example, physical health (Sirois, 2006). In sum,
chronic procrastination is conceptually self-regulatory failure and, consequently, disables
individuals from guiding their goal-directed activities toward health lifestyles.
Of course, this study is limited by sample size and demographics. We had a small
number of respondents, all traditional age students (as opposed to older working adults),
and all respondents came from Spain. Given the number of cross-cultural studies between
Spanish and, say, English-speaking respondents on chronic procrastination that replicate
the same results (e.g., Diaz-Morelas et al., 2006; Ferrari & Diaz-Morales, 2007a, 2007b;
Ferrari et al., 2007), we are confident that our results may generalize. Still, future studies
need to replicate and expand our results. We also encourage future studies to dig deeper
into understanding the link between chronic procrastination and mental-health, as this topic
needs greater breadth and depth in application and understanding (Ferrari, 2010).
Nevertheless, the present study found a link between these maladaptive lifestyles which
impact on the lives of many individuals.
References
Díaz-Morales, J.F., Ferrari, J. R., Díaz, K., & Argumedo, D. (2006). Factorial structure of
three procrastination scales with Spanish adult population. European Journal of
Psychological Assessment, 22, 132–137.
Ferrari, J.R. (2001). Procrastination as self-regulation failure of performance: Effects of
cognitive load, self-awareness, and time limits on "working best under pressure."
European Journal of Personality 15, 391-406.
Ferrari, J.R. (2010). Still procrastinating: the no regrets guide to getting it done. New
Jersey: John Wiley & Sons.
Ferrari... / Individual Differences Research, 201x, Vol. x, No. x, pp. xx-xx
4
Ferrari, J.R., & Díaz-Morales, J.F. (2007a). Perceptions of self-concept and self-
presentation by procrastinators: Further evidence. The Spanish Journal of
Psychology, 10, 91-96.
Ferrari, J.R., & Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2007b). Procrastination: Different time orientations
reflect different motives. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 707–714.
Ferrari, J.R., & Pychyl, T.A. (2007). Regulating speed, accuracy, and judgments by
indecisives: Effects of frequent choices on self-regulation failure. Personality and
Individual Differences, 42, 777-787.
Ferrari, J.R., Díaz-Morales, J.F., O’Callaghan, J., Díaz, K., & Argumedo, D. (2007).
Frequent behavioral delay tendency by adults: International prevalence rates of
chronic procrastination. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 458–464.
Ferrari, J.R., Johnson, J.L., & McCown, W.G. (1995). Procrastination and task
avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Plenum, now Springer.
Grossarth-Maticek, R., & Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Self-regulation and mortality from
cancer, coronary heart disease, and other causes: A prospective study. Personality
and Individual Differences, 19, 781–795.
Marqués, M.J., Ibáñez, M.I., Ruipérez, M. A., Moya, J., & Ortet, G. (2005). The Self-
Regulation Inventory (SRI): Psychometric properties of a health-related coping
measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1043-1054.
McCown, W., & Johnson, J (1989). Adult Inventory of Procrastination. In JR Ferrari et
al (1995) Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment. New
York: Plenum, now Springer.
Schouwenburg, H. C., Lay, C. H., Pychyl, T. A., & Ferrari, J. R. (2004). Counseling the
procrastinator in academic settings. Washington: APA.
Sirois, F. M. (2006). ‘‘I’ll look after my health, later’’: A replication and extension of the
procrastination–health model with community-dwelling adults. Personality and
Individual Differences, 43, 15-26.
Tice, D. M., & Bratslavsky, E. (2000). Giving in to feel good: The place of emotion
regulation in the context of general self-control. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 149–159.
... The intentional postponement of action is known as procrastination, and it is a universal phenomenon (Chu & Choi, 2005;Tuckman, 1991). Procrastination can occur for many reasons such as postponing an action to avoid stress (Chu & Choi, 2005), or self-regulatory failure leading to poor well-being (Ferrari & Díaz-Morales, 2014), and the delay may be due to reaching a goal (Lay, 1986;Steel, Brothen, & Wambach, 2001). In the past two decades, there has been an extensive increase in the interest in procrastination (Ko & Chang, 2019). ...
... In the past two decades, there has been an extensive increase in the interest in procrastination (Ko & Chang, 2019). Worldwide, approximately 20% to 25% of adolescents and adults suffer from chronic procrastination tendencies in their work and personal lives (Balkis & Duru, 2007;Ferrari & Díaz-Morales, 2014;Ko & Chang, 2019). While, it was reported that 70% of college students consider themselves academic procrastinators (Schouwenburg, Lay, Pychyl, & Ferrari, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
The main aim of the current study was the examines the relationship between procrastination, Negative Emotions, and mental well-being. Participants from Saudi Arabia (n = 886; females 344, 38.8% and 542 males, 61.2%; Mage = 24.33, SD = 5.68). Participants completed online questionnaires on the Irrational Procrastination Scale (IPS), the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21), and the Mental Health Continuum-Short Form (MHC-SF). As hypothesized, males procrastinated more than females. Furthermore, single ones procrastinated more than married ones. Depending on the study assumption that procrastination and negative emotional symptoms are related to poorer mental well-being, procrastination is related to higher levels of DASS-21 and decreased well-being. These findings suggest that age, gender, depression, stress, anxiety, and Well-being were predictive of procrastination. Findings add to the psychological literature and improve a better comprehension of the complicated associations between Procrastination, Negative Emotions, and Well-Being. El objetivo principal del presente estudio fue examinar la relación entre la procrastinación, las emociones negativas y el bienestar mental. Participantes de Arabia Saudita (n = 886; mujeres 344, 38.8% y 542 hombres, 61.2%; Medad = 24.33, SD = 5.68). Los participantes completaron cuestionarios en línea sobre la Escala de procrastinación irracional (IPS), las Escalas de estrés por depresión, ansiedad (DASS-21) y el Formulario corto continuo de salud mental (MHC-SF). Según la hipótesis, los hombres postergaron las cosas más que las mujeres. Además, los solteros postergaban más que los casados. Dependiendo de la hipótesis del estudio de que la procrastinación y los síntomas emocionales negativos están relacionados con un peor bienestar mental, la procrastinación está relacionada con niveles más altos de DASS-21 y una disminución del bienestar. Estos hallazgos sugieren que la edad, el género, la depresión, el estrés, la ansiedad y el bienestar eran factores predictivos de la procrastinación. Los hallazgos se suman a la literatura psicológica y mejoran una mejor comprensión de las complicadas asociaciones entre la procastinación, las emociones negativas y el bienestar.
... Procrastination reduces the self-efficiency of scholars, adversely affects their achievement, and can fade their capacity to accomplish course assignments while some scholars are affected more than others in these areas (Wilkinson & Sherman, 1991). According to Ferrari and Díaz-Morales (2014), there are three characteristics of the academic procrastinator. The first main feature is that they delay in beginning or finishing an assignment. ...
... The mediator variable that could theoretically be expected is procrastination. Procrastination is defined as the tendency to unnecessarily and intentionally put off a task or a decision [53]. Most people do not feel like doing anything when they are depressed, which may easily induce procrastination [54]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to examine whether there is a difference in the sequential effect of depression on academic performance through peer relationships in Chinese adolescents based on gender and physical activity by using longitudinal data. Data from 7170 people from the China Education Panel Survey (CEPS) were used for analysis. The analysis showed that the indirect effect of depression on academic performance through peer relationships varied depending on gender and physical activity. Specifically, in male students experiencing depression, there was an indirect effect on academic performance through peer relationships in both high and low physical activity groups. In female students experiencing depression, the indirect effect on academic performance through peer relationships was offset if they participated more in physical activity. In other words, for female students participating in physical activity, improvement in peer relationships did not lead to higher academic performance, whereas for male students, improvement in academic performance could be anticipated by improving peer relationships regardless of participation in physical activity. Moreover, since peer relationships play a critical role in improving the academic performance of adolescents experiencing depression, it is important to develop a physical activity or sports program to strengthen peer relationships to improve academic performance.
... Procrastination and cramming is completely common practice and widely justifiable among students and yet perhaps officially denied by their institution [1,12]. Along with this practice, especially in the long term, will caused sleep deprivation [13], blood pressure disorders [14], physical-mental-emotional disruption [15] and also substance abuse [16], e.g., tobacco or caffeine, in order to accomplish good marks and passing the exam [1]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Aims: To analyze the practice of cramming among pre-clinical medical students during the remedial examination based on their gender and place of residence, along with a review of the literature on the factors that foster this practice. Study Design: Simple cross-sectional study. Place and Duration of Study: Faculty of Medicine, Universitas Kristen Indonesia, Jakarta- Indonesia, throughout August 2022. Methodology: Analysis of demographics data, gender and place of residence, of the cramming and non-cramming students, with the results of total 36 remedial exam scores for courses/block in the second, fourth, and sixth semesters. Results: Out of 2413, 739 (30.62%) were male and 1674 (69.37%) were female, and 1054 (43.68%) live in boarding houses and 1359 (56.31%) live in their own private house. Cramming conducted by 1553 students (64.35%) consisting of 505 males (32.51%) and 1049 female (67.54%). Based on the total number of participants taking the remedial exam, the prevalence of cramming was slightly higher in male student (68.33%) compared to female student (62.66%). Based on their place of residence, most of the perpetrator live in their own house (n=935 or 60.20%) and 39.80% live in boarding houses (n=619). Statistic analysis using Pearson chi-square on the gender differences and place of residence in cramming behavior both were proved significant (P=0.007 and P=0.000). The average passing percentage of cramming perpetrator is only 43.13% with very low mean score (50.13). There is a reduction in the prevalence of cramming as the students reach higher level of education. Conclusion: Cramming among medical students still a common practice although the success story regarding this activity is still questionable because there are still more perpetrators who fail the exam and do not reach the lower threshold value of passing.
... 66). While Steel's definition is an accurate description for chronic procrastinators, Ferrari, who is another leading researcher in the study of procrastination (Ferrari, 2001;Ferrari & Diaz-Morales, 2014;Goroshit et al., 2020;Tibbett and Ferrari, 2019), explains that many people consider their procrastination as something that is helpful, believing they work best under pressure (Ferrari, 2001). Klingsieck (2013) names this "arousal procrastination" due to a perceived increase in motivation to act at the last minute. ...
Chapter
Relationship difficulties are at the core of why most people seek counselling. The same is true in the context of higher education. Many students who seek counselling at university report experiencing issues in their intimate relationships. Transitioning to university is a major life milestone. This is a time filled with possibilities, when individuals evaluate and make important life and career choices. Also, for some, this time represents the beginning of new relationships with romantic partners. While these intimate connections can be a source of security and foster well-being, some individuals experience relationship issues that interfere with their academic performance, life satisfaction, and future success. Evidence shows that both negative relationship quality and relationship break-ups are strongly associated with poor mental health outcomes contributing to academic failure, and sometimes leading to, university attrition, and economic burden. More specifically, relationship difficulties are a significant contributor to anxiety, depression, and suicidality. This chapter will present a theoretical discussion of the literature to highlight the importance of understanding intimate relationships in the context of higher education and the effect of relationship issues (including conflict, lack of relationship skills, self-handicapping and self-sabotage, and personality traits) on the mental health of individuals, with implications for research and practice.
Article
Full-text available
Detection and treatment of clinically relevant forms of procrastination would be greatly facilitated by diagnostic criteria as formulated for psychological disorders in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5). In the present article, the steps for deriving and validating diagnostic criteria for pathological procrastination are described. In an online survey of a random sample of N = 10,000 German university students, 990 answered 13 items derived from the attempts in the literature to define procrastination, the Aitken Procrastination Inventory (API) and the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9). A subset of six items related to the first factor onset delay of the API was selected by Best Subset Multiple Regression (BSMR). A latent class analysis (LCA) of these six items sorted the students into six clusters. A cluster of pathological procrastinators (10%) was separated from the clusters of less impaired habitual , average , and occasional delayers . In addition, a cluster of unconcerned delayers (10%), with strong procrastination tendencies but little personal disadvantages, and a small cluster of fast performers (2%) emerged. The pathological procrastinators differed from all other clusters significantly on nine of the 13 items. They were older, had studied longer but had fulfilled less of their study obligations and were more depressed. The answer options of the six questions were collapsed into two categories ( procrastination feature present for at least half a year or absent ). These criteria were used for the clinical diagnosis of pathological procrastination. For a diagnosis, two fixed criteria ( delaying important tasks needlessly and strong interference with personal goals ) plus at least two of four additional criteria ( time spent procrastinating , time pressure , physical and psychological complaints , below performance potential ) must be met. This diagnostic rule captured 92% from the cluster of pathological procrastinators and 10% of the habitual delayers , but no one from the remaining clusters. Using these diagnostic criteria for clinical diagnosis and intervention decisions will facilitate the comparison and integration of the results from future studies of procrastination.
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to obtain empirical evidence the role of self-regulation on student's academic procrastination. The research method used is correlational quantitative. The sample in this study amounted to 120 students who came from students of SMK Ibrahimy 2. The scale used was Tuckman and Zimmermant. The method used to analyze the data in this study is to use statistical analysis methods using the Spearmen Test and Test . To simplify the calculation, the JAMOVI program is used. Based on the output of the hypothesis test results, the value is known, the significance value is p = 0.009 (<0.001) it can be interpreted that there is a relationship between self-regulation and academic procrastination. The results of the hypothesis test on Spearman's rho resulted in a value of -0.215, from the results of the correlation coefficient test it was known that the relationship between self-regulation variables and academic procrastination was a negative relationship.
Chapter
The mental health of students in higher education is of major concern, given the high levels of psychological distress that is known to exist amongst this cohort. In this literature review, evidence is presented based on research across three decades: it reveals a link between insecure attachment bonding in early childhood to maladaptive perfectionism as a means to feel worthy, oftentimes leading to procrastination as an unhelpful strategy to avoid the anxiety of possibly not meeting expectations. As a result, the student may experience conflicting “parts”—the part that wants to achieve high standards and the part that appears to self-sabotage—and this internal conflict appears to lead to high levels of shame, anxiety and depression. The mediator showing to effectively manage the cycle of shame that ensues when these “parts” are engaged is mindfulness and self-compassion. Suggestions for how to create a non-shaming, safe and encouraging environment include workshops that teach students and teachers about the autonomic nervous system and how to self-regulate emotions through the use of mindfulness and self-compassion techniques. As the research reveals, creating safety is the key to good outcomes both academically and from a mental health perspective.
Article
Full-text available
One of the elements constituting the globally growing interest in perfectionism and procrastination is today’s education systems. In this study, which employed phenomenological model as one of the qualitative research methods, it was aimed to identify the motives for the perfectionism and procrastination of Science High School students within the framework of their own experiences and to determine what they need to overcome them. The data obtained through semi-structured interviews with 21 students selected by purposive sampling method were subjected to content analysis. In the analysis, a multidimensional perspective was adopted without considering a particular theoretical perspective on perfectionism and procrastination. As a result of the data analysis, it was seen that in addition to the standards the students determined for themselves, others' expectations were also among the motives of the students for perfectionism in academic tasks. Students need to overcome their fears of failure, get rid of academic procrastination and social support mechanisms with a view to coping with this situation. In addition, students procrastinate academic tasks due to the nature of the task, motivational problems, inadequacies in self-regulation skills, and fear of failure or fear of fail to be perfect. In order to cope with procrastination behaviors, they need to learn a number of both behavioral and cognitive regulations and increase their motivations.
Article
Full-text available
Effects of cognitive load, objective self-awareness and time limits on the self-regulation of performance speed and accuracy were investigated between procrastinators and non-procrastinators. In experiment 1 chronic procrastinators completed fewer items (slow speed) and made more errors (less accuracy) than non-procrastinators under high but not low cognitive load conditions when the time span was limited and brief. In experiment 2 chronic procrastinators performed slower than non-procrastinators under a 2 second, but not under no limit, 1 second, or 4 second time limit conditions. Chronic procrastinators compared to non-procrastinators also performed more slowly and made more performance errors under objective self-awareness conditions regardless of the length of time. These experiments indicate that chronic procrastinators regulate ineffectively their performance speed and accuracy when they ‘work under pressure’ (defined by high cognitive load, objective self-awareness, and imposed time limitations). Copyright © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
Full-text available
Because procrastination concerns a person’s ability to meet deadlines, temporal dimension is clearly important to this personality construct. In the present study, the characteristic profile of arousal and avoidant motives of procrastinators were related to past, present, and future time conceptions. Participants (140 women, 135 men; mean age=49.4, SD=5.55) completed measures of arousal and avoidance procrastination, and time orientation. Results indicated that avoidant procrastination was associated negatively with present-fatalistic time orientation, and arousal procrastination was associated positively with present-hedonist and negatively with future time orientations. The variance accounted for by time orientations was modest, yet provides further evidence that there are distinct motives for chronic procrastination.
Article
Full-text available
Adult men (n = 582) and women (n = 765) from six nations (Spain, Peru, Venezuela, the United Kingdom, Australia, and the United States) completed two reliable and valid measures of chronic procrastination. Because both arousal and avoidant procrastination types were significantly related across the entire sample (r = .72, p < .001) and within each national sample, regression analyses calculated “pure” arousal and “pure” avoidant procrastinators, controlling for the scale scores of the other scale. Results indicated no significant sex or nationality differences within and between nations on self-reported arousal or avoidant procrastination. Overall, 13.5% and 14.6% of men and women self-identified as either arousal or avoidant procrastinators, respectively. These findings suggest that the tendency toward frequent delays in starting or completing tasks may be prevalent across diverse populations in spite of their distinct cultural values, norms, and practices.
Article
Full-text available
The present study explored the factorial structure of three well-known procrastination measures: the General Procrastination Scale (GP; Lay, 1986), the Adult Inventory of Procrastination (AIP; McCown & Johnson, 1989), and the Decisional Procrastination Scale (DP; Mann, 1982) with 502 Spanish adults between 25 and 67 years old. The principal component analysis indicated a one-factor structure for GP and DP, and two factor components for AIP. The internal consistency of the three scales was satisfactory. Results suggested that the Spanish versions of the three procrastination scales were effective and reliable. Finally, a principal component analysis with the three scales indicated a four-factor model of procrastination, namely: dilatory behaviors, indecision, lack of punctuality, and lack of planning.
Article
Full-text available
Initial investigations into the links between procrastination and health in student samples implicated stress-related and behavioural pathways. However, it is unknown if these relations are the same for com-munity-dwelling adults, or if alternative measures of procrastination and health behaviors will yield the same results. To replicate and extend previous findings 254 adults recruited from the community and the Internet completed self-report measures of procrastination, health, stress, wellness and household safety behaviours. Consistent with previous work, procrastination was associated with higher stress, more acute health problems, and the practice of fewer wellness behaviours. Procrastinators also reported fewer house-hold safety behaviours, and less frequent dental and medical check-ups. The structural equation modeling analyses revealed that stress fully mediated the procrastination–health relationship, but health behaviours did not when the combined effect with stress was considered. These findings suggest that in addition to hin-dering a variety of health-related behaviours, procrastination may confer additional risk for increased stress, and consequently more health problems.
Article
Full-text available
Effects of cognitive load, objective self-awareness and time limits on the self-regulation of performance speed and accuracy were investigated between procrastinators and non-procrastinators. In experiment 1 chronic procrastinators completed fewer items (slow speed) and made more errors (less accuracy) than non-procrastinators under high but not low cognitive load conditions when the time span was limited and brief. In experiment 2 chronic procrastinators performed slower than non-procrastinators under a 2 second, but not under no limit, 1 second, or 4 second time limit conditions. Chronic procrastinators compared to non-procrastinators also performed more slowly and made more performance errors under objective self-awareness conditions regardless of the length of time. These experiments indicate that chronic procrastinators regulate ineffectively their performance speed and accuracy when they ‘work under pressure’ (defined by high cognitive load, objective self-awareness, and imposed time limitations). Copyright © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
Understanding how emotion regulation is similar to and different from other self-control tasks can advance the understanding of emotion regulation. Emotion regulation has many similarities to other regulatory tasks such as dieting, and abstaining from smoking, drugs, alcohol, ill-advised sexual encounters, gambling, and procrastination, but it differs in a few important respects. Emotion regulation is similar to other kinds of self-regulation in that it consists of three components: standards, monitoring, and strength. Emotion regulation involves overriding one set responses with another, incompatible set, just like with other types of self-control. And like other regulatory tasks, emotion regulation can fail either because of underregulation or because of misregulation. Although emotion regulation is similar in many respects to other regulatory tasks, it is a special case of self-regulation in that it can often undermine attempts at other kinds of self- control. Specifically, focusing on regulating moods and feeling states can lead to a failure of self-control in other areas.
Article
Two studies examined individual differences in indecision on performance speed, accuracy, and subsequent self-control. In Study 1, indecisives (n = 36) took longer yet maintained accuracy as decisives (n = 39) on a Stroop task anticipating feedback (no feedback, success, or failure). Indecisives in the no feedback condition depleted their self-regulatory resources while maintaining accuracy. In Study 2 the number of Stroop trials increased with no performance feedback. Indecisives (n = 25) compared to decisives (n = 25) maintained accuracy yet took significantly longer and experienced depleted self-control. Prior choice exertion by indecisives to regulate effectively performance accuracy depleted capacity for subsequent self-control, and indecisives seem aware of lowered self-regulation energy. A self-control strength model may be important in understanding indecision.