Content uploaded by Peter Donald Griffiths
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Peter Donald Griffiths on Apr 30, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
Infection prevention as “a show”: A qualitative study of nurses’ infection prevention
behaviours
Carole Jackson DHC, King’s College London
Karen Lowton, PhD, King’s College London
Peter Griffiths, PhD University of Southampton
This is a pre-print version of a paper published as:
Jackson, C., Lowton, K., Griffiths, P., 2013. Infection prevention as “a show”: A qualitative study of nurses’ infection
prevention behaviours. International Journal of Nursing Studies online early
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2013.07.002.
Copyright is vested in the publisher of the paper. You may not copy or redistribute
this work in any way without their permission.
Infection prevention as “a show”: A qualitative study of nurses’ infection prevention
behaviours
Background; Control of infection and prevention of healthcare associated infections is an
ongoing issue worldwide. Yet despite initiatives and strategies to reduce the burden that these
infections cause, healthcare workers’ practice is still reported as suboptimal and these
infections persist. Much of the research to date has primarily focused on predicting infection
prevention behaviours and factors associated with guideline compliance. While this has given
valuable insight, an investigation aiming to understand and explain behaviours that occur in
everyday practice from the perspective of the actors themselves may hold the key to the
challenges of effecting behaviour change. This study questioned “How can nurses’ infection
prevention behaviour be explained?” This paper presents one of three identified themes
‘Rationalizing dirt-related behaviour’.
Design; This interpretative qualitative study uses vignettes, developed from nurses’ accounts
of practice, to explore nurses’ reported infection prevention behaviours
Participants; Registered nurses working in an acute hospital setting and had been qualified
for over a year. They were recruited while studying part-time at a London University.
Methods; Twenty semi-structured interviews were undertaken using a topic guide and
vignettes. Interviews were transcribed verbatim and analysed using the framework method.
Results; The findings demonstrate that participants were keen to give a good impression and
present themselves as knowledgeable practitioners, although it was evident that they did not
always follow procedure and policy. They rationalized their own behaviour and logically
justified any deviations from policy. Deviations in others were criticised as irrational and
explained as superficial and part of a ‘show’ or display. However, participants also gave a
presentation of themselves: a show or display that was influenced by the desire to protect self
and satisfy patient scrutiny.
Conclusions; this study contributes to the identification and explanation of nurses’ infection
prevention behaviours which are considered inappropriate or harmful. Behaviour is
multifaceted and complex, stemming from a response to factors that are outside a purely
‘scientific’ understanding of infection and not simply understood as a deficit in knowledge.
This calls for educational interventions that consider beliefs, values and social understanding
of dirt and infection.
Key words- infection prevention, healthcare associated infection, qualitative research,
compliance, nurse, Goffman,
What is already known about the topic?
• Healthcare associated infections are a continuing problem within healthcare, and are
costly to both health services and users.
• Current policy and guidance provide education and monitoring tools to record and
audit infection prevention practices but do not examine the root cause of non-
compliance and inappropriate behaviour.
What this paper adds?
• Inappropriate infection prevention does occur in practice but is attributed to others’
behaviour rather than own
• Behaviour is often self-protecting and may be part of a socially constructed reality
where a show is performed to convince the audience that practice is based on
knowledge
• Complex social behaviour requires multifaceted interventions which existing policy
and guidance do not always provide.
Introduction
Infection control and prevention of healthcare associated infections are an essential part of
healthcare. While there is a body of work that examines factors affecting compliance with
guidelines and is aimed at predicting infection prevention behaviours, some behaviour that
occurs in everyday practice remains unexplained (Pittet 2004). Examining such behaviours
may provide a key insight into the challenges of behaviour change and may ultimately inform
new initiatives aimed at improving practice, increasing quality of patient care and enhancing
infection prevention.
As far back as 1860, Florence Nightingale emphasised the importance of hygiene, cleanliness
and standards of care, yet despite this infections in hospitals and other healthcare settings
continue to be a major concern for health services (Department of Health (DH) DH 2009).
While today’s hospitals are much cleaner, safer places than in the 19th and early 20th
centuries, dirt and infection still threaten patient safety (DH 2009) through transmission
within the hospital environment. However, despite current scientific knowledge and policies,
beliefs and practices associated with cleanliness do not always accord with the implications
of the ‘rational’ scientific approach or even immediate objective evidence (Morrow at al
2011). For example, despite a clear recognition of the importance of hand washing in
reducing transmission of microorganisms, compliance by health professionals is often poor
and protective equipment is not always used appropriately (Pittet 2000).
In 2011 the World Health Organisation (WHO 2011) reported that healthcare associated
infections accounted for 16 million additional days in hospital throughout Europe with total
costs estimated at approximately €7 billion, while in the USA the estimated total cost per year
was $6.5 billion. In the UK, the cost of treatment and management of healthcare associated
infections has continued to rise with recent estimates of €53.9 million per year, attributed to
the resulting increased length of stay (WHO 2011).
In the first decade of the 21st century, the control of healthcare acquired infection, most
notably the globally problematic Meticillin-resistant Staphylococcus aureus (MRSA),
became a major focus of UK health policy as rates of infection were perceived to be too high
(National Audit Office (NAO) 2009). Repeated UK government initiatives (DH 2001a,
2001b, 2003, 2004, 2005, 2007, 2008) led to some reported improvements in control (NAO
2009); however evidence suggests that infection spread continues to be poorly understood by
healthcare workers and the general population, with practices not underpinned by sound
knowledge and evidence (Easton et al 2007, Nichols and Badger 2008, Morrow et al 2011).
Furthermore, although the need to understand infection prevention behaviour has been
identified as a key factor in improving practice and a significant step towards modifying
behaviour (Pittet 2004), little research has been undertaken regarding the motivation behind
specific behaviours. Some studies have identified why certain procedures and practices are
not carried out, for example handwashing, but few have considered workers’ behaviours as a
whole or investigated the key determinants to behaviour and infection prevention practices
(Pittet 2004, Whitby et al 2006).
Despite training and education, compliance with good practice remains variable. Evidence
suggests this is affected by many factors, including perception of one’s own practice and
intention, motivation, perception of threat and social or peer pressure (Chan et al 2002, Stein
et al 2003, Pittet 2004, Akyol et al 2006). Additionally, knowledge does not necessarily
correlate with good practice; low compliance with standard precautions has been noted in
those who reported a high level of conflict between providing patient care and the need to
protect themselves (Gould 2004). It has been argued that appropriate responses to infection
only occur when there is a perceived risk and when efficacy is expected (Jenner et al 2002).
Jenner et al (2002) cite self-protection as a motivating factor even when the main
organizational purpose is patient protection and infection reduction. Personal responsibility
and attitudes are predictors of intention to practice hand hygiene, with behaviour, to some
extent, being predicted by perceived behaviour, control and intent (Jenner et al 2002).
Behaviour, which is influenced by both rational and irrational thoughts, may therefore not be
congruent with policy. Paradoxically, policies perceived as rational by government agencies
may be adhered to even when healthcare workers believe they are not effective, or may be
adapted to accommodate irrational fears (Kennedy et al 2004). Any behaviour deemed
inappropriate warrants further investigation and may require considerable examination and
interpretation before the rationale behind it can be explained. Healthcare workers by the very
nature of their role may find that their behaviour is influenced and conflicted by automated
thinking and what is learnt through education and training, or seen in clinical practice (Curtis
2007); this conflict may play a significant part in how they behave. Curtis (2007) discusses
how disgust of dirt is part of human nature, with dirt and disgust both stemming from the
cultural construction in which we live combined with an element of “gut feeling” (p661).
Similarly infection prevention and hygiene behaviours carried out by healthcare workers as a
group may actually be a socially constructed concept which can be understood by identifying
the interpretations, knowledge and reality that have informed the behaviour.
This paper presents one of three themes identified in a larger study that aimed to understand
nurses’ infection prevention behaviours within acute hospitals by eliciting nurses’
explanations of observed behaviours. The main study gained insight into perceptions of risk
regarding infection prevention, the behaviours nurses adopted to reduce risk and the belief
systems they were operating from.
We present here one theme in which participants explained their own and others’ behaviours
in different terms; specifically they could recognise inappropriate behaviour in others but not
in themselves. There was a sense that participants were at ease rationalizing their own
behaviour but that they were unable to do this for others. The notion that a display or ‘show’
was being performed also emerged with some recognition from participants that this may be
driven by both the desire to appear knowledgeable and an awareness of patient scrutiny.
These findings, and the explanations they offer, have significant potential to impact and
influence on nurses’ infection prevention behaviour (Jackson 2011).
Methods
This interpretative qualitative study used in-depth interviews in order to explore nurses’
perceptions of risk and contagion, and the explanations they provided for their own and
others’ behaviours. Twenty individual interviews were conducted over a 14 month period by
CJ. Registered nurses, who were undertaking part-time post qualification education at
university, were recruited. Inclusion criteria were, qualified for one year or more, working in
an acute setting, not studying a course that had infection prevention as its focus. The sample
was both convenient and purposeful. Ethical approval was granted by King’s College London
(ref 06/07-7).
Interviews which lasted between 30-45 minutes were conducted in two stages. In the first
stage eight participants were interviewed using a topic guide developed from the literature.
The topic guide had 3 foci: perception of risk and contagion in the hospital setting; concerns
relating to transmission of infection to family and friends with precautions taken to prevent
this; and whether policies were appropriate and influential. Participants were also asked to
recall an incident where they had observed or had carried out infection prevention behaviours
that they deemed inappropriate, for example wearing gloves and aprons when not required.
Following transcription of these interviews, two vignettes were developed that represented
observed practices. Both vignettes included similar observations based on behaviour
identified by the first 8 participants, only behaviours identified by two or more participants
were considered for the vignette development. Each vignette represented an acute ward
setting divided into four bays. In each bay a specific behaviour was described that could have
been considered inappropriate or outside the recommended policy and guidelines, for
example the use of gloves and protective clothing where there was no identified infection risk
or for carrying out menial tasks where there was no actual patient contact. These vignettes
were used with the topic guide to elicit explanations of the behaviours from the remaining 12
interviewees (table 1). Participants were asked if they recognised the behaviours in the
vignettes, if they considered them appropriate or inappropriate and what explanations they
could offer for the behaviours described.
In bay one, the beds are being made, both staff members are wearing gloves. One wears the
same gloves for the whole bay, the other wears double gloves and after 4 beds removes the
outer pair and continues to make the remaining 4 with one pair on.
In the next bay a nurse is recording the blood pressures of all the patients. She is wearing
gloves and an apron which she keeps on for all 8 patients. One patient is having a bed bath;
her nurse is wearing gloves and an apron to wash her.
Table 1-Excerpt from vignette
Interviews were audio taped and transcribed verbatim and all potentially identifying text
anonymised, participants’ details were kept confidential and all were allocated pseudonyms
to maintain anonymity. All interviews were analysed using a set of predetermined analytical
phases as part of the Framework method (Ritchie and Spencer 1994). Familiarization of the
data occurred by a process of immersion with initial themes recorded. By drawing on the
emerging themes and the interview topic guide, and by recording any recurrence or prevailing
patterning, a thematic framework was identified. Indexing of the data was then completed by
applying the framework to the verbatim transcripts of the interviews. Finally the data were
considered as a whole. In the final interviews participants confirmed what others had said and
with no new information or themes emerging it was decided that enough data had been
generated to answer the question. Three main themes emerged. Participants were sent
transcripts of their own interviews for comment as part of the member checking process. The
final 6 participants were also sent emerging themes for comment and confirmation. No
negative responses or discrepancies were received from any participants regarding the
content of the interviews, analysis or emerging themes. In order to increase credibility an
independent academic reviewed all transcripts and identified comparable themes within them.
Participants
Participants ranged in age from 24-53 years with the largest group (40%) being between 24-
29 years. Thirteen women and seven men were interviewed. Level of previous nursing
education was University diploma (roughly equivalent to an associate degree, 60%),
bachelors degree (30%) and postgraduate degree (10%). Years of experience in nursing
ranged from 1½ to 20.
Findings
The findings present the theme ‘Rationalizing dirt related behaviours’. This theme, which
has 3 subthemes, identifies important aspects of behaviour and considers participants’
explanations and rationalization of these; both their own and that of others they reported
observing. Participants explained their own and others’ behaviours in different terms;
significantly they could recognise inappropriate behaviour in others but not in themselves.
There was a sense that participants were at ease rationalizing and logically justifying their
own behaviour if it was outside recommended policy and guidelines, but that they were
unable to do this for others. The notion that a display or ‘show’ was being performed
emerged with some recognition that this may be driven by both the desire to appear
knowledgeable and an awareness of patient scrutiny.
Rationalizing and rationalizing the irrational
The study found that nurses rationalize their own behaviour even if they recognise that it does
not fit with recommended practice, ultimately always seeing their own practice as correct.
The implication that the participant’s own practice was in line with infection prevention
policy at the time was clear from the first interview; all participants presented themselves as
knowledgeable practitioners who understood infection prevention practices. Alan for example
stated that his own practices were good and followed policy:
“My own infection control practices are good and I follow the policies. I’m always careful
when patients are in side rooms and make sure I don’t bring anything out, making sure I’ve
washed my hands” Alan
This was reiterated in all interviews; participants did not recognise their own practice as
anything other than correct. Participants also gave the impression that it was important that
their knowledge of the scientific rationale and good infection prevention practices were
recorded, presenting themselves as knowledgeable doers who carried out correct procedures.
For example, Carol commented that she never worried about cross infection because her own
practices were so good, while Betty saw her hand hygiene as very thorough:
“…and I’ve never worried and I think that’s probably more down to my own practices than
luck” Carol.
“No, I think my hand washing is probably a bit more thorough, up to the top of your uniform
and spending about five minutes washing your hands if you’ve something that’s really
infected” Betty.
Participants implied that ‘normal’ hygiene rules do not apply if something is perceived as
somehow ‘more’ infected. The behaviour is rationalized; because it is ‘more’ infected, a
distinction is made between ‘infected’ and ‘more infected’. The latter poses a greater risk and
therefore behaviour beyond rational policy is required and justified. Only one participant saw
her practice as less than ideal, identifying that she perhaps was a risk taker towards herself
but never at the expense of a patient; she also has a presentation she is trying to convey; ‘I do
take risks with myself, however I am a good nurse and would never put my patients at risk’.
For all the other participants their practice seemed to be beyond reproach. However, as the
discussion developed it became apparent that nurses’ behaviour did deviate from policy if
they saw any risk to themselves, for example:
“as a nurse, as a professional practitioner, it is up to you to rationalize what you deem is
good and safe.....and as long as you rationalize and are aware of the consequences of your
action, I think that’s more important rather than just following guidelines” Peter.
This change to procedure was then rationalized so participants could maintain the perception
that their practice was in keeping with policy and infection prevention. Nurses’ presentation
that they fully complied with policy seemed to be the default position; only when behaviour
was examined much more closely did the “what I say I do” and the “what I really do” become
differentiated.
Participants associated any deviation in their behaviour outside hygiene and infection
prevention policies with the desire to avoid dirt, germs, bacteria and anything that was
unknown. For participants dirt could contain germs and bacteria, which while not actually
responsible for a named infection, could still potentially be a risk to them and they were
protecting themselves. Participants identified that patient contact also involved the risk of
contact with infection and dirt. However infection had tightly defined boundaries and could
be clearly identified whereas dirt could compose of germs, bacteria and waste products but
not necessarily any identified infection. Germs and bacteria were considered as dirty and
unclean, but not infectious until identified as such by microbiological testing. Infection, once
identified, could be treated accordingly with actions that were supported by the policy
recommendations. When there was an identified infection the behaviour was clear; a threat
had been identified and there were specific procedures to follow. Self was threatened by
infections and, as there was risk involved, precautions were needed. A definitive diagnosis of
infection gave the participants the information they needed to decide what precautions to
take. Most felt that their own practice and that of others improved when there was an
identified infection. For example, Betty worked in an area where she considered others’
practice to be generally poor, however she reported that infection did improve other nurses’
practice:
“If something is obviously infected, they [nurses] are better because they want to protect
themselves as well” Betty.
However if acknowledging a situation where no infection risk existed, participants
rationalized the irrationality of any of their own inappropriate protective behaviour by
explaining that they were protecting themselves from dirt and uncleanliness which was
unpleasant and unknown. They also identified how they assessed risk of the activity and
acted appropriately, for example Irene felt that she knew about risk activities and would
always make the correct judgement:
“Yes, and, you know, you know the difference between a major risk and something that if you
wash your hands and say, you know, you rationalize it” Irene.
The participants consistently presented themselves and their practice as beyond reproach. The
presentation to the researcher was one of a knowledgeable practitioner who rationalized their
own practice. Infection prevention practices allowed them to rationalize the irrational or
indeed to rationalize the human nature instinct to protect even when evidence demonstrated
that it was not required. When they chose to ignore or deviate from any formal hygiene or
infection prevention policy, rather than acknowledging that this was irrational, they found
ways to justify it, identifying a risk beyond the remit of the policy.
Rationalizing or condemning irrational behaviour in others
Having clearly identified their own practice and rationalized all aspects, participants
subsequently condemned any inappropriate behaviour they had witnessed in others as
irrational, or rationalized it as being a self protection mechanism in a situation where there
was no risk. The participants appeared not to see any similarities between their own practice
when they acted outside the policies and ‘rules’, and the practice of others who were
perceived as doing the same; they did not acknowledge that others may have similar reasons
for their behaviour. Olivia commented that
“If you are washing somebody, you are just washing......So, what are you protecting from? I
have no idea what they are protecting themselves from personally. The person is not having
any skin disease and the person is just like me and you” Olivia
The behaviours of others were clearly divided into either; the irrational, described as lack of
thinking and ridiculous practice which went against common sense and was completely
without explanation; or rationalized as protection, for example:
“I think that’s what it is; they’re seeing it as a protection for themselves. Yes, which is
ridiculous” Irene.
This is in sharp contrast to their own behaviour which while acknowledged as self protection
was not recognised as anything other than acceptable. In addition, the protective behaviour of
others was seen purely as protection for self, without any thought or consideration for the
patient:
“It goes back to that thing about protecting yourself rather than thinking about the wider
environment and the implications for the people” Steven.
While the participants had not made the concern for patients explicit in the discussion
regarding their own behaviours, perhaps demonstrating that their ‘good’ practice following
policy and rules implicitly meant that they had the patient’s interests at heart. Other nurses’
inappropriate behaviour was seen as being driven by a desire to follow their own rules and to
act outside the recognised behaviour, even though they knew the scientific rationale behind
the policies and procedures. It was not generally seen as a lack of education in qualified
nurses, more of an attitude or mindset where the desire to protect self took precedence over
all scientific rationale. In addition participants described observing inappropriate behaviour
of others such as glove wearing, which in an attempt to protect self, actually increased risk
because they failed to change the gloves between patients, for example,
“Yes, their main concern would be themselves, although if they looked at it a bit more, they
are thinking, ‘That patient has got this, so we don’t want to spread it around.’ Although they
are [spreading it]” Kenneth
The participants saw this incorrect wearing of protective clothing as self protecting and poor
practice. All participants agreed that they themselves would only wear gloves when needed,
either because there was infection or there was risk of contact with dirt, whereas they felt that
others carrying out inappropriate protection practices did so because they were not thinking
about the scientific rationale behind the practice and only considering themselves. No thought
was being given to the patient in terms of how they felt or the increased risk that
inappropriate behaviour could pose. Some participants even thought the behaviour may
simply be driven by laziness or a desire to save time by using gloves as an alternative to hand
washing:
“Or maybe they just can’t be bothered to wash their hands thoroughly, hand washing again,
you know. Some people take off these gloves and just one pump of alcohol gel and that’s it.
That is not disinfected properly,” Linda
The participants recognised and always condemned any incorrect and inappropriate
behaviour in others. When they recognised that their own behaviour or practice was the same
or similar, they explained their actions as a defence against dirt and as an avoidance strategy.
They did not however offer this as an explanation for others’ behaviour.
The display of practice: is it all a show?
The final subtheme identified that role modelling and leading by example, whether to peers
or patients, was a recurring topic and suggested that the display of correct procedure was the
basis of improving practice. Although there was concern voiced by participants that an
incorrect display would do more harm than good, any practice witnessed often enough could
be taken as the norm and become part of routine practice. Roger verbalized how when
patients see certain procedures they come to accept them as the norm:
[Patients think] “they are [nurses] just doing it because they’re being asked to follow the
procedures and that’s all it is.’ So that maybe it exactly” Roger
When considering others’ inappropriate behaviour, participants identified that this was an
attempt by the other staff to give the impression that infection prevention and transmission
were taken seriously. There seemed to be an assumption that the overt display of wearing
protective clothing by nurses was to give the impression to patients that they were being
thorough, and taking precautions. Even the participants themselves accepted that sometimes
they were trying to give the right impression:
“at times, even in the clinical area, they [patients] do watch us as health professionals. They
do watch us as to how we provide care.... You see. They do watch us how we provide care, so
that also should give us an indication that it’s something that is what they’re thinking about
seriously” Geoffrey,
“You could argue, and I think that...., whether there should be a lip service to the fact that,
that we wear gloves, observationally, if someone is checking you out, or alternatively if
you’re seen to wear gloves, then that seems to be more, more of an indicator that you take
these things” [seriously]…Steven
The concept of making the right impression or performing for the patients, were recurring
themes that participants recognised both in their own practice and in others, for example
“So you’re doing it [wearing gloves] to prevent yourself being basically assessed by the
patient as incompetent or not maintaining hygiene standards....... We’re under thorough
scrutiny by patients. They see everything you do...So I think it’s a bit of both really. Yes, so
I think we do that to satisfy the patients” Linda
This overt wearing of protective clothing and the impression it gave was reiterated by Roger,
who on the one hand felt that patients may be offended by being washed by someone wearing
gloves asking “What is wrong with me?” while on the other hand the patient may feel that
the nurses were only following procedure and that it was nothing personal, because they
knew the nurses and knew they were preventing infection – even if in reality there was no
infection risk and the display was not actually necessary: However the data suggests an
acknowledgement that wearing gloves to wash a patient could be offensive and may make the
patient feel dirty, with most participants recognising that wearing gloves to carry out simple
procedures could be interpreted in different ways:
“I mean I won’t go to every patient with gloves and apron because it makes them feel like
they are dirty when they are not” Kenneth.
“I think they feel, in one way they probably feel embarrassed because they think that people
think that they’re dirty or sometimes there are reasons why you should wear gloves and
aprons” Peter.
Although the idea that patients are dirty is often what participants were saying when they
discussed protective behaviour; if they suspect dirt they take precautions. Carol sums this up
quite succinctly:
“Well I have seen nurses wash patients as well with gloves on, … that to me is awful, because
to me that says that you’re not clean, so I would imagine that some of the patients feel that
way. I’ve never had a patient complain to me about a nurse not wearing gloves” Carol
Carol implies that patients could pick up on this and get the impression that they are being
perceived as dirty or unclean and there was some recognition that a number of patients
preferred the use of gloves, seeing it as less personal or that the correct procedure was being
carried out. However patients can only see this as correct procedure if that is the impression
they have been given, i.e. if they are convinced by the performance. If the display has been
consistently that gloves are worn for all patient contact then this will become accepted as the
norm.
Discussion
For participants it seemed important that their own good practice and presentation were
acknowledged in the first instance before any further discussion regarding inappropriate
behaviour or less desirable practice could be considered. This presentation of self in the
initial stages of the interview has some links with Goffman’s (1959) work on presentation of
self in everyday life which discusses how those providing a service present themselves in
their role and give a particular impression. For Goffman this presentation is used to give a
good impression of the service and the part they play in it. It seemed important to participants
that their own good practice and presentation was acknowledged in the first instance before
any further discussion regarding inappropriate behaviour or less desirable practice could be
considered.
Having given this presentation of themselves it then became apparent that despite their
reports of correct procedures at all times, policy was not always carried out if risk to self was
identified. Whilst identified infection simplified behaviour because the rules were clear and
protection was needed against harmful micro-organisms (Whitby et al 2006), where there was
no actual identified infection participants rationalized their behaviour as protection from the
unknown and the unpleasantness of dirt. This change to procedure was then rationalized so
participants could maintain the perception that their practice was in keeping with policy and
infection prevention.
Participants failed to see the similarities between their practice and the practice of others and
offered different reasons for this behaviour. They understood their own behaviour because
they knew the reason for it, they were responding to a certain set of external circumstances
that justified their behaviour. This logical justification allowed them to rationalize their own
behaviour and continue to see it as appropriate and as long as the rationale was sufficient it
was permissible. They also suggested that in some instances healthcare workers are only
really paying lip service to any form of infection prevention by carrying out some form of
practice that is either incorrect or incomplete. The actual process is not being thought through
and there is an appearance of correct behaviour but only on a superficial level. The early
work of Roth (1957) considered infection prevention practices that were not based on rational
scientific evidence but had become a ritual of expected and agreed behaviour. Roth identified
that inconsistencies in practice, both in compliance and correct usage, rendered the practice to
be of no benefit at best and a risk at worst. Similarly participants in the current study saw the
incorrect wearing of protective clothing not only as symbolic and self protecting, but also
poor practice and a vehicle to increase the risk of infection. Where participants themselves
had acted in the same way, they accepted that their behaviour was based on an avoidance or
disgust of others rather than a scientific rationale; they did not however recognise the same
level of thinking in others and the attribution of behaviour differed (Morrow et al 2011).
The results of this study demonstrate similarities to the work of Morrow et al (2011) who
considered causal attribution theory when trying to determine the causes of behaviour in
relation to the control of MRSA. They demonstrated that when clinicians attributed MRSA
acquisition they consistently overestimated the extent to which this occurred in areas other
than their own, and as a direct result on the actions of others. Conversely, they
underestimated the role that they themselves played in this acquisition and the extent to
which transmission occurred in their own clinician areas. MRSA acquisitions and
transmission was seen as something that occurred elsewhere and as a result of the actions of
others. Morrow et al (2011) concluded that although in some cases transmission had occurred
elsewhere, when there was any degree of uncertainly acquisition was attributed to other
settings. Rather as the current study, Morrow et al also identified that staff commonly
demonstrated favourable bias towards their own performances. When considering risks both
studies identified externals factors, such as others poor infection prevention practice, as
presenting the greatest risk. When attributing our own behaviour we have information
available that we do not have when observing others. We are aware of our own external
influences and internalise less, furthermore there is a tendency to attribute behaviour to
whatever makes us look the best, a self serving attribution process. Participants identified that
they were at risk from others within the healthcare setting, and regardless of education and
understanding they are ultimately part of the wider population and their perceptions and the
reality they constructed was influenced by external forces. However while participants saw
the patients as ‘others’ who are vulnerable and the cause of disease (Joffe 1999) they also
perceived a risk to themselves from these ‘others’ through contact with dirt, uncleanliness
and the unknown. Distinct groups are identified from the data; self whose practice is rational
or can be rationalized, and the ‘others’ who in this instance are both staff members and the
patient. Crucially however, risk to self from others was highlighted rather than risk to others
from the participants.
The final subtheme draws more significantly on the impact that patients have on this
behaviour. The description of a display that nurses are giving evolves where nurses, under
patient scrutiny, are putting on a show of knowing what to do, giving the right impression,
and carrying out the correct procedures. This again has resonance with the work of Goffman
(1959) who suggests that any individual playing a part is asking the audience to take the
impression they are giving seriously. The audience is asked to accept that the actor has the
attributes the performance suggests and that the task they are performing has “the
consequences that are implicitly claimed” (p28). This behaviour may however be nothing
more than a show, especially if the performance is not based on evidence and best practice,
but a desire to please or reassure the audience.
However the data in this study also suggest that wearing gloves as a part of the display,
particularly when not required, could be interpreted by patients as a sign that they were dirty.
Gloves, aprons and uniforms and their symbolic reference have been previously noted
(Douglas 1966, Twigg 2000, Van Dongens 2001) with both Twigg (2000) and Van Dongen
(2001) acknowledging that gloves can symbolically say to the patient “you are
contaminated”. It was recognised by both these authors that care should be taken not to cause
offence and that workers often felt guilty about using protective clothing because of the
connotations attached to it. Twigg (2000) concluded that nurses and care workers use gloves
to protect against this pollution and this has been demonstrated here. Nevertheless, while it
has been suggested that that this protection may go beyond hygiene and may actually be more
to do with professional barriers (Douglas 1966, Twigg 2000), this was not a consistent theme
in these findings where only one participant mentioned looking professional and maintaining
professional boundaries. These findings strongly suggest that one of the prime reasons for
glove wearing was to demonstrate and provide an impression of a knowledgeable
practitioner. Participants reported being very aware of the patients’ scrutiny and recognised a
culture where patients were actively encouraged to question practice, as seen in the
‘cleanyourhands’ campaign which is still active in the UK today (DH 2004, NPSA 2008).
This campaign which consisted of posters, leaflets and badges, aimed to give both staff and
patients ownership of the campaign by making infection prevention everybody’s business.
Significantly, it encouraged patients to challenge workers to clean their hands essentially
providing constant observation to ultimately affect behaviour. Thus the desire to satisfy the
patients’ scrutiny may take precedence over rational thought and common sense. Nurses, who
inappropriately use infection prevention measures confidently, may in fact be playing a part.
By asking the audience to believe that they are someone who knows the subject and who
takes the correct actions to prevent infection spread, they are asking them to believe that they
know and understand that rationale. The individual or actor may be completely taken in by
the act or conversely may be cynical about it while still carrying it out (Goffman 1959).
Each of the participants had a presentation of themselves that they were trying to convey. In
the clinical setting this performance has a readily available audience for the majority of the
time. Their actions are constantly observed by an audience made up of patients, who are often
new and have not seen the show before, and other disciplines who may not all be convinced
by the performance (Goffman 1959). A nurse putting on apron and gloves is sending a
message, ‘I know what I am doing’, and although elements of the show are not ‘rational’
scientifically they are about creating the best impression. However not everyone within the
audience may be convinced by the performance. While those without the informing
knowledge, for example patients, may be comforted by the actions, other observers with more
accurate scientific knowledge may in fact be horrified by the behaviour. The audience is then
divided into those who believe the performance and the message it is giving, and those who
recognise the action, but are not convinced by the performance.
Limitations
It is acknowledged that self report of practice is flawed, particularly in relation to infection
prevention, as espoused practice and actual practice can differ significantly (Jenner et al
2006, Nichols and Badger 2008). However this study is strengthened by the use of vignettes
which can reduce socially desirable responses (Hughes and Huby 2002). By allowing
participants to discuss their own practice, the practice of others and the vignette character,
some of the potential for the disparity between actual and reported behaviour may have been
eradicated.
Conclusions
This paper shows that infection prevention behaviours among nurses can be understood
though a lens of social theory. Nurses make judgements about their own infection prevention
behaviours which permit deviations from policy because of post hoc rationalization whereby
the behaviour is reinterpreted in the light of motivation and intentions. Consequently the
transgression is not registered as such. Judgements about the behaviours of others differ and
others’ practice was explained as being at a superficial level and part of a show or display.
Deviations are dismissed as simply ‘irrational’. The interpretation made of both own and
other behaviours shed light onto the complex social construction of behaviour that is guided
by a framework that is removed from a purely scientific rationale. Infection prevention
behaviour was described in terms of “a show” that is performed to cast the actor in a good
light rather than to strictly comply with guidelines, echoing the classic work of Irving
Goffman.
The participants in this study demonstrated that they had the knowledge and education
required to understand the principles of infection prevention procedures, the transmission of
disease and risk of contagion. However their behaviour exists outside what is taught and
accepted as correct. It is insufficient to say that education can change this behaviour; this
behaviour (and the reasons for it) has to be recognised first and foremost by those carrying it
out before an attempt can be made to change it. This complex behaviour requires
multifaceted interventions which existing policy and guidance do not completely provide.
This study has shown that nurses are not aware of the discrepancy between their own practice
and practice which meets policy requirements; they lack insight and self awareness and while
they may recognise aspects of the ‘show’ they fail to recognise the part that they themselves
play in it. The show and the behaviours within it form part of the socially constructed reality
of the clinical area; healthcare workers see relevance to self in terms of risk to self and have
developed practices and displays which negate this. Reflection on these practices may be a
constructive way towards gaining self awareness and knowledge from experience (Schon
1987, 1991, Johns 2000 Forrest 2008). Rather as the study's participants recollected practice
they had seen, reflection allows the practitioner to recall their own actions and experiences,
thus enhancing self awareness and allowing own practice to be examined. Reflective sessions
or clinical supervision programmes (Driscoll 2000) would allow healthcare workers to firstly
express their fears regarding dirt and infection before considering whether their own
behaviour actually meets the policy requirements or is in fact not based on the scientific
rationale.
Finally this study only interviewed nurses and as other healthcare workers are involved in
infection prevention it would be useful to examine their behaviours as well. Perhaps more
significantly and it would be worth examining patients’ perception of nurses’ behaviours,
how much they observe and direct the show, and ultimately whether they are convinced by it.
References
Akyol, A., Ulusoy, H., Ozen, I., 2006. Handwashing: a simple, economical and effective
method for preventing nosocomial infections in intensive care units. Journal of Hospital
Infection 62, 395-405.
Chan, R., Alexander, M., Chan, E., Chan, V., Ho, B., Lai, C., Lam, P., Shit, F., Yiu, I., 2002.
Nurses’ knowledge of and compliance with universal precautions in an acute care hospital.
International Journal of Nursing 39,157–163.
Curtis, V., 2007. Dirt, Disgust and disease: a natural history of hygiene. Journal of
Epidemiology and Community Health 61, 660-664.
Curtis, V., Danquah L.O., Aunger, R.V., 2009. Planned, motivated and habitual behaviour: an
eleven country review. Health Education Research 24 (4), 655-673.
Department of Health, 2001a. The epic project: developing national evidence-based
guidelines for preventing healthcare associated infections. Journal of Hospital Infection 47,
Supplement S3-S82.
Department of Health, 2001b. Standard principles for preventing hospital-acquired infections.
Department of Health, London.
Department of Health, 2003. Winning Ways :Working together to reduce Healthcare
Associated Infection in England. Department of Health, London.
Department of Health, 2004. Towards cleaner hospitals and lower infection rates. Department
of Health, London.
Department of Health, 2005. Saving Lives. A delivery programme to reduce healthcare
associated infection (HCAI) including MRSA. Department of Health, London.
Department of Health, 2007. Saving Lives: reducing infection, delivering clean and safe care.
Department of Health, London.
Department of Health, 2008. Clean, safe care, reducing infections and saving lives.
Department of Health, London.
Department of Health, 2009. The Health and Social Care Act 2008. Code of Practice for
health and adult social care on the prevention and control of infections and related guidance.
Department of Health, London.
Douglas, M., 1966. Purity and danger - an analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo.
Routledge and Keegan Paul, Oxon, New York.
Driscoll, J., 2000. Practicing clinical supervision: a reflective approach. Bailliere Tindall,
Edinburgh.
Easton, P.M., Sarma, A., Williams, F.L.R., Marwick, C.A., Phillips, G., Nathwani, D., 2007.
Infection control and management of MRSA: assessing the knowledge of staff in an acute
hospital setting. Journal of Hospital Infection 66, 29-33.
Forrest, M.E.S., 2008. On becoming a critically reflective practitioner. Health Information
and Libraries Journal 25 (3), 229–232.
Goffman, E., 1959. The presentation of self in everyday life. Penguin, London.
Gould, D., 2004. Systematic observation of hand decontamination. Nursing Standard 18 (47),
39-44.
Hughes, R. J., Huby, M., 2002. The application of vignettes in social and nursing research.
Journal of Advanced Nursing 37 (4), 382-386.
Jackson, C., 2011. Unclean: a qualitative study of nurses’ reported infection control
behaviours. Unpublished research-based thesis submitted to King’s College London
For the Degree of Doctorate in Health Care.
Jenner, E.A., Watson, P.W.B., Miller, L.R., Jones, F.A., Scott, G.M., 2002. Explaining hand
hygiene practice: an extended application of the theory of planned behaviour. Psychology,
Health & Medicine 7, 311-326.
Jenner, E.A., Fletcher, B.C., Watson, P., Jones, F.A., Miller, L., Scott, G.M., 2006.
Discrepancy between self-reported and observed hand hygiene behaviour in healthcare
professionals. Journal of Hospital Infection 63 (4), 418–22.
Jervis, L.L., 2001. The pollution of incontinence and the dirty work of caregiving in a U.S.
nursing home. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 15 (1), 84-99.
Joffe, H., 1999. Risk and the other. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Johns, C., 2000. Becoming a reflective practitioner. Blackwell Science, Oxford.
Kennedy, A.M., Elward, A.M., Fraser, V.J., 2004. Survey of knowledge, beliefs and practices
of neonatal intensive care unit healthcare workers regarding nosocomial infections, central
venous catheter care and hand hygiene. Infection Control and Hospital Epidemiology 25 (9),
747-752.
Morrow, E., Griffiths, P., Rao, G., Flaxman, D., 2011. “Somebody else’s problem”: staff
attributions of cause and risk of meticillin-resistant staphylococcus aureus. American Journal
of Infection Control 39 (4), 284-91.
Morse, J. M., 1989. Strategies for sampling. In J.M. Morse (eds) Qualitative nursing research:
a contemporary dialogue. Aspen, Rockville MD.
Moscovici, S., 2000. Social representations, explorations in social psychology. Polity Press,
Oxford.
M’Tero, S.S., Sayed, M., Tyrell, D.A.J., 1981. Quantitative studies on preventing the spread
of micro-organisms in a hospital isolation unit. Journal of Hospital Infection 2, 317-328.
Naikoba, S., Hayward, A., 2001. The effectiveness of interventions aimed at increasing
handwashing in healthcare workers a systematic review. Journal of Hospital Infection 47,
173-180.
National Audit Office, 2000. The management and control of hospital acquired infection in
acute NHS trusts in England. National Audit Office, London.
National Audit Office, 2009. Reducing healthcare associated infections in hospitals in
England. National Audit Office, London.
National Patient Safety Agency, 2008. Clean your hands campaign.
http://www.npsa.nhs.uk/cleanyourhands/ accessed 27/06/2013
Nichols, A., Badger, B., 2008. An investigation of the division between espoused and actual
practice in infection control and of the knowledge sources that may underpin this division.
British Journal of Infection Control 9, 11-15.
Pittet, D., 2004. The Lowbury lecture: behaviour in infection control. Journal of
Hospital Infection 58, 1-13.
Pittet, D., 2000 Improving compliance with hand hygiene in hospital. Infection Control and
Hospital Epidemiology 21 (6), 381-386.
Ritchie, J., Spencer, L., 1994. Qualitative data analysis for applied policy research, In:
Bryman, A., Burgess, R.G. (eds) Analysing qualitative data. Routledge, London, pp 173-194.
Roth, J., 1957. Ritual and magic in the control of contagion. America Sociological Review
22, 310-4.
Schon, D. A., 1987. Educating the reflective practitioner. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco,
California.
Schon, D.A., 1991. The reflective practitioner 2nd ed. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, California.
Stein, A.D., Makarawo, T.P., Ahmad, M.F., 2003. A survey of doctors’ and nurses’ attitudes
and compliance with infection control guidelines in Birmingham teaching hospitals. Journal
of Hospital Infection 54, 68-73.
Twigg, J., 2000 Carework as a form of bodywork. Ageing and Society 20, 389-411.
Van Dongen, E., 2001. It isn’t something to yodel about, but it exists! Faeces, nurses, social
relations and status with a mental hospital. Ageing and Health 5 (3), 205-215.
Whitby, M., McLaws, M. L., Ross, R. W., 2006. Why healthcare workers don’t wash their
hands: a behavioural explanation. Infection Control and Hospital Epidemiology 27 (5), 484–
492.
World Health Organisation, 2011. Report on the Burden of Endemic Health Care-Associated
Infection Worldwide. World Health Organisation, Geneva.