ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The current study provides a thorough content analysis of use of force studies published in peer-reviewed journals between 1995 and 2008. The most commonly used explanatory factors are discussed in terms of their influence on police officers' decisions to use force during encounters with suspects. Based on the empirical evidence summarized, it appears that few suspect and encounter characteristics are highly influential in determining use of force by police. Moreover, most of the variables used throughout the literature seem to have a mixed relationship with or appear to be poor predictors of use of force by police. We offer possible explanations for the inconsistent findings and suggestions for future research in this area.
Content may be subject to copyright.
214 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force: A Review of the Evidence (2010).
Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7(2). pp. 214-239
© 2010 Southwestern Association of Criminal Justice
UNDERSTANDING POLICE USE OF FORCE:
A REVIEW OF THE EVIDENCE
Charles F. Klahm IV
St. Joseph’s College
Rob Tillyer
University of Texas at San Antonio
Abstract
The current study provides a thorough content analysis of use of force studies published
in peer-reviewed journals between 1995 and 2008. The most commonly used explanatory
factors are discussed in terms of their influence on police officers’ decisions to use force during
encounters with suspects. Based on the empirical evidence summarized, it appears that few
suspect and encounter characteristics are highly influential in determining use of force by
police. Moreover, most of the variables used throughout the literature seem to have a mixed
relationship with or appear to be poor predictors of use of force by police. We offer possible
explanations for the inconsistent findings and suggestions for future research in this area.
Key Words: Police, Force, Violence, Discretion
INTRODUCTION
The police are tasked with making a variety of decisions and the duty to address
“something-that-ought-not-to-be-happening-and-about-which-someone-had-better-do-
something-now” (Bittner, 1974: 30). This broad mandate involves many different functions,
including crime fighter (Manning, 1978, 1992), order maintenance or peacekeeper (Greene,
2000; Kelling & Moore, 1988; Wilson, 1968; Wilson & Kelling, 1982), service delivery (Eck &
Rosenbaum, 1994), problem solver (Eck & Spelman, 1987; Goldstein, 1979, 1990; Kelling &
Moore, 1988), and dispenser of force (Bittner, 1970,1974; Muir, 1977). This potpourri of roles
produces definitions of police work that are not consistent and present a significant challenge to
understanding police work from a scientific perspective.
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 215
Arguably the most defining characteristic of police work is their ability to use force to
enforce the law (Bittner, 1970); they are uniquely situated and authorized to employ various
levels of force to compel specific responses from citizens. These actions have been studied
since the “discovery” of discretion in the middle of the 20th Century. Initially, police use of
“normal” and non-lethal force only received scant attention, and it was not until the mid 1980’s
that non-lethal force became a popular topic for academics. Since then, more research attention
has been focused on examining the extent, nature, and correlates of non-lethal police force.
Unfortunately, due to its rare occurrence (NIJ, 1999), use of force is not well understood despite
the fact that scholars have been researching it for nearly sixty years.
Sherman (1980) and Riksheim and Chermak (1993) initially reported on the state of
knowledge regarding police behavior broadly, and use of force specifically, by summarizing
existing studies of police behavior. Since these pioneering pieces, the use of force literature has
experienced significant growth over the last twenty years. Thus, a comprehensive update on the
correlates of use of force by police is necessary. We aim to accomplish this goal by
summarizing and grouping the primary correlates of use of force by borrowing from the
template employed by Riksheim and Chermak (1993) and focusing on research within the past
twenty years. This comprehensive review will assist in forming the foundation for a new wave
of research questions and generate a research agenda that studies arguably the most defining
aspect of police work.
METHODOLOGY
To effectively catalog and understand the correlates of force, we borrow from the
pioneering and widely cited work of Sherman (1980) and Riksheim and Chermak (1993) and
examine various studies of use of force by police published between 1995 and 2008. This study
period was selected for several reasons. First, in 1995, a comprehensive definition for use of
force within the academic community was presented (Garner, Schade, Hepburn, & Buchanan,
1995); thus, it was expected that most, if not all, studies after 1995 would use that definition of
force in their assessment of this phenomenon. Second, in the mid-1990s, the use of force
continuum became a prominent measure of force among scholars studying police use of force.
As a result, scholars began to include nonviolent police behaviors in their measures of force that
were not included in earlier studies. Last, Riksheim and Chermak’s (1993) replication of
Sherman’s (1980) literature review reported on studies conducted between 1980 and 1993.
Therefore, their review provided a detailed synopsis of the field’s knowledge regarding police
use of force up to the mid-1990’s, but no recent comprehensive review has been undertaken.
A comprehensive and scientific methodology was instituted to identify all relevant
studies. Initially, multiple Boolean search terms were created from a combination of
words/phrases, such as “police”, “use of force”, “use of violence”, and “forceful encounters”.
These search terms were then used to gather literature consolidated in the Criminal Justice
Periodicals Index, which searches peer-reviewed journals publishing studies on criminal justice,
broadly, and policing, specifically. Forty-one studies were originally identified, each directly
216 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
addressing a dimension of use of force by police. Of these, twenty-eight used multivariate
techniques: twenty-four
1
analyzed incidents where police resorted to force during an encounter
with a suspect; two studies examined officer attitudes or perceptions regarding the use of force
2
;
one focused on internal affairs investigations for use of force
3
; and one evaluated agency level
rates of reported use of force incidents
4
. The remaining thirteen studies did not conduct
multivariate analyses and only provided a general overview of use of force in terms of
univariate descriptive statistics or bivariate relationships. These studies were removed from
further consideration, as the state of research has changed recently to require more rigor in
analysis with multivariate analysis now considered the minimum threshold for scientific study.
Two additional studies were removed from consideration because the samples focused on
deported illegal immigrants, which are very different from the suspects described in traditional
policing studies. Finally, three studies that relied on vignette analysis were removed due to
limitations associated with those data. The remaining twenty-three studies using multivariate
analyses were analyzed and summarized in Table 1.
Across the twenty-three studies a total of 212 different independent variables were
employed to explain various dimensions of police use of force. Due to space limitations, all
212 are not discussed, rather the discourse here is limited to the most commonly used variables
throughout the literature. Similar to Riksheim and Chermak (1993), these factors are grouped
by suspect, encounter, and officer characteristics. The constellation of factors used to predict
police force are discussed in this order to reflect the nested structure of police-citizen encounter
data. That is, data relating to police-citizen encounters correspond to a natural, hierarchical
structure in which suspect and encounter characteristics are nested within officers, which in turn
are nested within agencies and communities.
SUSPECT CHARACTERISTICS
Suspect characteristics are frequently examined by studies exploring use of force by
police. Suspect demographics (i.e., race/ethnicity, gender, and age) are common foci, but
demeanor, social class, and the use of drugs/alcohol are also variables of interest in more
contemporary inquiries. Each of these factors is reviewed in detail below.
Race/Ethnicity
Similar to studies examining other criminal justice decision points, the race/ethnicity of
1
Alpert, Dunham, & MacDonald (2004); Burke & Mikkelsen (2004); Crawford & Burns (1998); Engel, Sobol, & Worden (2000);
Garner, Maxwell, & Heraux (2002); Kaminski, Digiovanni, & Downs (2004); Kop & Euwema (2001); Lawton (2007); McCluskey &
Terrill (2005); McCluskey, Terrill, & Paoline (2005); Morabito & Doerner (1997); Norris, Birkbeck, & Gabaldon (2006); Paoline &
Terrill (2004); Paoline & Terrill (2007); Phillips & Smith (2000); Phillips, Rodriguez, & Hagan (2002); Phillips, Hagan, & Rodriguez
(2006); Schuck (2004); Sun & Payne (2004); Terrill & Mastrofski (2002); Terrill & Reisig (2003); Terrill, Paoline, & Manning
(2003); Terrill (2005); Terrill, Leinfelt, & Kwak (2008)
2
Holmes, Reynolds, Holmes, & Faulkner (1998); Son, Davis, & Rome (1998)
3
McElvain & Kposowa (2004)
4
Alpert & MacDonald (2001)
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 217
TABLE 1. SUMMARY OF RESEARCH FINDINGS ASSOCIATED WITH USE OF
FORCE BY VARIABLE
Variables
Positive
relationship
Negative
relationship
Mixed
findings
No
relationship
# of
studies
Suspect
characteristics
Race/Ethnicity
(minority)
2
--
7
8
17
Gender (male)
8
--
6
3
17
Age (older)
--
7
3
5
15
Demeanor
4
--
2
7
13
Social class (lower)
5
--
2
2
9
Intoxication
7
--
5
3
15
Encounter
characteristics
Weapon
4
--
5
1
10
Proactive contact
4
--
5
1
10
Resistance
8
--
1
1
10
Arrest
5
--
1
--
6
Other officers
3
1
4
2
10
Other citizens
1
--
3
8
12
Conflict
4
--
2
1
7
Officer
characteristics
Race (non-white)
--
--
2
9
11
Gender (Male)
2
--
3
10
15
Age
--
2
--
1
3
Experience
--
4
4
5
13
Education
--
2
2
1
5
218 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
the suspect occupies a considerable amount of research attention. This is particularly salient in
the context of force due to the historically contentious relationship between minority
communities and the police. The empirical evidence is mixed on this issue, but several studies
documented that a suspect’s race/ethnicity did not influence whether an officer used force
during an encounter (Engel et al., 2000; Lawton, 2007; McCluskey et al., 2005; McCluskey &
Terrill, 2005; Morabito & Doerner, 1997; Phillips & Smith, 2000; Sun & Payne, 2004).
Importantly, some of these findings were consistent across multiple models; for example, Engel
et al. (2000) estimated nine models and race/ethnicity was not statistically significant in any of
the analyses. Similarly, Phillips and Smith’s (2000) findings of no race/ethnicity effect were
consistent across two models, and Sun and Payne (2004) derived the same finding across three
models. Moreover, Terrill (2005) examined behavioral sequences between the suspects and
officers in his sample and reported that suspect race/ethnicity did not affect whether an officer
skipped levels on the force continuum or increased or decreased the amount of force they used
during an encounter.
In spite of the strong evidence suggesting that a suspect’s race/ethnicity does not
influence police use of force, some studies have reported contradictory findings. For example,
Terrill and Mastrofski (2002) found that non-white citizens were more likely to be subjected to
some form of force than their white counterparts (see also Terrill et al., 2003). Moreover,
several other studies have found that suspect race/ethnicity produced mixed results, depending
on the model that was estimated. Garner et al. (2002) discovered that Black suspects were more
likely to have force used against them in situations of compliance, but race/ethnicity was not a
factor in encounters involving resistance. Several other studies have also produced mixed
results (Kaminski et al., 2004; Paoline & Terrill, 2004, 2007; Schuck, 2004; Terrill et al., 2008).
For instance, Terrill and Reisig (2003) initially reported that minority suspects were more likely
to have force used against them than white suspects; however, when neighborhood contextual
factors were introduced into the model, suspect race/ethnicity no longer retained significance.
Gender
The gender of the suspect has also received considerable attention in studies of force and
consistently demonstrates that male suspects are more likely to have forced used against them
during police-citizen encounters (Garner et al., 2002; McCluskey et al., 2005; McCluskey &
Terrill, 2005; Phillips & Smith, 2000; Sun & Payne, 2004; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002; Terrill &
Reisig, 2003; Terrill et al., 2003). Some studies indicated mixed results for gender (Crawford
& Burns, 1998; Paoline & Terrill, 2004, 2007; Schuck, 2004; Terrill, 2005). For example,
Kaminski et al. (2004) discovered that officers were no more or less likely to use a firm grip on
male suspects compared to females; however, officers were more likely to use a higher level of
force on male suspects compared to their female counterparts. Finally, a few select studies
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 219
reported that suspect gender was not related to use of force (Engel et al., 2000; Lawton, 2000;
Morabito & Doerner, 1997).
Age
The third suspect demographic, age, has been inconsistently linked to the use of force.
Of those studies that reported a relationship between age and use of force, the majority of
empirical evidence suggested that law enforcement officers were less likely to use force on
older suspects (McCluskey & Terrill, 2005; McCluskey et al., 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2007;
Phillips & Smith, 2000; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002; Terrill & Reisig, 2003; Terrill et al., 2003).
The evidence is not completely consistent, however, as Paoline and Terrill (2004) reported that
male officers were less likely to use verbal and physical force against older citizens, whereas
being older only reduced the likelihood of physical force when the encounter involved a female
officer. Similarly, Crawford and Burns (1998) found that officers were less likely to use a
physical restraint against younger citizens, but were more likely to use some form of nonlethal
force (see also Terrill, 2005). They also reported that citizen age did not influence whether an
officer issued a verbal command, used chemical spray or a firearm. Similar non-significant
findings were presented in other studies (Engel et al., 2000; Garner et al., 2002; Kaminski et al.,
2004; Terrill et al., 2008). For example, Sun and Payne (2004) discovered that police in their
sample were no more or less likely to respond coercively to older citizens when resolving
domestic disputes.
Demeanor
Apart from suspect demographics, suspect demeanor within the context of the police-
citizen encounter has also received a considerable amount of research attention. Collectively,
the evidence is mixed with some studies reporting disrespectful suspects were more likely to
have force used against them, others citing no demeanor effect, and still others reporting
inconsistent results within the same study. For example, Engel et al. (2000) reported that
disrespectful citizens were more likely to be subjected to force than their respectful counterparts
across nine different models. They also examined how demeanor interacted with other factors,
but these terms did not achieve statistical significance. Sun and Payne (2004) also reported that
officers were more likely to resolve a dispute by responding coercively when the citizen
involved was disrespectful (see also Garner et al., 2002 and Kaminski et al., 2004).
Contrary to these findings, other studies have suggested inconsistent results. For
example, in one study, poor suspect demeanor did not influence the use of a verbal command or
firearm, but angry or aggressive suspects were more likely to have a chemical spray or
nonlethal weapon used against them (Crawford & Burns, 1998). Similarly, Terrill (2005)
reported a null effect for demeanor in three of the four models he estimated but he found that
officers were less likely to jump levels of force (both up and down the continuum) when
confronted with a disrespectful suspect (see explanation on p. 132). Finally, a group of studies
220 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
reported that suspect demeanor was not related to use of force (McCluskey et al., 2005;
McCluskey & Terrill, 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2004, 2007; Phillips & Smith, 2000; Terrill et
al., 2003). For example, Terrill and Mastrofski (2002) reported that suspects who were
disrespectful toward the police in language or gesture were no more or less likely to have force
used against them than their more polite counterparts.
Studying suspect demeanor has also been criticized due to its operationalization and
measurement (Engel, Klahm, & Tillyer, 2010). The most pervasive problem throughout the
literature is that demeanor is measured as a number of different behaviors, all of which may not
be equivalent but are nonetheless treated as reflecting the same conceptual idea. This might
account for the divergent results reported across studies. Aside from this issue, in most
instances, suspect demeanor is measured according to a third party assessment (observers) and
thus might not truly reflect how the officer involved in the encounter perceived his or her
behavior.
Social Class
Social class is a classic consideration for assessments of equal treatment by the police for
all citizens. The majority of research examining suspect social class and use of force was
inconclusive with some research suggesting that there was no relationship between social class
and use of force (McCluskey et al., 2005; Sun & Payne, 2004), and other studies suggesting that
an officer’s propensity to use force was influenced by the social class of the citizen involved in
the encounter (McCluskey & Terrill, 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2007; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002;
Terrill et al., 2003; Terrill & Reisig, 2003). Finally, others have reported inconsistent effects
for this factor in the same study (see Paoline & Terrill, 2004 and Terrill, 2005). It is important
to heed Friedrich’s (1980) caution that it is difficult to disentangle the effects of race/ethnicity
and social class. Thus, any findings regarding social class should be considered tenuous at best.
Moreover, similar to the issue concerning demeanor, most of these studies used measures of
social class that were based on an observer’s perception of the suspect, which may have been
influenced by the neighborhood context and incongruent with the officer’s assessment.
Intoxication
Suspect’s use of alcohol and/or drugs (i.e., intoxication) and its relationship with police
behavior has a long history of research (Reiss, 1971; Friedrich, 1980). The body of
contemporary research offers a somewhat mixed picture of a relationship between intoxication
and use of force. Several studies have reported that suspects who were under the influence of
drugs or alcohol at the time of their encounter with police were more likely to have force used
against them than their sober counterparts (Engel et al., 2000; McCluskey & Terrill, 2005;
McCluskey et al., 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2007; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002; Terrill et al., 2003;
Terrill et al., 2008). Conversely, other studies have suggested a less direct relationship. For
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 221
example, Crawford and Burns (1998) reported that suspects under the influence of alcohol were
more likely to have a verbal command levied at them, whereas drug intoxication had no effect
on this outcome. Similarly, they reported that suspects under the influence of drugs were more
likely to experience a nonlethal weapon attack but that police were no more or less likely to use
this type of force on suspects under the influence of alcohol (see also Garner et al., 2002;
Lawton, 2007; Paoline & Terrill, 2004; and Terrill, 2005). Other studies have reported null
findings including Morabito and Doerner (1997) who reported that officers were no more or
less likely to use OC spray against intoxicated suspects prior to or after policy changes
regarding the deployment of OC spray (see also Phillips & Smith, 2000 and Schuck, 2004).
ENCOUNTER CHARACTERISTICS
Research has also explored the influence of encounter characteristics on the use of force
by police. Encounters characteristics reflect factors, not directly linked to the suspect, that vary
across police-citizen situations. These factors include the presence of a weapon during the
encounter, if the officer proactively initiated the contact, the suspect resisted, if an arrest
occurred, the presence of other officers or other citizens, and if there was conflict between the
officer and citizen within the encounter. The evidence regarding each of these elements is
summarized below.
Weapon
It seems intuitive that suspects possessing a weapon would be more likely to have force
used against them due to the inherent danger they might pose to the officer and/or public. Few
studies, however, actually assess the impact of this characteristic and the empirical evidence
regarding its effect is mixed. A handful of studies have, indeed, found that suspects brandishing
a weapon were more likely to have force used against them (McCluskey et al., 2005; Paoline &
Terrill, 2007; Sun & Payne, 2004; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002). Other studies, however, have
reported mixed results (e.g., Crawford & Burns, 1998; Kaminski et al., 2004; Morabito &
Doerner, 1997; and Terrill et al., 2003). For example, Paoline and Terrill (2004) discovered
that female officers were no more or less likely to resort to verbal or physical force when the
suspect involved in the encounter was carrying a weapon; however, their male counterparts
were more likely to use physical force when a suspect was wielding a weapon, but no more or
less likely to use verbal force. Lastly, contrary to expectation, a single study found that
possessing a weapon did not influence an officer’s likelihood of using force (McCluskey &
Terrill, 2005).
222 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
Proactive Contact
Very few studies prior to the mid-1990s examined the effect that an officer initiating
contact had on the likelihood of using force (Riksheim & Chermak, 1993; Sherman, 1980).
Since then, several studies have included such a measure and the empirical evidence is mixed.
Several studies found that when police proactively initiate an encounter they were more likely
to apply force (McCluskey & Terrill, 2005; McCluskey et al., 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2007;
Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002), whereas others reported inconsistent effects across models. For
example, Garner et al. (2002) discovered that proactively entering an encounter was not related
to force when the suspect was complaint, but was predictive of force when the citizen resisted
(see also, Paoline & Terrill, 2004; Terrill, 2005; and Terrill et al., 2003). Most recently, Terrill
et al. (2008) reported that their findings were dependent upon the analytical technique they
used. The results of an ordinal regression model indicated that officers were less likely to use
force during a proactive encounter; however, a logistic regression model produced no
statistically significant effects. Finally, Engel et al. (2000) reported no relationship between
proactively engaging a citizen in an encounter and use of force.
Resistance
Terrill and Mastrofski (2002) and Garner et al. (2002) drew attention to the fact that
police-citizen encounters are dynamic in nature. As such, they emphasized the importance of
capturing suspect behaviors occurring during police encounters that might precipitate the use of
force. Failure of early studies to capture the dynamic nature of these encounters constrained our
understanding regarding how certain factors are related to police use of force (Terrill &
Mastrofski, 2002). Prior to opining this sentiment, a single study (see Crawford & Burns, 1998;
reported mixed results) reviewed here included a measure attempting to account for the
dynamic nature of these events, but since then several studies have by including measures of
suspect resistance. The empirical evidence suggests that resistant suspects were more likely to
experience a forceful outcome compared to their compliant counterparts (McCluskey & Terrill,
2005; McCluskey et al., 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2004, 2007; Schuck, 2004; Terrill et al., 2003;
Terrill et al., 2008). For example, Terrill and Mastrofski (2002) reported suspects who
demonstrated passive, verbal, defensive, or active resistance were more likely to have force
used against them. The lone exception to this general finding is Lawton (2007) who reported
no effect for this factor.
Arrest
The arrest of a suspect was not considered in early assessments of use of force (Riksheim
& Chermak, 1993; Sherman, 1980), but more contemporary studies have employed a measure
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 223
of arrest to determine if this factor influences the likelihood of an officer using force. The
empirical evidence is fairly consistent, suggesting that officers were, in fact, more likely to
employ force when an arrest was made (McCluskey & Terrill, 2005; McCluskey et al., 2005;
Paoline & Terrill, 2007; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002; Terrill et al., 2003). Paoline and Terrill
(2004) specified this relationship by reporting that both male and female officers were more
likely to use physical force when they arrested the suspect involved in the encounter; however,
male officers were less likely to employ verbal force when an arrest was initiated, whereas the
likelihood of a female officer using verbal force was not influenced.
Importantly, one significant limitation of these studies is the inability to determine
whether force was used before an arrest was initiated; thus, the temporal ordering component of
scientific research is not achieved. Moreover, some organizational policy dictates the use of
handcuffing once a suspect is arrested thus eliminating discretion and requiring officers to use
that level of force (see Terrill et al., 2003 footnote on page 1022). Finally, arrest is also
associated with other officer behaviors captured on the use of force continuum. For example,
verbal commands and pat-downs are actions officers engage in when affecting an arrest.
Presence of Other Officers/Citizens
Between 1980 and 1993, only one study examined the impact of other officers on the use
of force (Riksheim & Chermak, 1993). More recently, scholars have become increasingly
interested in how this factor might influence use of force situations. The empirical evidence, to
date, is mixed, as some studies found that force was more likely to occur as the number of
officers involved increases (Garner et al., 2002; Paoline & Terrill, 2007; and Terrill &
Mastrofski, 2002), others reported that force is negatively related to the presence of officers
(Lawton, 2007), and some suggested that there is no relationship (Engel et al., 2000;
McCluskey, et al., 2005).
Other studies have produced mixed results for the effect of this factor. For example,
Terrill et al. (2003) reported that the location of the encounter was influential, a positive
relationship in one location, but a null relationship in another jurisdiction. Paoline and Terrill
(2004) reported that the relationship depended on the operationalization of force as presence of
officers was positively related to physical force, but not to verbal force. Finally, Phillips and
Smith (2000) discovered a negative relationship only when more than three officers were
present (see also Terrill, 2005).
The presence of other citizens has also only recently become a focus of research, as only
two studies had considered this factor on police officer decision-making prior to 1993
(Riksheim & Chermak, 1993). The recent evidence is rather consistent suggesting that the
number of bystanders has no influence on an officer’s likelihood of using force (McCluskey et
al., 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2004, 2007; Schuck, 2004; Terrill, 2005; Terrill & Mastrofski,
2002; Terrill et al., 2003; Terrill et al., 2008). Engel et al. (2000), however, reported conflicting
results, suggesting that the police were more likely to use force against a suspect as the number
of bystanders increased. Finally, Crawford and Burns (1998) found that bystanders increased
224 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
the likelihood of using physical restraints but had no influence on the use of chemical agents,
nonlethal weapons, or firearms (see also Garner et al., 2002 and Phillips & Smith, 2000).
Conflict
Conflict within the encounter is also a relatively new characteristic included in
assessments of force, as none of the studies reviewed by Sherman (1980) or Riksheim and
Chermak (1993) considered this factor. The collective empirical evidence unsurprisingly
suggests that officers were more likely to use force against a suspect if he/she was engaged in a
conflict with another citizen at the time of the encounter (McCluskey & Terrill, 2005;
McCluskey et al., 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2007; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002). Other studies
produced mixed results; for example, Terrill et al. (2003) concluded that this factor varied by
jurisdiction, and Paoline and Terrill (2004) reported its effect varied by the operationalization of
force (i.e., verbal vs. physical). Finally, Engel et al. (2000) reported that conflicts between
citizens did not influence an officer’s likelihood of using force.
OFFICER CHARACTERISTICS
Officer characteristics reflect the intrinsic uniqueness of the officer involved in the
encounter. Previous summaries considered race/ethnicity, gender, age, length of experience,
and education of officers (Riksheim & Chermak, 1993; Sherman, 1980). Only a handful of
early studies examined these factors, but by the time Riksheim and Chermak (1993) conducted
their summary, over twenty independent findings were reported for officer characteristics.
More recently, officer characteristics have received considerable attention in use of force
research.
Race/Ethnicity
Early studies reported that officer race/ethnicity was not related to the likelihood or
appropriateness of police use of force in general, or the use of deadly force, specifically
(Friedrich, 1980; Geller & Karales, 1981). More recently, this finding has been confirmed by a
series of research studies (Lawton, 2007; McElvain & Kposowa, 2004; McCluskey et al., 2005;
McCluskey & Terrill, 2005; Morabito & Doerner, 1997; Paoline & Terrill, 2004, 2007; Terrill
& Mastrofski, 2002). For example, Crawford and Burns (1998) reported that officer
race/ethnicity did not influence the likelihood of an officer using a verbal command, physical
restraint, chemical spray, non-lethal weapon, or firearm.
While the majority of research indicates no consistent relationship between officer
race/ethnicity and use of force, a few recent studies have produced divergent results suggesting
Black and White officers differed in their use of force practices. For example, one of Sun and
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 225
Payne’s (2004) models found that Black officers were more likely than White officers to
respond coercively when asked to resolve interpersonal conflicts between citizens.
Interestingly, officer race/ethnicity was no longer statistically significant once interaction terms
and neighborhood level characteristics were introduced into the model. Similarly, Garner et al.
(2002) reported mixed results, as they discovered that Hispanic officers were more likely than
White officers to use force, while Black officers and those classified as “Other” were no more
or less likely to use force compared to their White counterparts. These relationships, however,
only pertained to the prevalence of force. When severity of force was their outcome measure,
officer race/ethnicity was not a significant predictor. Thus, officer race/ethnicity appears to have
no consistent effect on use of force by police.
Gender
Similarly, most studies indicate officer gender is not related to use of force by police.
Arguably the most thorough analysis of gender differences and use of force involved six
different models and two different analytic techniques (Paoline & Terrill, 2004). Results
indicated only one significant difference between male and female officers in their sample; male
officers were more likely to use higher levels of force against male suspects whereas suspect
gender was unrelated to the level of force female officers used. No other statistically significant
gender differences were reported in the likelihood or type of force used despite the fact that
male and female officers were influenced by other factors differentially. McCluskey and Terrill
(2005) found that after controlling for the number and type of complaints filed against officers,
officer gender was not related to use of force in their sample. These findings have been
supported by several other studies (Crawford & Burns, 1998; Kaminski et al., 2004; Lawton,
2007; McCluskey et al., 2005; Paoline & Terrill, 2007; Phillips & Smith, 2000; Sun and Payne,
2004; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002; Terrill et al., 2008).
Despite the fact that most studies find no significant difference in how often or the type
of force employed by male and female officers, some have produced results suggesting gender
differences in the amount of force male and female officers used or the likelihood that they
resorted to force. For example, Garner et al. (2002) found that male officers were more likely
to use force and employ more severe types of force than female officers. Similarly, McElvain
and Kposowa (2004) found that male officers were more likely to be investigated by internal
affairs for incidents involving higher levels of force than female officers. Finally, Kop and
Euwema (2001) found that male officers were more likely to resort to force than female officers
in their sample; however, they found no gender differences in officer attitudes toward use of
force (see also Morabito & Doerner, 1997). Thus, while not consistent in every study, the
overwhelming amount of evidence suggests that officer gender is not related to use of force.
226 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
Age
Officer age has surprisingly received relatively little attention in empirical studies. One
possible explanation is that officer age and years of experience are highly correlated with one
another thus requiring only one of the two measures to be included in the analysis. Exceptions
to this pattern include Crawford and Burns’ (1998) finding that officer age was unrelated to an
officer’s propensity to use any of type of force analyzed. Conversely, Garner et al. (2002)
reported that older officers were less likely to use force and, when they did, they used less
severe types of force compared to younger officers. Moreover, McElvain and Kposowa (2004)
reported that older officers were less likely to have been investigated by internal affairs for use
of force incidents compared to younger officers. The cumulative evidence regarding the
relationship between officer age and use of force is inconclusive, as too few studies have
considered this factor in their analysis.
Experience
Officer’s level of experience has received a considerable amount of research attention
with mixed results. Evidence has accumulated suggesting a negative relationship between
officer experience and use of force: officers with more experience were less likely to use less
force (Paoline & Terrill, 2007; Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002). Kop and Euwema (2001) also
discovered that officers with more experience held less favorable attitudes toward the use of
force relative to their counterparts with fewer years of service and were less likely to use force.
Likewise, McElvain and Kposowa (2004) found that officers with more years of experience
were less likely to have been investigated by internal affairs for a use of force incident.
Conversely, several other studies reported that officer experience had no influence on use
of force decisions. For example, Sun and Payne (2004) found that officers with more years of
experience were no more or less likely to use force than those with fewer years of service. This
finding was also reported in several other studies (Lawton, 2007; McCluskey et al., 2005;
McCluskey & Terrill, 2005; Terrill et al., 2008).
Finally, further complicating matters, officer experience also produced mixed results in
the same study depending on how use of force was operationalized. For example, Crawford
and Burns (1998) found that officers with more years of experience were less likely to use a
restraining hold and a firearm than officers with fewer years of experience, but were no more or
less likely to use a verbal command, chemical spray, or nonlethal weapon. Other studies found
that officer experience produced mixed results as well (Kaminski et al., 2004; Morabito &
Doerner, 1997; Paoline & Terrill, 2004). The relationship between use of force and officer
experience is unclear and often contingent on the data examined and the operationalization of
the dependent variable.
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 227
Education
One of the recommendations offered by the President’s Commission on Law
Enforcement and Administration of Justice was that police agencies seek to hire college-
educated personnel. Seemingly, those achieving a higher level of educational attainment
possess better decision-making skills and will make better police officers (Worden, 1990). This
assumption has received relatively little attention and the empirical evidence produced by the
few studies that have examined this factor was mixed. A single study reported here found that
an officer’s level of education did not influence the likelihood that he or she used force (Sun &
Payne, 2004), while others have found a negative relationship between force and an officer’s
level of education (Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002). For example, Paoline and Terrill (2007)
reported that officers with a college degree were less likely to use verbal and physical force
compared to their colleagues with only a high school degree. Similarly, officers with some
college were less likely to engage in verbal force compared to their less educated counterparts
but no more or less likely to use physical force. Other studies have reported mixed results
including Morabito and Doerner (1997) who reported that officers with a Bachelor’s degree
were no more or less likely to use OC spray prior to policy changes but were more likely to
deploy this form of nonlethal force after departmental policy changes. Similar mixed results
pertaining to the effects of officer education on the likelihood of using force were reported by
Paoline and Terrill (2004).
DISCUSSION
Based on the empirical evidence to date, it appears that few suspect and encounter
characteristics are highly influential in determining use of force by police. For example, male
suspects, those who were intoxicated, offered resistance, or arrested during their encounter with
police were much more likely to experience police force. A word of caution is warranted, as the
overall consistency of these factors should be tempered with the caveat that several studies also
reported mixed findings or no relationship for these factors as well. Despite this, the general
trend for these factors suggests force is more likely occur when these characteristics are present.
The overwhelming majority of variables used throughout the literature seem to have a mixed
relationship (i.e., suspect race/ethnicity, suspect gender, suspect age, weapon, etc.) or appear to
be poor predictors (i.e., other citizens present, officer race, officer gender, etc.) of use of force
by police. Explaining the relative inconsistency of variables across studies is not an easy task,
but a necessary one if the field of police studies wishes to further its understanding of the nature
and extent of this phenomenon. We offer some plausible explanations that might put the
inconsistent findings reported here in context.
First, while this body of research has improved the state of knowledge regarding
correlates of force, as with all knowledge regarding police behavior, methodological issues
continue to be relevant. Despite the continuity in definition offered by Garner et al. (1995),
there is an on-going, pervasive problem with scholars failing to provide a consistent
228 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
operationalization and measurement of force throughout the literature. This deficiency leads to
an assortment of police behaviors (e.g., verbal, physical, violent, and nonviolent ones) being
measured as force and might account for why there is little consistency in terms of the reported
effects of exogenous variables across studies. Further, this inconsistency raises concerns about
how research findings should be interpreted. For example, it is unclear whether extant findings
are related to nonviolent, violent, or both types of force. As a result, not only are research
results inconsistent across studies, but also there is no way to ensure the results are explaining
the same phenomenon.
Even among those studies that provided definitions of force, the degree of specificity
varies considerably. For example, Terrill and Reisig (2003: 299) defined force as “… acts that
threaten or inflict physical harm on suspects”, whereas Williams and Westall (2003: 471)
5
defined it as “any act or behavior that compelled a person into submission”. Unlike Terrill and
Reisig’s (2003) definition, Williams and Westall’s does not clearly convey the types of police
officer behavior that constitute force and leaves the meaning of “submission” ambiguous. Such
inconsistencies might account for disparate findings across studies. Future research needs to
address the operationalization of force in an effort to generate consensus throughout the
literature.
A second concern centers on possible omitted variable biases. The failure to consistently
include a measure of crime seriousness might account for some of the disparate findings
reported here. Notwithstanding a few inquiries (Alpert et al., 2004; Engel et al., 2000; Lawton,
2007), use of force studies have generally been silent on the importance of this factor.
Considering this is one of the most robust predictors of criminal justice decision-making
(Gottfredson & Gottfredson, 1988) and studies have consistently shown that those who commit
more serious offenses are more likely to be arrested (Brown & Frank, 2006; Novak & Engel,
2005), charged (Meithe, 1987), receive longer sentences (Koons-Witt, 2002; Steffensmeier,
Ulmer, & Kramer, 1998), and victims and witnesses to crimes are more likely to notify the
police when they perceive a serious crime has been committed (Felson, Messner, & Hoskin,
1999), it is crucial to further explore the importance of this factor in force encounters.
Similarly, only one study reviewed here made an attempt to determine whether the officer
involved in the encounter had specific preexisting knowledge regarding the suspect that might
heighten his/her sense of urgency and result in a greater likelihood of resorting to force. Garner
et al. (2002) included measures that tapped into whether the suspect was known to be violent,
possess a weapon, and a member of a gang. Given the relative consistency of prior criminal
record in other areas of criminal justice research, it seems logical for scholars focusing on
police use of force to make an attempt to include such measures in the future.
5
The findings from this study were not reported here, as multivariate analysis was not conducted. However, their
operationalization of force exemplifies the concern being articulated. Of the 41 studies conducted between 1995 and 2008
that were located as a part of this research only twelve provided a proper operational definition of force. The remaining
studies merely identified behaviors captured in their measure or failed to address operationalization all together.
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 229
The third concern deals with recent analytic improvements (i.e., hierarchical modeling)
that may require a re-assessment of findings generated using traditional unilevel, multivariate
models. Assessing police-citizen encounters by considering suspect, encounter, and officer
factors requires consideration of the inherent nested nature of the data. Specifically, data
collected on police-citizen encounters correspond to multiple levels of aggregation: suspect and
encounter level factors correspond to one level of aggregation (level-1), while officer
characteristics correspond to a higher level of aggregation (level-2). This logic also applies to
higher other factors such as neighborhood or organizational factors (level-3). Ideally, suspect
and encounter characteristics should be modeled at a different level of aggregation from officer
characteristics in order to assess their independent impact on outcomes (i.e., use of force)
(Terrill & Mastrofski, 2002).
Failing to acknowledge this issue violates the assumption that each outcome is
independently influenced by the predictors in the model (Hanushek & Jackson, 1977). This
likely leads to error terms at level-1 being correlated across officers at level-2 and may lead to
invalid parameter estimates (Luke, 2004). Moreover, multicollinearity between level-1 and
level-2 units and a biased F-test may result from a failure to properly model nested data
(Wooldredge, Griffin, & Pratt, 2001). Studies that do not acknowledge the hierarchical nature
of the police-citizen encounters might report biased estimates regarding the correlates of police
use of force (as well as other decisions) and report statistical artifacts rather than actual
statistical relationships.
To date, the accepted method of studying use of force has been to analyze data using
pooled variance analytical techniques for categorical and limited dependent variables (i.e.,
ordered probit and multinomial logit models). The only exceptions to date are Lawton (2007),
McCluskey and Terrill (2005), and Terrill and Reisig (2003), who used hierarchical linear
modeling (HLM or HGLM) techniques to estimate the effects of the endogenous variables on
police use of force. Two of these studies, however, modeled encounter level factors at level-1
and community/contextual level factors at level-2 (i.e., violent crime rate, heterogeneity
measures, concentrated disadvantage, homicide rate, etc.), thus failing to address the nested
nature of the data (i.e., suspect and encounter factors nested within officers). In an attempt to
overcome this issue, McCluskey and Terrill (2005) modeled encounter level factors at level-1
and officer characteristics at level-2 to assess the independent effects of the variables operating
at the different levels while controlling for the potential correlated error. Future research should
follow their lead and explore the use of HGLM if the discipline of police studies wishes to
broaden its understanding of policing outcomes in general, and use of force, specifically.
In addition to methodological issues, a consistent, yet underdeveloped, theme in policing
research is the impact of organizational and contextual characteristics on encounter outcomes.
While scholars have been attracted to these factors for some time, few include meaningful
measures in their analysis. Sherman (1980) defined contextual characteristics as attributes of
the community that influence how police carry out their role. His review identified several
community level factors that might affect police officer decision-making, such as the area’s
political climate, economic status, and basic demographic characteristics. However, he
230 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
discovered that few of these measures were employed in use of force studies. By the time
Riksheim and Chermak (1993) replicated his review, scholars had increasingly estimated the
effects of community level factors in use of force research, but still only a total of fourteen
findings were reported. Aside from studies that merely included a jurisdictional measure,
community level factors were incorporated in only four studies, accounting for nine different
findings (see Lawton, 2007; McCluskey et al., 2005; Sun & Payne, 2004; and Terrill & Reisig,
2003).
Similarly, organizational factors have long been discussed by scholars sanctimoniously,
but not given their due attention. Organizational characteristics are factors intrinsic to the
agency, not the individual, but may influence officer decision-making. Sherman’s (1980)
review found a total of five findings reported in police use of literature prior to 1980 and, by
1993, the number of findings reported throughout the literature had increased to thirteen
(Riksheim & Chermak, 1993). Organizational characteristics have received scant attention in
the more recent use of force research, as only two studies reported here included true measures
of such factors (Alpert & MacDonald, 2001; Terrill et al., 2003). Several studies compared
results across departments or jurisdictions, but without providing measures that tap
organizational differences, the findings are difficult to interpret (Sherman, 1980). That is,
merely identifying that outcomes vary across agencies does not speak to any of the
characteristics of those agencies, which might explain why outcomes vary.
As advancements in analytical techniques continue to allow for more sophisticated
modeling of data, it would be expected that contextual and organizational factors experience a
“rebirth” throughout the literature. Considering we can assess the independent influences of
characteristics operating at different levels (i.e., encounter, officer, organizational, and
neighborhood context), this seems like an intuitive avenue for future research. These new and
improved methods might allow for the testing of complex theoretical frameworks such as
Klinger’s (1997) ecological perspective, which suggests police behavior is based on a
constellation of factors ranging from encounter characteristics and personal experiences to
community and work contexts.
IMPORTANCE OF UNDERSTANDING POLICE USE OF FORCE
Understanding the nature and extent of use of force by police is extremely important for a
variety of reasons. The phrase ‘police use of force’ has a negative connotation that implies
cruel, harsh, or brutal treatment, and there is evidence suggesting that these incidents erode
community attitudes toward and trust in police (Thompson & Lee, 2004). Thus, use of force
incidents often serve to exacerbate the historically contentious relations between the police and
certain segments of society. In particular, African American communities have a long-standing
tense relationship with the police and some suggest that Hispanic communities are also at odds
with police (Huang & Vaughn, 1996; Walker, 1997).
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 231
In addition to the deleterious effects on police-community relations, police use of force
incidents can be very costly for police organizations in terms of civil litigation payouts and
subsequent resource expenditures. The very nature of their work makes police organizations
susceptible to civil suits, especially claims of excessive use of force (Barrineau, 1994).
Although estimating the total amount of money paid out annually for use of force claims is a
difficult endeavor due to a lack of reliable data (del Carmen, 1993), a substantial number of law
suits are filed against police each year claiming excessive use of force (Skolnick & Fyfe, 1993).
Of equal concern is recent evidence suggesting that the number of suits filed against police
organizations has been increasing since the 1980’s (see Kappeler & Kappeler, 1992 and
Kappeler, Kappeler, & del Carmen, 1993).
Finally, organizational policy, in theory, should be predicated on empirical research. As
such, it is imperative that we fully understand the nature and extent of police use of force as
well as the factors related to its use. Only then can training protocols be tailored to its
appropriate use and policy formulated to instruct officers when they can and should use force.
232 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
REFERENCES
Alpert, G. and J. MacDonald. (2001). Police use of force: An analysis of organizational
characteristics. Justice Quarterly, Vol. 18, pp. 393-409.
Alpert, G., R. Dunham and J. MacDonald. (2004). Interactive Police-Citizen Encounters that
Result in Force. Police Quarterly, Vol. 7 (4), pp. 475-488.
Barrineau, H. (1994). Civil Liability in Criminal Justice. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson Publishing.
Bittner, E. (1970). The Functions of the Police in Modern Society. Rockville, MD: National
Institute of Mental Health, Center for Studies of Crime and Delinquency.
Bittner, E. (1974). Florence Nightingale in Pursuit of Willie Sutton: A Theory of the Police. In
H. Jacob. (Ed.). The Potential for Reform of Criminal Justice. London: Sage.
Brown, R. and J. Frank. (2006). Race and Officer Decision Making: Examining Differences In
arrest Outcomes Between Black and White Officers. Justice Quarterly, 23, 96-126.
Burke, R. and A. Mikkelsen. (2005). Burnout, job stress and attitudes towards the use of force
by Norwegian police officers. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and
Management, Vol. 28(2), 269-278.
Crawford, C. and R. Burns. (1998). Predictors of the Police Use of Force: The Application of a
Continuum Perspective in Phoenix. Police Quarterly, Vol. 1(1), pp. 41-63.
del Carmen, R. (1993). Civil Liabilities in Law Enforcement: Where are we and Where Should
we go From Here? American Journal of Police, 12(4), pp. 87 99.
Eck, J. and W. Spelman. (1987). Who Ya Gonna Call? The Police as Problem-Busters. Crime
Delinquency, Vol. 33(1), pp. 31-52.
Eck, J. and D. Rosenbaum. (1994). The new police order: effectiveness, equity, and efficiency
in community policing. In D. Rosenbaum (Ed.). The Challenge of Community Policing:
Testing the Promises. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. pp. 3-23.
Engel, R., J. Sobol and R. Worden. (2000). Further Exploration of the Demeanor Hypothesis:
The Interaction Effects of Suspects' Characteristics and Demeanor on Police Behavior.
Justice Quarterly 17(2), pp. 235 258.
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 233
Engel, R., C. Klahm and R. Tillyer (2010). Citizen Demeanor, Race and Traffic Stops: A
review of the Evidence. In White, M. and Rice, S. (Eds.), Race, Ethnicity and Policing:
The Issues, Methods, Research and Future. New York, NY: New York University Press.
Felson, R., S. Messner, and A. Hoskin. 1999. The Victim-Offender Relationship and Calling the
Police in Assaults. Criminology, Vol. 37, pp. 901 917.
Friedrich, R. (1980). Police Use of Force: Individuals, Situations, and Organizations. The
Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 452:82-97.
Garner, J., T. Schade, J. Hepburn and J. Buchanan. (1995). Measuring the continuum of force
used by and against the police. Criminal Justice Review, Vol. 20, pp. 146 168.
Garner, J., C. Maxwell and C. Heraux. (2002). Characteristics associated with the prevalence
and severity of force used by the police. Justice Quarterly, Vol. 19, pp.705-746.
Geller, W. and K. Karales. (1981). Shootings of and by Chicago Police: Uncommon Crises
Part:1 Shootings by Chicago Police. The Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology,
Vol.
22(4), pp. 1813-1866.
Goldstein, H. (1979). Improving Policing: A Problem-Oriented Approach. Crime and
Delinquency 25: 236-258.
Goldstein, H. (1990). Problem-Oriented Policing. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.
Gottfredson, M. and D. Gottfredson. (1988). Decisionmaking in Criminal Justice: Toward the
Rational Exercise of Discretion. New York: Plenum Press.
Greene, J. (2000). Community Policing in America: Changing the Nature, Structure, and
Function of the Police. In J. Horney (Ed.), Criminal Justice 2000, Vol. 3: Policies,
Processes, and Decisions of the Criminal Justice System, National Institute of Justice,
Washington, DC.
Hanushek, E. and J. Jackson. (1977). Statistical Methods for Social Scientists. New York:
Academic Press.
Holmes, S., M. Reynolds, R. Holmes and S. Faulkner. (1998). Individual and situational
determinants of police force: An examination of threat presentation. American Journal of
Criminal Justice, 23(1), 83106.
234 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
Huang, W. and M. Vaughn. (1996). Support and Confidence: Public Attitudes Toward the
Police. In T. Flanagan and D. Longmire (Eds). Americans View Crime and Justice: A
National Public Opinion Survey. London: Sage.
Kaminski, R., C. Digiovanni and R. Downs. (2004). The Use of Force between the Police and
Persons with Impaired Judgment. Police Quarterly, Vol. 7(3), pp. 311-338.
Kappeler, S. and V. Kappeler. (1992). A research note on section 1983 claims against the
police: Cases before the federal district courts in 1990. American Journal of Police, Vol.
11(1), pp. 65 73.
Kappeler, V., S. Kappeler, and R. del Carmen. (1993), A content analysis of police civil
liability cases: decisions of the federal district courts, 1978-1990, Journal of Criminal
Justice, Vol. 21 pp.325-37.
Kelling, G. and M. Moore. (1988). From political reform to community: the evolving strategy
of police. In J. Green and S. Mastrofski (Eds.), Community Policing: Rhetoric or Reality,
Praeger, New York, NY, pp.1-26.
Klinger, D. (1997). Negotiating Order in Patrol Work: An Ecological Theory of Police
Response to Deviance. Criminology, 35(2), pp. 277-306.
Koons-Witt, B. (2002) Gender and justice: The Effect of gender and gender-related factors on
the decision to incarcerate before and after sentencing guidelines. Criminology, Vol.
40(2), pp. 297-328.
Kop, N. and M. Euwema. (2001). Occupational stress and the use of force by Dutch police
officers. Criminal Justice and Behavior, Vol. 28, pp.631-652.
Lawton, B. (2007). Levels of nonlethal force: an examination of individual, situational and
contextual factors. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency,
Vol. 44(2), pp. 163-184.
Luke, D. (2004). Multilevel Modeling. (Sage University Papers Series on Quantitative
Applications in the Social Sciences, series no. 07-143). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 235
Manning, P. (1978). The Police: Mandate, Strategies, and Appearances. In P. Manning and J.
Van Maanen (Eds.). (1978). Policing a View From the Street, pp. 7 32.
Reprinted in V. Kappeler (Ed.). (2006). The Police and Society: Touchstone Readings.
Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press.
Manning, P. (1992). Economic Rhetori and Policing Reform. Criminal Justice Research
Bulletin, Vol. 7(4), pp. 1 8.
Reprinted in V. Kappeler (Ed.). (2006). The Police and Society: Touchstone Readings.
Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press.
McCluskey, J. and W. Terrill. (2005). Departmental and Citizen Complaints as Predictors of
Police Coercion. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and
Management, Vol. 28(3), pp. 513 529.
McCluskey, J., W. Terrill and E. Paoline III. (2005). Peer Group Aggressiveness and the Use of
Coercion in Police-Suspect Encounters. Police Practice and Research: An International
Journal, Vol. 6(1), pp. 19 37.
McElvain, J., and A. Kposowa. (2004). Police officer characteristics and internal affairs
investigations for use of force allegations. Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 32, 265-279.
Meithe, T. (1987). Charging and plea bargaining practices under determinate sentencing: An
investigation of the hydraulic displacement of discretion. Journal of Criminal Law
Criminology. Vol. 78, pp. 155176.
Morabito, E. and B. Doerner. (1997). Police Use of Less-than-lethal Force: Oleoresin Capsicum
(OC) Spray. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategy and Management, Vol.
20(4), pp. 680-697.
Muir Jr., W. (1977). Police: Streetcorner Politicians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.
National Institute of Justice, (1999). Use of force by police: Overview of national and local
data. Washington, DC: United States Department of Justice.
Norris, M., C. Birkbeck and L. Gabaldon (2006). Social Geometry and Force: A Partial Test of
Black’s Theory of Law With Mexican, U.S., and Venezuelan Police. Journal of
Contemporary Criminal Justice. Vol. 22(4), pp. 324 346.
236 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
Novak, K. and R. Engel. (2005). Disentangling the influence of suspects’ demeanor and mental
disorder on arrest. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies &
Management, 28(3), pp. 493-512.
Paoline III, E. and W. Terrill. (2004). Women Police Officers and the Use of Coercion. Women
and Criminal Justice, Vol. 15(3/4), pp. 97 119.
Paoline III, E. and W. Terrill. (2007). Police Education, Experience, and the Use of Force.
Criminal Justice and Behavior, Vol. 34(2), pp. 179 196.
Phillips, S., N. Rodriguez and J. Hagan. (2002). Brutality at the Border? Use of Force in the
Arrest of Immigrants in the United States. International Journal of the Sociology of Law,
Vol. 30, pp. 285 306.
Phillips, S., J. Hagan and N. Rodriguez (2006). Brutal Borders? Examining the Treatment of
Deportees During Arrest and Detention. Social Forces, Vol. 85(1), pp. 93 109.
Phillips, T. and P. Smith. (2000). Police Violence Occasioning Citizen Complaint: An
Empirical Analysis of TimeSpace Dynamics’, British Journal of Criminology, Vol. 40,
pp. 480 496.
Reiss Jr., A. (1971). The Police and the Public, Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.
Riksheim, E. and S. Chermak. (1993). Causes of Police Behavior Revisited. Journal of
Criminal Justice Vol. 21, pp. 353 382.
Schuck, A. (2004). The masking of racial and ethnic disparity in police use of physical force:
The effects of gender and custody status. Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 32, pp. 557
564.
Sherman, L. (1980). Causes of Police Behavior: the Current State of Quantitative Research.
Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, Vol. 17(1) pp. 69 100.
Skolnick, J, and J. Fyfe. (1993). Above the law: Police and the excessive use of force. New
York: The Free Press.
Son, I., M. Davis and D. Rome. (1998). Race and its effect on police officersperceptions of
misconduct. Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 26(1), pp. 21 28.
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 237
Steffensmeier, D., J. Ulmer and J. Kramer. (1998). The interaction of race, gender, and age in
criminal sentencing: The punishment cost of being young, black, and male. Criminology,
Vol. 36, pp. 763797.
Sun, I., and B. Payne. (2004). Racial differences in resolving conflicts: A comparison between
Black and White police officers. Crime and Delinquency, Vol. 50, pp. 516 541.
Terrill, W. and S. Mastrofski. (2002). Situational and Officer Based Determinants of Police
Coercion. Justice Quarterly, Vol. 19(2), pp. 215 248.
Terrill, W., E. Paoline III and P. Manning. (2003). Police Culture and Coercion. Criminology,
Vol. 41(4), pp. 1003 1034.
Terrill, W. and M. Reisig. (2003). Neighborhood Context and Police Use of Force. Journal of
Research in Crime and Delinquency, Vol. 40(3), pp. 291 321.
Terrill, W. (2005). Police Use of Force: A Transactional Approach. Justice Quarterly, Vol.
22(1), pp. 107 138.
Terrill, W., F. Leinfelt and D. Kwak. (2008). Examining Police Use of Force: A Smaller
Agency Perspective. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies &
Management, Vol. 31(1), pp. 57 76.
Thompson, B. and J. Lee. (2004). Who Cares If Police Become Violent? Explaining Approval
of Police Use of Force Using a National Sample. Sociological Inquiry, Vol. 74(3), pp.
381 410.
Walker, S. (1997). Complaints Against the Police: A Focus Group Study of Citizen Perceptions,
Goals, and Expectations, Criminal Justice Review, Vol. 22 pp. 207226.
White, M. and J. Ready. (2007). The TASER as a Less Lethal Force Alternative: Findings on
Use and Effectiveness in a Large Metropolitan Police Agency. Police Quarterly, 10(2),
pp. 170-191.
Williams, J. and D. Westall. (2003). SWAT and non-SWAT police officers and the use of force.
Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 31, pp. 469 474.
Wilson, J. (1968). Varieties of Police Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, J. and G. Kelling. (1982), "Broken windows: the police and neighborhood safety",
Atlantic Monthly, pp.29-38.
238 Klahm & Tillyer —Understanding Police Use of Force (2010)
!
Wooldredge, J., T. Griffin, and T. Pratt. (2001). Considering Hierarchical Models for Research
on Inmate Behavior: Predicting Misconduct with Multilevel Data. Justice Quarterly, Vol.
18(1), pp. 203-231.
Worden, R. (1990). A Badge and a Baccalaureate: Polices, Hypotheses, and Further Evidence.
Justice Quarterly, Vol. 7(3), pp. 565-592.
The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 7 (2) 239
BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCHES
Charles F. Klahm is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Criminal Justice at St.
Joseph’s College, New York. His research interests include understanding police behavior,
especially their use of force, the use of evolving technology in policing, as well as how
technology influences crime prevention initiatives.
Rob Tillyer is an Assistant Professor at the University of Texas at San Antonio in the
Department of Criminal Justice. His research interests include the use of discretion within the
criminal justice system, police officer decision-making, victimization, crime prevention, and
criminological theories of crime. Recent journal articles have appeared in Justice Quarterly,
Journal of Criminal Justice and Policing.
... Findings like these are consistent and challenge the narrative that police (and non-police proxies) use more force towards minorities (Engel et al. 2000;Geller et al. 2021;James et al. 2014James et al. , 2013James et al. , 2016James et al. , 2018aKlahm and Tillyer 2010;Klinger 1994;Worrall et al. 2021). Furthermore, it is important to evaluate decision-making as a function of police-public relations with thorough experimental methodologies, especially when widespread criticisms of police may be sensationalized in non-systematic news and social media coverage. ...
... In the current experimental study, compliance by the on-screen actor was the strongest inoculator against participant touching the mock handgun. Additionally, none of the participants fired the mock handgun in any of the randomly assigned compliance scenarios, which is consistent with prior research (e.g., Engel et al. 2000;James et al. 2014James et al. , 2018aKlahm and Tillyer 2010;Klinger 1994;Novak and Engel 2005;Pickett and Nix 2019;Worden and Shepard 1996). The multivariate model, to this point, found that participants were significantly less likely to touch the mock handgun when the on-screen actor, Black or White, was compliant. ...
... Validated scales, of this kind, could demonstrate the complexities of police-public engagements. To that end, the demeanor and actions of members of the public have continually shown to affect police-public contacts, and, therefore, subsequent simulator-based research should observe these effects (Engel et al. 2000;James et al. 2018a;Klahm and Tillyer 2010;Klinger 1994;Novak and Engel 2005;Pickett and Nix 2019;Worden and Shepard 1996). ...
Article
The objective of this manuscript is to explore demographic, legal, and situational considerations on use-of-force decisions. The influence of implicit bias was measured in real time using a police training simulator in an experimental study with university students (n = 115). Participants were randomly assigned to one of four scenarios which varied according to the on-screen actor’s race (e.g., White/Black) and behavior (i.e., compliance/attack). Bivariate and multivariate regression models were used to estimate the effect of implicit bias on the decision to consider and use simulated lethal force. Actor behavior, independent of actor race, most influenced participant responses. These results suggest that simulated police-public interactions offer significant value in the assessment of implicit bias, particularly in the context of use-of-force decisions. Furthermore, absence of bias is inconsistent with current news and social media narratives about the existence of bias in officer decision-making.
... The existing literature on the use of force consistently exemplifies what impact civilian resistance has on the severity of force (Fridell and Lim 2016;Gau et al. 2010;Lawton 2007;Stroshine and Brandl 2019;Terrill and Mastrofski 2002). It also illustrates the methodological diversity, data sources, and analytical measurement that has been used to enhance our understanding of force interactions (Garner et al. 2002;Hollis 2018;Hollis and Jennings 2018;Klahm et al. 2014;Klahm and Tillyer 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Police brutality has become a contentious topic in both printed and social media. This has been fuelled by incidents in the United States where police officers were accused of torturing civilians during arrests. The purpose of this study is to provide an expert witness account of the use of force by law enforcement officials in a correctional setting. The study evaluates the effectiveness of the force factor analysis (resistance vs. sequential levels) that is used internationally to measure the appropriate force allowed in each situation. It further measures the force factor analysis against the different regulatory frameworks that provide oversight of the use of force by law enforcement agencies. A hermeneutics strategy was used to construct a criminological view of the incident. An inductive conventional content analytical approach was used for the subjective interpretation of text data content through the systematic classification process of coding and identifying themes or patterns. The finding indicates that the Resistance Force Comparative Scale is only effective if consideration is given to other environmental dynamics that have an impact on the situation at hand. The effects of prisoner resistance on the severity of force used by correctional staff members are explored, along with implications and recommendations.
... In the same direction, some research suggests that departments with more female officers tend to present fewer police shooting deaths (Carmichael & Kent, 2015). Nevertheless, contrasting findings indicate nonsignificant findings regarding the impact of an officer's gender (Cojean et al., 2023;Hine et al., 2018b;Hoffman & Hickey, 2005;Klahm & Tillyer, 2010;Lawton, 2007;Noppe, 2018;Paoline et al., 2021;Paoline III & Terrill, 2007;Rydberg & Terrill, 2010;Schell-Busey et al., 2023;Schuck & Rabe-Hemp, 2005;Stroshine & Brandl, 2020;Terrill et al., 2008). ...
... Providing real-time solutions to non-trivial problems is part of the police force's daily routine (Suchman 2007) which sometimes necessitates the use of constraint for the preservation of public order. Literature on the subject shows that the decision to use legitimate violence by law enforcement officers is based on several factors (Bolger 2015;Klahm and Tillyer 2010). Whilst the absolute rationality defended by Descartes (1693) has long been idealised, the decision to use legitimate violence is marked by elements over which we have no control (e.g. the environment or the behaviour of the suspect) and according to a particular chronology (Terrill 2003). ...
Article
Our study aimed to investigate a link between impulsivity and use of force amongst French gendarmes. We expected that higher scores on an impulsivity scale (UPPS) would lead to a higher level of coercion, both in frequency and intensity. We also sought to verify whether the French National Gendarmerie was in line with current literature regarding the influence of age, gender, seniority, and level of education on the use of force. Questionnaires were sent to members of the French National Gendarmerie via professional e-mail addresses. Responses were collected over a 1-month period. The questionnaires were accepted when the responses to an impulsivity scale (UPPS) and to a scenario had been completed. Questions on demographic data were also included. The sample consisted of 326 French gendarmes. Participants were divided into three age groups: 18–25 (N = 43, 13.2%), 26–40 (N = 139, 42.6%), and > 41 (N = 144, 44.2%). Seventy-six women (23.3%) and 250 men (76.7%) responded. Most participants were career NCOs (N = 265, 81.3%); the others were volunteer assistant gendarmes or gendarmes under contract (N = 25, 7.67%) or commissioned officers (N = 36, 11.0%). Only urgency and lack of premeditation seemed to have an influence on the use of force, particularly regarding younger novice officers, increasing significantly the means of coercion used. On the other hand, gender and education level had only a slight non-significant influence, whilst age and seniority significantly reduce the use of force. Older and more experienced officers used legitimate violence to a lesser extent. This study shows the strong influence of age and years of service on the use of force. These results indicate a need for more extensive initial training. For some of its subscales, impulsivity also had a direct influence on the amount and intensity of coercion used by law enforcement officers in their daily work. Therefore, courses on impulsivity control could be useful for police officers. Further research should be conducted on actual police interventions to avoid any bias related to behavioural intent.
... conclude that economic inequality and racial variables are unrelated to police killings, as other environmental variables can explain why blacks are more likely to be killed in the US(Jacobs and O'Brien 1998: 853)853.In a survey of multivariate analyses of the determinants of police use of fatal and nonfatal force in the US,Klahm and Tillyer (2010) point out that despite several decades of research, evidence is still mixed, even for essential structural variables such as race and social class. While the authors do not document negative findings, positive findings are countered by mixed results (race/ethnicity: 2 positive, 7 mixed, 8 no relationship; social class: 5 positive, 2 mixed, 2 no relationship). ...
Book
Full-text available
This PRIF Report analyzes police use of lethal force in Brazil and the Philippines, two countries with comparatively high levels of police lethality and upward trends in recent years after political shifts toward iron-fisted policies.Based on competing explanations proposed by conflict theory and consensus theory, it analyzes how far variation can be explained by structural variables such as demography, economy, and crime levels on police use of deadly force at the subnational level. The results challenge both theories, suggesting that politics may be a relevant trigger for coercive policing.
Article
Police officers have a great deal of discretion in any given police-citizen encounter (Rydberg and Terrill in Police Q 13(1):92–120, 2010). However, especially in use-of-force cases, it is a controversial facet of police work. Previously, scholars have pointed out the importance of empirical research on factors that influence police officers’ use of force decisions. The present scoping review provides an update of recent empirical studies. The review spans 2006–2023 and analysed 102 articles published in major journals. The findings generally showed a wide range of possible factors influencing police decision-making in use-of-force cases in interactions with citizens. Understanding police discretion in use-of-force decisions appears to be complex. Future research should aim to develop an integrative theoretical framework for understanding police use of force encompassing different levels of analysis.
Thesis
Full-text available
The New York Times reported that Black Lives Matter was the third most-read subject of 2020. These articles brought to the forefront the question of disparity in arrest rates for darker-skinned people. Questioning arrest disparity is understandable because virtually everything known about disproportionate arrest rates has been a guess, and virtually all prior research on disproportionate arrest rates is questionable because of improper benchmarking (the denominator effect). Current research has highlighted the need to switch from demographic data to skin tone data and start over on disproportionate arrest rates research; therefore, this study explored the relationship between skin tone and disproportionate arrest rates. This study also sought to determine which of the three theories surrounding disproportionate arrests is most predictive of disproportionate rates. The current theories are that disproportionate arrests increase as skin tone gets darker (stereotype threat theory), disproportionate rates are different for Black and Brown people (self-categorization theory), or disproportionate rates apply equally across all darker skin colors (social dominance theory). This study used a quantitative exploratory quasi-experimental design using linear spline regression to analyze arrest rates in Alachua County, Florida, before and after the county’s mandate to reduce arrests as much as possible during the COVID-19 pandemic to protect the prison population. The study was exploratory as no previous study has used skin tone analysis to examine arrest disparity. The findings of this study enhance the understanding of the existence and nature of disparities in arrest rates and offer a solid foundation for additional studies about the relationship between disproportionate arrest rates and skin color.
Chapter
The use of firearms by the police is a course of action that is probably the most severe option at their disposal. Other than in the USA, the use of firearms by the police is only taken in the rarest of occasions in the European jurisdictions. This chapter begins with an outline of the research pertaining to this subject area. Subsequently, the situation concerning the use of firearms by the police in Germany and Europe is described. Building on this foundation, the authors describe several fundamental scientific findings on aspects of particular importance before (basic and advanced training), during (deciding, experiencing, and hitting), and after shooting (consequences for shooters and targeted persons). The chapter concludes with an overview of the current state of research with regard to various problems associated with the use of firearms by the police.
Article
Full-text available
Since the 1960s, one of the major reform efforts in law enforcement has been to increase the number of Black Americans within police agencies and on patrol in the streets. The general premise behind these efforts has been that increased diversity will improve police–community relations and will decrease biased police behavior, particularly against Black citizens. Policies seeking to reform policing through increasing the numbers of African American officers have been implemented with little empirical evidence that an officer's race (or ethnicity) is actually related to their behavior towards citizens, in particular arrest deci-sions. Using data from systematic social observations of police–citizen encoun-ters in Cincinnati, OH, this study examines the influence of officer race on arrest outcomes, focusing on the behavior of Black officers. Findings suggest that officer race has direct influence on arrest outcomes and there are substan-tive differences between White and Black officers in the decision to arrest. In general, White officers in our study were more likely to arrest suspects than Black officers, but Black suspects were more likely to be arrested when the decision maker was a Black officer. Keywords officer race; arrest; police discretion; citizen race Robert A. Brown is an Assistant Professor in the School of Public and Environmental Affairs at Indiana University Purdue University Indianapolis. His research interests include officer decision making and police control mechanisms. James Frank is an Associate Professor in the Division of Criminal Justice at the University of Cincinnati. Recently he has directed police observation studies examining the work routines of street-level officers in the Cincinnati Police Division and 21 small-town police agencies. His research interests include officer decision making and citizens' attitudes toward the police. Correspondence to: Robert A. Brown,
Article
Violent, but non-lethal, encounters between police and civilians are an important yet neglected social problem. Even more neglected are the spatial and temporal dynamics of these events. The paper draws on Giddens's structuration theory to operationalize these variables and test their explanatory power. A multivariate analysis of data derived from citizen complaints files shows that time-space variables are important predictors of the use of major physical force by police against civilians. An explanation for this result is developed using Giddens's concept of practical consciousness. The findings of the paper suggest a need to redirect criminological and policing research towards the exploration of time-space environments.
Article
"This book . . . examines the problem of police corruption . . . in such a way that the stereotype of the crude, greedy cop who is basically a grown-up delinquent, if not an out-and-out robber, yields to portraits of particular men, often of earnest good will and even more than ordinary compassion, contending with an enormously demanding and challenging job."—Robert Coles, New Yorker "Other social scientists have observed policemen on patrol, or have interviewed them systematically. Professor Muir has brought the two together, and, because of the philosophical depth he brings to his commentaries, he has lifted the sociology of the police on to a new level. He has both observed the men and talked with them at length about their personal lives, their conceptions of society and of the place of criminals within it. His ambition is to define the good policeman and to explain his development, but his achievement is to illuminate the philosophical and occupational maturation of patrol officers in 'Laconia' (a pseudonym) . . . . His discussions of [the policemen's] moral development are threaded through with analytically suggestive formulations that bespeak a wisdom very rarely encountered in reports of sociological research."—Michael Banton, Times Literary Supplement
Article
This essay reviews the rise of community-and problem-oriented policing as major vehicles to improve the effectiveness of police efforts in communities and as means of reforming police organiza-tions. The essay considers the historical development of various mod-els of policing, examining the assumptions embedded in each of these often-competing emphases. The essay goes on to review extant research on the impacts of community policing on communities, police organizations, police work, and police officers. Findings from various studies suggest that community and problem-oriented polic-ing have had modest impacts on community crime but larger impacts on the quality of interaction between the police and the public. In addition, extant research suggests that police organizations are slow-ly adopting the philosophy and practices of community and problem-oriented policing and have shown some change in police structure and service delivery. Changes associated with problem solving within police agencies are less evident in the research literature. More often than not, the police are using traditional approaches to respond to problems identified in community settings. Finally, the research liter-ature suggests that police officers' conception of their roles and their attachment to police work are improving with the adoption of com-munity and problem-oriented policing roles.